Abstract
Phenotypic plasticity can either hinder or promote adaptation to novel environments. Recent studies that have quantified alignments between plasticity, genetic variation, and divergence propose that such alignments may reflect constraints that bias future evolutionary trajectories. Here, we emphasize that such alignments may themselves be a result of natural selection and do not necessarily indicate constraints on adaptation. We estimated developmental plasticity and broad sense genetic covariance matrices (G) among damselfly populations situated along a latitudinal gradient in Europe. Damselflies were reared at photoperiod treatments that simulated the seasonal time constraints experienced at northern (strong constraints) and southern (relaxed constraints) latitudes. This allowed us to partition the effects of (1) latitude, (2) photoperiod, and (3) environmental novelty on G and its putative alignment with adaptive plasticity and divergence. Environmental novelty and latitude did not affect G, but photoperiod did. Photoperiod increased evolvability in the direction of observed adaptive divergence and developmental plasticity when G was assessed under strong seasonal time constraints at northern (relative to southern) photoperiod. Because selection and adaptation under time constraints is well understood in Lestes damselflies, our results suggest that natural selection can shape the alignment between divergence, plasticity, and evolvability.
Keywords: Adaptation, developmental bias, G‐matrix, genetic constraints, latitude, life history, phenotypic plasticity, time constraints
Whether phenotypic plasticity hinders or facilitates adaptation in new environments has been debated, and recently interest in this issue has intensified due to the increasing need to understand species’ responses to global environmental change (Whitlock 1996; Price et al. 2003; West‐Eberhard 2003; Ghalambor et al. 2007; Lande 2009; Chevin et al. 2010; Walters et al. 2012; Levis and Pfennig 2016; Uller et al. 2018; Levis and Pfennig 2019; Noble et al. 2019). One argument is that, because phenotypic plasticity can rapidly generate phenotypes that are better matched to novel environmental settings, plasticity can allow organisms to survive the initial stages of change, leaving an opportunity for subsequent genetic adaptation through natural selection (Waddington 1953; West‐Eberhard 2003; Lande 2009; Levis and Pfennig 2016). This mechanism, in turn, might have important implications for rates of genetic diversification (Gomez‐Mestre and Buchholz 2006; Pfennig et al. 2010; Susoy et al. 2015). The potential for plasticity to facilitate adaptation in new environments should thus depend on whether plastic responses move the phenotype closer to the new adaptive peak and whether there is additive genetic variation allowing traits to respond in the direction of selection. Adaptive evolution is therefore predicted to be further facilitated, if both genetic variation and phenotypic plasticity are aligned with the direction of multivariate selection generated by the environmental change (Lande 2009, but see Whitlock 1996; Walters et al. 2012).
Against this background, a prominent debate has surfaced about causation in the evolutionary process and why such an alignment might be expected in the first place (e.g., Gerhart and Kirschner 2007; Laland et al. 2015; Houle et al. 2017; McGlothlin et al. 2018; Uller et al. 2018; Svensson and Berger 2019; Jiang and Zhang 2020). Classic interpretations of an alignment between genetic variation and divergence typically invoke genetic constraints, suggesting that adaptation is constrained to occur along dimensions for which there is additive genetic variation (Lande 1979; Schluter 1996). Similarly, developmental plasticity is argued to play a leading role in evolution by biasing the evolutionary trajectory along certain developmental pathways predetermined by persistent selection during an organism's evolutionary history (West‐Eberhard 2003): a phenomenon often referred to as “developmental bias” (Uller et al. 2018). Indeed, the common influence of developmental bias during the course of evolution might be inferred from the fact that development tends to produce certain phenotypes more often than others (Uller et al. 2018). However, it is also possible that an alignment between developmental plasticity, genetic variation, and divergence could be created over relatively short time scales by contemporary natural selection shaping all three levels of biological variation simultaneously (Cheverud 1984; Schluter 1996; Houle et al. 2017; Noble et al. 2019; Svensson and Berger 2019). Hence, such an alignment does not need to reflect constraints on adaptation. Here, correlational selection can canalize the effects of both genetic (de novo mutations or segregating variation) and environmental perturbation on phenotypic expression, causing favorable trait combinations to occur and be selected more frequently than expected by chance. Indeed, recent theory suggests that such effects can accumulate within a few hundred generations (Draghi and Whitlock 2012; Jones et al. 2014). Alignments between developmental plasticity, genetic variation, and divergence may thus have dual causality. Determining the extent to which such alignments are a consequence of genetic/developmental constraints or the adaptive outcomes of contemporary natural selection is important for predicting evolutionary potential: the former alternative implies a limited capacity for adaptation to novel selection pressures, in contrast to the latter alternative that implies potential for rapid adaptive responses.
Because new environments often confer some change in selection on coadapted trait complexes, it is not obvious that alignments between ancestral plasticity and genetic variation will increase evolvability upon environmental change. Increased evolvability could nevertheless occur if exposure to an environmental change releases “cryptic genetic variation” (Paaby and Rockman 2014), such that genetic variation increases overall in new environments. A recent meta‐analysis by Noble et al. (2019) found that phenotypic plasticity was aligned with the amount of genetic (co)variance within and between traits, but whether genetic variation was quantified in the ancestral or a novel environment had no overall effect on the alignment or the total amount of expressed variation. These results were discussed with regard to the “plasticity first hypothesis,” stating that plasticity takes the lead in adaptive evolution (Levis and Pfennig 2016; Radersma et al. 2020). However, identifying unambiguous support for the idea that plasticity is an evolutionary driver, and not itself a product of selection, is difficult. In addition to information on plasticity and genetic (co)variation in the studied traits, knowledge of trait divergence and multivariate phenotypic selection is ultimately necessary (Levis and Pfennig 2016; Noble et al. 2019).
Correlated genetic and plastic responses to selection need to be studied using a multivariate approach, which can be achieved by examining the additive genetic variance and covariance between traits as summarized in a G‐matrix (Arnold et al. 2008). Quantifying G across alternative environments can provide information about how well plastic responses and additive genetic (co)variances are aligned. Making this type of analysis among multiple populations, locally adapted to the alternative environments for which multivariate selection is understood, offers the opportunity to test the prediction that the alignment between divergence, plasticity, and G is related to the differences in natural selection imposed by the respective environments. Here, we applied this approach in the damselfly Lestes sponsa (Odonata: Zygoptera), along a latitudinal gradient in Europe, to explore how multivariate selection mediated via seasonal time constraints affects the alignment between developmental plasticity, G, and divergence.
Seasonal environments impose strong selection for phenotypic plasticity in growth and development (Bradshaw and Holzapfel 2001, 2004; Sniegula et al. 2016, 2018), where photoperiod often is used as a reliable cue for tracking time and inducing adaptive plasticity (Tauber et al. 1986). At northern latitudes, a short growth season imposes strong time constraints on development and selects for a fixed strategy determining age and size at maturity, with fast development and a small size at emergence, and lack of standing genetic variation in growth rate (Rowe and Ludwig 1991; Abrams et al. 1996; Hoffmann and Parsons 1997; Dmitriew 2011). Support for these predictions has been found in several time‐constrained insect populations (Bradshaw and Holzapfel 2001, 2004; Blanckenhorn and Demont 2004; Nygren et al. 2008; Berger and Gotthard 2008; Gotthard 2008). The relaxed time constraints experienced at more southern latitudes, on the other hand, facilitate growth to a larger body size, a more flexible age at maturity, and leave room for alternative foraging strategies (e.g., the trade‐off between juvenile growth and mortality risk mediated by foraging effort: Werner and Gilliam 1984).
In the damselfly L. sponsa, the genetic and plastic responses of juvenile growth to seasonal time constraints (cued by photoperiod) follow these general patterns and are likely adaptive, with evidence for local adaptation in the use of photoperiodic cues (Sniegula and Johansson 2017; Sniegula et al. 2017). We manipulated photoperiod to simulate time constraints and induce changes in multivariate selection on growth and development in populations of L. sponsa sampled from a latitudinal gradient in Europe. We compared northern and southern populations for several life history and morphological traits to quantify (1) phenotypic plasticity in response to photoperiod, (2) population divergence in mean trait values, and (3) the broad‐sense genetic covariance matrix (G) measured at both photoperiods. This experimental design allowed us to assess effects of latitude (southern vs. northern origin) and photoperiod (northern photoperiod that induces strong time constraints vs. a more southern photoperiod with its more relaxed time constraints) on G and its potential alignment with developmental plasticity and adaptive divergence. We performed three main tests aimed at determining evolutionary causality:
We tested for the presence of an alignment between G, plasticity, and divergence, which would suggest a putative role for developmental bias and constraints in our study system. However, evidence against such a constraint includes previous work showing that plasticity in response to photoperiod and divergence along latitude in L. sponsa both reflect adaptive responses to seasonal time constraints (Sniegula and Johansson 2010; Sniegula et al. 2017).
Because an alignment between plasticity and G does not per se indicate constraints on adaptation, we tested if seasonal time constraints (an effect of photoperiod) affected the alignment between plasticity and G. As multivariate selection on the measured life history phenotypes is known to be induced by seasonal time constraints, an effect of photoperiod on the alignment would thus suggest natural selection as its causal driver. An effect of photoperiod on the alignment between G, plasticity, and divergence is, however, not informative as to whether natural selection shaped the alignment over long periods of time during the evolutionary history of L. sponsa, or whether the alignment has been shaped over a more recent and shorter time scale.
Our third test was therefore to examine whether differences in contemporary selection among our northern and southern populations had shaped the alignment. We did this by exploring if the effect of photoperiod on the alignment differed between the two types of populations. Because northern and southern populations differ in their recent evolutionary history, signified by stronger seasonal time constraints and observed concomitant responses to photoperiod in northern populations (Sniegula and Johansson. 2017; Sniegula et al. 2017), a stronger effect of photoperiod treatment on the potential alignment is predicted in northern populations. We note here that southern populations need to use photoperiodic cues to time their development and have experienced time constraints during their evolutionary history (e.g., during the latest ice age in central Europe), and indeed, also show plasticity in response to perceived time constraints (Sniegula and Johansson. 2017; Sniegula et al. 2017). Hence, the southern populations can also be expected to show effects of photoperiod on the alignment under the “contemporary selection hypothesis” (but less so than the northern populations).
Methods
STUDY SYSTEM
Our study organism was the obligatory univoltine damselfly Lestes sponsa (Hansemann 1823). In Europe, L. sponsa’s latitudinal distribution extends from northern Spain (Boudot and Kalkman 2015) to northern Sweden (Artportalen 2019). After a winter diapause, L. sponsa hatch in spring and the aquatic juvenile stage lasts for 2‐3 months after which the terrestrial adult stage has a lifespan of about a month (Corbet 1956). All growth occurs during the larval stage, but mass increase can occur in adults (Hyeun‐Ji and Johansson 2016). Because L. sponsa is time constrained, development time and size at emergence impact fitness (De Block and Stoks 2005). In addition, morphological traits such as wing morphology and thorax size have consequences for fitness components such as mating and survival (Swillen et al. 2009; Outomuro et al. 2016). Natural selection has apparently shaped adaptive phenotypic plasticity in this species. For example, across a latitudinal gradient covering central to northern Europe, individuals grown in a southern photoperiod take on average 25% longer to develop into adults when compared with those in a northern photoperiod, and this is an insufficient development time for the short season in the north (Sniegula and Johansson 2010). Also, in the congeneric L. viridis, time constraints result in lower mating success (De Block and Stoks 2005), suggesting that plasticity that accelerates development time until metamorphosis should be adaptive. Moreover, northern populations of L. sponsa are probably experiencing directional selection for faster growth and egg development time because these traits have lower additive genetic variance in the north (Sniegula et al. 2016).
