Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Mar 23;16(3):e0238336. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238336

Learning can be detrimental for a parasitic wasp

Valeria Bertoldi 1, Gabriele Rondoni 1, Ezio Peri 2, Eric Conti 1, Jacques Brodeur 3,*
Editor: Nicolas Desneux4
PMCID: PMC7987188  PMID: 33755694

Abstract

Animals have evolved the capacity to learn, and the conventional view is that learning allows individuals to improve foraging decisions. The parasitoid Telenomus podisi has been shown to parasitize eggs of the exotic stink bug Halyomorpha halys at the same rate as eggs of its coevolved host, Podisus maculiventris, but the parasitoid cannot complete its development in the exotic species. We hypothesized that T. podisi learns to exploit cues from this non-coevolved species, thereby increasing unsuccessful parasitism rates. We conducted bioassays to compare the responses of naïve vs. experienced parasitoids on chemical footprints left by one of the two host species. Both naïve and experienced females showed a higher response to footprints of P. maculiventris than of H. halys. Furthermore, parasitoids that gained an experience on H. halys significantly increased their residence time within the arena and the frequency of re-encounter with the area contaminated by chemical cues. Hence, our study describes detrimental learning where a parasitoid learns to associate chemical cues from an unsuitable host, potentially re-enforcing a reproductive cul-de-sac (evolutionary trap). Maladaptive learning in the T. podisiH. halys association could have consequences for population dynamics of sympatric native and exotic host species.

1. Introduction

Animal decision-making, which is involved in processes such as resource and habitat selection, mate choice and progeny allocation, relies on innate behaviour (instinct), stochastic processes, physiological feedbacks (e.g., hormonal signalling) and learning (reviewed in [1]). Insect parasitoids have been used as model systems to explore both proximate and ultimate perspectives of optimal foraging. In order to cope with spatial and temporal variability in resources, parasitic wasps have evolved the capacity to associate host-related chemical cues to host availability and suitability [26]. They further consolidate and improve this capacity through learning processes [710], resulting in increased reproductive success [3, 1113]. The probability of including a new stimulus in the behavioural repertoire of a parasitoid female depends on its reliability in host location [2], with oviposition having been shown to consolidate a change in foraging behaviour and host acceptance [3, 9, 14]. For example, scelionid parasitoids use host chemical cues on the egg surface or those deposited on plant surfaces by gravid females (footprints) to locate hosts in the habitat (reviewed by [15]). Following detection of host chemical cues, experienced scelionid females show stronger arrestment response (increased residence time, slower walking and increased turning tendency) than naïve females, a behaviour that is re-enforced by successful oviposition [16, 17].

Biological invasions generate novel interactions which can have negative consequences on populations of native species [1821]. This occurs, for example, when an invasive exotic species becomes accepted as a host by native parasitoids but is unsuitable for offspring development. The introduced species then acts as an egg sink [22] for indigenous parasitoids, and negatively impact their reproductive success. We hypothesized that such an evolutionary trap [23, 24] could be exacerbated if foraging parasitoid females learn to exploit cues from a novel but unsuitable host. To our knowledge, there are no examples where associative learning actually results in costs to the foraging success of an animal.

To test this hypothesis we used Telenomus podisi Ashmead (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae), a common egg parasitoid of several North American stink bugs (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) [25, 26]. As previously reported, T. podisi females accept and parasitize eggs of the brown marmorated stink bug, Halyomorpha halys Stål (Pentatomidae) [27], a recently established pest from eastern Asia [28], at a similar rate to that of its coevolved host, the predator Podisus maculiventris (Say) (Pentatomidae). But the parasitoid progeny rarely develop successfully in H. halys [27, 2934], except for a nonconforming T. podisi population in California [35]. Halyomorpha halys is progressively spreading in invaded areas, thereby increasing the probability of native T. podisi encountering this novel but unsuitable host. We examined the learning capacity of T. podisi towards its coevolved host, P. maculiventris, and non-coevolved host, H. halys, and discussed consequences on interacting species.

