Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Mar 24;16(3):e0248733. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248733

Your face scares me: Effects of Perceptual load and Social Anxiety on processing of threatening and neutral faces

Marios Theodorou 1,*, Nikos Konstantinou 2, Georgia Panayiotou 1
Editor: Christos Papadelis3
PMCID: PMC7990234  PMID: 33760893

Abstract

Social Anxiety Disorder is among the most widely studied psychiatric conditions. However, the role of attentional and emotional processes in the maintenance of the condition is still not well-established. This study addressed the impact of individual differences in Social Anxiety, by examining the effects of perceptual load and stimulus valence when processing faces vs objects, here used as distractors, within a letter-search task. In addition to RT and accuracy on the letter search task, heart rate, and skin conductance during the task and participants’ self-report emotional evaluation were assessed to help interpret performance effects. Results suggest that distractor stimuli that are either threatening or faces impair performance of high SA participants. Results demonstrate a hypervigilance for threatening faces in SA but indicate that this happens primarily when cognitive resources are available, that is, under low perceptual load.

Introduction

Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD), the clinical form of Social Anxiety (SA) is among the most widely studied psychiatric conditions. Information processing models of the etiology and maintenance of this condition generally suggest that social stimuli, including threatening facial expressions are anxiety-inducing for individuals with SA, activating excessive attentional vigilance and defensive mobilization [13].

From an evolutionary perspective, human faces, especially those representing threat (e.g. showing anger), are prominent stimuli for humans, with relevance to survival, as they may signal rejection by conspecifics. Öhman et al. [46], hypothesize a common emotional, cognitive and threat related neural pathway, involving brain areas such as the amygdala, for processing faces and other evolutionarily significant fearful stimuli like snakes. Because of their significance, such stimuli are prioritized and instigate automatic, bottom up processing [5, 7]. With regards to SA, Öhman et al.’s model [4, 6], suggests that genetic predisposition and prior experiences of submissiveness during social status conflicts have facilitated fear conditioning to angry faces in high SA individuals, producing hypervigilance towards them.

Based on Lang’s [8] bio-informational theory, semantic memories of emotional stimuli are associated with specific somatovisceral response patterns for survival-related actions. Attention processes share a common neural substrate with emotion, and therefore attentional orienting towards threatening stimuli engages motivational circuits to prepare the organism for action [9]. In anxiety disorders, including SAD, these adaptive orienting and defensive preparation processes are extreme and activated even by mildly threatening or ambiguous stimuli [1012]. Accordingly, models of SA [13], suggest that socially anxious individuals orient attention to perceived threat, which elevates bodily arousal, that in turn may verify, through conditioning, their fearful perceptions for social situations.

Existing data support, in their majority, the presence of attentional hypervigilance to threatening faces in SA, at least during early stages of processing, at exposure durations of up to 500 ms [1419]. This seems to hold compared to both low anxious individuals and those with high trait anxiety [20]. However, inconsistent findings also exist, with several studies failing to identify attentional vigilance toward threatening faces in SA. For example, Kolassa and Miltner [21], found interference in gender identification when viewing emotional faces, compared to neutral, suggesting hypervigilance, but this was not specific to angry faces or linked to SA levels. Similarly, de Jong, et al. [22] hypothesized that accuracy on a face identification task with rapid face presentation, would be lower on trials where an emotional stimulus preceded the target, due to emotion-induced blindness. An attentional blink effect suggested vigilance to emotional faces, but this occurred irrespective of emotional expression (angry/happy) and SA.

Other studies, in fact, find lower attention allocation to threatening compared to neutral/non-face stimuli, with these findings typically interpreted as motivated avoidance in SA for threatening social stimuli. Chen, et al. [23], using the dot-probe paradigm, found that individuals with SAD showed greater attentional bias to household objects at 500ms exposure-times compared to faces, regardless of their emotional expression. Using the ‘Face in the crowd’ task, Juth, et al. [24] found that participants detected happy faces faster than angry or fearful ones, regardless of SA level, indicating avoidance of negative expressions. Reasons for these mixed results remain currently unclear, perpetuating the question as to the circumstances under which hypervigilance to faces occurs in SA. Conditions such as type of task, stimulus presentation duration, SA severity, types of facial expressions examined and control conditions used may need to be considered for this question to be properly addressed.

First, whether processing emotional faces was a primary task, a secondary task or distractor may modulate attention allocation to face stimuli. Specifically, it has been shown that the processing load that a task imposes affects the processing of task-relevant and irrelevant information [2528]. According to Load Theory [29, 30] distractibility by task irrelevant stimuli is eliminated due to exhaustion of attentional resources, when the primary task is demanding, i.e. is characterized by high perceptual load. Conversely, when the primary task involves low perceptual load the remaining perceptual capacity is automatically allocated to task irrelevant stimuli. Even though a minority of studies has indicated that faces may represent a special case [31], attracting attention as distractors even under high load conditions, most results indicate that load effects hold for face stimuli as well [3235].

However, to the degree that high SA individuals preferentially process even mild threat, prioritizing it over any other task, they may show hypervigilance to threatening face distractors, even under high load primary tasks, unlike low SA participants. Soares et al. [36] found that only high SA participants were less accurate on a high load compared to a low load task with threatening faces as distractors, showing preferential attention to these faces. However, this hypothesis has not been extensively examined, and existing results are mixed. Fox, Yates and Ashwin [37], using a modified flanker task, found no difference between low and high SA groups on interference by fear-conditioned angry faces in high load conditions. Under low load, however, it was the low SA group that showed higher interference for fear-conditioned faces, an effect interpreted as attentional avoidance in SA.

Secondly, most studies that find attentional vigilance towards threatening faces in SA presented face stimuli briefly. Attention allocation may change over time, as proposed by the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis [15], so that automatic hypervigilance to threat during early pre-conscious stages of processing gives way to avoidance, an emotion regulatory process, once the stimulus becomes more conscious.

Thirdly, characteristics of the stimuli may have contributed to the inconsistent findings of attention biases in SA. There is some evidence that high SA individuals are not specifically vigilant towards angry faces, but may find any type of face, and especially neutral, or ambiguous as threatening [12, 38], raising questions as to the appropriate control condition for threatening face stimuli. Also, it remains to be clarified if biases in SA are indeed specific to faces, or if they generalize to other types of survival threat.

In the present study we aimed to address these questions, by examining the presence of attention bias towards threatening faces in SA during very early stages of processing, under varied perceptual load conditions, and using a balanced design that included face, non-face, threatening and neutral distractors, hoping to further specify the circumstances under which these biases occur.

The present study

We examine attention biases towards threatening faces in SA, using a design that targets very early processing, and varies stimulus characteristics (threatening vs neutral; face vs object) and primary task perceptual load. It was expected that, to the degree that threatening faces are biologically significant for everyone, but particularly for high SA individuals, these participants would show greater difficulty filtering out task irrelevant stimuli of this nature compared to low SA participants, showing preferential attention towards them even when they are to-be-ignored distractors. This will be apparent as slower RT on the primary task and increased emotional reactivity, physiologically and subjectively, to trials containing threatening faces distractors.

Participants high and low on SA performed a letter search task, similar to that used previously [36, 39], with very short stimulus presentation times of 100 ms, capturing stages when automatic attentional vigilance towards threat should occur. In order to identify if there is preferential attention to threatening faces specifically, we also included neutral faces, and objects, threatening (guns) and neutral (chairs) as distractors. This balanced design helps to decipher if it is stimulus emotionality that attracts attention (negative valence; high arousal) or if there is something particular to faces that renders them into more potent distractors than other stimuli. Diverging from Soares et al. [36], distractor stimuli were presented at the center of the letter display to increase the potency of their effects on the primary task. It was expected a) high SA individuals would be more distracted by the presence of threatening faces as distractors, compared to non-distractor trials and trials with other types of distractors (neutral faces, threatening and neutral objects), as indicated by slower RTs. Accuracy was also examined to identify any speed-accuracy trade-off effects. To the degree that high SA individuals prioritize the processing of threatening faces over any other tasks, it was also expected that b) this group and not the low SA participants might show distraction by threatening faces even under high perceptual load conditions.

In addition to performance measures, we utilized physiological indices of emotion-related arousal, assessing heart rate (HR) and skin conductance level (SCL) during the letter search task. Given prior findings [4042] that high SA participants exhibit greater autonomic reactivity to emotional faces than controls, we predicted that c) high SA participants would show higher HR and SCL in blocks with threatening faces than any other blocks, and compared to low SA participants.

Method

Participants

A power analysis was conducted using the software package, G*Power 3.1.9 [43] to ensure sufficient statistical power. To achieve power of 0.90 to detect a small effect size (f = .15) with a significance level of a = .05 the estimated total required number of 42 participants. In this study, ninety-six college students participated for extra credit and were screened for SA using the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory-23 (SPAI-23). Cronbach’s alpha for the SPAI-SP subscale was α = .94. Mean SP score for this sample was slightly higher (M = 23.35, SD = 11.63) than the respective original USA normative (M = 22, SD = 10.05) [44]. The bottom 25% (low SA group), and the top 25% (high SA group) of the distribution for SPAI-23 Social Phobia (SP) subscale was selected for the experiment. The final sample consisted of 46 participants (35 women), aged 18–24 (M = 20.67, SD = 1.38). All had normal or corrected to normal vision and provided written informed consent by signing the informed consent form. The study was approved by the Cyprus National Bioethics Committee (Ref: ΕΕΒΚ/ΕΠ/2014/31). All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Course credit was offered as an incentive for participation.

Measures and materials

SPAI-23: The Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory-23 [45] is an abbreviated version of the SPAI [46], assessing Social Phobia. It was standardized in the Greek language, showing good psychometric properties with adolescents [47], and college students [48].

A total of 64 images (four categories x 16 images; Table 1) were used as distractors. Faces (50% women, 50% angry, 50% neutral expression) were selected from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database [49]. Handguns were selected from the UCSD Vision and Memory lab object database [50], and chairs were created at the National Institute of Mental Health [51]. All stimuli were converted to grayscale to avoid confounding by color or luminosity. Faces were cropped around the face to avoid effects of the context and different hair styles using Adobe Photoshop (Adobe systems, Incorporated, San Jose, CA), so that only the central face area was visible. Normative ratings of the selected stimuli on valence, arousal, and dominance dimensions exist from an independent college sample [52].

Table 1. Stimuli selected for each category.

KDEF Threatening Faces: AF01ANS, AF07ANS, AF16ANS, AF20ANS, AF21ANS, AF23ANS, AF25ANS, AF31ANS, AM03ANS, AM05ANS, AM07ANS, AM08ANS, AM10ANS, AM14ANS, AM17ANS, AM29ANS.
KDEF Neutral Faces: AF01NES, AF07NES, AF16NES, AF20NES, AF21NES, AF23NES, AF25NES, AF31NES, AM03NES, AM05NES, AM07NES, AM08NES, AM10NES, AM14NES, AM17NES, AM29NES.
UCSD Threatening Objects (handguns): Colt 1911, COlt Gold Cup, Colt official police, CZ 85, CZ_75, hI sTANDARD, LLAMA 87, norinco NP, Norinco NZ85B, Para Ordnance P 14–45, Pietta 1873 Colt, Ruger 22 45, cal, S&W 4506, Taurus PT92, Uberti Schofield, handgun1.
NIMH Neutral Objects (chairs): 1_Chair_Original, 2_Chair_Original, 4_Chair_Original, 5_Chair_Original, 6_Chair_Original, 8_Chair_Original, 9_Chair_Original, 10_Chair_Original, 11_Chair_Original, 13_Chair_Original, 14_Chair_Original, 15_Chair_Original, 16_Chair_Original, 17_Chair_Original, 18_Chair_Original, 20_Chair_Original.

Apparatus

The experiment was controlled using the Cogent Toolbox (http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent.php) for Matlab (MathWorks, Inc.) on a PC running Microsoft Windows 7 attached to a 19” monitor (60 Hz refresh rate; resolution 800 × 600). A chin rest ensured an invariable distance of 60cm from the monitor.

Physiological data were collected continuously during the letter search task using BIOPAC MP150 for Windows and AcqKnowledge 3.9.0 software (Biopac Systems Inc., Santa Barbara, CA). Ag/AgCl shielded electrodes were placed on participants’ face and arms following standard procedures [53]. Electrocardiogram (ECG) recorded at the two inner forearms was filtered by a BIOPAC ECG100C bioamplifier recording HR between 40 and 140 beats per minute (BPM). SCL was recorded using a BIOPAC GSR100C transducer amplifier.

Procedure

Participants arrived at the lab, and sat in a dimly lit, sound attenuated room. Following informed consent, they were given instructions and fitted with physiological monitors. A 5-min adjustment period, during which participants were asked to sit quietly and relax, preceded the experiment to stabilize physiological recordings and familiarize them with the equipment. Next, participants completed the letter-search task for which they had to identify which of the two pre-specified target-letters (either X or Z) appeared in a display of other letters. Each trial began with a fixation cross (0.4° x 0.6°) presented at the center of the screen for 1000 ms, followed by presentation of eight letters (0.4° x 0.6°) for 100 ms. Letters were placed with equal probability in any of eight positions arranged in a circle of 2° in radius centered at fixation. The task included two load conditions: in the low load condition Os were presented in seven of the letter places (Fig 1). In the high load condition, seven letters with similar configuration to the X and Z target-letters were presented (K, F, V, T, L, N, H; Fig 2). A distractor (2.5° × 2.5°) was presented in the middle of the circle on 50% of the trials (randomly intermixed), based on evidence that participants tend to habituate to more frequent distractor presentations, which can obliterate distractor effects [35, 39, 54]. A smaller % of trials with distractors (20%) was pilot tested, yielding similar results, therefore a higher % was used to allow for more samples of each distractor type. Participants were instructed to search for a target letter “X” or “Z”, and respond as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the keys 1 or 2 on the numerical keypad for “X” or “Z” respectively, while ignoring task-irrelevant images. The target-letter was equally likely to appear in any of the eight positions of the circle.

Fig 1. An example trial sequence and timing in visual search task (low load distractor absent).

Fig 1

Fig 2. An example trial sequence and timing in visual search task (high load distractor absent).

Fig 2

Trials varying on distractor emotionality (threatening/neutral) and perceptual load (high/low) were presented in separate counterbalanced blocks. Distractor present and distractor absent trials were mixed randomly within all blocks. Each participant completed 8 blocks of 64 trials each (low load threatening faces, high load threatening faces, low load neutral faces, high load neutral faces, low load threatening objects, high load threatening objects, low load neutral objects, high load neutral objects), in counterbalanced fashion (ABBABAAB). Blocks were separated by 60s rest intervals. Prior to the experiment, participants completed two practice blocks of 16 trials each. After task completion, participants rated their subjective experience while viewing the distractor stimuli on valence (anchors: pleasant; unpleasant), arousal (anchors: intense; calm), and dominance (anchors: dominant; submissive), to verify that stimuli elicited the intended response.

Data reduction

Trials with RTs of less than 150 ms, more than 1,500 ms, or +/- 2.5SD from each participant’s mean, were considered outliers (8%) and were excluded from analyses. Mean accuracy was calculated as the percentage of correct trials per block. Only correct trials in each type of block were used for the calculation of mean RT [39, 54]. For HR and SCL, means were computed for each type of block and across all inter-trial intervals. Three participants (one low SA, two high SA) were removed from HR and SCL analyses due to poor signal. Mean physiology across all inter-trial intervals was the covariate for analyses on physiological measures to account for individual differences.

For performance effects, a repeated measures ANOVA in Statistical Package for the Social Sciences-25.0 examined separately RT and accuracy (% correct) as the dependent variables, with load (low, high), distractor presence (present, absent), distractor emotionality (threatening, neutral), and distractor type (face, object) as within-subject variables, and social anxiety (low, high) as a between subject variable. A similar ANOVA, with load, distractor emotionality, and distractor type, as within-subject variables, and social anxiety as a between subject variable examined effects on HR and SCL. Because these measures were taken as the average of each block across both distractor present and absent trials, distractor presence was not a variable in these analyses. The Benjamini-Hochberg procedure [55] was used to control for multiple testing bias (false positives). To adjust with the low effect sizes in attentional bias research [2], the false discovery rate was set at 0.10 [56, 57]. Effects are reported at a significance level of p < .05.

Results

Manipulation checks

Effects of load and distractor presence on performance

Table 2 presents mean RTs and Accuracy rates in each condition of distractor presence and load. A significant effect of load, F(1, 44) = 274.42, p < .01, η2 = .86, showed that participants were slower in high load (M = 741.15ms, SD = 151.64) compared to low load blocks (M = 540.17ms, SD = 126.94). A significant effect of distractor presence was also found, F(1, 44) = 28.79, p < .01, η2 = .40, so that participants were slower in distractor present trials (M = 649.30ms, SD = 141.38) compared to distractor absent trials (M = 632.02ms, SD = 137.19). A significant load x distractor presence interaction F(1, 44) = 9.46, p = .004, η2 = .18, indicated that the distractor presence effect was only significant in the low load blocks, F(1, 44) = 48.30, p < .01, η2 = .52, with participants being slower in distractor present trials (M = 552.98ms, SD = 129.22) compared to distractor absent trials (M = 527.36ms, SD = 124.65). Distractor type and emotionality did not show any significant effects on RT or an interaction with load.

Table 2. Mean RTs, accuracy rates, and ratings per distractor category (SD in parentheses).
Low Load
Trials with distractor Trials without distractor
TF NF TO NO TF NF TO NO
RT ms 559.2 (133.4) 545 (13.4) 558.5 (120.8) 549.3 (128.4) 526.6 (113.7) 525.7 (128.9) 526.04 (119.97) 531.12 (135.4)
% Acc 92% 95% 92% 94% 96% 94% 96% 95%
High Load
Trials with distractor Trials without distractor
TF NF TO NO TF NF TO NO
RT ms 743.9 (140.4) 742.2 (142.5) 750.9 (161.8) 745.5 (169.5) 740.5 (141.2) 734.6 (139.9) 734.7 (160.04) 736.9 (157.8)
% Acc 68% 69% 65% 67% 68% 73% 71% 69%
Ratings
TF NF TO NO
Valence 3.53 (.89) 4.05 (.71) 3.11 (1.21) 5.23 (1.29)
Arousal 5.78 (.95) 5.38 (.89) 6.31 (1.30) 4.20 (1.10)
Dominance 5.79 (1.79) 6.10 (2.82) 5.68 (2.22) 6.74 (1.61)

TF = Threatening Faces, NF = Neutral Faces, TO = Threatening Objects, NO = Neutral Objects Ratings: Note. Means indicate the average of the 16 images per category. Likert scales for valence (1 = very unpleasant, 9 = very pleasant), arousal (1 = very relaxed, 9 = very tense), dominance (1 = no control over the situation, 9 = full in control of the situation

With regards to accuracy, a significant effect of load, F(1, 44) = 262.23, p < .01, η2 = .86, showed that participants were more accurate in low load (M = .941, SD = .08) compared to high load trials (M = .687, SD = .14). A significant effect of distractor presence, F(1, 44) = 33.38, p < .01, η2 = .43, indicated that they were more accurate on distractor absent (M = .826, SD = .10) compared to distractor present trials (M = .802, SD = .11). There was no significant interaction between load x distractor emotionality, F(1, 44) = .76, p = .39, or type, F(1, 44) = 1.72, p = .20, suggesting that effects of load remain irrespective of these variables.

Subjective reactivity

Table 1 presents mean valence, arousal and dominance ratings for each distractor category. A Repeated Measures ANOVA with distractor emotionality (threatening, neutral), and distractor type (face, object), as within-subject variables, and SA (low, high) as a between subject variable showed no significant main or interactive effects of SA on ratings. With regards to valence, a significant interaction of distractor type x emotionality interaction, F(1, 42) = 35.20, p < .001, η2 = .46, indicated that participants, reported feeling more positive when viewing neutral compared to threatening faces. A significant interaction between distractor type and emotionality, F(1, 42) = 40.22, p < .001, η2 = .49, showed that they also reported less arousal in the presence of neutral compared to threatening faces F(1, 42) = 12.26, p < .0001, η2 = .23, and higher arousal in the presence of threatening compared to neutral objects, F(1, 42) = 72.23, p < .001, η2 = .63. A significant interaction between distractor type and emotionality, F(1, 42) = 8.06, p = .007, η2 = .16 indicated that participants reported less dominance in the presence of threatening compared to neutral faces, and threatening compared to neutral objects F(1, 42) = 18.60, p < .001, η2 = .31.

Physiological reactivity

A significant interaction between distractor type and distractor emotionality was found, F(1, 40) = 4.34, p = .044, η2 = .10. This interaction was broken down by examining the effect for each distractor type, and showed that participants exhibited higher HR in blocks with threatening (M = 79.92, SD = 13.69) compared to neutral faces (M = 79.72, SD = 11.86), F(1, 40) = 7.45, p = .009, η2 = .16.

Main hypotheses

Effects of SA on performance

Looking at the between group effects from the ANOVA examining RT, a significant interaction between distractor type and SA level was observed, F(1, 44) = 4.93, p = .03, η2 = .10. This was broken down by examining the distractor type effect in each SA group separately. Low SA participants were slower in RT on blocks with distractor objects compared to faces, F(1, 22) = 4.73, p = .041. Among high SA participants the distractor type effect was non-significant, F(1, 22) = 1.33, p = .26, though RT was numerically slower in blocks with distractor faces (M = 640.12, SD = 122.87) compared to objects (M = 630.22, SD = 133.23). Additionally, a significant distractor emotionality x SA level interaction was found, F(1, 44) = 4.14, p = .048, η2 = .09. When examining each SA group separately, low SA participants were equally fast in RT regardless of distractor emotionality, F(1, 22) = .27, p = .61, while in the high SA group an effect of distractor emotionality, F(1, 22) = 5.17, p = .033, η2 = .19, indicated participants were slower on blocks with threatening, compared to neutral distractors, as hypothesized (Fig 3). There were no significant interactions between SA and within-subjects variables with regards to accuracy.

Fig 3. Interaction between SA level and distractor emotionality (RT).

Fig 3

Error bars represent standard errors.

Effects of SA on physiology

Table 3 presents mean HR for each distractor category per SA group. There was a significant effect of SA level on HR, F(1, 40) = 8.94, p = .005, η2 = .18, so that there was higher overall heart rate in the high SA group (M = 81.01, SD = 12.11) compared to the low SA group (M = 78.45, SD = 12.78). This was modified by a significant two-way interaction between distractor type and SA level, F(1, 40) = 3.97, p = .05, η2 = .09. The interaction was broken down by examining the distractor type effect in each SA group. Distractor type was non-significant, F(1, 20) = 1.21, p = .29, in the low SA group, but significant in the high SA group, F(1, 19) = 5.99, p = .024, η2 = .24 so that in blocks with faces, high SA participants showed higher HR (M = 81.51, SD = 12.91), compared to blocks with objects (M = 80.51, SD = 11.30). A significant two-way interaction between distractor emotionality and SA level was also obtained, F(1, 40) = 5.70, p = .022, η2 = .13. For the low SA group there was no significant effect of distractor emotionality F(1, 20) = .004, p = .95; for high SA participants HR was higher in blocks with threatening distractors (M = 81.31, SD = 13.04) compared to neutral [M = 80.68, SD = 11.18; F(1, 19) = 18.55, p < .001, η2 = .49]. The 3-way SA x type x emotionality interaction was not significant, however, looking at the means in Table 2, suggests that the highest HR was for blocks with threatening faces. A non-significant trend for a three-way load x emotionality x SA interaction indicated additionally that for high SA participants the increased HR for threatening relative to neutral distractors only occurred under low perceptual load conditions F(1, 19) = 3.36, p = .074, η2 = .08. No significant effects of SA or interactions were found for SCL.

Table 3. Mean and standard deviation for HR per distractor category in low and high SA groups.
Mean SD
Low SA Threatening Faces 77.55 12.56
Objects 77.92 12.73
Neutral Faces 78.11 12.67
Objects 78.75 13.16
High SA Threatening Faces 82.40 14.72
Objects 81.08 11.36
Neutral Faces 81.41 11.11
Objects 80.69 11.25

Discussion

This study examined attention biases in SA, taking into consideration task perceptual load, stimulus type and valence, using a task focusing on early processing stages where vigilance is mostly expected. We used multiple dependent measures, including RT, psychophysiology and self-report to clarify the circumstances under which vigilance occurs. The study contributes three main findings, that converge to partially support our hypothesis that high SA individuals show increased vigilance towards threatening faces during early stages of processing. Findings additionally contribute useful insights about the specific circumstances during which such effects appear.

First, high SA participants showed increased distractibility by faces, evidenced in their RT. While low SA participants were more distracted by objects compared to faces, in the high SA group interference was equivalent for both objects and faces, showing that the significance of faces was greater for this group, in accord with our hypothesis. Looking at the means, one can observe that whereas for low SA participants distraction was mostly carried by threatening objects (i.e. guns), high SA participants found both faces and guns to be equally distracting, with threatening faces resulting in the slowest RTs albeit non-significantly so. Thus, threatening faces, in accord with our hypothesis and existing theories [46], hold special significance in SA. The very short stimulus presentation used may have permitted for early attentional vigilance, leading to slower RTs on the primary task to become apparent. We did not obtain the expected type x emotionality interaction in the high SA group, which would have documented more definitively that threatening faces attract significantly more attention than threatening objects, in spite of means being in the expected direction; this may be due to the particular type of threatening objects included: Guns may have signalled immediate survival threat for both high and low SA participants alike, evoking high levels of vigilance and preventing this difference from emerging more clearly. The special significance of threatening faces for high SA participants, in accord with our main hypothesis, is however, also corroborated by the increased HR on blocks with face distractors for this group only, especially threatening ones, which suggests that faces elicit defensive action preparation but only for those socially anxious. The reason that psychophysiological effects were seen on HR but not SCL may be due to the fact that HR is sensitive to both valence and arousal aspects of emotion [58], whereas SCL is more sensitive to arousal. What makes a threatening face elicit defensive action may entail both its arousal and aversiveness value.

Secondly, high SA individuals had slower RTs on trials with threatening than neutral distractors, showing that their attention was attracted by potentially survival related stimuli, irrespective of their type. Although this may be an appropriate response if the threat is real or significant, low SA participants did not show this general effect, remaining apparently focused on the primary task, as instructed irrespective of the emotionality of distractor stimuli. In contrast, even in the safe context of the lab, with no real danger present, socially anxious individuals were unable to filter out threatening distractors of any type, and showed slowing in their performance and defensive preparation; this is in accordance with [4, 5], Lang, and others who describe anxious individuals as having exaggerated defensive reactions even at low levels of threat.

Third, there was no significant interaction between stimulus type or emotionality with Load for either group. This suggests that perceptual load effects on RT held irrespective of type of distractor stimulus for both high and low SA participants, which is in contrast to our hypothesis that faces would hold such high significance for high SA participants as to over-ride load effects and occur even under high perceptual load. As indicated by multiple prior studies [3335], the high perceptual load task seemed to absorb available perceptual resources, prohibiting any significant levels of distraction, at least on RT measures. The marginal interaction showing that increased HR to threating stimuli in the high SA group occurred only under low perceptual load, concurs with this as well, as defensive mobilization seems to have taken place to a greater degree under conditions where threatening distractors could be more fully perceived, irrespective of how important they were to the individual. Alternatively, one may wonder whether failure to support this hypothesis may suggest that our high SA participants were not anxious enough to demonstrate extreme threat sensitivity. Although participants were non-clinical, the top quartile were selected as high SA were actually higher in SA compared to the SPAI-23 USA college sample, suggesting that significant levels of anxiety were present. Future studies should however, replicate these effects on participants with clinical levels of SAD to validate the present conclusions. Another possibility is that our face stimuli were not threatening enough for the anxious participants, as might be suspected by the absence of SA level x type interaction on subjective ratings. However, the HR and RT effects argue against this possibility and show that threatening faces were perceived as significant and arousing by our high SA group.

Credibility of these results is enhanced by the fact all our manipulations appeared to be effective. Analyses indicated that perceptual load was appropriately manipulated and produced expected effects. Participants were overall slower and less accurate on blocks where the task involved high rather than low perceptual load and when distractors were present. RT was negatively affected by distraction only on low load blocks, consistent with Load Theory [31, 32, 39, 54].

Distractor stimulus emotionality was also manipulated effectively, as shown by subjective and HR responses: Participants rated threatening stimuli, especially faces, as more negative, arousing and leading to less perceived dominance than neutral. Threatening stimuli, particularly faces, also led to increased HR relative to neutral faces and objects, concurring with the idea that such stimuli have significance to human beings and elicit defensive activation [4042].

Results of this study should be seen in light of its strengths and limitations. Among its strengths is the use of multiple measures of engagement with the stimuli, including performance and physiological indices. Our findings converge to demonstrate hypervigilance to threatening stimuli, especially threatening faces, for high SA individuals, as expected. The short time frames of stimulus presentation allowed us to capture vigilance effects during early processing, when they should be most apparent. Moreover, the balanced design and multiple manipulation checks permitted us to examine effects of threatening faces versus other emotional and neutral stimuli on attention and show that high SA individuals are more attentive to threat overall, but are most sensitive to threatening faces. Limitations include the non-clinical nature of the sample, which raises the need for replication among individuals with clinical levels of SA. Additionally, the fact that guns represented the threatening objects may have obliterated more specific attention biases to threatening faces, because guns may signal immediate survival threat and may be equally relevant for both participant groups, eliciting high levels of engagement. Using alternative aversive object stimuli in the future may extend and corroborate present findings. Another limitation pertains to the inability to examine gender effects, due to the primarily female nature of the student body at the University where the sample was recruited. Future research could use gender-balanced and larger community samples, reflecting a normative population. This research can also be extended to examine SA biases towards faces depicting for example disgust or fear.

In spite of some limitations, findings of this study contribute insights, supporting vigilance for threatening faces in SA at very short exposure durations, but indicating that this happens primarily when cognitive resources are available, that is under low perceptual load. It is also consistent with emotion theories [8, 9] and clinical models [13], regarding the cognitive prioritization of stimuli that threaten survival and the distinctive emotional mobilization for personally threatening stimuli in SA.

Data Availability

The data underlying this study contain sensitive participant information and cannot be shared publicly. Data access queries should be directed to the Center for Applied Neuroscience, University of Cyprus (can@ucy.ac.cy).

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Bögels SM, Mansell W. Attention processes in the maintenance and treatment of social phobia: hypervigilance, avoidance and self-focused attention. Clin Psychol Rev. 2004. November;24(7):827–56. 10.1016/j.cpr.2004.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Pergamin L, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, van IJzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133(1):1–24. 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Staugaard SR. Threatening faces and social anxiety: a literature review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010. August;30(6):669–90. 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Öhman A. Face the Beast and Fear the Face: Animal and Social Fears as Prototypes for Evolutionary Analyses of Emotion - ÖHman—1986—Psychophysiology—Wiley Online Library [Internet]. 1986. [cited 2020 Sep 14]. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00608.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Öhman A. Of snakes and faces: An evolutionary perspective on the psychology of fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology [Internet]. 2009. [cited 2020 Sep 14];50(6):543–52. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00784.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Öhman A, Soares SC, Juth P, Lindström B, Esteves F. Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: Serpents and hostile humans. Journal of Cognitive Psychology [Internet]. 2012. February 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];24(1):17–32. Available from: 10.1080/20445911.2011.629603 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.LeDoux JE. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:155–84. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lang PJ. A Bio-Informational Theory of Emotional Imagery. Psychophysiology [Internet]. 1979. [cited 2020 Sep 14];16(6):495–512. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb01511.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bradley MM. Natural selective attention: orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology. 2009. January;46(1):1–11. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00702.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bradley MM, Codispoti M, Cuthbert BN, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion. 2001. September;1(3):276–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lang P. Emotion and Motivation: Toward Consensus Definitions and a Common Research Purpose—[Internet]. 2010. [cited 2020 Sep 14]. Available from: https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1754073910361984 [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lira Yoon K, Zinbarg RE. Threat is in the eye of the beholder: Social anxiety and the interpretation of ambiguous facial expressions. Behaviour Research and Therapy [Internet]. 2007. April 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];45(4):839–47. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796706001033 10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Rapee RM, Heimberg RG. A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behav Res Ther. 1997. August;35(8):741–56. 10.1016/s0005-7967(97)00022-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Gilboa-Schechtman E, Foa EB, Amir N. Attentional Biases for Facial Expressions in Social Phobia: The Face-in-the-Crowd Paradigm. Cognition and Emotion [Internet]. 1999. May 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];13(3):305–18. Available from: 10.1080/026999399379294 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Mogg K, Philippot P, Bradley BP. Selective attention to angry faces in clinical social phobia. J Abnorm Psychol. 2004. February;113(1):160–5. 10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.160 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pishyar R, Harris LM, Menzies RG. Attentional bias for words and faces in social anxiety. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping [Internet]. 2004. March 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];17(1):23–36. Available from: 10.1080/10615800310001601458 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Helfinstein SM, White LK, Bar-Haim Y, Fox NA. Affective primes suppress attention bias to threat in socially anxious individuals. Behav Res Ther. 2008. July;46(7):799–810. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.03.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Klumpp H, Amir N. Examination of vigilance and disengagement of threat in social anxiety with a probe detection task. Anxiety Stress Coping [Internet]. 2009. May [cited 2020 Sep 14];22(3):283–96. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3712328/ 10.1080/10615800802449602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sposari JA, Rapee RM. Attentional Bias Toward Facial Stimuli Under Conditions of Social Threat in Socially Phobic and Nonclinical Participants. Cogn Ther Res [Internet]. 2007. February 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];31(1):23–37. Available from: 10.1007/s10608-006-9073-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mogg K, Bradley BP. Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behav Res Ther. 2002. December;40(12):1403–14. 10.1016/s0005-7967(02)00017-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kolassa I-T, Miltner WHR. Psychophysiological correlates of face processing in social phobia. Brain Research [Internet]. 2006. November 6 [cited 2020 Sep 14];1118(1):130–41. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0006899306023778 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jong PJ de Koster EHW, Wees R van, Martens S. Emotional facial expressions and the attentional blink: Attenuated blink for angry and happy faces irrespective of social anxiety. Cognition and Emotion [Internet]. 2009. December 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];23(8):1640–52. Available from: 10.1080/02699930802490227 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Chen YP, Ehlers A, Clark DM, Mansell W. Patients with generalized social phobia direct their attention away from faces. Behaviour Research and Therapy [Internet]. 2002. June 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];40(6):677–87. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005796701000869 10.1016/s0005-7967(01)00086-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Juth P, Lundqvist D, Karlsson A, Öhman A. Looking for Foes and Friends: Perceptual and Emotional Factors When Finding a Face in the Crowd. Emotion. 2005;5(4):379–95. 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Dillen LF, Heslenfeld DJ, Koole SL. Tuning down the emotional brain: an fMRI study of the effects of cognitive load on the processing of affective images. Neuroimage. 2009. May 1;45(4):1212–9. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Carmel D, Fairnie J, Lavie N. Weight and See: Loading Working Memory Improves Incidental Identification of Irrelevant Faces. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2012. August 15 [cited 2020 Sep 14];3. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3419462/ 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00286 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jenkins R, Lavie N, Driver J. Recognition memory for distractor faces depends on attentional load at exposure. Psychon Bull Rev. 2005. April;12(2):314–20. 10.3758/bf03196378 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Palermo R, Rhodes G. Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2007. January 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];45(1):75–92. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393206001655 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lavie N. Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1995;21(3):451–68. 10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lavie N, Dalton P. Load Theory of Attention and Cognitive Control [Internet]. The Oxford Handbook of Attention. 2014. [cited 2020 Sep 14]. Available from: https://www.oxfordhandbooks.com/view/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199675111.001.0001/oxfordhb-9780199675111-e-003 [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Lavie N, Ro T, Russell C. The role of perceptual load in processing distractor faces. Psychol Sci. 2003. September;14(5):510–5. 10.1111/1467-9280.03453 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Silvert L, Lepsien J, Fragopanagos N, Goolsby B, Kiss M, Taylor JG, et al. Influence of attentional demands on the processing of emotional facial expressions in the amygdala. Neuroimage. 2007. November 1;38(2):357–66. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lim S-L, Padmala S, Pessoa L. Affective learning modulates spatial competition during low-load attentional conditions. Neuropsychologia [Internet]. 2008. April [cited 2020 Sep 14];46(5):1267–78. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2440675/ 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Pessoa L, Padmala S, Morland T. Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage. 2005. October 15;28(1):249–55. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yates A, Ashwin C, Fox E. Does Emotion Processing Require Attention? The Effects of Fear Conditioning and Perceptual Load. Emotion [Internet]. 2010. December [cited 2020 Sep 14];10(6):822–30. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3491873/ 10.1037/a0020325 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Soares SC, Rocha M, Neiva T, Rodrigues P, Silva CF. Social anxiety under load: The effects of perceptual load in processing emotional faces. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Fox E, Yates A, Ashwin C. Trait anxiety and perceptual load as determinants of emotion processing in a fear conditioning paradigm. Emotion. 2012. April;12(2):236–49. 10.1037/a0025321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Cooney RE, Atlas LY, Joormann J, Eugène F, Gotlib IH. Amygdala activation in the processing of neutral faces in social anxiety disorder: is neutral really neutral? Psychiatry Res. 2006. November 22;148(1):55–9. 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2006.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Forster S, Lavie N. Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: the role of load. J Exp Psychol Appl. 2008. March;14(1):73–83. 10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Vrana SR, Gross D. Reactions to facial expressions: effects of social context and speech anxiety on responses to neutral, anger, and joy expressions. Biol Psychol. 2004. March;66(1):63–78. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2003.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Wieser MJ, Pauli P, Alpers GW, Mühlberger A. Is eye to eye contact really threatening and avoided in social anxiety?—An eye-tracking and psychophysiology study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders [Internet]. 2009. January 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];23(1):93–103. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0887618508000972 10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wangelin BC, Bradley MM, Kastner A, Lang PJ. Affective engagement for facial expressions and emotional scenes: The influence of social anxiety. Biol Psychol [Internet]. 2012. September [cited 2020 Sep 14];91(1):103–10. Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3407306/ 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods [Internet]. 2007. May 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];39(2):175–91. Available from: 10.3758/BF03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Schry AR, Roberson-Nay R, White SW. Measuring social anxiety in college students: a comprehensive evaluation of the psychometric properties of the SPAI-23. Psychol Assess. 2012. December;24(4):846–54. 10.1037/a0027398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Roberson-Nay R, Strong DR, Nay WT, Beidel DC, Turner SM. Development of an abbreviated Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory (SPAI) using item response theory: the SPAI-23. Psychol Assess. 2007. March;19(1):133–45. 10.1037/1040-3590.19.1.133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Turner SM, Beidel DC, Dancu CV, Stanley MA. An empirically derived inventory to measure social fears and anxiety: The Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989;1(1):35–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Panayiotou G, et al. Social Anxiety Disorder: Prevalence, co-morbidity and comparison of two screening measures in adolescents. Poster presented at the CAN annual conference, Nicosia, Cyprus. 2014;
  • 48.Theodorou M, & Panayiotou G,. Psychometric properties of the Social Phobia and Anxiety Inventory-23 in Greek-Cypriot college population. Poster presented at the CAN annual conference, Nicosia, Cyprus. 2015;
  • 49.Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Öhman A. The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institutet. 1998;91(630):2–2. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Konkle T, Brady TF, Alvarez GA, Oliva A. Conceptual distinctiveness supports detailed visual long-term memory for real-world objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General [Internet]. 2010. August [cited 2020 Sep 14];139(3):558–78. Available from: http://doi.apa.org/getdoi.cfm?doi=10.1037/a0019165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Zachariou V, Safiullah Z, Ungerleider L. The FFA can process a non-face category of objects as robustly as faces. Journal of Vision [Internet]. 2015. September 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];15(12):424–424. Available from: https://jov.arvojournals.org/article.aspx?articleid=2433532 [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Theodorou M, et al. Emotional dimensions reflected in ratings of pictures (faces and objects). Presented at the Annual Scientific Conference of the Center for Applied Neuroscience (CAN), September, Nicosia, Cyprus. 2016;
  • 53.Fridlund AJ, Cacioppo JT. Guidelines for Human Electromyographic Research. Psychophysiology [Internet]. 1986. [cited 2020 Oct 9];23(5):567–89. Available from: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00676.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Forster S, Lavie N. High perceptual load makes everybody equal: eliminating individual differences in distractibility with load. Psychol Sci. 2007. May;18(5):377–81. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01908.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodological) [Internet]. 1995. [cited 2020 Sep 14];57(1):289–300. Available from: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2346101 [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Pintzinger NM, Pfabigan DM, Tran US, Kryspin-Exner I, Lamm C. Attentional biases in healthy adults: Exploring the impact of temperament and gender. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry [Internet]. 2016. September 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];52:29–37. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0005791616300167 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.02.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vaskevich A, Luria R. Adding statistical regularity results in a global slowdown in visual search. Cognition [Internet]. 2018. May 1 [cited 2020 Sep 14];174:19–27. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010027718300180 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Witvliet CV, Vrana SR. Psychophysiological responses as indices of affective dimensions. Psychophysiology. 1995. September;32(5):436–43. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1995.tb02094.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Christos Papadelis

4 Mar 2021

Your face scares me: Effects of Perceptual load and Social Anxiety on processing of threatening and neutral faces

PONE-D-20-37708

Dear Dr. Theodorou,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Christos Papadelis, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. In your Methods section, please ensure you have specifically stated whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians of the minors included in the study or whether the research ethics committee or IRB specifically waived the need for their consent.

2. Please ensure that you include a title page within your main document. We do appreciate that you have a title page document uploaded as a separate file, however, as per our author guidelines (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title-page) we do require this to be part of the manuscript file itself and not uploaded separately.

Could you therefore please include the title page into the beginning of your manuscript file itself, listing all authors and affiliations.

3. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please move it to the Methods section and delete it from any other section. Please ensure that your ethics statement is included in your manuscript, as the ethics statement entered into the online submission form will not be published alongside your manuscript.

4. We note that Figures 1 and 2 include an image of a participant in the study. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license.

Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes.

Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

Please respond by return e-mail with an amended manuscript. We can upload this to your submission on your behalf.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, please either instruct us to remove the figure or supply a replacement figure by return e-mail for which you hold the relevant copyright permissions and subject consents. In some cases, you may need to specify in the text that the image used in the figure is not the original image used in the study, but a similar image used for illustrative purposes only. We can make any changes on your behalf.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors examine how individuals high in social anxiety, compared to those low in social anxiety, can more readily be distracted by threatening stimuli, especially faces. They also explore the role of perceptual oad. The study was well-designed, well-executed. It was also notable that the authors collected physiological measures as well. Most of the hypotheses were supported, and the authors had reasonable explanations for hypotheses that were not supported (e.g., why threatening faces were not significantly different from threatening objects for those high in SA; why no interaction with stimuli type and emotionality with perceptual load); they also used supplementary physiological measures (e.g., HR) to buttress their accounts. All in all, this one-study manuscript has all the strengths of a one-study manuscript (and all the limitations of a one-study manuscript). The authors also present a comprehensive literature review in the introduction which later allows them to be circumspect and contextualize their findings, describing how their findings dovetail with the rest of the literature.

One limitation that the authors do not address is the decision to focus on the bottom and top quartiles of the SA measures. They report that the average of their sample is a little high, compared to other normative samples. So, why not just a median split to get at the difference between hi and low SA? Or why not keep the SA measure as a continuous variable, and search for interactions? Keeping individual differences measures continuous, instead of splitting them, seems more common in personality and social psychology research. Anyhow, that issue would be good to explain.

Reviewer #2: On the whole I think this is a straightforward and cleanly executed study. The effects in question are to my knowledge novel and demonstrate an intriguing relationship between cognitive load and social anxiety. One interesting consideration that could be a useful way to explore the role of the face itself vs. the low-level distracting information in the face pattern could be consider either negating the contrast of the face or presenting the face image upside-down, both of which are known to reduce the efficiency of face recognition substantially. However, I think the results are presented here with appropriate nuance and the current design supports interesting conclusions that may lead to further work in this area.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Christos Papadelis

16 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-37708

Your face scares me: Effects of Perceptual load and Social Anxiety on processing of threatening and neutral faces  

Dear Dr. Theodorou:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Christos Papadelis

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Availability Statement

    The data underlying this study contain sensitive participant information and cannot be shared publicly. Data access queries should be directed to the Center for Applied Neuroscience, University of Cyprus (can@ucy.ac.cy).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES