Summary
Although males and females largely share the same genome and nervous system, they differ profoundly in reproductive investments and require distinct behavioral, morphological, and physiological adaptations. How can the nervous system, while bound by both developmental and biophysical constraints, produce these sex differences in behavior? Here, we uncover a novel dimorphism in Drosophila melanogaster that allows deployment of completely different behavioral repertoires in males and females with minimum changes to circuit architecture. Sexual differentiation of only a small number of higher order neurons in the brain leads to a change in connectivity related to the primary reproductive needs of both sexes—courtship pursuit in males and communal oviposition in females. This study explains how an apparently similar brain generates distinct behavioral repertoires in the two sexes and presents a fundamental principle of neural circuit organization that may be extended to other species.
Keywords: sexually dimorphic neurons, sex-specific neurons, sexual behavior, adaptive sex differences, courtship, tracking, egg laying, Drosophila
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
doublesex-expressing aDN neurons have sexually dimorphic inputs
-
•
Male aDNs receive visual inputs via LC10 visual projection neurons
-
•
Female aDNs receive olfactory inputs via the lateral horn
-
•
aDNs control visual courtship pursuit in males and communal egg laying in females
How does the nervous system produce sex differences in behavior? Nojima et al. report a sexually dimorphic neuronal cluster in the Drosophila brain that receives sex-specific sensory information: visual signals required for courtship pursuit in males and olfactory signals required for communal egg laying in females.
Introduction
Sexually reproducing species exhibit sex differences in social interactions to boost reproductive success and survival of progeny. Comparing and contrasting the anatomy, activity, and function of sexually dimorphic neurons in the brain of males and females across taxa are starting to reveal the fundamental principles of neural circuit organization underlying these sex differences in behavior. A variety of alternative neuronal circuit configurations have been proposed to generate sexually dimorphic behaviors.1 Many studies have identified sex differences in sensory inputs in various species; however, such differences in higher order brain circuits that organize species- and sex-specific instinctive behaviors in response to sensory cues are still poorly characterized.
Sex is determined early in an animal’s development and initiates many irreversible sexual differentiation events that influence how the genome and the environment interact to give rise to sex-specific morphology and behavior. In Drosophila, selective expression of two sex determination transcription factors (TFs), Doublesex (Dsx) and Fruitless (Fru), define cell-type-specific developmental programs that govern functional connectivity and lay the foundations through which innate sexual behaviors are genetically predetermined.2, 3, 4, 5, 6 Because both fru- and dsx-expressing neurons are essential for male and female reproductive behaviors, studies in the adult have focused on neurons that express these TFs to identify anatomical or molecular sex differences in neuronal populations.7 This allows us to gain entry to the neural circuits underlying sex-typical behaviors and identify the neuronal nodes that control component behaviors and behavioral sequencing.
Dsx proteins, which are part of the structurally and functionally conserved Doublesex and Male-abnormal-3 Related Transcription factors (DMRT) protein family, are critical for sex-specific differentiation throughout the animal kingdom.8 In the insect phylum, Dsx proteins act at the interface between sex determination and sexual differentiation, regulating a myriad of somatic sexual differences both inside and outside the nervous system.9 The dsx gene has functions in both sexes: its transcripts undergo sex-specific alternative splicing to encode either a male- or female-specific isoform.10,11 dsx expression is highly regulated in both male and female flies, as shown by its temporally and spatially restricted expression patterns through development, with only a select group of neurons expressing dsx.12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17 The dsx gene is expressed in some 150 and 30–40 neurons per hemisphere in the male and female brains, which reside in 10 and 7 to 8 discrete anatomical clusters, respectively.12,14,15,17,18 This restricted expression of dsx in higher order neurons in the brain suggests these neurons may act as key sex-specific processing nodes of sensory information.
To study the fundamental principles of neural circuit organization underlying sex differences in behavior, we identified and mapped dsx+ sexual dimorphisms in the CNS. Our analyses revealed that all dsx+ clusters are either sexually dimorphic or sex specific; none are sexually monomorphic. To examine higher order processing differences between the sexes, we focused on the dsx+ anterior dorsal neuron (aDN) cluster, as it is present in both sexes yet has sexually dimorphic dendritic arborizations associated with sensory perception. We show that these anatomical differences lead to sex-specific connectivity, with male aDN inputs being exclusively visual, while female inputs are primarily olfactory. Finally, we show that this unique sexually dimorphic neuronal hub that reroutes distinct sensory pathways gives rise to functionally distinct social behaviors between the sexes: visual tracking during courtship in males and communal egg-laying site selection in females.
Results
dsx-expressing neurons in the CNS are either sexually dimorphic or sex specific
To systematically investigate the anatomy of dsx-expressing neuronal clusters in the CNS of both sexes, we carried out a single cluster-level screen of dsx+ neurons employing genetic mosaic and intersectional approaches.19,20 We used image co-registration to compare individually labeled male and female clusters to identify anatomical sexual dimorphisms (data available through Virtual Fly Brain).21 After dissecting approximately 3,500 brains, we found that all dsx+ clusters examined in the brain showed anatomical sex differences in their cell numbers and/or neurite morphologies (Figure 1; Table S1; see Lee et al.,12 Robinett et al.,17 and Kimura et al.18 for cluster nomenclature). A similarly detailed analysis of dimorphism in the ventral nerve cord (VNC) is described in Figure S1 and Table S1. Collectively, our single-cluster mapping demonstrates that dsx+ neuronal clusters in the CNS are either sex specific or sexually dimorphic, while none are sexually monomorphic.
dsx+ aDNs have sexually dimorphic input sites
dsx expression is restricted to a small number of sexually dimorphic higher order neurons in the brain in both sexes, suggesting they may act as key processing nodes of sensory information that subserve sex-specific behaviors. The sexually dimorphic dsx+ aDN cluster, consisting of two neurons per hemisphere with their somata located anteriorly in the superior lateral protocerebrum (SLP), is a striking example of this functional organization (Figure 1).
Overt sexual dimorphisms in sensory connectivity with the aDNs were apparent when examining their neurites. All aDN neurites are restricted to the ipsilateral protocerebra and do not cross the midline (Figures 2A–2H). Male and female laterally localized neurites terminate in noticeably different neuropils, exhibiting clear sexually dimorphic arborization patterns. Male neurites are restricted to the anterior optic tubercle (AOTu), one of the central-brain optic glomeruli through which visual signals are conveyed via visual projection neurons (VPNs) from the optic lobe to the central brain.22, 23, 24, 25, 26 In the female, the lateral neurites are mainly in the posterior part of the superior lateral protocerebrum (pSLP), with minor processes in the superior clamp (SCL) and ventrolateral protocerebrum (VLP) (Figures 2A–2H). These neuropil regions receive multiple sensory inputs, including those from contact chemosensory, mechanosensory, olfactory, and auditory interneurons.27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 33 aDN medial neurites, in contrast, terminate in the same areas in both sexes, the superior medial protocerebrum (SMP) (Figures 2A–2C). These sex-specific neurite patterns make it a particularly interesting candidate for a node that could process sensory inputs differently between the sexes.
To determine the flow of information through the aDNs in males and females, we examined pre- and postsynaptic markers using a split-Gal4 hemi-driver combination, dsx∩VGlut, which specifically labels the aDN in the brain.34 In males, postsynaptic input (DenMark)35 signals were detected in the AOTu (Figures 2I and 2K), while in females, input sites were observed in the pSLP/SCL/VLP regions (Figures 2J and 2K). In contrast, aDN presynaptic output sites (Syb::EGFP)36 were restricted to the SMP in both sexes (Figures 2L–2N). These sex differences in dendritic arborization lead to dimorphic connectivity in aDN neurons and suggest they receive information from different sensory modalities in males and females.
Male aDNs receive inputs from visual projection neurons
aDNs receive extensive visual inputs from the AOTu in males, but not in females. Among more than 20 known classes of VPNs, a single class called the lobula columnar 10 (LC10) neurons send their axonal projections exclusively to the AOTu (Figure 3A).22 A subpopulation of LC10 neurons, LC10a, regulates visual aspects of male courtship behavior, whereas no clear behavioral function has been characterized in females.25 Intriguingly, the anatomy and physiological responses of these LC10 neurons are similar between the sexes.25 These findings led us to hypothesize that males and females might receive the same visual stimuli through LC10 neurons but produce distinct behavioral responses due to sex differences in their central-brain connectivity.
We examined connectivity between LC10s and aDNs in males using an LC10a-specific transgenic driver (Figure S2A).25 Co-registration of LC10a presynaptic sites with the postsynaptic dendritic fields of the aDNs revealed a clear overlap in males, but not in females (Figures 3B and 3C). GFP reconstitution across synaptic partners (GRASP)37 also detected proximity between LC10a and aDN in males, but not in females (Figures 3D–3G); control males showed no GFP signals (Figures S2B and S2C). These observations suggest that LC10a neurons connect to aDNs in males, but not in females. To corroborate the absence of LC10a inputs to the aDN in females, we plotted aDN and LC10a neurons in the same reference space using connectivity information from the densely reconstructed hemibrain connectome via the neuPrint (v1.1) database (https://neuprint.janelia.org/).38 We confirmed that female aDNs do not receive input from any of the approximately 450 LC10a single neurons annotated in the hemibrain dataset (Figure S2D).