2. Material and methods

2.1 Hosts and parasitoid

A colony of H. halys reared continuously on raw pumpkin seeds, carrots, green beans, grapes and potted soybean plants, was established from adults collected in Ontario (Canada) in 2012. The P. maculiventris colony was initially established using adults collected in Ontario in 2011 and 2012 and supplemented with bugs from Anatis Bioprotection (Canada). They were fed with live mealworm, Tenebrio molitor L. (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae) larvae reared in the laboratory, fresh green beans and bean plants. Nymphs were kept in plastic cylinders and fed with mealworm and green beans. For both stink bug species, freshly laid eggs (< 24 h old) were used for the experiments.

The T. podisi colony was established with individuals collected in 2011 and 2012 in Ontario. Adult parasitoids were provided with a 1:1 (vol/vol) honey:water solution rubbed on a small piece of ParaFilm®. Each week, 1–2 days-old egg masses of P. maculiventris (stuck on filter paper using Pritt® stick glue) were exposed to T. podisi females for 24 h to maintain the colony. After emergence, male and female parasitoids were kept together in glass tubes for mating and provided with the honey-water solution. Naïve (i.e. without oviposition experience), 3–8 days-old females were randomly assigned to the different experimental treatments. Females T. podisi from our laboratory colony are synovigenic (ovigeny index < 0.05), typically emerging with very few mature oocytes (mean of approximately 2.0 [36]), and can survive for up to 100 days under laboratory conditions [37]. Unpublished data (M. Gaudreau, pers. com.) further show that 4–8 day-old females have an average of 14.2 mature eggs in their abdomen, similar to the maximum number of mature eggs observed per female throughout their reproductive life.

All insects were reared in a growth chamber (Conviron E15) at 24±1°C, 50±5 percent relative humidity, under a 16L:8D photoperiod.

2.2 Treatments

To determine if T. podisi females exhibit learning behaviour the following four treatments were tested: (i) naïve parasitoid females foraging on H. halys traces; (ii) females with experience on H. halys traces and eggs, then tested on H. halys traces; (iii) naïve females foraging on P. maculiventris traces; and (iv) females with experience on P. maculiventris traces and eggs, then tested on P. maculiventris traces. The experiments were conducted from 10:00 to 14:00 at 24±1°C, 50±5 percent relative humidity, under a 16L:8D photoperiod. Between 39 to 43 replicates were conducted for each treatment.

2.3 Obtaining experienced females

Experienced parasitoid females were obtained in the following manner. A female of either H. halys or P. maculiventris in their pre-ovipositional phase (with a physogastric abdomen) was introduced in an experimental arena consisting of a Petri dish (5 cm diam., 1 cm height) placed upside-down on a filter sheath. The Petri dish cover had tissue mesh (0.01 cm holes) to prevent saturation of the atmosphere with volatiles released by the stink bug female. Females walked for 30 minutes on the filter paper to allow contamination with chemical traces. Females that occasionally walked on the Petri dish were discarded. Filter papers soiled by bug’s faeces were discarded. Once the stink bug female was removed, a small egg mass (5–6 eggs) of the same species was placed in the middle of the contaminated area. A naïve (i.e. without foraging and oviposition experience) T. podisi female was then introduced in the arena and continuously observed. Once she had oviposited the female was removed, isolated in a 1.5 mL tube for 1 h before being tested as an experienced parasitoid. Females that did not oviposit within 1 h were discarded.

2.4 Parasitoid female response to chemical cues

As previously described [38], bioassays were conducted on a large filter paper arena (20 x 20 cm) where parasitoid females could move freely on the surface. A 5 cm diam area at the centre was exposed to a female of H. halys or P. maculiventris in pre-ovipositional phase, as described above. For the assays, a T. podisi female was released in the middle of the contaminated area (without host eggs) and her behaviour recorded with a HDD video camera (Sony HDR-XR 500) placed 40 cm above the arena. The assay stopped when the wasp left the arena or after 10 minutes [38]. Individuals that flew away within 10 s after the release were excluded from the analysis (n = 6). Each arena was used to test 5 females.

We recorded the time spent by the female in the arena (total residence time) and the number of times the parasitoids went back to the contaminated area once left (number of re-encounters), indicative of a tortuous path associated with the searching behaviour of a parasitoid female [15, 39, 40].

