The initial encoding of visual information primarily from the contralateral visual field is a fundamental organizing principle of the primate visual system. Recently, the presence of such retinotopic sensitivity has been shown to extend well beyond early visual cortex to regions not historically considered retinotopically sensitive. In particular, human scene-selective regions in parahippocampal and medial parietal cortex exhibit prominent biases for the contralateral visual field.
Keywords: contralateral, fMRI, hippocampus, retinotopy, visual field biases
Abstract
The initial encoding of visual information primarily from the contralateral visual field is a fundamental organizing principle of the primate visual system. Recently, the presence of such retinotopic sensitivity has been shown to extend well beyond early visual cortex to regions not historically considered retinotopically sensitive. In particular, human scene-selective regions in parahippocampal and medial parietal cortex exhibit prominent biases for the contralateral visual field. Here, we used fMRI to test the hypothesis that the human hippocampus, which is thought to be anatomically connected with these scene-selective regions, would also exhibit a biased representation of contralateral visual space. First, population receptive field (pRF) mapping with scene stimuli revealed strong biases for the contralateral visual field in bilateral hippocampus. Second, the distribution of retinotopic sensitivity suggested a more prominent representation in anterior medial portions of the hippocampus. Finally, the contralateral bias was confirmed in independent data taken from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) initiative. The presence of contralateral biases in the hippocampus, a structure considered by many as the apex of the visual hierarchy, highlights the truly pervasive influence of retinotopy. Moreover, this finding has important implications for understanding how visual information relates to the allocentric global spatial representations known to be encoded therein.
SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Retinotopic encoding of visual information is an organizing principle of visual cortex. Recent work demonstrates this sensitivity in structures far beyond early visual cortex, including those anatomically connected to the hippocampus. Here, using population receptive field (pRF) modeling in two independent sets of data we demonstrate a consistent bias for the contralateral visual field in bilateral hippocampus. Such a bias highlights the truly pervasive influence of retinotopy, with important implications for understanding how the presence of retinotopy relates to more allocentric spatial representations.
Introduction
The segregation of visual information processing from the two visual hemifields, with biased representation of the contralateral visual field, is a fundamental feature of the human visual system (Wandell et al., 2007). Although historically considered a feature reserved for the earliest stages of visual cortex (V1–V4), recent work highlights privileged processing of contralateral visual space throughout the brain (Kravitz et al., 2013; Silson et al., 2015). Indeed, at least 20 separate maps of the visual field have been identified throughout cortex (Wandell et al., 2007; Swisher et al., 2007) and greater contralateral sensitivity has been reported in anterior regions of ventral temporal cortex (Hemond et al., 2007; Kravitz et al., 2010; Chan et al., 2010; Huang and Sereno, 2013), the postcentral sulcus (Sereno and Huang, 2006), and the default mode network (Szinte and Knapen, 2020). Further, retinotopic maps of the contralateral visual field have been reported in the frontal eye fields (Mackey et al., 2017), the frontal lobes (Hagler and Sereno, 2006; Silver and Kastner, 2009), and even the cerebellum (Van Es et al., 2019). Given the seemingly ubiquitous influence of contralateral visual encoding, we asked whether the human hippocampus, a structure critical for long-term episodic memory (Scoville and Milner, 1957; Squire, 1992) and spatial navigation (O'Keefe and Nadal, 1978) among many other cognitive functions, also exhibits a contralateral bias for visual space.
Although at first glance, the notion of retinotopic sensitivity within the hippocampus may seem surprising, there is growing evidence to suggest that such sensitivity may nonetheless exist.
For example, the hierarchical model of visual processing proposed by Felleman and Van Essen places the hippocampus at the apex of the visual hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991). More recent non-human primate models (Kravitz et al., 2011) highlight multiple visual pathways originating in primary visual cortex that converge on the hippocampus, providing multiple routes for the feed-forward encoding of retinotopic information. Functional imaging studies have confirmed many features of this model in humans (Kravitz et al., 2011; Margulies et al., 2009; Silson et al., 2015) by demonstrating the contralateral encoding of visual field position in structures thought to be anatomically connected with the hippocampus (Margulies et al., 2009). Specifically, the scene-selective parahippocampal place area (PPA), located in parahippocampal gyrus (PHG; Epstein and Kanwisher, 1998), and medial place area (MPA), located in medial parietal cortex (Silson et al., 2016) both exhibit biases for contralateral visual space.
Beyond the retinotopic nature of inputs to the hippocampus, a handful of studies provide neurophysiological support for retinotopic sensitivity in medial temporal lobe structures. For example, visually responsive cells have been recorded from the hippocampus and neighboring structures of non-human primates (Maclean et al., 1968; Desimone and Gross, 1979), and early electrophysiological recordings from the human hippocampal formation reported a pair of units with receptive fields in the contralateral upper visual field (Wilson et al., 1983). One recent fMRI study asked whether distinct regions of the hippocampus were associated with spatial memory relating to coarse-grained locations of the visual field (Jeye et al., 2018); however, their focus was on memory, they did not perform retinotopic mapping or test for a main effect of visual field location, and a nuanced pattern of results was found.
Given the evidence for retinotopically organized input, we predicted that human hippocampus would exhibit a contralateral bias during population receptive field (pRF) mapping. We tested this prediction directly, by estimating pRFs using fMRI (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008) in a sample of individual participants (n = 27). Consistent with our predictions, a significant contralateral bias was present in bilateral hippocampus at the group level. Further, the distribution of retinotopically sensitive voxels within the hippocampus highlighted a more prominent representation in anterior and medial portions. Finally, this contralateral bias was confirmed in an independent 7.0 Tesla retinotopy dataset, collected as part of Human Connectome Project (HCP) initiative (Benson et al., 2018; Szinte and Knapen, 2020).
Materials and Methods
Participants
Twenty-nine participants completed the initial fMRI experiment (21 females, mean age = 24.2 years). All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision and gave written informed consent. The National Institutes of Health Institutional Review Board approved the consent and protocol. (A further 181 participants were included in the HCP dataset, detailed below.)
fMRI scanning parameters
Participants were scanned on a 3.0T GE Sigma MRI scanner using a 32-channel head coil in the Clinical Research Center on the National Institutes of Health campus (Bethesda, MD). Across all participants, whole-brain coverage was acquired. Slices were orientated axially, such that the most inferior slice was below the temporal lobe. All participants completed six pRF mapping runs and six runs of a six category-localizer. All functional images were acquired using a BOLD-contrast-sensitive standard EPI sequence (TE = 30 ms, TR = 2 s, flip-angle = 65°, FOV = 192 mm, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, resolution 3 × 3 × 3 mm, slice gap = 0.3 mm, 28 slices). A high-resolution T1 structural image was obtained for each participant (TE = 3.47 ms, repetition time = 2.53 s, TI = 900 ms, flip angle = 7°, 172 slices with 1 × 1 × 1 mm voxels).
Visual stimuli and tasks
pRF mapping
During pRF mapping sessions a bar aperture traversed gradually through the visual field, while revealing randomly selected scene fragments from 90 possible scenes. During each 36 s sweep, the aperture took 18 evenly spaced steps every 2 s (1 TR) to traverse the entire screen. Across the 18 aperture positions all 90 possible scene images were displayed once. A total of eight sweeps were made during each run (four orientations, two directions). Specifically, the bar aperture progressed in the following order for all runs: left to right, bottom right to top left, top to bottom, bottom left to top right, right to left, top left to bottom right, bottom to top, and top right to bottom left. The bar stimuli covered a circular aperture (diameter = 20° of visual angle). Participants performed a color detection task at fixation, indicating via button press when the white fixation dot changed to red. Color fixation changes occurred semi-randomly, with approximately two-color changes per sweep (Silson et al., 2015). Stimuli for this and the other in-scanner task were presented using PsychoPy software (Peirce, 2007; RRID:SCR_006571) from a Macbook Pro laptop (Apple Systems).
Six category functional localizer
Participants completed six functional localizer runs. During each run, color images from six stimulus categories (scenes, faces, bodies, buildings, objects, and scrambled objects) were presented at fixation (5 × 5° of visual angle) in 16-s blocks [20 images per block (300 ms per image, 500 ms blank)]. Each category was presented twice per run, with the order of presentation counterbalanced across participants and runs. Participants responded via an MRI compatible button box whenever the same image appeared sequentially.
fMRI data processing
Preprocessing
All data were analyzed using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI) software package (Cox, 1996; RRID:SCR_005927). All functions and programs are readily available in the current version: AFNI binary version April 21, 2020. Before pRF and functional localizer analyses, all images for each participant were motion corrected to the first image of the first run (3dVolreg), after removal of the appropriate “dummy” volumes (eight) to allow stabilization of the magnetic field. Postmotion-correction data were detrended (3dDetrend) and, in the case of the localizer data, smoothed with a 5-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel (3dMerge).
pRF modeling
Detailed description of the pRF model implemented in AFNI is provided elsewhere (Silson et al., 2015). Briefly, given the position of the stimulus in the visual field at every time point, the model estimates the pRF parameters that yield the best fit to the data: pRF center location (x, y), and size (diameter of the pRF). Both Simplex and Powell optimization algorithms are used simultaneously to find the best time-series/parameter sets (x, y, size) by minimizing the least-squares error of the predicted time-series with the acquired time-series for each voxel.
Six category functional localizer
Analyses were conducted using a general linear model approach and the AFNI programs 3dDeconvolve and 3dREMLfit. The data at each time point were treated as the sum of all effects thought to be present at that time point and the time series was compared against a generalized least square (GLSQ) model fit with REML estimation of the temporal autocorrelation structure. Specifically, a response model was built by convolving a standard γ function with a 16-s square wave for each condition and compared against the activation time courses using GLSQ regression. Motion parameters and four polynomials accounting for slow drifts were included as regressors of no interest. To derive the response magnitude per condition, t tests were performed between the condition-specific β estimates (normalized by the grand mean of each voxel for each run) and baseline.
Anatomical alignment
In each participant, both the pRF and functional localizer data were first de-obliqued (3dWarp) before being aligned to the individual participant's high-resolution T1-weighted anatomic scan (align_epi_anat.py). Each participant's aligned data were then inspected visually to confirm alignment accuracy. Given prior work demonstrating that the collateral sulcus (Weiner et al., 2018) and the mid-fusiform sulcus (Weiner et al., 2014) provide accurate anatomic landmarks for the peak of scene-selective PPA and face-selective fusiform face area (FFA; Kanwisher et al., 1997), the results of the contrast scenes versus faces were overlaid onto each individual participants' anatomic scan and inspected. Accurate alignment was determined using the above criteria for 27/29 participants. Subsequent analyses included only the 27 participants who met this alignment criteria.
Hippocampal definitions
For each participant, the automated hippocampal segmentation provided by the output of Freesurfer four autorecon script (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) was used as a mask for the hippocampus. In order to divide the hippocampus into anterior, middle, and posterior sections we first sorted the voxel indices by the y-axis, which codes for cortical anterior-posterior position. These indices were then separated into equal thirds and the corresponding pRF parameters were sampled for further analysis.
Visual field coverage and visual field biases
The visual field coverage plots represent the group average sensitivity of each region of interest (ROI) to different positions in the visual field. To compute these, individual participant visual field coverage plots were first derived. To compute the visual field coverage plots, the pRF of each voxel is first plotted onto a square matrix representing the visual field (1000, 1000 pixels). Once all the pRFs from a given ROI have been plotted, the max operator procedure stores the maximum value at each pixel across all the pRFs. The resulting coverage plot thus represents the maximum envelope of all the pRFs within a ROI (Winawer et al., 2010). This procedure was completed in each participant separately before pixel values were averaged across participants. To compute visual field biases in individual participants and ROIs, we calculated the mean pRF sensitivity in the ipsilateral and contralateral visual fields, respectively.
Statistical analyses
Statistics were calculated using the R Studio package (version 1.3). For our analyses, we used repeated-measures ANOVAs to examine the presence of contralateral biases in the hippocampus. For each analysis, we established initially whether the ANOVA adhered to the assumptions of sphericity using Mauchly's test. When the assumption of sphericity was violated, the degrees of freedom for that main effect or interaction were corrected using the Greenhouse–Geisser correction to allow appropriate interpretation of the F value resulting from the ANOVA.
HCP retinotopy data
To confirm the contralateral biases in the hippocampus, we turned to the 7.0 Tesla retinotopy dataset collected as part of the HCP initiative (Benson et al., 2018). This dataset comprises high-resolution retinotopic data (1.6 mm isotropic) and a large sample size (n = 181). Full descriptions of this dataset are provided elsewhere (Benson et al., 2018), but briefly, participants completed six retinotopic mapping runs (2× rotating wedge, 2× expanding ring, 2× moving bar) in which the stimulus aperture presented a dynamic color texture (comprised of objects at different scales) on a pink noise background. Participants fixated centrally and indicated via button press when the fixation dot changed color. For consistency with our individual participant analyses we sampled the averaged data for the two bar runs only. Specifically, we sampled pRFs in the hippocampus from the group-averaged data derived by first computing the average time course for each voxel across participants and, second, fitting the linear Gaussian pRF model to these group-averaged time courses using custom python-based routines. Note that the pRF modeling implementation applied to the HCP data are different from that applied to the single participant data. Preprocessing on these data were identical to that used for the previous demonstration of retinotopic sensitivity within the default mode network (Szinte and Knapen, 2020). A mask for the hippocampus was taken from the Harvard/Oxford probabilistic atlas (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/Atlases).
Results
We tested the hypothesis that the human hippocampus would exhibit a spatial bias for the contralateral visual field during visual field mapping (Fig. 1A). Such a bias would mirror not only early visual cortex, but also, more anterior regions, such as medial parietal cortex and parahippocampal cortex that provide input to the hippocampus both directly and indirectly.
Biased representation of contralateral space in the hippocampus
Initially, we computed visual field coverage plots in each participant and ROI (left hippocampus, right hippocampus) from all suprathreshold pRFs (R2 > 0.1), before averaging these coverage plots across participants. These visual field coverage plots represent schematic visualizations of the sensitivity of a given brain region to different positions in the visual field, built by combining the best Gaussian receptive field model (position, size, and explained variance) for each voxel within an ROI. In our analyses, a max operator is used. This creates a coverage plot that reflects, at each point in the visual field, the maximum sensitivity (which we refer to as pRF value) from all of the receptive field models within an ROI (min = 0, max = 1) Thus, the coverage plot reflects the maximum envelope of all the pRFs.
The group average visual field coverage plots for the left and right hippocampus (Fig. 1B) demonstrate a striking contralateral bias for both hemispheres, respectively. From the average coverage plots alone, there is no clear evidence of any quadrant biases but note the numerically higher percentages of pRF centers in the upper visual field (Fig. 1B, inset).
To quantify these contralateral biases, we calculated the mean pRF value (see above) in the ipsilateral and contralateral visual field in each participant and ROI, respectively, and submitted these to a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with hemisphere (left, right) and visual field (ipsilateral, contralateral) as within-participant factors. The main effects of hemisphere (F(1,26) = 6.98, p = 0.02, partial η2 = 0.06) and visual field (F(1,26) = 21.44, p = 8.89−5, partial η2 = 0.07) were significant, reflecting on average larger pRF values in the right over left hemisphere and the contralateral over ipsilateral visual field, respectively. The hemisphere by visual field interaction was not significant (p > 0.05). A series of paired t tests confirmed a significant contralateral bias in both the left (t(26) = 2.50, p = 0.01) and right (t(26) = 3.22, p = 0.003) hippocampus (Fig. 1C). We examined the possibility that the larger pRF values in the right hemisphere may be because of sampling larger numbers of pRFs. Although on average a larger number of suprathreshold pRFs were sampled in the right hippocampus this difference was not found to be significant (paired t test across participants t(26) = 1.88, p = 0.06). Additional analyses were also conducted using a higher threshold for the pRF explained variance (R2 > 0.2). Despite resulting in fewer included participants (Left hemisphere (LH): n = 11; Right hemisphere (RH): n = 17), a series of paired t tests revealed a significant contralateral bias in both hemispheres (LH t(10) = 2.50, p = 0.03; RH t(16) = 4.49, p = 0.0003).
Retinotopic sensitivity and scene-selectivity in the hippocampus
Given prior work suggesting a place for the hippocampus in the scene-processing network (Maguire and Mullally, 2013; Hodgetts et al., 2016), we next sought to establish the relationship between the strength of retinotopic encoding (variance explained by the pRF model) and the degree of scene-selectivity within the hippocampus. In each participant and hemisphere, we calculated the correlation (Pearson's) between the variance explained by each voxel's pRF fit and that voxel's corresponding index of scene-selectivity (t value of the contrast scenes versus faces in a separate localizer task), before averaging correlation coefficients across participants. On average, a positive correlation was observed in each hemisphere, suggesting that the more retinotopically sensitive a voxel, the more scene-selective also (Fig. 2A). A series of t tests versus zero (i.e., no correlation) confirmed the significant positive correlation at the group level in both hemispheres (LH: t(26) = 3.88, p = 0.002, RH: t(26) = 3.23, p = 0.003).