To test whether aDN and LC10a neurons are functionally connected in males, we light-stimulated LC10a neurons expressing the red-light-activated channelrhodopsin CsChrimson39 while simultaneously recording fluorescence from aDNs expressing GCaMP6f.34,40 Initially, we did not see a response in aDNs in either sex with different stimulation patterns (Figures 3H and 3I, top, and S2E–S2H), in apparent contrast to their anatomical connection revealed by GRASP (Figures 3D and 3E). We reasoned that the response in aDNs might be masked by inhibition from other upstream neurons, potentially dependent on the male’s courtship arousal state.41 To test this possibility, we blocked inhibitory inputs by applying picrotoxin (PTX), an inhibitor of glutamate-gated chloride channels and gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA)-A receptors, and CGP54626, a GABA-B receptor antagonist, to the brain 15 min before the imaging experiment.41, 42, 43 After blocking inhibition, we observed a significant calcium response in male aDN dendrites during stimulation of LC10a, which increased with a higher concentration of PTX (Figures 3H and 3I). The apparent axonal responses in males were not significant, probably due to larger variability, with only some of the dendritic inputs leading to synaptic output from axon terminals. Light-evoked responses were male specific (Figures 3H and 3I) and could not be detected in controls (Figures S2I and S2J), even though both sexes showed spontaneous activity after application of PTX (data not shown), probably as a result of disinhibition of other inputs to aDNs. Therefore, anatomical and physiological results confirm that the male, but not female, aDNs receive visual inputs from LC10a VPNs. This striking dimorphism in aDN input sites generates different routing of sensory information between males and females.
Male aDNs function in visually guided courtship behavior
LC10a VPNs have been shown to extract object motion relative to the background, recognizing the current position of a potential mate during courtship.25 We therefore tested whether aDNs might be a necessary downstream target to mediate their role in courtship. We blocked evoked transmission from aDNs in male flies using expression of a TNT transgene44 and tested their courtship behavior. To specifically target aDNs, we combined the split-Gal4 hemi-driver dsx∩VGlut and a brain-specifically expressed FLP recombinase (Otd-FLP)45 with a Gal4/FLP-responsive TNT effector (Figure S3A).46
Courtship intensity and copulation success were markedly reduced in aDN-silenced males (dsx∩VGlut∩Otd > TNT) compared with genetic controls (Figures 4A and 4B). However, they exhibited normal latencies to initiate courtship (Figure 4C). Pronounced abnormalities were apparent when examining videos of aDN-silenced males (Videos S1 and S2). To quantify specific behavioral deficits, we trained an automated behavioral annotation system to detect the following male courtship behaviors: wing extension (a courtship-specific behavior used to generate song), approaching, facing, turning, circling, and contact (STAR methods). We observed a reduction in both approaching and facing indices and a trend for an increase in the contact index (a measure of the minimum distance between the male and female; Figures S3B–S3D). Importantly, all other indices examined were unaffected, suggesting specific rather than general deficits (Figures S3E–S3G). As approaching and facing behaviors both depend on being able to localize the female, we hypothesized that aDN-inactivated males have difficulties tracking females. We therefore measured how the male orients toward the female during courtship by examining the position of the female relative to the male while he was unilaterally extending his wing. aDN-silenced males exhibited an increase in the amount of misdirected courtship behavior (i.e., the male extended his wing when the female was not in front of him; Figures 4D and S3H; Videos S1 and S2). This defect is more discernible by examining the facing angle (Figures 4E and 4G) and distance between the male and female (Figures 4F and 4H), which were both increased compared to controls, a phenotype reminiscent of LC10a-silenced males.25 The minimum distance, on the other hand, was decreased (Figure 4I), showing that aDN-silenced males position themselves at a broader range of distances while singing to the female. These manipulations demonstrate that aDN-silenced males were impaired in their ability to orient and track female movement during courtship. These deficits are linked to a difficulty in locating and following the female, not a reduced motivation to court. aDN-silenced males are not blind in general, as they showed the same preference for light as controls in a phototaxis assay (data not shown). Our data are consistent with the male aDN playing a role in motion detection during male courtship behavior.
While courting, males unilaterally extend their wing closest to the female (ipsilateral wing) to produce courtship song.47 Although aDNs and LC10a neurons in males are both involved in visually guided courtship pursuit of the female, their roles in wing extension appear to differ. aDN-silenced males displayed normal levels of unilateral wing extension (Figure S3I) and, in contrast to LC10a-silenced males,25 exhibited normal ipsi- versus contralateral wing choice (Figure S3J). aDN-silenced males did, however, display an increase in bilateral wing extension (Figures 4J and S3K). This increase was due to an increase in the number of bouts (Figure 4K) and not the bout length (Figure S3I). To test whether these phenotypes depend on visual cues, we tested courtship behavior in the dark and found no significant differences between aDN-silenced males and controls (Figures S3M–S3R), suggesting that, in the absence of visual cues, aDN neurons are not necessary for normal courtship behavior. Bilateral thermogenetic activation of aDNs in solitary males did not induce any wing extensions (data not shown), in contrast to LC10a bilateral activation, which induces unilateral wing extensions.25 Our findings suggest that visually guided courtship behavior through LC10a VPNs consists of two separable components—visual tracking and wing choice—likely mediated by different sets of downstream neurons. aDN is involved in the former, but not the latter. We therefore set out to identify additional downstream targets of LC10a that might control wing choice.
The fru-expressing AL5a cluster is downstream of LC10a VPNs
To identify additional neurons downstream of LC10a, we examined a candidate neuronal cluster, AL5a, which innervates the AOTu and expresses the sex-determination gene fru.21,48,49 Interestingly, when AL5a is unilaterally activated, male flies frequently display unilateral wing extension,50 a behavioral phenotype reminiscent of unilateral LC10a activation.25 Based on these observations, we hypothesized that AL5a (also known as aSP-I and aSP11)21,49 is downstream of LC10a.
Co-registration of LC10a with the AL5a cluster revealed clear overlap in the AOTu region in males and females (Figures 5A–5C and S4A). To investigate the anatomical and physiological connectivity between LC10a and AL5a, we identified a Gal4 line that is expressed in AL5a neurons and confirmed fru+ AL5a AOTu innervation (Figures S4B and S4C).21,49 We found that AL5a input sites are associated with the AOTu (Figures 5D and 5E), and GRASP detected robust signals in both sexes between LC10a and AL5a neurons in the dorsal side of the AOTu (Figures 5F and S4D). Our findings suggest that the AL5a neurons connect with LC10a neurons in both sexes.
To confirm that LC10a and AL5a neurons are functionally connected, we optogenetically activated the LC10a neuronal population while simultaneously imaging calcium transients in the dendritic arbors of AL5a neurons in the AOTu. We observed a large calcium response in AL5a input sites in both sexes (Figures 5G, 5H, and S4E–S4K), suggesting that, unlike aDNs, AL5a receives inputs from LC10a in both sexes. Together, these data indicate AL5a neurons are downstream of LC10a neurons in both sexes, whereas connectivity from LC10a to aDN is male specific.
Female aDNs receive multimodal sensory inputs
While males utilize aDNs to process visual inputs, female aDNs have alternative inputs. Using ultrastructural connectivity, we found female aDNs process information from multiple sensory modalities, the most prominent of which are olfactory and thermo- and hygrosensory cues. We examined direct synaptic connectivity between annotated aDNs and circuit elements in the female hemibrain electron microscopy (EM) dataset (Figure 6A). In agreement with our light imaging results, we found the dendrites of aDNs span multiple neuropils in females (Figures 6A and 6B), whereas the axonal region is restricted to the SMP neuropil. Although over 80% of aDN inputs and outputs were restricted to the dendritic and axonal regions, respectively, many axo-axonic inputs and dendro-dendritic outputs were found (Figure 6C). We next examined the identity of the neurons pre- and postsynaptic to the aDN (Figures S5A and S5B; Table S2). We focused on the 30 neuron types with the largest contribution to the synaptic input budget (Figure 6D). A clear connection between olfactory input processing neurons and the aDN was apparent, as 9 of the neuron types are associated with the lateral horn (LH) and receive inputs from olfactory projection neurons (PNs) (Figures 6D and S5A). The LH is involved in innate odor responses as well as thermo- and hygrosensory responses.51 We also identified direct inputs from potential olfactory and thermo-/hygrosensory PNs into the aDN, including the multi-glomerular VP5+ adPN, known to have extensive innervation in the humid air sensing VP5 glomerulus in the antennal lobe (AL) as well as the subesophageal zone (SEZ) (Figures 6D and S5C).52 In addition to antennal sensory inputs, multiple neurons ascending through the lateral antennal lobe tract (lALT) synapse onto the aDN (IALT1 and IALT2; Figures 6D and S5D). The morphology of these lALT-tract axons is reminiscent of contact chemosensory and mechanosensory ascending neurons from the VNC.31,32 Together, this indicates that female aDNs receive multimodal sensory inputs, many of which are pre-processed olfactory and thermo- and hygrosensory cues inputting through the LH.
Female aDNs receive functionally relevant olfactory inputs
To analyze functional connectivity between the olfactory system and the aDNs, we expressed the optogenetic activator CsChrimson under the control of a broad antennal-lobe projection neuron driver line, GH146-LexA, which expresses in two-thirds of all second-order olfactory PNs,53,54 and monitored the activity of aDNs expressing GCaMP6f upon light stimulation. When PNs were optogenetically activated, female, but not male, aDNs showed an increase in fluorescence in both their dendritic and axonal compartments (Figures 6E–6G), with similar responses in both virgin and mated females. These responses confirm that female aDNs are downstream of the antennal-lobe PNs and, importantly, that this functional connectivity is female specific. To test whether this sex-specific response was at least in part olfactory in origin, we expressed the optogenetic activator CsChrimson under the control of a broad olfactory sensory neuron (OSN) driver line, Orco-LexA, which expresses in most OSNs in the basiconic and trichoid sensilla.55 We found that female, but not male, aDNs showed an increase in calcium signals during stimulation of OSNs. Intriguingly, these responses were more robust in 24–48 h post-mated females compared to virgin females, suggesting a change in aDN responses to odors after mating (Figures 6H and 6I) and predicting a role for aDN neurons in female post-mating behavior. These physiological data, in conjunction with the pattern of female-specific aDN connectivity in the brain, confirm that female aDNs respond to olfactory rather than visual input.
The female aDNs play a role in egg-laying site selection
When examining aDN connectivity, we found striking recurrent connectivity between aDNs and recently identified egg-laying circuitry (Figures 6D and 7A–7D).56 Indeed, the neuron with the most synaptic inputs to the aDNs, with axo-axonic connections making up 15.5% of the axonal input budget, is the oviposition excitatory neuron oviEN, which mediates external sensory signals involved in egg-laying site selection in mated females (Figures 7A and S5A).56 oviEN and aDN cell bodies are distinct and derived from different neuroblasts; however, their neuronal processes have nearly identical layouts (Figure 7B), suggesting they may integrate similar information (e.g., LH pre-processed olfactory). oviENs are excitatory cholinergic neurons, whereas aDNs are glutamatergic and could be inhibitory, excitatory, or both depending on postsynaptic receptor expression,57,58 suggesting there may be differences in the way these two types of neurons process sensory information. The oviposition inhibitory neuron (oviIN) also forms synapses onto the axons of the aDNs from both the ipsilateral and contralateral hemispheres (Figure 7C), together accounting for 18.4% of the aDNs’ axonal budget (Figure 6D). The oviIN conveys information about female mating status, potentially inhibiting aDN axonal output in virgin females only.56 We found that aDNs directly output onto oviDNa, a female-specific descending neuron controlling egg laying (Figures 7A and 7D),56 and an uncharacterized neuron SMP156, which appears highly integrated into the egg-laying circuitry (Figure 7A). aDNs devote the largest proportion of their axonal budget to SMP156, contributing the highest fraction to SMP156s inputs (5.7%). The oviEN contributes the 2nd highest (1.8%) to the SMP156 inputs, with oviIN neuron inputs being less prominent (0.86%). SMP156 outputs across both hemispheres in the inferior bridge (IB), a neuropil that spans the midline, implying it is involved in comparison and analysis of signals from different directions (Figures S6A and S6B).59 The prominent connectivity of SMP156 neurons with egg-laying circuitry suggests it might act as an integrator of egg-laying signals.
The connections between aDNs, egg-laying circuitry, and the olfactory system, as well as their mating state-dependent responses to olfactory signals, predict a role in the social aspects of egg laying. In a social context, females rely on olfactory pheromonal cues deposited by resident flies at shared egg-laying sites.60 We tested the hypothesis that the aDNs are necessary for this female-specific social and reproductive behavior by giving mated females the choice to lay eggs either on a patch marked with male pheromone extracts or a patch devoid of social cues (Figure 7E). Male pheromones attracted control females to lay their eggs on the pheromone-marked patch, while females in which aDNs where silenced by TNT did not distinguish between patches marked with pheromones or those devoid of them, resulting in a significant difference among genotypes (Figure 7F). Moreover, as silencing of aDNs did not have an effect on the number of eggs laid (Figure 7G) but did on the preference for pheromone-marked egg-laying sites, these data indicate that aDNs have a specific role in egg-laying site selection and not on fecundity. Finally, we asked whether the aDNs play a role in female pre-mating behaviors. Copulation latency and overall fertility were unaffected in aDN-silenced virgin females (Figures S6C–S6E). These data suggest a specific role for female aDNs in egg-laying site selection based on olfactory cues rather than a general role in mating behavior.
Discussion
We have identified a small cluster of two neurons per hemisphere in the central brain, which reconfigures circuit logic in a sex-specific manner. Perhaps most surprising is the seemingly unrelated behaviors these equivalent neurons control in each sex—visual tracking during courtship in males and communal egg laying in females. Ultimately, these circuit reconfigurations lead to the same end result—an increase in reproductive success. Our findings highlight a flexible strategy used to structure the nervous system, where relatively minor modifications in neuronal networks allow each sex to respond to their social environment in a sex-appropriate manner.
The behavioral function of the male aDN cluster appears to be related to visual aspects of courtship behavior. A set of visual projection neurons, LC10a, was previously identified as involved in tracking and following behaviors in the male during courtship; however, no apparent sex differences in their anatomy or their physiological responses to visual stimuli were detected.25 It would seem these sex differences in behavior arise from the sex-specific downstream connectivity of LC10a neurons in the central brain. Here, we identify aDNs connecting downstream to LC10a in males only. aDN inactivation mirrors visual tracking defects displayed upon LC10a inactivation (Figure 4); therefore, the male aDN cluster confers sex specificity to visually guided tracking of females during courtship.
We additionally identified AL5a neurons to be downstream of LC10a in both sexes. Interestingly, it has been reported that AL5a is likely upstream of the fru+ cluster Lv2/pIP-b/pIP8,48,49 thought to exchange and integrate visual information from the right and left hemispheres of the brain. This male-specific connectivity is compatible with a potential role for AL5a in mediating visual information necessary for wing choice during courtship, a behavior these neurons have been shown to elicit when activated.50
The two LC10a downstream clusters we identified, aDN and AL5a, also show differences in their anatomical connectivity and physiological responses. Whereas AL5a is downstream of LC10a in both sexes, aDN is only connected to LC10a in the male. Despite direct anatomical connectivity between LC10a and aDN in males (Figure 3), functional connectivity was only uncovered under conditions of pharmacological disinhibition. This observation might hint at inhibitory modulation of aDN that depends on the male’s internal state, e.g., his mating drive, or additional cues that influence his courtship arousal. A previous study found that, in sexually satiated males, calcium responses in courtship “decision-making” P1 neurons were absent when stimulating upstream neurons but could be restored to the levels observed in naive males by application of PTX.41 It is tempting to speculate that inhibition in the LC10a → aDN pathway is similarly linked to sexual arousal. In contrast, AL5a responses to LC10a stimulation occurred in the absence of PTX and were markedly larger in AL5a than in aDN (compare Figures 3H, 3I, 5G, and 5H). The variation in calcium signals could be due to the considerable difference in cell numbers comprising each cluster (2 aDN versus 24 AL5a) or due to inputs from different AOTu regions. aDNs sample from the whole glomerulus region, whereas the AL5a cluster is restricted to the dorsal part of the AOTu, suggesting they extract information from broad versus specific parts of the visual field, respectively. Future investigation will be aimed at linking the clusters’ anatomical differences with their differential processing of visual information to facilitate distinct behavioral roles.
In females, the aDN cluster does not receive visual information but appears to sample from a range of sensory modalities, with information received via the antennal lobe dominating its inputs, suggesting its involvement in a complex behavior requiring multisensory integration. One such behavior is female egg-laying site selection, which is critical to the success of offspring.61 For Drosophila, offspring survival rates depend on the selection of oviposition sites that are shared with conspecifics, a process known to rely on olfaction.60 We have shown that aDNs are highly integrated into circuitry known to regulate oviposition (Figure 7A).56 The excitatory oviEN, which is anatomically similar to the aDNs, responds to information about substrate suitability via gustatory and mechanosensory cues in the legs and directly influences aDN output. Silencing oviEN function suppresses egg laying itself, whereas silencing aDN does not affect the overall number of eggs laid. Instead, aDN-silenced females are no longer able to show a preference to lay eggs communally, losing a female-specific social behavior essential for offspring survival.62 While both oviEN and aDN output directly onto the oviposition motor program (through oviDNs), oviENs are the largest contributors to oviDN dendritic budgets, with aDN being relatively minor contributors. Thus, the aDN cluster acts as a modulator of egg laying choice, whereas the oviEN more generally affects the mechanics of egg laying.
As the oviposition of fertilized eggs is a female behavior that can only be displayed after mating, the behavioral programs required are likely inhibited in virgin females. The activity of the inhibitory neuron oviIN depends on female mating status and thus appears to act as a general inhibitor of egg-laying circuitry in virgin females.56 oviINs form axo-axonic synapses with both the aDN and oviEN, suggesting they gate their outputs by presynaptic inhibition in a state-dependent manner.63 Intriguingly, as both oviEN and oviIN form axo-axonic synapses with aDN, this suggests a potential gating mechanism by which their relative strengths inhibit or facilitate output from aDN onto downstream targets.
Consistent with aDNs’ behavioral function in egg-laying site selection, a female post-mating behavior, we found differences in the aDN physiological responses in mated versus virgin females. Stimulation of OSNs resulted in significantly stronger aDN calcium responses in mated females compared to virgins (Figures 6E–6I). This finding might hint at a state-dependent inhibition of olfactory inputs into aDN in females, potentially analogous to the inhibition of visual inputs to aDN observed in males. The difference in physiological responses between mated and virgin females was not observed when stimulating PNs, which are downstream of OSNs but upstream of aDN. There are different possible explanations for this discrepancy, including differences in the populations of neurons targeted by the driver lines used to target PNs versus OSNs or inhibition in virgin females occurring at the level of OSN to PN connectivity; therefore, activating PNs directly bypasses the state-dependent inhibition. In addition to state-dependent effects, there also seemed to be differences in the calcium responses in different neuronal compartments (Figures 6E–6I). This finding could be explained by the position of the input synapses of different upstream neurons into the aDN (e.g., dendritic versus axonic). The exact mechanism of how aDN integrates these different inputs and transforms them into an output that guides egg-laying site selection remains to be examined.
The principal output of the female aDN is the previously undescribed SMP156 neuron, which itself outputs primarily in the IB, where its axons show cross-hemisphere connectivity, suggesting it acts as integrators of sensory information from different directions. The major SMP156 output neuron type (IB011) projects to the lobula in the opposite hemisphere, potentially integrating olfactory and visual information as observed in other flying insects during pheromone orientation.64,65 Olfactory navigation requires comparisons of left and right inputs, e.g., when male moths orient themselves toward conspecific females in response to sex pheromones.66 Determination of position and direction applies to males pursuing females and females following pheromonal cues to locate a communal egg-laying site. We propose that the aDN cluster in females selectively integrates sensory information, relaying it to SMP156, which confers directionality and processes information relevant to locating an appropriate egg-laying site. In the absence of a male connectome for comparison, we can only speculate about potential shared downstream connectivity. As the male aDN output sites are mainly overlapping with female sites in the SMP (Figures 2J and 2L), it is possible that the male visual pathway also inputs into SMP156, or a similar neuron associated with the IB, potentially feeding back onto visual pathways, supporting appropriate tracking of the female. A male connectome and more genetic tools will help reveal the full extent of downstream functional connectivity and convergence between the sexes.
As fundamental features of most animal species, sexual dimorphisms and sex differences have particular importance for the function of the nervous system. These innate sex-specific adaptations are built during development and orchestrate interactions between sensory information and specific brain regions to shape the phenotype, including the emergent properties of the sex-specific neural circuitry. Evolutionary forces acting on these neural systems have generated adaptive sex differences in behavior.67 In Drosophila, males compete for a mate through courtship displays, while a female’s investment is focused on the success of their offspring. These sex-specific behaviors are guided by the perception and processing of sensory cues, ensuring responses lead to reproductive success. In this study, we have shown how a sex-specific switch between visual and olfactory inputs underlies adaptive sex differences in behavior and provides insight on how similar mechanisms may be implemented in the brains of other sexually dimorphic species.
STAR★Methods
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Antibodies | ||
Anti-GFP Polyclonal (rabbit) | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A6455; RRID: AB_221570 |
Anti-GFP Monoclonal (mouse) | Sigma-Aldrich | G6539; RRID: AB_259941 |
Anti-mCherry Monoclonal 16D7 (rat) | Thermo Fisher Scientific | M11217; RRID: AB_2536611 |
Anti-Brp (nc82) Monoclonal (mouse) | Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank (University of Iowa) | RRID: AB_2314866 |
Anti-CadN DN-Ex #8 (mouse) | Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank (University of Iowa) | RRID: AB_528121 |
Goat Anti-rabbit Alexa 488 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A11034; RRID: AB_2576217 |
Goat Anti-rat Alexa 546 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A11081; RRID: AB_2534125 |
Goat Anti-rat Alexa 633 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A21094; RRID: AB_2535749 |
Goat anti-mouse Alexa 488 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A28175; RRID: AB_2536161 |
Goat Anti-mouse Alexa 546 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A11030; RRID: AB_2534089 |
Goat Anti-mouse Alexa 633 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | A21050; RRID: AB_2535718 |
Chemicals, peptides, and recombinant proteins | ||
N-hexane | Fisher Scientific | CAS Number-110-54-3 |
BD Difco | Fisher Scientific | CAS Number-9002-18-0 |
Yeast extract powder | Fisher Scientific | CAS Number-8013-01-02 |
Formaldehyde | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# 47608-250ML-F |
Normal Goat Serum | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# G9023 |
Phosphate buffered saline (PBS) | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# P3183-10PAK |
Triton X-100 | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# T8787-100ML |
All-trans retinal | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# R2500 |
NaCl | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# S7653 |
KCl | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# P9333 |
NaHCO3 | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# S6297 |
NaH2PO4 | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# S8282 |
CaCl2 | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# 21115 |
MgCl2 | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# M1028 |
N-Tris | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# T5691 |
Trehalose | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# T9531 |
Glucose | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# G7528 |
Sucrose | Sigma-Aldrich | Cat# S0389 |
Vectashield mounting medium | Vector Laboratories | Cat# H-1000 |
Deposited data | ||
doublesex+ clones | This study | https://v2.virtualflybrain.org |
Experimental models: organisms/strains | ||
Drosophila: wild-type Canton-S | Gift from Jeffrey Hall | N/A |
Drosophila: dsxGal4.DBD | Pavlou et al.34 | N/A |
Drosophila: dsxGal4 | Rideout et al.16 | N/A |
Drosophila: VGlutdVP16.AD | Gao et al.68 | N/A |
Drosophila: VT029314-LexA | Ribeiro et al.25 | N/A |
Drosophila: R35D04-p65.AD; R22D06-Gal4.DBD | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_68336 |
Drosophila: fruNP21 | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_30027 |
Drosophila: GMR22D04-Gal4 | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_48981 |
Drosophila: fruFLP | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_66870 |
Drosophila: UAS > mCherry > ReaChR | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_53740 |
Drosophila: 10xUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_32185 |
Drosophila: 40xUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_32195 |
Drosophila: UAS-DenMark, UAS-Syb::EGFP | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_33064 |
Drosophila: UAS-CD4::spGFP1-10, lexAop-CD4::spGFP11 | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_58755 |
Drosophila: UAS-GCaMP6f | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_42747 |
Drosophila: 13xlexAop2-IVS-myr::GFP | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_32210 |
Drosophila: 13xLexAop2-IVS-CsChrimson::tdTomato | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_82183 |
Drosophila: UAS > stop > TNT | Bloomington DSC | RRID: BDSC_67690 |
Drosophila: GH146-LexA | Gift from Tzumin Lee | N/A |
Drosophila: Orco-LexA::VP16 | Gift from Tzumin Lee | N/A |
Drosophila: Trh-p65.AD | Gift from Gerald Rubin | N/A |
Drosophila: hs-FLP(22); FRTG13, UAS-mCD8::GFP | Gift from Daisuke Yamamoto | N/A |
Drosophila: hs-FLP(22); FRTG13, tubP-Gal80 | Gift from Daisuke Yamamoto | N/A |
Drosophila: Otd-FLP | Gift from David Anderson | N/A |
Drosophila: UAS > stop > mCD8::GFP | Gift from Barry Dickson | N/A |
Software and algorithms | ||
Fiji | open source | https://fiji.sc |
Image stabilizer | From Kang Li | http://www.cs.cmu.edu/∼kangli/code/Image_Stabilizer.html |
CMTK Registration Toolkit | From Gregory Jefferis | https://github.com/jefferis/fiji-cmtk-gui |
Neuroglancer Hub | Google; Janelia EM; Janelia Scientific Computing | https://neuroglancerhub.github.io/ |
neuPRINT | Janelia EM | https://neuprint.janelia.org/ |
Blender v2.8.2 | Blender | https://www.blender.org/download/releases/2-82/ |
navis v2.2.-blender interface | Python Software Foundation GitHub | https://pypi.org/project/navis/https://github.com/schlegelp/navis |
FlyLight | HHMI Janelia | http://splitgal4.janelia.org/cgi-bin/splitgal4.cgi |
NeuronBridge | HHMI Janelia | https://neuronbridge.janelia.org |
Amira 5.4.2 | Thermo Fisher Scientific | RRID: SCR_007353 |
Caltech Fly Tracker | Eyjolfsdottir et al.69 | http://www.vision.caltech.edu/Tools/FlyTracker/ |
JAABA | Kabra et al.70 | http://jaaba.sourceforge.net/ |
GraphPad Prism 8 | GraphPad Software, La Jolla, CA | https://www.graphpad.com/scientific-software/prism/ |
ImageJ | open source | https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/ |
Adobe Illustrator CC | Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA | https://www.adobe.com/uk/products/illustrator.html |
MATLAB | The Mathworks, Natick, MA | https://uk.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html |
LabView | National Instruments | https://www.ni.com/en-gb/shop/labview.html |
any2ufmf | open source | http://ctrax.sourceforge.net/any2ufmf.html |
R | R Development Core Team, 2020 | https://www.r-project.org/ |
Calcium imaging analysis scripts | This study | https://github.com/AR2202/2-photon |
Python (scipy and statsmodels) | Python Software Foundation | https://www.python.org/ |
Behavioral analysis scripts | This study | https://github.com/aaron-allen/aDN_behaviour and https://github.com/aaron-allen/goodwin-lab-tracking |
Resource availability
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Stephen F. Goodwin (stephen.goodwin@cncb.ox.ac.uk).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Data and code availability
Scripts used for behavioral analysis are available from https://github.com/aaron-allen/aDN_behaviour and https://github.com/aaron-allen/goodwin-lab-tracking and those for calcium imaging analysis from https://github.com/AR2202/2-photon. Single neuron and cluster images described in this study will be uploaded to a publicly available database hosted by Virtual Fly Brain (https://v2.virtualflybrain.org) following publication. Requests for further details of the software and raw data should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Stephen F. Goodwin (stephen.goodwin@cncb.ox.ac.uk).
Experimental model and subject details
Fly strains
All Drosophila melanogaster strains were reared at 25°C and 40%–50% humidity on standard cornmeal-agar food in 12:12 h light:dark cycle. Flies were aged 3-8 days post eclosion. Sexes of the flies used are stated in each Figure and Legend. Flies used for optogenetic activation experiments were transferred to food containing 1 mM all-trans retinal during adulthood39. For GRASP experiments, flies were reared at 18°C and aged 14-20 days post eclosion37. Fly strains used in this study (the full genotype list is available in Table S3) include wild-type Canton-S; dsxGal4; hs-FLP(22), FRTG13, UAS-mCD8::GFP and FRTG13, tubP-Gal80 (Daisuke Yamamoto); dsxGal4.DBD; VGlutdVP16.AD; VT029314-LexA; R35D04-p65.AD and R22D06-Gal4.DBD; R22D04-Gal4 (BDSC #48981); GH146-LexA (Tzumin Lee); Orco-LexA (Tzumin Lee); Otd-FLP (David Anderson); UAS > mCherry > ReaChR (BDSC #53743); 10xUAS-IVS-mCD8::GFP (BDSC #32186); UAS > stop > mCD8::GFP (Barry Dickson); UAS-DenMark; UAS-Syb::EGFP; UAS-CD4::spGFP1-10, lexAop-CD4::spGFP11; UAS-GCaMP6f; 13XlexAop2-CsChrimson::tdTom) (BDSC #82138), and UAS > stop > TNT. See Table S3 for the concrete genotypes of the flies used in each experiment.
Method details
Generation of mosaic clones
Somatic clones were produced using the MARCM method as described previously19. The flies used for MARCM analysis were obtained from crosses between y, w, hs-FLP(22); FRTG13, UAS-mCD8::GFP; dsxGal4 and y, w, hs-FLP(22); FRTG13, tubP-Gal80; UAS-mCD8::GFP. To generate mosaic clones, chromosomal recombination was induced by applying three heat-shock treatments to embryos and larvae 24, 36 and 48 h after egg-laying at 38 °C each for 10-15 min. In another set of MARCM experiments, the flies were obtained from crosses between y, hs-flp; FRTG13, UAS-mCD8::GFP; fruNP21 and y, w, hs-FLP(22); FRTG13, tubP-Gal80; UAS-mCD8::GFP. Heat-shock was applied to embryos 24 h after egg laying at 38°C for 7 min.
Immunohistochemistry
After a brief pre-wash of adult flies in 100% EtOH to remove hydrophobic cuticular chemical compounds, brains and VNCs were dissected in PBS at RT (20-25°C), collected in 2 mL sample tubes and fixed with 4% formaldehyde (Sigma-Aldrich) in PBS (Sigma-Aldrich) for 20 min at RT. After fixation, tissues were washed in 0.7% PBS/Triton X-100 (Sigma-Aldrich) (PBT) 3 times each for 20 min at RT. After blocking in 10% normal goat serum (Sigma-Aldrich) in PBT (NGS/PBT) overnight (8-12 h) at RT, tissues were incubated in primary antibody solutions for 72 h at 4°C (1:1000, rabbit anti-GFP, Thermo Fisher Scientific; 1:100, mouse anti-GFP, Sigma-Aldrich; 1:1000, rat anti-mCherry, Thermo Fisher Scientific; 1:10, mouse anti-Brp, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank; 1:50, rat anti-CadN, Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank). After 4 washes in PBT for 1 h each at RT, tissues were incubated in secondary antibody solutions for 48 h at 4°C (1:500, anti-rabbit Alexa Fluor 488, anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 488, anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 546, anti-rat Alexa Fluor 546, anti-rat Alexa Fluor 633, anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 546, anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 633, Thermo Fisher Scientific). After 4 washes in PBT for 1 h each at RT, 70% glycerol in PBS was added to the sample tubes, which were subsequently transferred to −20°C and kept for at least 8 hr for tissue clearing. Specimens were mounted in Vectashield (Vector Laboratories).
Confocal image acquisition and processing
Confocal image stacks were acquired on a Leica TCS SP5 confocal microscope at 1024 × 1024-pixel resolution with a slice size of 1 μm. Water-immersion 25x and oil-immersion 40x objective lenses were used for VNC and brain images, respectively. Z stack images were generated, and signals in unrelated regions, background noise and unexpected tissue debris were erased using Fiji (https://fiji.sc/). Images were registered on to intersex template brain and VNC using the Fiji Computational Morphometry Toolkit (CMTK) Registration GUI (https://github.com/jefferis/fiji-cmtk-gui;)21). 3D volume-rendered images were generated using Amira 5.4.2 (Thermo Fisher Scientific; https://www.thermofisher.com/amira-avizo). The schematic drawings of the AOTu and SLP (Figures 2D and 2H) are made by modifying the JFRC2 template brain and neuropil labels downloaded from the Virtual Fly Brain database.71,72
Representations of anatomical hemibrain data
Volumetric data of neurons and neuropils was obtained from the hemibrain project’s neuPrint neuroglancer plugin (HHMI Janelia Fly EM; GoogleAI; https://neuroglancerhub.github.io/; https://neuprint.janelia.org/)38. Blender v2.8.2 with navis v2.2.-blender interface (https://pypi.org/project/navis/; https://github.com/schlegelp/navis)73 and custom python-based scripts have been used to assemble and render anatomical 3D representations. Connectivity data and synapse locations were obtained from the neuPrint database and have been processed with navis v2.2 based custom python scripts inside the blender python 3.7 console. All scripts are available upon request.
Neuron identification/classification
Neuron clusters are based on neuPrint v1.1 except for oviDNa2, oviEN and lALT1-lALT5 clusters which are described here.
aDN (541347811, 604070433), oviDNa (550655668), and oviIN (485934965) were identified previously and are annotated on neuPrint. We have compared their morphologies with our light microscopic images in case of aDN, and driver line stacks published previously56 and hosted on FlyLight (http://splitgal4.janelia.org/cgi-bin/splitgal4.cgi) for the neurons described below.
To identify oviEN and oviDNa2 neurons, we assessed connectivity data of oviDNa, oviIN and aDN to identify SMP550 (452689494) as not only the strongest input to aDN, but also to oviDNa. SMP550 is also the only strongly connected upstream neuron matching the morphology of the oviEN neurons56 as seen in stable split lines 49443 and 65426 (http://splitgal4.janelia.org/cgi-bin/splitgal4.cgi).
oviDNa and SLP410 (450971893) are downstream neurons of aDN which are morphologically similar. Their morphology matches neurons labeled by SS46540 and SS35666 published in Wang et al.56 as suggested by neuronbridge (https://neuronbridge.janelia.org). Since Wang et al.56 suggested 2 oviDNa neurons per hemisphere, we propose that they correspond to the neurons named oviDNa and SLP410 in the hemibrain dataset. SMP550/oviEN are also strong inputs to SLP410/oviDNa2, further corroborating this claim.
The newly identified clusters of VNC ascending neurons projecting through the lALT tract, clusters lALT1-5, are based on clustering of morphological features.
Anatomical and connectivity data of neurons obtained from neuPrint has been used to identify PNs and LH associated neurons following published criteria52,73,74. In brief: LH associated neurons have dendritic projections inside the LH and receive olfactory PN input. They also have outputs outside the LH. Projection neurons convey information from sensory processing neuropils to the protocerebrum. They are putatively olfactory when they receive inputs in the antennal lobe. Projection neurons of other, potentially thermo-/hygro-, modalities, ascend from the SEZ which is not present in the hemibrain dataset and have been classified according to Marin et al.52
Calcium imaging
In vivo calcium imaging of flies expressing GCaMP6f was performed at 5.92 frames per second, 256 X 256 pixels resolution, using a Two-Photon microscope (Scientifica) controlled by ScanImage 3.8 software.75 Fluorescence was excited by a Ti-Sapphire laser (Chameleon Ultra II, Coherent) at ∼140 fs pulses, 80 MHz repetition rate, centered on 910 nm. Male and female flies were kept separately from eclosion. All flies except those used as non-retinal controls were aged on food containing 1mM all-trans-retinal. Mated females were generated by mating virgin females with Canton S males 24-48h before the imaging experiment. 3 to 8-day-old adult virgin male, virgin female and mated female flies were anaesthetized on ice and mounted in a custom 3D-printed recording chamber using dental wax. The head capsule was opened using fine forceps and the brain was bathed in carbogenated (95% O2, 5% CO2) haemolymph-like solution containing 103mM NaCl, 3mM KCl, 5mM TES, 26mM NaHCO3, 1mm NaH2PO4, 1.5mM CaCl2, 4mM MgCl2, 10mM trehalose, 10mM glucose29. For the experiments in Figures 3H, 3I, S2E, and S2F, the recording solution additionally contained 50 μM CGP54626 (Sigma Aldrich) and 150-300 μM Picrotoxin (Sigma Aldrich). Optogenetic stimulation was delivered by a red LED (Multicomp OSW-6338, 630 nm) with a stimulation frequency of 40 Hz and individual pulse duration of 10 ms unless stated otherwise. Images were corrected for X and Y movement using image stabilizer in Fiji (http://www.cs.cmu.edu/∼kangli/code/Image_Stabilizer.html). All experiments that showed substantial movement in Z were discarded. Regions of interest (ROI) were selected manually. Subsequent analysis was carried out using custom scripts in MATLAB and python. To obtain average ΔF/F traces, average fluorescence in the ROI was measured against the average baseline fluorescence from 10 s to 200 ms before the onset of the optogenetic stimulation. Experiments were aligned to the optogenetic stimulus. Four optogenetic stimuli were delivered per fly at an inter-stimulus interval of 20 s, and within-fly averages were subsequently averaged across flies. Within-fly averages were used for subsequent statistical analysis and n is reported as the number of flies. Area under the curve (AUC) was calculated as the integral of the ΔF/F traces from the beginning until 1 s after the end of the optogenetic stimulus with a baseline of 1 s before the stimulus.
Courtship assays
Individual virgin males were collected and aged for 5–7 days post-eclosion while virgin females were aged for 3–5 days post-eclosion at 25°C in groups of 3-5 flies. Courtship assays were carried out at 25°C where individual females were introduced into one side of a round courtship chamber with a retractable divider in the middle (20 mm diameter × 2 mm height) with an individual naive male on the other side. The arenas were constructed out of custom cut acrylic (https://southernacrylics.co.uk/) with 20 chambers per plate and an arrangement of 4x5 chambers. Chamber plates were mounted in a custom 3D printed support and were backlight with the FLFL-Si200-IR24 infrared backlight (http://www.falcon-illumination.com/productdetail_FLFL.php). Videos were recorded using the Basler ace A2440-75um camera (cat# 35927) with a 35mm VIS-NIR fixed focal length lens (cat# 67716) and a UV/VIS cut-off filter (cat# 89834) from Edmund Optics (https://www.edmundoptics.co.uk/). The uncompressed AVI videos were recorded at a resolution of 2400x1600 pixels, 8-bit grayscale, 25 frames per second, for 1 hour using LabVIEW software (https://www.ni.com/en-gb/shop/labview.html).
Automated behavior tracking
Uncompressed AVI video files were converted to μFMF video files using ‘any2ufmf’ software (http://ctrax.sourceforge.net/any2ufmf.html). The μFMF video files were tracked with Caltech FlyTracker (http://www.vision.caltech.edu/Tools/FlyTracker/)69 with the following settings: num_chunks = 1, num_cores = 1, max_minutes = 15, save_JAABA = 1. Courtship behaviors were annotated using the Janelia Automatic Animal Behavior Annotator, JAABA (http://jaaba.sourceforge.net/)70. Classifiers were trained for the following behaviors:
-
•
‘Approaching’ – focal fly was approaching the other fly (TP = 96.8%, FN = 3.2%, TN = 98.5%, FP = 1.5%, nP = 2715, nN = 16169).
-
•
‘Facing’ – the focal flies head was oriented toward the other fly, while not being on the opposite side of the chamber (TP = 100.0%, FN = 0.0%, TN = 95.2%, FP = 4.8%, nP = 4490, nN = 7937).
-
•
‘Contact’ – the leg, proboscis, or head of the focal fly contacted any part the other fly (TP = 93.9%, FN = 6.1%, TN = 98.1%, FP = 1.9%, nP = 2684, nN = 10789).
-
•
‘Circling’ – the focal fly walked sideways while facing and being close to the other fly (TP = 71.6%, FN = 28.4%, TN = 98.5%, FP = 1.5%, nP = 638, nN = 9746).
-
•
‘Turning’ – the focal fly turned their body to orient toward the other fly while not moving forward (TP = 89.6%, FN = 10.4%, TN = 96.6%, FP = 3.4%, nP = 414, nN = 1540).
-
•
‘Wing extension’ – the focal fly extended a wing beyond their body (TP = 98.3%, FN = 1.7%, TN = 98.6%, FP = 1.4%, nP = 1575, nN = 10926).
(TP – true positive, FN – false negative, TN – true negative, FN – false negative, nP – number of annotated positive frames, nN – number of annotated negative frames).
Classifiers were applied to the tracking data using the ‘JAABADetect’ function.
Female fertility
% Fertility is the proportion of females that produce viable progeny. Females tested for fertility were collected at eclosion, stored in groups of 3-5 and aged for 5 days. They were then introduced individually into food vials containing three wild-type virgin males aged 5-7 days. All vials were scored for presence of larval progeny after 10 days. Vials containing a dead female were discounted.
Female egg-laying preference assay
Pheromone extraction
Pheromones were extracted from the cuticle of five-day old male Canton-S flies with n-hexane (Fisher Scientific). The flies were collected under CO2 anesthesia on the day of eclosion and left to mature in groups of 10-15 in 25 mm x 95 mm rearing vials. Prior to the extraction the flies were anesthetized on ice and transferred into 2 mL glass screw cap vials. 12 μL of n-hexane was added to the vial per fly and the vial was vortexed for 3 min after which the supernatant was carefully transferred into a clean vial. Due to minor evaporation and absorption of the hexane by the fly bodies, the remaining supernatant contained approximately 10 μL of hexane extract per fly, which was the dose used to represent a single fly in the behavioral experiments.
Oviposition experiments
The flies were collected on the day of eclosion and left to mature for five days before mating them in groups of 10 males with 10 females in rearing vials for approximately four hours. All genotypes were mated to Canton-S males. After mating all females were kept on a 3% bacteriological agar substrate overnight (Fisher Scientific) with a small amount of yeast extract paste (Fisher Scientific), to provide food and moisture to enhance oviposition. On the day of the experiment the females were individually transferred into oviposition assays (57 × 38 × 17 mm) containing a 3% agar middle zone unsuitable for oviposition and two oviposition zones of 0.75% agar containing 100 mM of sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich) on either end of the assay. The oviposition zones enclosed a 3 mm filter disc (Chromatography paper, Whatman) containing either the odor treatment or the solvent as control. To prevent odor saturation, the assays were covered with Parafilm (Sigma-Aldrich) that was punctured several times with a fine needle above the oviposition zones. After 24 hours the number of eggs laid per oviposition zone was counted under a stereomicroscope, and the oviposition indices were calculated as follows: (Eggs side A - Eggs side B) / (Eggs side A + Eggs side B).
Quantification and statistical analysis
Analysis of courtship data
Analysis of courtship data was conducted in R (https://www.r-project.org/). Unless otherwise stated, all functions are from either ‘base’ R or the ‘stats’ package. Data wrangling and plotting were conducted using the ‘tidyverse’ set of packages76, including ‘readr’, ‘dplyr’, ‘ggplot2’. Time to copulation was assessed by manual inspection of the videos. Tracking data was sub-sectioned to remove frames for which the flies were copulating. Courtship initiation was defined as exhibiting any combination of the courtship (JAABA) behaviors (excluding facing) for a total of 3 s over a 6 s window. For all subsequent analyses all frames before courtship initiation were removed. Courtship index is the percentage of time males exhibit any courtship (JAABA) behavior. Similarly, individual courtship (JAABA) behavior indices are defined as the percent of time exhibiting that behavior. Cumulative copulation was analyzed using the Kaplan-Meier method with the ‘survfit’ function form the ‘survival’ package77 and plotted with the ‘ggsurvplot’ function from the ‘survminer’ package78. Statistical significances were assessed with a log-rank test using the ‘survdiff’ function from ‘survival’, and the ‘pairwise_survdiff’ function from ‘survminer’ for pairwise differences.
Probability density function plots were generated by taking the mean of individual density curves (generated with the ‘density’ function with default parameters) per genotype of the ‘facing_angle’ and ‘dist_to_other’ features generated by the FlyTracker software. Facing angle (‘facing_angle’) is defined as the angle between the line bisecting the focal fly in the direction in which it is facing (see inset in Figure 4E), and the line between the centroids of the two flies. Distance to the other fly (‘dist_to_other’) is the distance between the centroids of the two flies. Statistical differences were assessed using the 2-sided Kolmogorov–Smirnov test79 using the ‘ks.test’ function with default parameters.
Bilateral wing extension was defined as both wings being extended at an angle greater than 15° (‘min_wing_ang’ > 0.26 radians). This value was chosen as 97.5% of individuals had a mean lesser wing angle of less than 15° (Figure S4K). Bilateral bout length and the number of bouts were determined using the ‘rle’ package. Unilateral wing extension was defined as one wing extended more than 35° (‘max_wing_ang’ > 0.61 radians) while the other was extended less than 15° (‘min_wing_ang’ < 0.26 radians).
Ipsilateral wing extension was defined as having a left wing angle of greater than 35° (‘wing_l_ang’ > 0.61 radians) while the female was located to the left of the male’s body axis, or right wing angle of greater than 35° (‘wing_r_ang’ > 0.61 radians) while the female was to the right of the male’s body axis. Vice versa for contralateral wing extension. Wing choice index was defined as the amount of time the ipsilateral wing was extended, minus the amount of time the contralateral wing was extended, all divided by the sum of time either wing was extended25.
Statistical significance for Figures 4A, 4B, 4G–4K, and S4A–S4J were assessed with one-way ANOVAs conducted using the ‘aov’ function, followed by post hoc 2-sided t tests (‘t.test’ function). All statistical values (for ANOVAs, t tests, and other mentioned statistical tests) were adjusted for multiple testing (for each behavior component) and multiple comparison (for each genotype) using the Holm-Bonferroni method (‘p.adjust’ function with method = “holm”).
Statistical analysis of oviposition
The oviposition indices were first analyzed for a preference to the pheromone extract over the control treatment using a two-tailed Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests with the null hypothesis assuming no preference to either of the treatments (mu = 0). Afterward, the preferences to the pheromone extracts were compared across genotypes using a generalized linear model (GLM) with a quasibinomial error distribution. In order to use the quasibinimioal error distribution the data was analyzed using a ‘cbind’ including the number of eggs laid on the side with the pheromone extract and the number of eggs laid on the side with the control treatment, generating values between 0 (preference for control treatment) and 1 (preference for odour treatment). Afterward, a GLM with a negative binomial error distribution was run to test for differences among genotypes for the total number of eggs laid. The model assumptions were checked by estimation of overdispersion and inspections of model residuals. All analyses were carried out in R (v. 3.6.1). The GLMs were performed using lme4 and car80,81 for model comparison based on χ2 likelihood ratios82 and the data was visualized using ggplot2.76
Statistical analysis of calcium imaging data
ΔF/F are reported as mean and SE. Statistical significance was tested by the Kruskal-Wallis test, followed by pairwise comparisons with the Mann-Whitney-U test using the scipy and statsmodels packages in python and significance levels were adjusted for multiple comparisons using the Holm-Bonferroni method. Significance levels are reported as follows: p ≥ 0.01: ns, p < 0.01: ·, p < 0.05: ∗, p < 0.01: ∗∗, p < 0.001: ∗∗∗.
Acknowledgments
We thank A. Barrios, A. Dornan, S. Heinze, G. Jefferis, M. Juusola, D. Shepherd, and the Goodwin Lab for helpful discussions and critical reading of the manuscript. We are grateful to D. Anderson, B. Dickson, T. Lee, G. Rubin, D. Yamamoto, and the Bloomington Stock Center (NIH P40OD018537) for sharing important reagents with us. We thank A. de Haan for genotyping and S. Waddell for support with EM reconstruction. N.O. was supported by a Wellcome Trust Collaborative Award (203261/Z/16/Z) to S. Waddell. This work was supported by grants from the BBSRC (BB/T001348/1) to T.N. and S.F.G. and a Wellcome Trust Senior Investigator Award to S.F.G. (106189/Z/14/Z).
Author contributions
Conceptualization, T.N., A.R., A.M.A., M.C.N., and S.F.G.; methodology, T.N., A.R., A.M.A., N.O., T.A.V., M.C.N., J.-C.B., and S.F.G.; investigation, T.N., A.R., A.M.A., N.O., T.A.V., and M.C.N.; resources, S.F.G.; writing, T.N., A.R., A.M.A., N.O., T.A.V., J.-C.B., M.C.N., and S.F.G.; supervision, M.C.N. and S.F.G.; funding acquisition, S.F.G.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Published: January 27, 2021
Footnotes
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.12.047.
Contributor Information
Megan C. Neville, Email: megan.goodwin@cncb.ox.ac.uk.
Stephen F. Goodwin, Email: stephen.goodwin@cncb.ox.ac.uk.
Supplemental information
References
- 1.Yang T., Shah N.M. Molecular and neural control of sexually dimorphic social behaviors. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2016;38:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.04.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Luo S.D., Shi G.W., Baker B.S. Direct targets of the D. melanogaster DSXF protein and the evolution of sexual development. Development. 2011;138:2761–2771. doi: 10.1242/dev.065227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dalton J.E., Fear J.M., Knott S., Baker B.S., McIntyre L.M., Arbeitman M.N. Male-specific Fruitless isoforms have different regulatory roles conferred by distinct zinc finger DNA binding domains. BMC Genomics. 2013;14:659. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-14-659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Neville M.C., Nojima T., Ashley E., Parker D.J., Walker J., Southall T., Van de Sande B., Marques A.C., Fischer B., Brand A.H. Male-specific fruitless isoforms target neurodevelopmental genes to specify a sexually dimorphic nervous system. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Vernes S.C. Genome wide identification of fruitless targets suggests a role in upregulating genes important for neural circuit formation. Sci. Rep. 2014;4:4412. doi: 10.1038/srep04412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Clough E., Jimenez E., Kim Y.A., Whitworth C., Neville M.C., Hempel L.U., Pavlou H.J., Chen Z.X., Sturgill D., Dale R.K. Sex- and tissue-specific functions of Drosophila doublesex transcription factor target genes. Dev. Cell. 2014;31:761–773. doi: 10.1016/j.devcel.2014.11.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Asahina K. Sex differences in Drosophila behavior: qualitative and quantitative dimorphism. Curr. Opin. Physiol. 2018;6:35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cophys.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Verhulst E.C., van de Zande L. Double nexus--Doublesex is the connecting element in sex determination. Brief. Funct. Genomics. 2015;14:396–406. doi: 10.1093/bfgp/elv005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Clough E., Oliver B. Genomics of sex determination in Drosophila. Brief. Funct. Genomics. 2012;11:387–394. doi: 10.1093/bfgp/els019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Baker B.S., Wolfner M.F. A molecular analysis of doublesex, a bifunctional gene that controls both male and female sexual differentiation in Drosophila melanogaster. Genes Dev. 1988;2:477–489. doi: 10.1101/gad.2.4.477. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Burtis K.C., Baker B.S. Drosophila doublesex gene controls somatic sexual differentiation by producing alternatively spliced mRNAs encoding related sex-specific polypeptides. Cell. 1989;56:997–1010. doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(89)90633-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lee G., Hall J.C., Park J.H. Doublesex gene expression in the central nervous system of Drosophila melanogaster. J. Neurogenet. 2002;16:229–248. doi: 10.1080/01677060216292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Billeter J.C., Villella A., Allendorfer J.B., Dornan A.J., Richardson M., Gailey D.A., Goodwin S.F. Isoform-specific control of male neuronal differentiation and behavior in Drosophila by the fruitless gene. Curr. Biol. 2006;16:1063–1076. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Rideout E.J., Dornan A.J., Neville M.C., Eadie S., Goodwin S.F. Control of sexual differentiation and behavior by the doublesex gene in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:458–466. doi: 10.1038/nn.2515. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sanders L.E., Arbeitman M.N. Doublesex establishes sexual dimorphism in the Drosophila central nervous system in an isoform-dependent manner by directing cell number. Dev. Biol. 2008;320:378–390. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2008.05.543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rideout E.J., Billeter J.C., Goodwin S.F. The sex-determination genes fruitless and doublesex specify a neural substrate required for courtship song. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:1473–1478. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Robinett C.C., Vaughan A.G., Knapp J.M., Baker B.S. Sex and the single cell. II. There is a time and place for sex. PLoS Biol. 2010;8:e1000365. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kimura K., Sato C., Koganezawa M., Yamamoto D. Drosophila ovipositor extension in mating behavior and egg deposition involves distinct sets of brain interneurons. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0126445. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126445. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lee T., Luo L. Mosaic analysis with a repressible cell marker for studies of gene function in neuronal morphogenesis. Neuron. 1999;22:451–461. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80701-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Luan H., Peabody N.C., Vinson C.R., White B.H. Refined spatial manipulation of neuronal function by combinatorial restriction of transgene expression. Neuron. 2006;52:425–436. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.08.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cachero S., Ostrovsky A.D., Yu J.Y., Dickson B.J., Jefferis G.S. Sexual dimorphism in the fly brain. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1589–1601. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.07.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wu M., Nern A., Williamson W.R., Morimoto M.M., Reiser M.B., Card G.M., Rubin G.M. Visual projection neurons in the Drosophila lobula link feature detection to distinct behavioral programs. eLife. 2016;5:e21022. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Panser K., Tirian L., Schulze F., Villalba S., Jefferis G.S.X.E., Bühler K., Straw A.D. Automatic segmentation of Drosophila neural compartments using GAL4 expression data reveals novel visual pathways. Curr. Biol. 2016;26:1943–1954. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.05.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Omoto J.J., Keleş M.F., Nguyen B.M., Bolanos C., Lovick J.K., Frye M.A., Hartenstein V. Visual input to the Drosophila central complex by developmentally and functionally distinct neuronal populations. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:1098–1110. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.02.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ribeiro I.M.A., Drews M., Bahl A., Machacek C., Borst A., Dickson B.J. Visual projection neurons mediating directed courtship in Drosophila. Cell. 2018;174:607–621.e18. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.06.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lamaze A., Krätschmer P., Chen K.F., Lowe S., Jepson J.E.C. A wake-promoting circadian output circuit in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2018;28:3098–3105.e3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2018.07.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ruta V., Datta S.R., Vasconcelos M.L., Freeland J., Looger L.L., Axel R. A dimorphic pheromone circuit in Drosophila from sensory input to descending output. Nature. 2010;468:686–690. doi: 10.1038/nature09554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kohl J., Ostrovsky A.D., Frechter S., Jefferis G.S. A bidirectional circuit switch reroutes pheromone signals in male and female brains. Cell. 2013;155:1610–1623. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Clowney E.J., Iguchi S., Bussell J.J., Scheer E., Ruta V. Multimodal chemosensory circuits controlling male courtship in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;87:1036–1049. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kallman B.R., Kim H., Scott K. Excitation and inhibition onto central courtship neurons biases Drosophila mate choice. eLife. 2015;4:e11188. doi: 10.7554/eLife.11188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Kim H., Kirkhart C., Scott K. Long-range projection neurons in the taste circuit of Drosophila. eLife. 2017;6:e23386. doi: 10.7554/eLife.23386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Tsubouchi A., Yano T., Yokoyama T.K., Murtin C., Otsuna H., Ito K. Topological and modality-specific representation of somatosensory information in the fly brain. Science. 2017;358:615–623. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dolan M.J., Belliart-Guérin G., Bates A.S., Frechter S., Lampin-Saint-Amaux A., Aso Y., Roberts R.J.V., Schlegel P., Wong A., Hammad A. Communication from learned to innate olfactory processing centers is required for memory retrieval in Drosophila. Neuron. 2018;100:651–668.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pavlou H.J., Lin A.C., Neville M.C., Nojima T., Diao F., Chen B.E., White B.H., Goodwin S.F. Neural circuitry coordinating male copulation. eLife. 2016;5:e20713. doi: 10.7554/eLife.20713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nicolaï L.J., Ramaekers A., Raemaekers T., Drozdzecki A., Mauss A.S., Yan J., Landgraf M., Annaert W., Hassan B.A. Genetically encoded dendritic marker sheds light on neuronal connectivity in Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107:20553–20558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010198107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Estes P.S., Ho G.L., Narayanan R., Ramaswami M. Synaptic localization and restricted diffusion of a Drosophila neuronal synaptobrevin--green fluorescent protein chimera in vivo. J. Neurogenet. 2000;13:233–255. doi: 10.3109/01677060009084496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Gordon M.D., Scott K. Motor control in a Drosophila taste circuit. Neuron. 2009;61:373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.12.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Scheffer L.K., Xu C.S., Januszewski M., Lu Z., Takemura S.Y., Hayworth K.J., Huang G.B., Shinomiya K., Maitlin-Shepard J., Berg S. A connectome and analysis of the adult Drosophila central brain. eLife. 2020;9:e57443. doi: 10.7554/eLife.57443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Klapoetke N.C., Murata Y., Kim S.S., Pulver S.R., Birdsey-Benson A., Cho Y.K., Morimoto T.K., Chuong A.S., Carpenter E.J., Tian Z. Independent optical excitation of distinct neural populations. Nat. Methods. 2014;11:338–346. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chen T.W., Wardill T.J., Sun Y., Pulver S.R., Renninger S.L., Baohan A., Schreiter E.R., Kerr R.A., Orger M.B., Jayaraman V. Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature. 2013;499:295–300. doi: 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Zhang S.X., Miner L.E., Boutros C.L., Rogulja D., Crickmore M.A. Motivation, perception, and chance converge to make a binary decision. Neuron. 2018;99:376–388.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.06.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Seeholzer L.F., Seppo M., Stern D.L., Ruta V. Evolution of a central neural circuit underlies Drosophila mate preferences. Nature. 2018;559:564–569. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0322-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Hamasaka Y., Wegener C., Nässel D.R. GABA modulates Drosophila circadian clock neurons via GABAB receptors and decreases in calcium. J. Neurobiol. 2005;65:225–240. doi: 10.1002/neu.20184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Sweeney S.T., Broadie K., Keane J., Niemann H., O’Kane C.J. Targeted expression of tetanus toxin light chain in Drosophila specifically eliminates synaptic transmission and causes behavioral defects. Neuron. 1995;14:341–351. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90290-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Asahina K., Watanabe K., Duistermars B.J., Hoopfer E., González C.R., Eyjólfsdóttir E.A., Perona P., Anderson D.J. Tachykinin-expressing neurons control male-specific aggressive arousal in Drosophila. Cell. 2014;156:221–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.11.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.von Philipsborn A.C., Liu T., Yu J.Y., Masser C., Bidaye S.S., Dickson B.J. Neuronal control of Drosophila courtship song. Neuron. 2011;69:509–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pan Y., Meissner G.W., Baker B.S. Joint control of Drosophila male courtship behavior by motion cues and activation of male-specific P1 neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2012;109:10065–10070. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207107109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kimura K., Hachiya T., Koganezawa M., Tazawa T., Yamamoto D. Fruitless and doublesex coordinate to generate male-specific neurons that can initiate courtship. Neuron. 2008;59:759–769. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Yu J.Y., Kanai M.I., Demir E., Jefferis G.S., Dickson B.J. Cellular organization of the neural circuit that drives Drosophila courtship behavior. Curr. Biol. 2010;20:1602–1614. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kohatsu S., Koganezawa M., Yamamoto D. Female contact activates male-specific interneurons that trigger stereotypic courtship behavior in Drosophila. Neuron. 2011;69:498–508. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Heimbeck G., Bugnon V., Gendre N., Keller A., Stocker R.F. A central neural circuit for experience-independent olfactory and courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:15336–15341. doi: 10.1073/pnas.011314898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Marin E.C., Büld L., Theiss M., Sarkissian T., Roberts R.J.V., Turnbull R., Tamimi I.F.M., Pleijzier M.W., Laursen W.J., Drummond N. Connectomics analysis reveals first-, second-, and third-order thermosensory and hygrosensory neurons in the adult Drosophila brain. Curr. Biol. 2020;30:3167–3182.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Stocker R.F., Heimbeck G., Gendre N., de Belle J.S. Neuroblast ablation in Drosophila P[GAL4] lines reveals origins of olfactory interneurons. J. Neurobiol. 1997;32:443–456. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(199705)32:5<443::aid-neu1>3.0.co;2-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lai S.L., Awasaki T., Ito K., Lee T. Clonal analysis of Drosophila antennal lobe neurons: diverse neuronal architectures in the lateral neuroblast lineage. Development. 2008;135:2883–2893. doi: 10.1242/dev.024380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lai S.L., Lee T. Genetic mosaic with dual binary transcriptional systems in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:703–709. doi: 10.1038/nn1681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Wang F., Wang K., Forknall N., Patrick C., Yang T., Parekh R., Bock D., Dickson B.J. Neural circuitry linking mating and egg laying in Drosophila females. Nature. 2020;579:101–105. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-2055-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Liu W.W., Wilson R.I. Glutamate is an inhibitory neurotransmitter in the Drosophila olfactory system. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2013;110:10294–10299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1220560110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Felsenberg J., Jacob P.F., Walker T., Barnstedt O., Edmondson-Stait A.J., Pleijzier M.W., Otto N., Schlegel P., Sharifi N., Perisse E. Integration of parallel opposing memories underlies memory extinction. Cell. 2018;175:709–722.e15. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.08.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Shih C.T., Sporns O., Yuan S.L., Su T.S., Lin Y.J., Chuang C.C., Wang T.Y., Lo C.C., Greenspan R.J., Chiang A.S. Connectomics-based analysis of information flow in the Drosophila brain. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:1249–1258. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Duménil C., Woud D., Pinto F., Alkema J.T., Jansen I., Van Der Geest A.M., Roessingh S., Billeter J.C. Pheromonal cues deposited by mated females convey social information about egg-laying sites in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Chem. Ecol. 2016;42:259–269. doi: 10.1007/s10886-016-0681-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cury K.M., Prud’homme B., Gompel N. A short guide to insect oviposition: when, where and how to lay an egg. J. Neurogenet. 2019;33:75–89. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2019.1586898. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Billeter J.C., Wolfner M.F. Chemical cues that guide female reproduction in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Chem. Ecol. 2018;44:750–769. doi: 10.1007/s10886-018-0947-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Cattaert D., El Manira A. Shunting versus inactivation: analysis of presynaptic inhibitory mechanisms in primary afferents of the crayfish. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:6079–6089. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-14-06079.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Verspui R., Gray J.R. Visual stimuli induced by self-motion and object-motion modify odour-guided flight of male moths (Manduca sexta L.) J. Exp. Biol. 2009;212:3272–3282. doi: 10.1242/jeb.031591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Pansopha P., Ando N., Kanzaki R. Dynamic use of optic flow during pheromone tracking by the male silkmoth, Bombyx mori. J. Exp. Biol. 2014;217:1811–1820. doi: 10.1242/jeb.090266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Hildebrand J.G. Olfactory control of behavior in moths: central processing of odor information and the functional significance of olfactory glomeruli. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1996;178:5–19. doi: 10.1007/BF00189586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Tosches M.A. Developmental and genetic mechanisms of neural circuit evolution. Dev. Biol. 2017;431:16–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2017.06.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gao S., Takemura S.Y., Ting C.Y., Huang S., Lu Z., Luan H., Rister J., Thum A.S., Yang M., Hong S.T. The neural substrate of spectral preference in Drosophila. Neuron. 2008;60:328–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.08.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Eyjolfsdottir E., Branson S., Burgos-Artizzu X., Hoopfer E.D., Schor J., Anderson D.J., Perona P. Detecting social actions of fruit flies. In: Fleet D., Pajdla T., Schiele B., Tuytelaars T., editors. Computer Vision – ECCV 2014, 8690. Springer, Cham; 2014. pp. 772–787. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kabra M., Robie A.A., Rivera-Alba M., Branson S., Branson K. JAABA: interactive machine learning for automatic annotation of animal behavior. Nat. Methods. 2013;10:64–67. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Jenett A., Gerald R.M., Ngo T.-T.B., Shepherd D., Murphy C., Dionne H. A GAL4-Driver Line Resource for Drosophila Neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Ito K., Shinomiya K., Ito M., Armstrong J.D., Boyan G., Hartenstein V. A Systematic Nomenclature for the Insect Brain. Neuron. 2014;81:755–765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bates A.S., Schlegel P., Roberts R.J.V., Drummond N., Tamimi I.F.M., Turnbull R., Zhao X., Marin E.C., Popovici P.D., Dhawan S. Complete connectomic reconstruction of olfactory projection neurons in the fly brain. Curr. Biol. 2020;30:3183–3199.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Frechter S., Bates A.S., Tootoonian S., Dolan M.J., Manton J., Jamasb A.R., Kohl J., Bock D., Jefferis G. Functional and anatomical specificity in a higher olfactory centre. eLife. 2019;8:e44590. doi: 10.7554/eLife.44590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Pologruto T.A., Sabatini B.L., Svoboda K. ScanImage: flexible software for operating laser scanning microscopes. Biomed. Eng. Online. 2003;2:13. doi: 10.1186/1475-925X-2-13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Wickham H. Springer; 2016. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Therneau T.M., Grambsch P.M. Springer-Verlag; 2000. Modeling Survival Data: Extending the Cox Model. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Kassambara A., Kosinski M., Biecek P. 2019. Survminer: drawing survival curves using ‘ggplot2’.https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/survminer/index.html [Google Scholar]
- 79.Marsaglia G., Tsang W.W., Wang J. Evaluating Kolmogorov’s distribution. J. Stat. Softw. 2003;8:1–4. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Bates D., Mächler M., Bolker B., Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015;67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Fox J., Weisber S. Sage Publications; 2011. An R Companion to Applied Regression. [Google Scholar]
- 82.Hothorn T., Bretz F., Westfall P. Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Biom. J. 2008;50:346–363. doi: 10.1002/bimj.200810425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Scripts used for behavioral analysis are available from https://github.com/aaron-allen/aDN_behaviour and https://github.com/aaron-allen/goodwin-lab-tracking and those for calcium imaging analysis from https://github.com/AR2202/2-photon. Single neuron and cluster images described in this study will be uploaded to a publicly available database hosted by Virtual Fly Brain (https://v2.virtualflybrain.org) following publication. Requests for further details of the software and raw data should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Stephen F. Goodwin (stephen.goodwin@cncb.ox.ac.uk).