2.5 Statistical analyses

Generalized linear models (GLMs with Gaussian distribution for time data or Poisson distribution for count data) were fitted to test the effects of host herbivore species (H. halys or P. maculiventris), parasitoid previous experience (experienced or naïve) and their interaction on total parasitoid residence time and number of re-encounters with the contaminated area. Residence time data were subjected to Box-Cox transformation for normalization before the analyses. Significance of the model terms was evaluated by means of F test or Likelihood Ratio Test [41]. Significance of the different variable levels was assessed using the Tukey method for multiple comparisons procedure, adopting a significance level α = 0.05. The potential influence of chemical residuals from a parasitoid female on the behaviour of the following female was evaluated as a random effect in mixed models. This influence was negligible and therefore models with only fixed effect were included. Analyses were conducted under R statistical environment [42].

3. Results

Total residence time of T. podisi varied depending on the insect species footprint (Gaussian GLM: F(1, 159) = 53.78, P < 0.0001, the previous experience (F(1, 158) = 16.83, P < 0.0001) and the interaction of these two factors (F(1, 157) = 4.28, P = 0.04). For the T. podisi—P. maculiventris association, parasitoid females that had previously experienced the chemical traces left by their native host and had been rewarded with an oviposition did not have a greater residence time in the experimental arena than naïve females. In contrast, females with a rewarded experience on the exotic H. halys stayed significantly longer in the arena than naïve wasps when tested on chemical footprints of H. halys (Fig 1A). However, both naïve and experienced parasitoids displayed higher residence time on chemical traces of their coevolved host P. maculiventris than those of the exotic H. halys (Fig 1A).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

(A) Residence time and (B) number of re-encounters with the host-contaminated arena of naive and experienced wasps. The four treatments were: T. podisi naïve females tested on H. halys traces (HH_naï; N = 40); T. podisi females experienced on H. halys traces and eggs, then tested on H. halys traces (HH_exp; N = 43); naïve T. podisi females tested on P. maculiventris traces (PM_naï; N = 39); T. podisi females experienced on P. maculiventris traces and eggs, then tested on P. maculiventris traces (PM_exp; N = 39). Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between treatments (p < 0.05; GLM followed by Tukey method for multiple comparison procedure).

The frequency of re-encounter with the area contaminated by chemical cues varied depending on the insect species footprint (Poisson GLM: X2 = 35.65, P < 0.0001), the previous experience (X2 = 26.39, P < 0.0001) and the interaction of these two factors (X2 = 12.3, P = 0.0005). Experienced T. podisi females re-entered the contaminated area more frequently than naïve females (Fig 1B). Naïve females tested on H. halys footprints rarely returned to the contaminated area. However, following a rewarded experience on H. halys, the number of re-encounters with this species was similar to the number of re-encounters of naïve females tested on P. maculiventris (Fig 1B). Both naïve and experienced T. podisi females returned more frequently to a patch contaminated by P. maculiventris than H. halys (Fig 1B).

4. Discussion

Our findings indicate that T. podisi females exhibit increased foraging behaviour following experience with chemical traces and oviposition in both its coevolved and novel host, probably due to associative learning [11, 43]. This capacity permits a female parasitoid to capture and retain information about host availability in the habitat and to adjust her foraging behaviour accordingly. However, the value of learning and its consequences on the reproductive success of T. podisi are opposite when exploiting a suitable (P. maculiventris) vs. unsuitable (H. halys) host. Exploiting H. halys eggs is maladaptive for T. podisi because it incurs significant costs to foraging females and leads to a cul-de-sac for their progeny [27]. Telenomus podisi females are not attracted to plants infested by H. halys under laboratory conditions [44], whereas T. podisi was the most common parasitoid sampled from H. halys eggs under field conditions in Canada [33]. Halyomorpha halys not only represents an egg sink for T. podisi, but also a ‘time sink’ (sensu [27]) since females increase time foraging in areas contaminated by the unsuitable host. The waste of time is further amplified when females protect the egg mass (patch guarding) from competitors and predators during several hours following oviposition [45].