SAMPLING AND REARING OF POPULATIONS
In 2018, we collected eggs from L. sponsa females that had been sampled from three latitudes in Europe: northern Sweden (northern: 66°N; n = 34 females), central Sweden (central: 59°N; n = 36), and north‐western Poland (southern: 54°N; n = 38) on 1 August, 23‐27 July and 8‐14 August, and 29‐30 July, respectively. Although Poland is not in southern Europe, we refer to these populations as north, central, and south (i.e., in a latitudinal context) for simplicity. We have no data about the level of genetic divergence among these populations, but development time, growth rate, and additive genetic variance for growth rate and egg development time differ significantly between populations of L. sponsa that span the latitudinal range used in our study (Sniegula et al. 2016). Because of the last ice age, the northern population probably split from the southern ones about 5000 years ago. We sampled seven populations: three, two, and two sites from the north, central, and south regions, respectively. All females were collected as mating pairs to form full‐sib families. Mated females were allowed to lay eggs in individual cups (see Supplementary Information 1 for details on animal husbandry). Eggs were allowed to overwinter in their native condition, in one of three climate chambers representing the temperature and photoperiod (derived lake model FLake [Lake Model FLake 2009]), of the southern, central, and northern sampling locations. We selected a combination of source individuals and thermo‐photoperiods to give five experimental groups: (1) southern (temperature‐photoperiod) and native (genetic origin of eggs), (2) central (temperature‐photoperiod) and native (genetic origin of eggs), (3) northern (temperature‐photoperiod) and native (genetic origin of eggs), (4) southern (temperature‐photoperiod) and northern (genetic origin of eggs), and (5) northern (temperature‐photoperiod) and southern (genetic origin of eggs). Note that because photoperiod is the main developmental cue in L. sponsa (Norling 2018), and because degree days (i.e., the total temperature sum over development) until emergence does not differ much between latitudes (Supplementary Information 2), we refer to photoperiod as the main environmental difference between latitudinal sample locations throughout the manuscript. On 20 November 2018, we initiated spring conditions for the eggs (i.e., water temperature >10°C, the threshold temperature for spring hatching in L. sponsa; Corbet 1956), and then simulated weekly changes of spring and summer temperatures and photoperiods until the last larva had emerged. Hence, eggs and larvae received continuous changes (weekly) in temperature and photoperiod during development and growth, simulating the conditions at south, central, and northern latitudes. Six larvae per female and treatment (n = 1156 larvae) were reared individually (see, data archiving) until emergence, after which individuals were weighed and preserved in 95% ethanol. We measured eight traits for three to six larvae from each clutch: larval development time between hatching and emergence, adult body mass at emergence, head width, thorax length, thorax width, abdomen length, tibia length (third leg on the right side), and wing length (posterior right wing). Exact landmarks, used to characterize the phenotype (see Supplementary Information 3), were measured from the first larvae that emerged from each clutch, with one clutch of eggs typically consisting of 50‐100 eggs.
STATISTICS
We assume that offspring within clutches are full‐sibs given apparent sperm precedence in damselflies (Corbet 1999, p. 521), although some clutches in L. sponsa may be half‐sibs (Johansson et al. 2020). Maternal effects on larval life history traits are low in L. sponsa (<1% in a study by Sniegula et al. 2016), suggesting that our quantitative genetic analysis of full‐sibs will mostly describe broad sense genetic variation, but does not exclude the presence of maternal environmental effects.
All traits were first mean‐standardized (Hansen and Houle 2008). Fitting multivariate response models that included all eight original traits proved unfeasible. As tibia length, abdomen length, thorax length and width, and head width were positively correlated, we therefore calculated a composite trait that described variation in metric size: metric size = (tibiaL × abdomenL × thoraxL × thoraxW × headW)1/5. Hence, we analyzed four traits: (1) metric size, (2) (linearized) body mass1/3, (3) wing length, and (4) development time. As all traits were approximately normally distributed, we assumed Gaussian response variables in all analyses.
We first tested whether there was significant latitudinal variation in each trait among the seven populations when reared at their respective native photoperiods. We applied mixed models using restricted maximum likelihood (REML) estimation and the Kenward‐Rodgers correction for the denominator degrees of freedom using lme4 (Bates et al. 2015) and car (Fox and Weisberg 2019) packages in R version 3.6.1 (R Core Team 2019), with latitude and sex as fixed effects and sample population as a random effect, making sure that each population serves as an independent observation of the effect of latitude on trait variation (see Supplementary Information 4 for model specification and output).
Next, we estimated whether there was (1) significant genetic differentiation in the four traits, (2) an effect of photoperiod treatment on trait expression (i.e., plasticity), and (3) significant genetic differentiation in the use and interpretation of photoperiod to cue development (i.e., a latitude:photoperiod treatment interaction). We used mixed models for each trait excluding the two populations from central latitude (that were reared only at their own native photoperiod). The data from the central latitude populations were thus used only to examine if there was a latitudinal cline in expressed life history traits: populations from two latitudes will (almost) always differ in one way or another. These models thus compared the populations from northern and southern latitude that had been reared in the common garden experiment including both the northern (long) and southern (short) photoperiods, and the changes in temperature. Latitude, photoperiod treatment, and sex were included as fully crossed fixed effects, and population crossed with photoperiod treatment was included as a random effect to ensure the correct level of replication for the effects of latitude and the latitude:photoperiod interaction. The family identity crossed with offspring sex was included as an additional random effect (model specification and output in Supplementary Information 5).
To compare G‐matrices, we used multivariate response models including the four traits in a Bayesian mixed modeling framework incorporating Markov Chain Monte Carlo simulations using the MCMCglmm package (Hadfield 2010) in R (model specification in Supplementary Information 6). We blocked out effects of population, sex, and photoperiod by adding these variables as fully crossed fixed effects. To estimate G, for each combination of latitude (northern and southern latitude) and photoperiod treatment (northern and southern day length), we fitted family identity as a latitude‐ and photoperiod‐specific random effect, with broad sense genetic (co)variances approximated as twice the (co)variance among full‐sibs. To retrieve corresponding trait heritabilities, the residual covariance matrix was fitted for latitude and photoperiod treatment. There was little indication of sex‐specific genetic variance in any of our single‐trait analyses (Supplementary Information 5). Likewise, there was no statistical support for population‐specific genetic covariance structure between populations within each latitude (results not shown). Hence, we estimated the four G‐matrices by averaging across sexes and populations within each latitude. We used weak and unbiased priors for the covariance matrices as recommended by Hadfield (2019) (Supplementary Information 6). There was much greater variation in development time compared to the morphological traits and all calculations and comparisons (described further below) were thus based on variance standardized matrices. In our models, genetic variance is expressed as units of phenotypic standard deviations and, therefore, twice the family variance in trait expression corresponds to (broad sense) trait heritability. We ran models for 550,000 iterations, discarding the first 50,000 iterations (as burn‐in) and stored every 500th run, resulting in 1000 uncorrelated posterior estimates of G for each latitude:photoperiod combination. These posterior estimates were used to calculate posterior modes (the most probable value) and Bayesian 95% credible intervals for all matrix parameters as well as to calculate P‐values for all matrix comparisons (see below).
First we compared trait‐specific genetic variation and heritability across the four latitude:photoperiod combinations. We then compared overall genetic variation by summing the variance across all four traits (i.e., the trace of G). We then inspected the orientation of G by performing eigen‐analysis of G and its corresponding genetic correlation matrix, R (the latter removing the influence of differences in overall genetic variance across traits). We explored how the first eigen‐vector, explaining most of the variation in G and R (Gmax and Rmax from here on), loaded on the four original traits, giving a representation of the orientation of the major axis of genetic variation and the genetic correlation structure across the four latitude:photoperiod combinations. We also explored how much of the total variance in G and R was explained by Gmax and Rmax, giving a representation of matrix dimensionality. We quantified (dis)similarity in the orientation of Gmax and Rmax, respectively, across the four groups by calculating pairwise vector correlations:
(1) |
where g1 and g2 are Gmax or Rmax for group 1 and group 2, |g1| and |g2| are the corresponding vector norms, and is the correlation between the vectors, ranging from 0 (maximum dissimilarity; vectors are completely orthogonal in multivariate trait space) to 1 (perfect similarity; vectors are identical).
Testing for statistical significance of correlations for Gmax and Rmax directly is problematic as the estimated vector correlations are bounded between 0 and 1. Therefore, to formally test for differences in the orientation of G, and simultaneously test the hypothesis that G is aligned with adaptive plasticity, we compared the alignment between G and the direction of multivariate developmental plasticity between the four latitude:photoperiod combinations via two complementary approaches (see also Berger et al. 2013; Lind et al. 2015). First, we calculated the alignment between Gmax and the multivariate vector of developmental plasticity for all four latitude:photoperiod combinations separately:
(2) |
where DP is the multivariate vector of developmental plasticity, given by the changes in trait means in response to photoperiod treatment, and is the angle between Gmax and developmental plasticity in multivariate trait space, ranging from 90 (maximum dissimilarity; vectors are completely orthogonal in multivariate trait space) to 0 (perfect similarity; vectors are identical). Second, following Hansen and Houle (2008), we quantified evolvability (e) of G along the vector of multivariate developmental plasticity
(3) |
where DP’ is the transpose of DP. eDP thus gives the expected response to selection along the plasticity vector for a unit strength of selection acting in the same multivariate direction as plasticity. We calculated posterior modes and 95% credible limits for all these metrics based on the Bayesian posterior estimates. We performed two‐sided hypotheses tests for the effect of latitude (eq. 4a), photoperiod (eq. 4b), and for differences in the effect of photoperiod between geographic origins (eq. 4c), by computing effect sizes for both alignment and evolvability based on the 1000 stored posterior estimates:
(4a) |
(4b) |
(4c) |
where NN and NS are estimates from northern populations raised at northern and southern photoperiod, and SN and SS are estimates from southern populations raised at northern and southern photoperiod. Bayesian P‐values were calculated by counting the fraction of stored iterations where the calculated effect overlapped 0 and then multiplying this fraction by a factor of 2 (to retrieve the two‐sided P‐value). As the alignment between Gmax and the vector of developmental plasticity essentially is a fraction (eq. 2), posterior estimates were logit transformed to achieve normality and to relativize computed differences prior to calculating 95% credible intervals and P‐values.
To test whether evolvability in the direction of multivariate plasticity was greater than expected by chance for each latitude:photoperiod combination, we computed evolvability for random matrices drawn from a multivariate normal distribution using the mvrnorm function in R. Matching random matrices were created for each of our 1000 stored posterior estimates of the empirically derived matrices. To do this, we first calculated the summed genetic variance for the four traits of the estimated matrix (i.e., the trace of G) for each stored iteration. We then created a random unstructured G‐matrix by sampling the same number of full‐sib families as in our original data (NN = 39, NS = 33, SN = 42, and SS = 43) from a multivariate normal distribution with the same amount of total genetic variance as for the estimated matrix, but assuming homogeneous trait variances and covariances = 0. We then calculated pairwise evolvabilities for the empirical matrix and the resulting unstructured random matrix for each of the 1000 stored iterations (eq. 3) and used these to calculate two‐sided Bayesian P‐values for each latitude:photoperiod combination as described above.
Results
LATITUDINAL VARIATION, DEVELOPMENTAL PLASTICITY, AND GENETIC DIFFERENTIATION IN TRAIT MEANS
There was significant latitudinal variation in all four traits when larvae were raised at their native photoperiod (P < 0.01 for all traits, Supplementary Information 4), with southern populations generally entering metamorphosis at an older age and larger size (Fig. 1A). In accordance, we identified significant differentiation in the three morphological traits in the common garden experiment (all P < 0.05, Supplementary Information 5), with southern populations being larger. All four traits showed strong plastic responses to photoperiod treatment, with a northern photoperiod generally rendering the faster development and smaller body size that is expected under perceived time constraints (all P < 0.01, Supplementary Information 5). There was a significant latitude‐by‐photoperiod interaction for development time (P = 0.009, F 1:2.48 = 56.7) and a marginally nonsignificant interaction for body mass (P = 0.066, F 1:2.52 = 9.70), signifying genetic divergence along the direction of multivariate plasticity and local adaptation in the use of photoperiodic cues to program development (Supplementary Information 5). Consistent with the putative effect of strong time constraints, the northern populations, evolving under stronger seasonal time constraints at northern latitude, showed a stronger response to photoperiod (Fig. 1B).
G‐MATRIX COMPARISONS
When comparing the variance‐standardized broad sense genetic (co)variance matrix, G, across latitudes (northern vs. southern Europe) and photoperiod treatments (northern vs. southern day length), there was a tendency for an increase in broad sense genetic variance and heritability for development time in southern populations raised at the novel northern photoperiod. However, there was no concomitant increase in the northern populations when they were reared at a southern photoperiod (Fig. 1B). Moreover, there was no tendency for genetic variance to increase in novel photoperiod when considering all four traits, either separately or when summing the total genetic variance over all traits (i.e., the trace of G), suggesting little evidence for revealed “cryptic” genetic variation in a novel photoperiod (summary in Table S7A).
When exploring the orientation of G and R, we found pronounced effects of photoperiod treatment on the trait loadings of Gmax and Rmax (summary in Table S7B). Here, growth in a northern photoperiod, signaling the strong time constraints experienced at northern latitude, led to positive correlations between development time and the three morphological traits, as expected in a scenario with a trade‐off between age and size at maturity and multivariate stabilizing selection favoring a fixed growth maximization strategy under seasonal time constraints. In the southern photoperiod, however, giving the perception of relaxed time constraints as experienced at southern latitude, development time and the three morphological traits were uncorrelated. The change in Gmax and Rmax caused genetic covariance structure to be more similar across populations from different latitudes raised at the same photoperiod, than for the same population raised at different photoperiods (Fig. 1B and Table 1). We quantified this (dis)similarity by estimating vector correlations of Gmax and Rmax, respectively, across latitudes and photoperiod treatments (Table S7B). This analysis showed that, indeed, vector correlations between matrices from populations reared at the same photoperiod were close to unity, whereas the novelty of the photoperiod treatment or latitude had relatively little effect on similarity of Gmax or Rmax (Table S7B).
Table 1.
ALIGNMENT BETWEEN G AND MULTIVARIATE DEVELOPMENTAL PLASTICITY
Latitudinal origin had no significant effect on alignment or evolvability (P MCMC > 0.6). However, the photoperiod treatment had a large influence on the alignment between Gmax and plasticity; development in the northern photoperiod (increased time constraints) caused genetic covariances to align with the direction of multivariate developmental plasticity, and this increase in alignment (relative to southern photoperiod and relaxed time constraints) was significant (P MCMC = 0.048; Fig. 2A). Similarly, northern photoperiod tended to increase evolvability in the direction of plasticity (relative to the southern photoperiod), although the effect was not statistically significant (P MCMC = 0.10; Fig. 2B). Moreover, comparing evolvability for the estimated G‐matrices and unstructured (random) matrices containing the same amount of total genetic variation showed that evolvability in the direction of multivariate plasticity was greater than expected by chance for the northern photoperiod (northern latitude: P MCMC = 0.01, southern latitude: P MCMC < 0.001), but not for the southern photoperiod (northern latitude: P MCMC = 0.26, southern latitude: P MCMC = 0.27) (Fig. 2B). We found no evidence suggesting that the effect of photoperiod differed between northern and southern populations (both origin:photoperiod interactions: P MCMC > 0.8), although we note that the power of our analysis would only pick up very pronounced differences as being statistically significant. In summary, developmental plasticity and evolvability were aligned only when G was measured at the photoperiod that signals strong multivariate selection via seasonal time constraints. However, as the alignment in northern and southern populations showed a similar response to photoperiod, we did not find evidence to suggest that any relatively recent change in selection at the different latitudes has affected the alignment.
Discussion
ALIGNMENT CAUSED BY SELECTION
Our analyses of populations of the damselfly L. sponsa uncovered that G was aligned with the direction of (multivariate) adaptive developmental plasticity and divergence in response to seasonal time constraints, only when G was estimated in the photoperiodic conditions that signal seasonal time stress. This suggests that the observed alignment is not a result of pure constraints, but rather implies that changes in natural selection imposed by seasonality have been instrumental in shaping the observed alignment between G and phenotypic plasticity. Indeed, insects that experience seasonal time constraints pay large fitness costs of suboptimal growth strategies due to intensified costs of postponing age at maturity and are therefore expected to show (i) fixed growth maximization strategies (Fig. 1B) (Rowe and Ludwig 1991; Abrams et al. 1996) and (ii) adaptive developmental plasticity in response to photoperiod (e.g., Bradshaw and Holzapfel 2001; Gotthard 2008), as shown previously in Lestes (Johansson and Rowe 1999; De Block and Stoks 2005; Sniegula et al. 2014). A scenario where selection shapes plasticity in both trait means and their genetic (co)variances will facilitate adaptation when G and plasticity are aligned also with the selection pressures imposed in the new environment so that (1) expressed phenotypes are moved closer toward the new trait optima, and (2) there is abundant genetic variation for the phenotypes in the direction of increased fitness, leaving opportunity for subsequent genetic assimilation (Lande 2009; Chevin et al. 2010, but see Whitlock 1996; Walters et al. 2012). However, if selection in the new environment is truly novel, then ancestral plasticity may be maladaptive and selected against (Ghalambor et al. 2015) even if it was originally derived from past selection. The extent to which such “misaligned” ancestral plasticity will limit future adaptation should depend on how fast the new selection pressures can reshape genetic (co)variances and phenotypic plasticity.
PAST OR CONTEMPORARY SELECTION
Theory suggests that alignments between phenotypic plasticity and genetic variation can, rather rapidly, develop if both levels of biological variation are shaped by correlational selection on underlying interacting genetic loci (Arnold et al. 2008; Draghi and Whitlock 2012; Jones et al. 2014). However, our comparison of northern populations (currently experiencing strong seasonal time constraints) and southern populations (currently experiencing more relaxed selection from seasonality) found no support for a difference in alignment between G, plasticity, and divergence. We note, however, that statistical power to identify more nuanced differences in the alignment was limited in our study, as often is the case for quantitative genetic designs. The result suggests that past selection, rather than contemporary selection following the evolutionary split of our northern and southern populations during the last ice age, has caused the observed alignment. Our study thus implies that the observed plasticity in trait means and in G itself is ancestral, but leaves open the question of how much developmental constraints may bias future adaptive potential to seasonality under climate change. Even though we did find that the northern populations have evolved a stronger plastic response in trait means to photoperiod cues than southern populations, southern populations also show strong responses to photoperiod (Fig. 1B). This may suggest that southern populations either currently also experience nontrivial selection to interpret cues from long day lengths or experienced such selection pressures in the not so distant past and have since then been under relaxed selection. Both these explanations are compatible with the observed effect of photoperiod on the alignment in both northern and southern populations and leave open the possibility that strong directional selection under climate change could result in a rapid change in the alignment between plasticity and G. Admittedly, photoperiod was different in the ancestral populations of L. sponsa during the last ice age when this species withdrew to the south. However, photoperiod would still have signaled time constraints because photoperiod varies over the growth season and affects southern Lestes populations (Johansson and Rowe 1999; De Block and Stoks 2005; Sniegula et al. 2014).
CRYPTIC GENETIC VARIATION
A population that enters a novel environment may exhibit an increase in genetic variation via release of “cryptic” genetic variation (McGuigan Sgro 2009; Paaby and Rockman 2014). An obvious scenario for release of cryptic genetic variation is when a population expands its range margin into new environments (Nadeau and Urban 2019). As a release of cryptic genetic variation reveals novel phenotypes upon which selection can act, this process has been proposed to have the potential to facilitate adaptation to novel environments (Hayden et al. 2011; McGuigan et al. 2011; Zheng et al. 2019). In our study, raising the populations at the nonnative photoperiod should be interpreted as novel conditions because both types of populations have not encountered these photoperiodic schemes for thousands of generations. Our 2 × 2 study design allowed us to separate the effects on G stemming from novelty of the photoperiod cue (i.e., southern populations raised at northern photoperiod, and vice versa) from those stemming from photoperiod per se. However, we found no general support for a release of cryptic variation in the studied life history traits, nor an effect of environmental novelty on G and its alignment with plasticity. In a meta‐analysis by Wood and Brodie (2015), environmental novelty did not affect differences in G between environments significantly. Similarly, in the meta‐analysis by Noble et al. (2019), the alignment between G and plasticity was unaffected (on average) by a change in the environment. Wood and Brodie (2015) concluded that their nonsignificant results might be caused by the absence of an objective metric of environmental novelty. Our study supports this conclusion and sheds further light on both these meta‐analyses as our 2 × 2 design allowed us to partition the effects of environmental novelty and photoperiod to show that only the latter had strong effects on G and its alignment with plasticity. As for many similar studies quantifying selection and genetic variation in ancestral and novel environments, one reason for why there was no excess of “cryptic” genetic variation released at the novel photoperiods in our study could be that selection in nature is still acting on the genes underlying responses to photoperiod, or has done so in the recent evolutionary past (see discussion above), even though the applied experimental day lengths are not experienced currently in the natural populations.
Conclusions
The evolutionary causality of alignments between phenotypic plasticity and additive genetic variance is of fundamental importance for realized adaptive potentials in changing environments. Here, we have shown that the alignment between G and developmental plasticity in phenotypes under multivariate selection from seasonal time constraints increases in an environment manipulated to increase such time constraints. This suggests that the type and strength of multivariate selection perceived by the organism can reshape alignments between plasticity and G. Although several studies have found alignments between plasticity and G (Noble et al. 2019), the causal explanation for such alignments cannot be provided by correlational studies (Houle et al. 2017; McGlothlin et al. 2018; Uller et al. 2018; Rohner et al. 2019) and our study suggest that this association can be the result of past changes in the selective environment rather than hard constraints on developmental trajectories. Our study did, however, not find any evidence suggesting that more recent changes in selection occurring between northern and southern populations have affected the alignment. Interestingly, the meta‐analysis by Noble et al. (2019) showed that the alignment between plasticity and G can strongly depend on the type of environmental change studied. Hence, more studies are needed to explore how past and present environmental selection shapes changes in plasticity, G, and their alignment, preferably using simultaneous information on multivariate selection and (adaptive and nonadaptive) genetic divergence in the studied phenotypes.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
FJ, PCW, and DB designed the study. FJ and SS collected the data. DB analyzed the data. FJ, PCW, SS, and DB wrote this article.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Associate Editor: M. R. Walsh
Handling Editor: D. W. Hall
Supporting information
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Thanks to C. Marshall for help with morphology measurements. Thanks also to A. Laurila for comments on a previous version of this article. FJ was supported by grant 2016–04015 from the Swedish Research Council (VR). DB was supported by grants 2015–05223 and 2019–05024 from the Swedish Research Council (VR) 2016–04015.
DATA ARCHIVING
Data are deposited on Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.jsxksn07z).
LITERATURE CITED
- Abrams, P. A. , Leimar O., Nylin S., and Wiklund C.. 1996. The effect of flexible growth rates on optimal sizes and development times in a seasonal environment. Am. Nat. 147:381–395. [Google Scholar]
- Arnold, S. J. , Burger R., Hohenlohe P. A., Ajie B. C., and Jones A. G.. 2008. Understanding the evolution and stability of the G‐matrix. Evolution 62:2451–2461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Artportalen (Swedish Species Observation System) . 2019. ArtDatabanken. dataset/occurrence. http://www.gbif.se/ipt/resource?r=artdata
- Bates, D. , Bolker M. M. B., and Walker S.. 2015. Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
- Berger, D. , and Gotthard K.. 2008. Time stress, predation risk and diurnal–nocturnal foraging trade‐offs in larval prey. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62:1655–1663. [Google Scholar]
- Berger, D. , Postma E., Blanckenhorn W. U., and Walters R. U.. 2013. Quantitative genetic divergence and standing genetic(co) variance in thermal reaction norms along latitude. Evolution 67:2385–2399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blanckenhorn, W.U. , and Demont M.. 2004. Bergmann and converse bergmann latitudinal clines in arthropods: two ends of a continuum? Integr. Comp. Biol. 44:413–424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boudot, J.‐P. , and Kalkman V. J., eds. 2015. Atlas of the European dragonflies and damselflies. KNNV Publishing, Zeist, The Netherlands. [Google Scholar]
- Bradshaw, W. E. , and Holzapfel C. M.. 2001. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 98:14509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradshaw, W. E. , and Holzapfel C. M. 2004. Evolution 38:1748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheverud, J. M. 1984. Quantitative genetics and developmental constraints on evolution by selection. J. Theor. Biol. 110:155–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chevin, L.‐M. et al. 2010. Phenotypic plasticity in evolutionary rescue experiments. Phil Trans. R Soc. Lond. B 368:201120089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbet, P. S. . 1956. The life‐histories of Lestes sponsa (Hansemann) and Sympetrum striolatum (Charpentier) (Odonata). Tijds. Entomol. 99:217–229. [Google Scholar]
- Corbet, P. 1999. Dragonflies: behavior and ecology of odonata. Harley Books, Colchester, U.K. [Google Scholar]
- De Block, M. , and Stoks R.. 2005. Fitness effects from egg to reproduction: bridging the life history transition. Ecology 86:185–197. [Google Scholar]
- Dmitriew, C. M. 2011. The evolution of growth trajectories: what limits growth rate? Biol. Rev. 86:97–116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Draghi, J. A. , and Whitlock M. C.. 2012. Phenotypic plasticity facilitates mutational variance, genetic variance and evolvability along the major axis of environmental variation. Evolution 66:2891–2902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox, J. , and Weisberg S.. 2019. An {R} companion to applied regression. 3rd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Available via https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/. [Google Scholar]
- Gerhart, J. , and Kirschner M.. 2007. The theory of facilitated variation. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA 104:8582–8589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghalambor, C. K. , Mackay C. K., Carroll S. P., and Reznick D. N.. 2007. Adaptive versus non‐adaptive phenotypic plasticity and the potential for contemporary adaptation in new environments. Funct. Ecol. 21:394–407. [Google Scholar]
- Ghalambor, C. K. , Hoke K. L., Ruell E. W., Fischer E. K., Reznick D. N., and Hughes K. A.. 2015. Non‐adaptive plasticity potentiates rapid adaptive evolution of gene expression in nature. Nature 525:372–375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez‐Mestre, I. , and Buchholz D. R.. 2006. Developmental plasticity mirrors differences among taxa in spadefoot toads linking plasticity and diversity. Proc. Nat Acad. Sci. USA 103:19021–19026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotthard, K. 2008. Adaptive growth decisions in butterflies. BioScience 58:222–230. [Google Scholar]
- Hadfield, J. D. 2010. MCMC methods for multi‐response generalized linear mixed models: the MCMCglmm R package. J. Stat. Softw. 33:1–22.20808728 [Google Scholar]
- Hadfield, J. D. 2019. MCMCglmm course notes. Available via https://cran.prroject.org/web/packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/CourseNotes.pdf.
- Hansen, T. F. , and Houle D.. 2008. Measuring and comparing evolvability and constraint in multivariate characters. J. Evol. Biol. 21:1201–1219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayden, E. J. , Ferrada E., and Wagner A.. 2011. Cryptic genetic variation promotes rapid evolutionary adaptation in an RNA enzyme. Nature 474:92–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoffmann, A. , and Parsons P.. 1997. Extreme environmental change and evolution. 1st ed. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, U.K. [Google Scholar]
- Houle, D. , Bolstad G. H., van der Linde K., and Hansen T. F.. 2017. Mutation predicts 40 million years of fly wing evolution. Nature 548:447–450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyeun‐Ji, L. , and Johansson F.. 2016. Compensating for a bad start: compensatory growth across life stages in an organism with a complex life cycle. Can. J. Zool. 94:41–47. [Google Scholar]
- Jiang, D. , and Zhang J.. 2020. Fly wing evolution explained by a neutral model with mutational pleiotropy. Evolution 74:2158–2167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson, F. , and Rowe L.. 1999. Life history and behavioral responses to time constraints in a damselfly. Ecology 80:1242–1252. [Google Scholar]
- Johansson, F. , Berger D., Höglund J., Meyer‐Lucht Y., Rödin‐Mörch P., Sniegula S., and Watts P. C.. 2020. High variation in last male sperm precedence and genital morphology in the emerald damselfly Lestes sponsa . Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 130:497–506. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, A. G. , Bürger R., and Arnold S. J.. 2014. Epistasis and natural selection shape the mutational architecture of complex traits. Nat. Commun. 5:3709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lake Model FLake . 2009. FLake online. http://www.flake.igb-berlin.de/index.shtml
- Laland, K. N. , Uller T., Feldman M. W., Sterelny K., Müller G. B., Moczek A., Jablonka E., and Odling‐Smee J.. 2015. The extended evolutionary synthesis: its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 282:20151019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande, R. 1979. Quantitative genetic analysis of multivariate evolution, applied to brain: body size allometry. Evolution 33:402–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lande, R. 2009. Adaptation to an extraordinary environment by evolution of phenotypic plasticity and genetic assimilation. J. Evol. Biol. 22:1435–1446. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levis, N. A. , and Pfennig D. W.. 2016. Evaluating ‘Plasticity‐First’ evolution in nature: key criteria and empirical approaches. Trends Ecol. Evol. 31:563–574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levis, N. A. , and Pfennig D. W. 2019. Plasticity‐led evolution: evaluating the key prediction of frequency‐dependent adaptation. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 286:20182754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lind, M. I. , Yarlett K., Reger J., Carter M. J., and Beckerman A. P.. 2015. The alignment between phenotypic plasticity, the major axis of genetic variation and the response to selection. Proc. R. Soc. B 282:20151651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGlothlin, J. W. , Kobiela M. E., Wright H. V., Mahler D. L., Kolbe J. J., Losos J. B., and Brodie E. D.. 2018. Adaptive radiation along a deeply conserved genetic line of least resistance in anolis izards. Evol. Lett. 2:310–322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuigan, K. , and Sgro C. M.. 2009. Evolutionary consequences of cryptic genetic variation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24:305–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuigan, K. , Nishimura N., Currey M., Hurwit D., and Cresko D. W. A.. 2011. Cryptic genetic variation and body size evolution in threespine stickleback. Evolution 65:1203‐1211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadeau, C. P. , and Urban M. C.. 2019. Eco‐evolution on the edge during climate change. Ecography 42:1280–1297. [Google Scholar]
- Noble, D. W. A. , Radersma R., and Uller T.. 2019. Plastic responses to novel environments are biased towards dimensions with high additive genetic variation. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 116:13452–1313461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norling, U. 2018. Constant and shifting photoperiods as seasonal cues during larval development of the univoltine damselfly Lestes sponsa (Odonata: Lestidae). Int. J. Odontol. 21:129–150. [Google Scholar]
- Nygren, G. H. , Bergstrom A., and Nylin S.. 2008. Latitudinal body size clines in the butterfly Polyommatus icarus are shaped by gene‐environment interactions. J. Insect. Sci. 8:1‐13. [Google Scholar]
- Outomuro, D. , Söderquist L., Cortázar‐Chinarro M., Lundgren C., Nilsson‐Örtman V., and Johansson F.. 2016. Antagonistic natural and sexual selection on wing shape in a scrambling damselfly. Evolution 70:1582–1595. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paaby, A. B. , and Rockman M. V.. 2014. Cryptic genetic variation: evolution's hidden substrate. Nat. Rev. Genet. 15:247–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfennig, D. W. , Matthew A., Wund E., Snell‐Rood E. C., Cruickshank T., Schlichting C. D., and Moczek A. P.. 2010. Phenotypic plasticity's impacts on diversification and speciation. Trend Ecol. Evol. 25:459–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Price, T. D. , Qvarnström A., and Irwin D. E.. 2003. The role of phenotypic plasticity in driving genetic evolution. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 270:1433–1440. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team . 2019. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna. Available via https://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- Radersma, R. , Noble D. W. A., and Uller T.. 2020. Plasticity leaves a phenotypic signature during local adaptation. Evol. Lett. 4:360–370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohner, P. T. , Roy J., Schäfer M.A., Blanckenhorn W.U., and Berger D.. 2019. Does thermal plasticity align with local adaptation? An inter‐specific comparison of wing morphology in sepsid flies. J. Evol. Biol. 32:463–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe, L. , and Ludwig D.. 1991. Size and timing of metamorphosis in complex life histories: time constraints and variation. Ecology 72:413–427. [Google Scholar]
- Schluter, D. 1996. Adaptive radiation along lines of least genetic resistance. Evolution 50:1766–1774. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sniegula, S. , and Johansson F.. 2010. Photoperiod affects compensating developmental rate across latitudes in the damselfly Lestes sponsa. Ecol. Entomol. 35:149–157. [Google Scholar]
- Sniegula, S. , Drobniak S. M., Gołąb M. J., and Johansson F.. 2014. Photoperiod and variation in life history traits in core and peripheral populations in the damselfly Lestes sponsa . Ecol. Entomol. 39:137‐148. [Google Scholar]
- Sniegula, S. , Gołąb M. J., Drobniak S. M., and Johansson F.. 2016. Seasonal time constraints reduce genetic variation in life history traits along a latitudinal gradient. J. Anim. Ecol. 85:187–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sniegula, S. , Gołąb M. J., and Johansson F.. 2017. Cannibalism and activity rate in larval damselflies increase along a latitudinal gradient as a consequence of time constraints. BMC Evol. Biol. 17:167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sniegula, S. , Gołąb M. J., Drobniak S. M., and Johansson F.. 2018. The genetic variance but not the genetic covariance of life‐history traits changes towards the north in a time‐constrained insect. J. Evol. Biol. 31:853–865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Susoy, V , Ragsdale E. J., Kanzaki N., and Sommer R. J.. 2015. Rapid diversification associated with a macroevolutionary pulse of developmental plasticity. eLife. 4:e05463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Svensson, E. I. , and Berger D.. 2019. The role of mutation bias in adaptive evolution. Trend Ecol. Evol. 34:422–434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swillen, I. , De Block M., and Stoks R.. 2009. Morphological and physiological sexual selection targets in a territorial damselfly. Ecol. Entomol. 34:677–683. [Google Scholar]
- Tauber, M. J. , Tauber C. A., and Masaki S.. 1986. Seasonal adaptation of insects. Oxford Univ. Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Uller, T. et al. 2018. Developmental bias and evolution: a regulatory network perspective. Genetics 209:949–996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waddington, C. H. 1953. Genetic assimilation of an acquired character. Evolution 7:118–126. [Google Scholar]
- Walters, R. , Blanckenhorn W. U., and Berger D.. 2012. Forecasting extinction risk of ectotherms under climate warming: an evolutionary perspective. Funct. Ecol. 26:1324–1338. [Google Scholar]
- Werner, E. E. , and Gilliam J. F.. 1984. The ontogenetic niche and species interactions in size‐structured populations. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 15:393–425. [Google Scholar]
- West‐Eberhard, M. J. 2003. Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford Univ. Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Whitlock, M. C. 1996. The red queen beats the jack‐of‐all‐trades: the limitations on the evolution of phenotypic plasticity and niche breadth. Am. Nat. 148:S65–77. [Google Scholar]
- Wood, C. W. , and 3rd Brodie E. D.. 2015. Environmental effects on the structure of the G‐matrix. Evolution 69:2927–2940. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng, J. , Payne J. L., and Wagner A., 2019. Cryptic genetic variation accelerates evolution by opening access to diverse adaptive peaks. Science 365:347–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data are deposited on Dryad (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.jsxksn07z).