Distribution of retinotopic sensitivity within the hippocampus
Prior work suggests functional differences throughout the hippocampus, and a particularly common finding has been scene-selective responses in the medial (rather than lateral) aspect of anterior hippocampus (Zeidman and Maguire, 2016). To explore the spatial distribution of retinotopic sensitivity within the hippocampus, we sorted the voxel indices of each hippocampus first by the x-axis, which codes for left-right, and then by the y-axis, which codes for anterior-posterior within the brain. Next, we computed the correlation (Pearson's) between each voxel's position along that axis and the strength of retinotopic encoding (pRF explained variance), before averaging correlation coefficients across participants and testing against zero (i.e., no correlation; Fig. 2B). In both hemispheres, there was a significant correlation between absolute x-position and retinotopic sensitivity (LH: t(26) = 2.41, p = 0.02, RH: t(26) = 2.26, p = 0.03), reflecting better pRF model fits medially, as well as, significant negative correlations between y-position and retinotopic sensitivity, reflecting greater explained variance anteriorly (LH: t(26) = 7.52, p = 5.42−8, RH: t(26) = 7.91, 2.17−8).
We next sought to establish whether a contralateral bias would be present in subsections of the hippocampus. Accordingly, we divided each participant's hippocampus into equal thirds along the y-axis (see Materials and Methods). These were subsequently labeled as anterior, middle, and posterior sections (Fig. 2C). The group average visual field coverage plots for each section are depicted for the left hippocampus (Fig. 2D) and right hippocampus (Fig. 2E). At the group level, a clear contralateral bias is evident in the anterior and middle sections of both hemispheres, whereas the posterior sections exhibit no such bias.
To quantify these biases, we computed the mean pRF value in both the ipsilateral and contralateral visual fields in each individual participant and ROI. These values were submitted to a three-way repeated-measures ANOVA with hemisphere (left, right), section (anterior, middle, posterior), and visual field (ipsilateral, contralateral) as within-participant factors. The main effects of hemisphere (F(1,26) = 8.75, p = 0.006, partial η2 = 0.05), section (F(2,52) = 23.49, p = 5.38−8, partial η2 = 0.16), and visual field (F(1,26) = 20.14, p = 0.0001, partial η2 = 0.02), were significant, reflecting on average larger pRF values in the right hemisphere, in anterior and middle over posterior sections and in the contralateral over ipsilateral visual field, respectively. Only the section by visual field interaction (F(2,52) = 5.75, p = 0.01, partial η2 = 0.008, GG-corrected) was significant. All other interactions were not significant (p > 0.05, in all cases).
To explore this further, we conducted a series of two-way ANOVAs with section and visual field as factors in each hemisphere separately. In the left hemisphere, only the main effect of section (F(2,52) = 25.58, p = 1.83–8, partial η2 = 0.21) was significant (p > 0.05, in all other cases). A series of paired t tests revealed a significant contralateral bias in the middle (t(26) = 1.96, p = 0.02), but not the anterior (t(26) = 1.61, p = 0.10) or posterior sections (t(26) = 0.14, p = 0.44), although note the numerically larger contralateral bias in the anterior section (Fig. 2F). In the right hemisphere, both the main effects of section F(2,52) = 11.28, p = 8.51−5, partial η2 = 0.12) and visual field (F(2,52) = 9.99, p = 0.003, partial η2 = 0.04) were significant, as was their interaction (F(2,52) = 5.52, p = 0.01, partial η2 = 0.01, GG-corrected). Again, a series of paired t tests revealed significant contralateral biases in both the anterior (t(26) = 4.00, p = 0.0004) and middle (t(26) = 2.88, p = 0.007) but not the posterior section (t(26) = 0.96, p = 0.34; Fig. 2G).
Reduced signal posteriorly could explain lack of contralateral bias
While the data suggest that the strength of retinotopic sensitivity is reduced more posteriorly in the hippocampus, it is important to consider the impact of signal strength on these patterns of results. First, we calculated the temporal signal-to-noise (tSNR) of the pRF runs for each participant. Next, we computed the median tSNR values in each section of the hippocampus and submitted these values to a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with hemisphere and section as factors (same levels as above). The main effect of section was significant (F(2,52) = 110.54, p = 1.54−13, partial η2 = 0.40, GG-corrected), reflecting larger tSNR values more anteriorly, whereas the main effect of hemisphere and the hemisphere by section interaction were not significant (p > 0.05 in both cases). Given the non-significant effect of hemisphere, tSNR values were averaged across hemispheres before being submitted to a one-way ANOVA with section as the only factor. The main effect of section was significant (F(2,52) = 110.54, p = 1.54−13, partial η2 = 0.43, GG-corrected). A series of paired t tests confirmed that tSNR decreased significantly from anterior to posterior in the hippocampus (anterior vs middle: t(26) = 10.56, p = 6.65−11; anterior vs posterior: t(26) = 11.32, p = 1.49−11; middle vs posterior: t(26) = 8.16, p = 1.19−8).
Contralateral bias in hippocampus not because of spillover from either lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) or PHG
The hippocampus is located lateral and ventral of the LGN, which is known to contain a retinotopic organization. We thus wanted to rule out the possibility that the contralateral biases observed in the hippocampus were not because of spillover from the LGN. To rule out this possibility we inspected the location of our hippocampal responses with respect to those in the LGN. Figure 3 demonstrates this relationship for a representative participant. While robust recruitment of the LGN is evident, such recruitment is clearly posterior of the hippocampal recruitment, which was consistently strongest in anterior medial portions.
The hippocampus is also located anterior and dorsal of the PHG. Prior work from our group and others has demonstrated the strong influence of retinotopy in the PHG and in the PPA in particular. Given the known proximity between the PHG and the hippocampus we sought to rule out the possibility that these retinotopically sensitive responses measured within the hippocampus were because of spillover of responses from PHG. In each participant, we examined the responses within the hippocampus with respect to those measured from PHG. The explained variance of the pRF model for a representative participant is shown in Figure 4A. While robust fits to the pRF model are evident in early visual cortex, extending anteriorly into ventral temporal cortex and encompassing the PPA, two small clusters of suprathreshold voxels are also evident within the hippocampus. These clusters, particularly the more anterior cluster, are spatially separated from responses in ventral temporal cortex and are unlikely to reflect spillover from PHG. Both clusters exhibit pRF centers located well within the contralateral visual field Figure 4B.
Replication of contralateral bias in a high-resolution independent dataset
Our individual participant analyses demonstrate that, when considered as a single structure, the human hippocampus exhibits a significant bias for contralateral visual space when measured through pRF mapping. We next sought to confirm these findings in independent data by taking advantage of the large sample (n = 181) and high-resolution (1.6 mm isotropic) 7.0 Tesla retinotopy data collected as part of the HCP initiative (Benson et al., 2018).
Using the group average pRF fitted data (from the bar runs only), we sampled pRF parameters (R2, x-position, eccentricity and pRF size) from a mask of the hippocampus. Enlarged views of the hippocampus with each pRF parameter overlaid in false color are shown in Figure 5. Many of the features present in the individual participant data are also evident here, despite these data being acquired across different scanners, field strengths, resolutions, and visual stimulus setups, while also being analyzed using different processing pipelines. These data demonstrate (1) that voxels are fit well by the pRF model throughout the hippocampus, with clear clusters evident in anterior medial sections (Fig. 5A,B) hippocampal pRFs exhibit largely contralateral visual field centers (Fig. 5B,C) pRFs are relatively eccentric with few representing the fovea and (2) pRFs range is size but with very few small pRFs. For completeness, we calculated the visual field coverage in each hemisphere from all supratheshold pRFs (R2 > 0.1) from the HCP data. In both hemispheres, a clear contralateral bias is evident (Fig. 5E). Again, there is no clear evidence for any quadrant biases but note that unlike our individual participant analyses the HCP data contains a higher percentage of lower visual field centers (percentage of pRF centers inset). These data complement the individual participant analyses reported above and highlight the contralateral bias exhibited by the human hippocampus during visual field mapping.
Discussion
Here, using pRF data from two independent sources we demonstrate a consistent contralateral bias in the human hippocampus during visual field mapping. These data demonstrate that the influence of retinotopy is present and measurable even at the very highest level of the visual hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991; Kravitz et al., 2011, 2013) and suggest that retinotopy be considered as a visuospatial representation that is available to the hippocampus.
Anatomical connectivity with the hippocampus implies retinotopic sensitivity
A contralateral bias of visual space was implied by direct and indirect connections between the hippocampus and antecedent regions of the visual hierarchy. Tract-tracing studies in non-human primates positioned the hippocampus at the highest level in the visual hierarchy (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991). The regions with which it is connected are responsive to visual stimuli, with early neurophysiological studies identifying visually responsive units in parahippocampal structures in non-human primates (Maclean et al., 1968; Desimone and Gross, 1979) and humans (Wilson et al., 1983). More recently, functional neuroimaging has demonstrated contralateral pRFs in multiple regions thought to connect directly and/or indirectly with the hippocampus (Silson et al., 2015). Specifically, contralateral biases have been reported in scene-selective PPA, OPA, and MPA, located on the ventral, lateral, and medial surfaces, respectively (Silson et al., 2015, 2016).
We found that retinotopic pRFs were detectable in the human hippocampus, lateralized to each contralateral hemisphere, using two independent datasets with distinct stimuli. We did not find any evidence for a systematic mapping of visual space in the hippocampus, a hallmark of early visual cortex. However, the absence of a retinotopic map should not imply the absence of retinotopic sensitivity. Indeed, prior work from our group (Silson et al., 2015, 2016) and others (Elshout et al., 2018) has demonstrated robust and reliable retinotopically driven responses in occipitotemporal and medial parietal cortices without clear evidence for accompanying retinotopic maps. Moreover, this could be because of technical limitations: given the organizational scale of the hippocampus relative to current fMRI voxel sizes it is possible that finding map-like organization in hippocampus requires using even smaller fMRI voxels. The coarse representation of contralateral visual space reported here is consistent with a very recent study employing ultra-high resolution and connective field modeling to demonstrate fine-grained visuotopic connectivity between V1 and the hippocampus (Knapen, 2021). The question of whether the contralateral biases reported here (and elsewhere; Knapen, 2021) reflect retinotopic inputs into the hippocampus or retinotopic neurons within the hippocampus itself cannot be answered by the current fMRI data but remains an important and open question for future research.
Distribution of retinotopic sensitivity across the hippocampus
Studies of visual scene perception and discrimination have highlighted the potentially key role played by the anterior medial portion of the hippocampus (Hodgetts et al., 2016; Zeidman and Maguire, 2016). Our results were consistent with this. Not only did we observe, on average, a significant positive correlation between retinotopic sensitivity and medial, lateral position within the hippocampus, but also, a significant negative correlation between retinotopic sensitivity and anterior-posterior position. Subsequent analyses of separate hippocampal sections also suggested more prominent retinotopic sensitivity anteriorly, but these are to be interpreted with caution as follow-up analyses also revealed that tSNR drops systematically in more posterior regions.
Our data demonstrated a significant positive relationship between retinotopic sensitivity and scene-selectivity, suggesting that the well-established preferential response of the hippocampus to scene stimuli involves processing in retinotopic space. Interestingly, similar positive relationships between scene-selectivity and retinotopy have been reported within scene-selective MPA in medial parietal cortex (Silson et al., 2016), which is thought to provide input to the hippocampus (Margulies et al., 2009; Kravitz et al., 2011).
Visuospatial encoding in the hippocampus
What information might the hippocampus be encoding or processing? The hippocampus directly encodes an animal's spatial location in an allocentric (world-centered) reference frame (O'Keefe and Dostrovsky, 1971). Visual input contributes to the formation of these representations (Chen et al., 2013), and indeed, recent findings have demonstrated that neuronal populations in both CA1 and V1 encode the rodent's subjective estimate of its position along a linear track (Saleem et al., 2018). However, to our knowledge, a contralateral visual field bias has never been identified in the rodent hippocampus. Mice have large visual fields, with retinotopic organization having been identified in both striate and extra-striate cortex (Dräger, 1975; Zhuang et al., 2017). Given that entorhinal cortex has direct connections with retinotopically organized regions including V1 (Wang et al., 2011; Morimoto et al., 2020), we hypothesize that a contralateral visual field bias exists in the population-level activity of rodent hippocampus. This could be investigated using recently developed tools for presenting visual stimuli to head-fixed rodents (Dombeck et al., 2010), to disambiguate place cell (allocentric or spatiotopic) responses from retinotopic responses.
There is increasing evidence that primate hippocampus and entorhinal cortex encode not only physical location, but also visual space in multiple reference frames (Zeidman and Maguire, 2016; Meister, 2018; Nau et al., 2018a; Rolls and Wirth, 2018). In brief (Meister and Buffalo, 2018), primate spatial view cells were found to encode positions on a video screen, or the position of the video screen in the room (Feigenbaum and Rolls, 1991; Georges-François et al. 1999). More recently, entorhinal grid cells (Hafting et al., 2005) have been found to have firing fields covering gaze direction or visual space in non-human primates (Killian et al., 2012; Wilming et al., 2018) and in humans (Julian et al., 2018; Nau et al., 2018b). Our results demonstrate that retinotopy complements these other visuospatial representations in the hippocampus.
Functional significance of multiple visuospatial representations
What functions might be served by the presence of multiple visuospatial representations in the hippocampus? Insights may be gained from neuropsychological studies on patients with specific lesions to the hippocampus. Such patients have been found to be impaired at discriminating images of similar three-dimensional scenes, or scenes from different viewpoints (Lee et al., 2005a,b; Baxter, 2009; Suzuki, 2009; Aly et al., 2013) and they are impaired at extrapolating beyond the view (Mullally et al., 2012). Thus, the hippocampus may be required for complex visual tasks, which require forming an internal representation or model of the stimuli. Our results suggest this may be subserved by conjunctive retinotopic and allocentric representations in the hippocampus.
Neuropsychological theories have been proposed to explain these findings in patients. In particular, scene construction theory (Hassabis and Maguire, 2007) proposes that the hippocampus and connected regions form internal models of scenes, facilitating cognitive functions including vision, navigation, imagination, and episodic memory (Zeidman and Maguire, 2016). Under this account, the hippocampus could be considered a node in the scene-processing network (Maguire and Mullally, 2013; Hodgetts et al., 2016), that is functionally connected to antecedent scene-selective regions (Margulies et al., 2009; Silson et al., 2016) and these regions exhibit prominent biases for contralateral visual space (Silson et al., 2015). Thus, the left hippocampus may contribute information from the right visual field to the formation of a scene representation, and vice versa. Our initial pRF modeling employed scene stimuli whereby multiple scene fragments were presented at each location. While this paradigm was used to try and prevent participants from mentally “filling-in” the scenes, it is possible that scene fragments were namable and generated internal representations. On the other hand, the stimulus employed under the HCP initiative (Benson et al., 2018) could be considered far more abstract (objects at multiple scales on a pink-noise background).
An alternative perspective on hemifield-specific responses recognizes that the hippocampus guides behavior, and this behavior may include eye movements. The level of hippocampus activity has been found to correlate with the number of fixations when novel face images are presented, suggesting a role for the hippocampus in sampling information (Liu et al., 2017). A recent proposal, the spatiotemporal similarity hypothesis, explains this by suggesting that that the hippocampus represents stimuli that co-occur in space and time, and it uses these joint representations to generate visual predictions and guide eye movements (Turk-Browne, 2019). Predictive coding is a computational framework which formalizes these notions and comes in multiple forms. Particularly relevant is active inference (Friston et al., 2015), which treats the brain as a deep hierarchical forward model that predicts sensory information and infers the causes of sensations by taking actions (such as sampling new information). Under this account, the purpose of a visual saccade is to test a hypothesis (i.e., reduce uncertainty) about what might be “out there” beyond the current view (Parr and Friston, 2018). The contribution of the hippocampus is proposed to be encoding transitions between discrete states, such as sequences of eye gaze positions (Mirza et al., 2016). Our results might suggest that left hippocampus encodes potential sequences of eye movements related to the right visual field, and vice versa (although in the tasks we present here, any such motor plans could not be enacted, as subjects were required to fixate centrally).
Finally, the hippocampus may also encode temporal regularities, sequences or transition probabilities in the environment (Kumaran and Maguire, 2006; Garvert et al., 2017; Stachenfeld et al., 2017). The pRF stimuli were highly predictable, traversing gradually on a predetermined trajectory through the visual field. It is therefore possible that the responses were elicited by predictions related to the sequence of stimuli in the contralateral visual field. An interesting future experiment could test this hypothesis by manipulating the predictability of the retinotopic mapping stimuli and measuring its impact on the contralateral biases measured as a result.
Taken together, our data highlight that retinotopic sensitivity, and the contralateral encoding of visual information in particular, is present even at the level of the human hippocampus. Whether such sensitivity reflects retinotopic input or the activity of retinotopic neurons in the hippocampus remains unclear. Likewise, how the hippocampus incorporates this retinotopic information with the allocentric and global spatial representations that the hippocampus supports is an important goal of future work, but it is possible that such a representation provides a means for the hippocampus to compare ongoing sensory inputs with past events. Indeed, the seemingly ubiquitous encoding of retinotopic information within brain regions that subserve divergent functions suggests the brain may use retinotopy as a means to facilitate neural communication.
Footnotes
This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institutes of Health, National Institutes of Mental Health (ZIAMH-002909). We thank Matthias Nau for thoughtful comments on the manuscript.
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
References
- Aly M, Ranganath C, Yonelinas AP (2013) Detecting changes in scenes: the hippocampus is critical for strength-based perception. Neuron 78:1127–1137. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.04.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baxter MG (2009) Involvement of medial temporal lobe structures in memory and perception. Neuron 61:667–677. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benson NC, Jamison KW, Arcaro MJ, Vu AT, Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Van Essen DC, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Winawer J, Kay K (2018) The Human Connectome Project 7 Tesla retinotopy dataset: description and population receptive field analysis. J Vis 18:23. 10.1167/18.13.23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan AW, Kravitz DJ, Truong S, Arizpe J, Baker CI (2010) Cortical representations of bodies and faces are strongest in commonly experienced configurations. Nat Neurosci 13:417–418. 10.1038/nn.2502 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen G, King JA, Burgess N, O'Keefe J (2013) How vision and movement combine in the hippocampal place code. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:378–383. 10.1073/pnas.1215834110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox RW (1996) AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29:162–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desimone R, Gross CG (1979) Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque. Brain Res 178:363–380. 10.1016/0006-8993(79)90699-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dombeck DA, Harvey CD, Tian L, Looger LL, Tank DW (2010) Functional imaging of hippocampal place cells at cellular resolution during virtual navigation. Nat Neurosci 13:1433–1440. 10.1038/nn.2648 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dräger UC (1975) Receptive fields of single cells and topography in mouse visual cortex. J Comp Neurol 160:269–289. 10.1002/cne.901600302 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumoulin SO, Wandell BA (2008) Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex. Neuroimage 39:647–660. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elshout JA, van den Berg AV, Haak KV (2018) Human V2A: a map of the peripheral visual hemifield with functional connections to scene-selective cortex. J Vis 18:22–22. 10.1167/18.9.22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Epstein R, Kanwisher N (1998) A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392:598–601. 10.1038/33402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1:1–47. 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feigenbaum JD, Rolls ET (1991) Allocentric and egocentric spatial information processing in the hippocampal formation of the behaving primate. Psychobiology 19:21–40. [Google Scholar]
- Friston K, Rigoli F, Ognibene D, Mathys C, Fitzgerald T, Pezzulo G (2015) Active inference and epistemic value. Cogn Neurosci 6:187–214. 10.1080/17588928.2015.1020053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garvert MM, Dolan RJ, Behrens TE (2017) A map of abstract relational knowledge in the human hippocampal–entorhinal cortex. Elife 6:e17086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Georges-François P, Rolls ET, Robertson RG (1999) Spatial view cells in the primate hippocampus: allocentric view not head direction or eye position or place. Cereb Cortex 9:197–212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hagler DJ Jr, Sereno MI (2006) Spatial maps in frontal and prefrontal cortex. Neuroimage 29:567–577. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.08.058 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser M-B, Moser EI (2005) Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature 436:801–806. 10.1038/nature03721 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hassabis D, Maguire EA (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn Sci 11:299–306. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hemond CC, Kanwisher NG, De Beeck HPO (2007) A preference for contralateral stimuli in human object-and face-selective cortex. PLoS One 2:e574. 10.1371/journal.pone.0000574 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodgetts CJ, Shine JP, Lawrence AD, Downing PE, Graham KS (2016) Evidencing a place for the hippocampus within the core scene processing network. Hum Brain Mapp 37:3779–3379. 10.1002/hbm.23275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang RS, Sereno MI (2013) Bottom-up retinotopic organization supports top-down mental imagery. Open Neuroimag J 7:58–67. 10.2174/1874440001307010058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeye BM, MacEvoy SP, Karanian JM, Slotnick SD (2018) Distinct regions of the hippocampus are associated with memory for different spatial locations. Brain Res 1687:41–49. 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.02.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Julian JB, Keinath AT, Frazzetta G, Epstein RA (2018) Human entorhinal cortex represents visual space using a boundary-anchored grid. Nat Neurosci 21:191–194. 10.1038/s41593-017-0049-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM (1997) The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17:4302–4311. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killian NJ, Jutras MJ, Buffalo EA (2012) A map of visual space in the primate entorhinal cortex. Nature 491:761–764. 10.1038/nature11587 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knapen T (2021) Topographic connectivity reveals task-dependent retinotopic processing throughout the human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 118:e2017032118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz DJ, Kriegeskorte N, Baker CI (2010) High-level visual object representations are constrained by position. Cereb Cortex 20:2916–2925. 10.1093/cercor/bhq042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz DJ, Saleem KS, Baker CI, Mishkin M (2011) A new neural framework for visuospatial processing. Nat Rev Neurosci 12:217–230. 10.1038/nrn3008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kravitz DJ, Saleem KS, Baker CI, Ungerleider LG, Mishkin M (2013) The ventral visual pathway: an expanded neural framework for the processing of object quality. Trends Cogn Sci 17:26–49. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumaran D, Maguire EA (2006) An unexpected sequence of events: mismatch detection in the human hippocampus. PLoS Biol 4:e424. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0040424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee ACH, Buckley MJ, Pegman SJ, Spiers H, Scahill VL, Gaffan D, Bussey TJ, Davies RR, Kapur N, Hodges JR, Graham KS (2005a) Specialization in the medial temporal lobe for processing of objects and scenes. Hippocampus 15:782–797. 10.1002/hipo.20101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee ACH, Bussey TJ, Murray EA, Saksida LM, Epstein RA, Kapur N, Hodges JR, Graham KS (2005b) Perceptual deficits in amnesia: challenging the medial temporal lobe 'mnemonic' view. Neuropsychologia 43:1–11. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu ZX, Shen K, Olsen RK, Ryan JD (2017) Visual sampling predicts hippocampal activity. J Neurosci 37:599–609. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2610-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacLean PD, Yokota T, Kinnard MA (1968) Photically sustained on-responses of units in posterior hippocampal gyrus of awake monkey. J Neurophysiol 31:870–883. 10.1152/jn.1968.31.6.870 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackey WE, Winawer J, Curtis CE (2017) Visual field map clusters in human frontoparietal cortex. Elife 6:e22974. 10.7554/eLife.22974 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maguire EA, Mullally SL (2013) The hippocampus: a manifesto for change. J Exp Psychol Gen 142:1180–1189. 10.1037/a0033650 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Villringer A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Petrides M (2009) Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:20069–20074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meister M (2018) Memory system neurons represent gaze position and the visual world. J Exp Neurosci 12:1179069518787484. 10.1177/1179069518787484 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meister ML, Buffalo EA (2018) Neurons in primate entorhinal cortex represent gaze position in multiple spatial reference frames. J Neurosci 38:2430–2441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mirza MB, Adams RA, Mathys CD, Friston KJ (2016) Scene construction, visual foraging, and active inference. Front Comput Neurosci 10:56. 10.3389/fncom.2016.00056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morimoto MM, Uchishiba E, Saleem AB (2020) Topographic organization of feedback projections to mouse primary visual cortex. BioRxiv 198440. doi: 10.1101/2020.07.12.198440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullally SL, Intraub H, Maguire EA (2012) Attenuated boundary extension produces a paradoxical memory advantage in amnesic patients. Curr Biol 22:261–268. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nau M, Julian JB, Doeller CF (2018a) How the brain's navigation system shapes our visual experience. Trends Cogn Sci 22:810–825. 10.1016/j.tics.2018.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nau M, Schröder TN, Bellmund JL, Doeller CF (2018b) Hexadirectional coding of visual space in human entorhinal cortex. Nat Neurosci 21:188–190. 10.1038/s41593-017-0050-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J, Dostrovsky J (1971) The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res 34:171–175. 10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O'Keefe J, Nadal L (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Oxford University. [Google Scholar]
- Parr T, Friston KJ (2018) The discrete and continuous brain: from decisions to movement—and back again. Neural Comput 30:2319–2347. 10.1162/neco_a_01102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls ET, Wirth S (2018) Spatial representations in the primate hippocampus, and their functions in memory and navigation. Prog Neurobiol 171:90–113. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2018.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleem AB, Diamanti EM, Fournier J, Harris KD, Carandini M (2018) Coherent encoding of subjective spatial position in visual cortex and hippocampus. Nature 562:124–127. 10.1038/s41586-018-0516-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scoville WB, Milner B (1957) Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 20:11–21. 10.1136/jnnp.20.1.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno MI, Huang RS (2006) A human parietal face area contains aligned head-centered visual and tactile maps. Nat Neurosci 9:1337–1343. 10.1038/nn1777 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silson EH, Chan AWY, Reynolds RC, Kravitz DJ, Baker CI (2015) A retinotopic basis for the division of high-level scene processing between lateral and ventral human occipitotemporal cortex. J Neurosci 35:11921–11935. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0137-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silson EH, Steel AD, Baker CI (2016) Scene-selectivity and retinotopy in medial parietal cortex. Front Hum Neurosci 10:412. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silver MA, Kastner S (2009) Topographic maps in human frontal and parietal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 13:488–495. 10.1016/j.tics.2009.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Squire LR (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 99:195–231. 10.1037/0033-295x.99.2.195 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stachenfeld KL, Botvinick MM, Gershman SJ (2017) The hippocampus as a predictive map. Nat Neurosci 20:1643–1653. 10.1038/nn.4650 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki WA (2009) Perception and the medial temporal lobe: evaluating the current evidence. Neuron 61:657–666. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.02.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swisher JD, Halko MA, Merabet LB, McMains SA, Somers DC (2007) Visual topography of human intraparietal sulcus. J Neurosci 27:5326–5337. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0991-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szinte M, Knapen T (2020) Visual organization of the default network. Cereb Cortex 30:3518–3527. 10.1093/cercor/bhz323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turk-Browne NB (2019) The hippocampus as a visual area organized by space and time: a spatiotemporal similarity hypothesis. Vision Res 165:123–130. 10.1016/j.visres.2019.10.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Es DM, Van Der Zwaag W, Knapen T (2019) Topographic maps of visual space in the human cerebellum. Curr Biol 29:1689–1694. 10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA (2007) Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56:366–383. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Q, Gao E, Burkhalter A (2011) Gateways of ventral and dorsal streams in mouse visual cortex. J Neurosci 31:1905–1918. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3488-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Golarai G, Caspers J, Chuapoco MR, Mohlberg H, Zilles K, Amunts K, Grill-Spector K (2014) The mid-fusiform sulcus: a landmark identifying both cytoarchitectonic and functional divisions of human ventral temporal cortex. Neuroimage 84:453–465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiner KS, Barnett MA, Witthoft N, Golarai G, Stigliani A, Kay KN, Gomez J, Natu VS, Amunts K, Zilles K, Grill-Spector K (2018) Defining the most probable location of the parahippocampal place area using cortex-based alignment and cross-validation. Neuroimage 170:373–384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winawer J, Horiguchi H, Sayres RA, Amano K, Wandell BA (2010) Mapping hV4 and ventral occipital cortex: the venous eclipse. J Vision 10:1–1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilming N, König P, König S, Buffalo EA (2018) Entorhinal cortex receptive fields are modulated by spatial attention, even without movement. Elife 7:e31745. 10.7554/eLife.31745 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson CL, Babb TL, Halgren E, Crandall PH (1983) Visual receptive fields and response properties of neurons in human temporal lobe and visual pathways. Brain 106:473–502. 10.1093/brain/106.2.473 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeidman P, Maguire EA (2016) Anterior hippocampus: the anatomy of perception, imagination and episodic memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 17:173–182. 10.1038/nrn.2015.24 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhuang J, Ng L, Williams D, Valley M, Li Y, Garrett M, Waters J (2017) An extended retinotopic map of mouse cortex. Elife 6:e18372. 10.7554/eLife.18372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]