Maladaptive learning has been reported in a number of social hymenopteran insects (bees and wasps) when copying the foraging decisions of conspecifics leads to the exploitation of already deprived food resources ([46] and references therein). It was also suggested [47] that new genetic combinations following hybridization in fishes and birds could negatively impact learning capacities, potentially leading to postzygotic reproductive isolation. Maladaptive learning has also been reported in parasitoid species used as biological control agents. When reared on alternative host species, artificial diets or artificial rearing units, parasitoids may partly loose their capacity to find and exploit the natural pest species [48, 49]. For example, it was shown [50] that prior experience of Exeristes roborator (Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae) when reared in an artificial arena significantly altered the behavioural response of parasitoid females to their host, the European pine shoot moth, Rhyacionia buoliana (Lepidoptera: Tortricidae), when released in forests. The present study documents an original case of detrimental learning where a parasitoid learns to associate chemical cues from an unsuitable host, thereby re-enforcing a reproductive cul-de-sac (evolutionary trap).

Both naïve and experienced T. podisi females showed a higher response to chemical traces of P. maculiventris than of H. halys. This pattern was expected because derived stimuli from coevolved hosts are likely to evoke strong innate responses by naïve individuals; indeed the innate response can be stronger than the learned response in the location and acceptance of highly suitable hosts [51]. However, we cannot exclude an effect of the rearing host (P. maculiventris), as shown in other parasitoid species [5254]. For instance, similar residence time between naïve vs. experienced wasps when exposed to P. maculiventris chemical traces could be partly explained by T. podisi females having already gained experience when developing in and emerging from P. maculiventris eggs.

Associative learning of cues from a novel but unsuitable host would also exacerbate the negative effects of the evolutionary trap on T. podisi populations. It may contribute to modify the community structure in areas invaded by H. halys through direct and indirect ecological effects [27, 55]. Recent exposure to H. halys may lead to an increase in T. podisi’s rate of parasitism on the invasive host (host switching), and a consequent decrease in parasitism of indigenous pentatomid species (apparent predation/parasitism; (+, -) type interaction). Additional research is required to determine the extent to which detrimental learning would affect the population dynamics of native stink bugs and parasitoids. We only tested females 1 h after their experience with host chemical cues and it would be important to determine how long they exhibit such learned behaviour under natural conditions, considering ecological factors such as the relative densities of native and exotic stink bugs and the persistence of the chemical cues in the footprints.

This original case of maladaptive learning arises from a situation where a native parasitoid encounters a new potential host species as a result of a biological invasion. On one hand, there is no operational ecological filter that stops host location and acceptance and, on the other hand, there is a strong physiological filter that prevents parasitoid development [27, 32]. Accordingly, a parasitoid could escape such an evolutionary trap by evolving (i) behavioural capacities to prevent acceptance of an unsuitable resource or (ii) physiological capacities to successfully reproduce in the novel host species [27, 56].

Supporting information

S1 File. R script.

R scripts and supplementary figures.

(PDF)

S1 Data. T. podisi data.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Paul K. Abram, Jeremy N. McNeil and three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments and Josée Doyon for technical assistance.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by Innovation Staff Exchange (RISE) H2020-MSCA-RISE-2015 of the European Union with the project Impact of invasive alien true bug species in native trophic webs INVASION (GA 690952) awarded to E.P, E.C and J.B.

References

  • 1.Papaj D.R. & Lewis A.C. (1993) Insect learning: Ecology and evolutionary perspectives. London, UK: Chapman & Hall. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Vet L.E.M. & Dicke M. (1992) Ecology of infochemical use by natural enemies in a tritrophic context. Annual Review of Entomology 37: 141–172. https://10.1146/annurev.en.37.010192.001041 [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Turlings T.C., Wäckers F.L., Vet L.E.M., Lewis W.J. & Tumlinson J.H. (1993) Learning of host-finding cues by hymenopterous parasitoids. In: Insect learning. Springer, pp 51–78. 10.1007/BF00994314 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Godfray H.C.J. (1994) Parasitoids: behavioral and evolutionary ecology. Princeton, USA: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fatouros N.E., Dicke M., Mumm R., Meiners T. & Hilker M. (2008) Foraging behavior of egg parasitoids exploiting chemical information. Behavioral Ecology 19:677–689. 10.1093/beheco/arn011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Haverkamp A. & Smid H.M. (2020) A neuronal arms race: the role of learning in parasitoid-host interactions. Current Opinion in Insect Science 42: 47–54 10.1016/j.cois.2020.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Arthur A.P. (1971) Associative learning by Nemeritis canescens (Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae). The Canadian Entomologist 103: 1137–1141. 10.4039/ent1031137-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lewis W. & Tumlinson J.H. (1988) Host detection by chemically mediated associative learning in a parasitic wasp. Nature 331:257. 10.1038/331257a0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vet L.E.M. & Groenewold A.W. (1990) Semiochemicals and learning in parasitoids. Journal of Chemical Ecology 16:3119–3135. 10.1007/BF00979615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dukas R. (2008) Evolutionary biology of insect learning. Annual Review of Entomology 53:145–160. 10.1146/annurev.ento.53.103106.093343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Papaj D.R. & Prokopy R.J. (1989) Ecological and evolutionary aspects of learning in phytophagous insects. Annual Review of Entomology 34:315–350. 10.1146/annurev.en.34.010189.001531 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Dukas R. & Duan J.J. (2000) Potential fitness consequences of associative learning in a parasitoid wasp. Behavioral Ecology 11:536–543. 10.1093/beheco/11.5.536 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Nieberding C.M., Van Dyck H. & Chiitka L. (2018) Adaptive learning in non-social insects: from theory to field work, and back. Current Opinion in Insect Science 27: 75–81. 10.1016/j.cois.2018.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kruidhof H.M., Pashalidou F.G., Fatouros N.E., Figueroa I.A., Vet L.E., Smid H.M. et al. (2012) Reward value determines memory consolidation in parasitic wasps. PLoS One 7:e39615. 10.1371/journal.pone.0039615 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Colazza S., Peri E., Salerno G. & Conti E. (2010) Host Searching by egg parasitoids: Exploitation of host chemical cues. In: Consoli FL, Parra JRP, Zucchi RA eds Egg parasitoids in agroecosystems with emphasis on Trichogramma. Springer; Netherlands, Dordrecht, pp 97–147. 10.1007/978-1-4020-9110-0_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Peri E., Salerno G., Slimani T., Frati F., Conti E., Colazza S. et al. (2016) The response of an egg parasitoid to substrate-borne semiochemicals is affected by previous experience. Scientific reports 6:27098. 10.1038/srep27098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Abram P.K., Cusumano A. Abram K., Colazza S. & Peri E (2017). Testing the habituation assumption uderlying models of parasitoid foraging behavior. PeerJ. 5, e3097. 10.7717/peerj.3097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Schlaepfer M.A., Runge M.C. & Sherman P.W. (2002) Ecological and evolutionary traps. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 17:474–480. 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02580-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Berthon K. (2015) How do native species respond to invaders? Mechanistic and trait-based perspectives. Biological Invasions 17:2199–2211. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00730.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cameron E.K., Vila M. & Cabeza M. (2016) Global meta-analysis of the impacts of terrestrial invertebrate invaders on species, communities ans ecosystems. Glob. Ecol Biogeography 25: 596–606. 10.1111/geb.12436. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bradley B.A., Laginhas B.B., Whitlock R., Allen J.M., Bates A.E., Bernatchez G., et al. (2019) Disentangling the abundance-impact relationship for invasive species. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 116:9919–9924. 10.1073/pnas.1818081116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hoogendoorn M. & Heimpel G.E. (2002) Indirect interactions between an introduced and native ladybird beetle species mediated by a shared parasitoid. Biological Control 25: 224–230. 10.1016/S1049-9644(02)00101-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Schlaepfer M.A., Sherman P.W., Blossey B. & Runge M.C. (2005) Introduced species as evolutionary traps. Ecology Letters 8:241–246. 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00730.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Robertson B.A., Rehage J.S. & Sih A. 2013. Ecological novelty and the emergence of evolutionary traps. Trends Ecol Evol 28, 552–560. 10.1016/j.tree.2013.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Yeargan K.V. (1979) Parasitism and predation of stink bug eggs in soybean and alfalfa fields. Environmental Entomology 8: 715–719. 10.1093/ee/8.4.715 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Koppel A.L., Hebert D.A., Kuhar T.P. & Kamminga K. (2009) Survey of stink bug (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) egg parasitoids in wheat, soybean, and vegetable crops in southeast Virginia. Environmental Entomology 38: 375–379. 10.1603/022.038.0209 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Abram P.K., Gariepy T.D., Boivin G. & Brodeur J. 2014. An invasive stink bug as an evolutionary trap for an indigenous egg parasitoid. Biological Invasions 16:1387–1395. 10.1007/s10530-013-0576-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hoebeke E.R. & Carter M.E. (2003) Halyomorpha halys (Stǻl) (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae): a polyphagous plant pest from Asia newly detected in North America. Proceedings of the Entomological Society of Washington 105: 225–237. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jones A.L., Jennings D.E., Hooks C.R. & Shrewsbury P.M. (2014) Sentinel eggs underestimate rates of parasitism of the exotic brown marmorated stink bug, Halyomorpha halys. Biological control 78:61–66. 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2014.07.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Haye T., Fischer S., Zhang J. & Gariepy T. (2015) Can native egg parasitoids adopt the invasive brown marmorated stink bug, Halyomorpha halys (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae), in Europe? Journal of Pest Science 88:693–705. 10.1007/s10340-015-0671-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cornelius M.L., Dieckhoff C., Vinyard B.T. & Hoelmer K.A. (2016) Parasitism and predation on sentinel egg masses of the Brown Marmorated Stink Bug (Hemiptera: Pentatomidae) in three vegetable crops: Importance of dissections for evaluating the impact of native parasitoids on an exotic pest. Environmental Entomology 45:1536–1542. 10.1093/ee/nvw134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Konopka J.K., Poinapen D., Gariepy T. & McNeil J.N. (2018) Understanding the mismatch between behaviour and development in a novel host-parasitoid association. Scientific reports 8:15677. 10.1038/s41598-018-33756-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Gariepy T.D., Bruin A., Konopka J., Scott-Dupree C., Fraser H., Bon M.-C. et al. (2019) A modified DNA barcode approach to define trophic interactions between native and exotic pentatomids and their parasitoids. Molecular Ecology 28:456–470. 10.1111/mec.14868 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Costi E., Wong W., Cossentine J., Acheampong S., Maistrello L., Haye T., et al. (2020) Variation in levels of acceptance, developmental success, and abortion of Halyomorpha halys eggs by native North American parasitoids. Biological Control 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2020.104351 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Tognon R., Aldrich J.R., Sant’Ana J. & Zalom F.G. (2019) Conditioning Native Telenomus and Trissolcus (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae) egg parasitoids to recognize the exotic brown marmorated stink bug (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae: Halyomorpha halys). Environmental Entomology 48:211–218. 10.1093/ee/nvy186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Zhou Y. Abram, P.K. Boivin, G. & Brodeur, J. (2014) Increasing host age does not have the expected negative effects on fitness parameters of an egg parasitoid. Entomologia Experimentalis et Applicata 151: 106–111. 10.1007/s10096-013-1992-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Abram P. K., Parent J. P., Brodeur J. & Boivin G. (2016). Size-induced phenotypic reaction norms in a parasitoid wasp: an examination of life-history and behavioural traits. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 17: 620–632. 10.1111/bij.12658 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Conti E., Salerno G., Bin F. & Vinson S.B. (2004) The role of host semiochemicals in parasitoid specificity: a case study with Trissolcus brochymenae and Trissolcus simoni on pentatomid bugs. Biological Control 29:435–443. 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2003.08.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Conti E., Salerno G., Bin F., Williams H.J. & Vinson S.B. (2003) Chemical Cues from Murgantia histrionica eliciting host location and recognition in the egg parasitoid Trissolcus brochymenae. Journal of Chemical Ecology 29:115–130. 10.1023/a:1021980614512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Borges M., Colazza S., Ramirez-Lucas P., Chauhan K.R., Moraes M.C.B. & Richard Aldrich J. (2003) Kairomonal effect of walking traces from Euschistus heros (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae) on two strains of Telenomus podisi (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae). Physiological Entomology 28:349–355. 10.1111/j.1365-3032.2003.00350.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Smith GM (2007) Analysing ecological data. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.R Core Team (2020) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Smid H.M. & Vet L.E.M. (2016) The complexity of learning, memory and neural processes in an evolutionary ecological context. Current Opinion in Insect Science 15: 61–69. 10.1016/j.cois.2016.03.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Martorana L., Brodeur J. Foti M.C. Agro A., Colazza S. & Peri E. (2019) Egg parasitoid exploitation of plant volatiles induced by single or concurrent attack of a zoophytophagous predator and an invasive phytophagous pest. Scientific Reports 9: 18956. 10.1038/s41598-019-55396-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Guerra-Grenier E., Abram P.K. & Brodeur J. (2020) Asymmetries affecting structure and outcome of aggressive contests between solitary egg parasitoids: the effect of natal host species. Behavioral Ecology 31: 1391–1400 10.1093/beheco/araa096 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Leadbeater E. & Chitka L. (2009) Social information use in foraging insects. In Jarau S. & Hrncir M. (eds) Food exploitation by social insects. Boca Raton, Florida: CRC Press. pp. 135–146. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Rice A.M. & McQuillan M.A. (2018) Maladaptive learning and memory in hybrids as a reproductive isolating barrier. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Proceedings of the Royal Socoiety of London B 285 20180542. 10.1098/rspb.2018.0542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Prokopy R.J. & Lewis W.J. (1993) Application of learning to pest management. In Papaj D.R. & Lewis A.C. (eds) Insect learning: Ecology and evolutionary perspectives. London, UK: Chapman & Hall. pp 308–342. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Leung K., Ras E., Ferguson K.B., Ariens S., Babendreier D., Bijma P., et al. (2020) Next generation biological control: the need for integrating genetics and genomics. Biological Reviews 95: 1838–1854. 10.1111/brv.12641 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wardle A.R & Borden J.H. (1986) Detrimental effect of prior conditioning on host habitat location by Exeristes roborator. Naturwissenschaften 73: 559–560. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vet L.E.M. (1995) Parasitoid foraging: the importance of variation in individual behaviour for population dynamics. In: Floyd R.B. (ed.) The Nicholson Centenary Meeting: Frontiers of population ecology, CSIRO Publishing, Melbourne, Australia, pp 254–256. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.König K., Krimmer E., Brose S., Gantert C., Buschlüter I., König C., et al. (2015) Does early learning drive ecological divergence during speciation processes in parasitoid wasps? Proceedings of the Royal Society B 282: 20141850. 10.1098/rspb.2014.1850 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Botch P.S., Delfosse E.S. (2018) Host-Acceptance behavior of Trissolcus japonicus (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae) reared on the invasive Halyomorpha halys (Heteroptera: Pentatomidae) and nontarget species. Environmental Entomology 47:403–411. 10.1093/ee/nvy014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Boyle S.M., Weber D.C., Hough-Goldstein J. & Hoelmer K.A. (2020) Parental host species affects behavior and parasitism by the pentatomid egg parasitoid, Trissolcus japonicus (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae). Biological Control 149: 104324. 10.1016/j.biocontrol.2020.104324 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Heimpel G., Neuhauser C. & Hoogendoorn M. (2003) Effects of parasitoid fecundity and host resistance on indirect interactions among hosts sharing a parasitoid. Ecology Letters 6:556–566. 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00466.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Robertson B.A. & Blumstein D.T. 2019. How to disarm an evolutionary trap. Conservation Science and Practice 1:e116. 10.1111/csp2.116 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

S1 File. R script.

R scripts and supplementary figures.

(PDF)

S1 Data. T. podisi data.

(XLSX)

Data Availability Statement

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES