Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Mar 25;16(3):e0249045. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249045

The gender gap in aversion to COVID-19 exposure: Evidence from professional tennis

Zuzanna Kowalik 1,#, Piotr Lewandowski 1,2,*,#
Editor: Valerio Capraro3
PMCID: PMC7993799  PMID: 33765075

Abstract

We study the gender differences in aversion to COVID-19 exposure using a natural experiment of the 2020 US Open. It was the first major tennis tournament after the season had been paused for six months, held with the same rules and prize money for men and women. We analyze the gender gap in the propensity to voluntarily withdraw because of COVID-19 concerns among players who were eligible and fit to play. We find that female players were significantly more likely than male players to have withdrawn from the 2020 US Open. While players from countries characterized by relatively high levels of trust and patience and relatively low levels of risk-taking were more likely to have withdrawn than their counterparts from other countries, female players exhibited significantly higher levels of aversion to pandemic exposure than male players even after cross-country differences in preferences are accounted for. About 15% of the probability of withdrawing that is explained by our model can be attributed to gender.

Introduction and motivation

The COVID-19 pandemic has affected men and women differently. Compared to men, women are less likely to become severely ill or die from COVID-19 [1], but are as likely to be infected. In 35 high-income countries with available data, women constitute the majority (52%) of those infected (according to the COVID-19 Sex-Disaggregated Data Tracker data). Gender differences in engaging in workplace interactions that are critical for the spread of infectious diseases transmitted by the respiratory or close-contact route, such as COVID-19 [2], are among the social factors that contribute to this gender gap. Compared to male workers, female workers have higher occupational exposure to contagion, and are more likely to work in highly exposed occupations, such as health professionals, care, and personal care workers [3]. At the same time, the COVID-19 crisis has affected the employment outcomes of women more than those of men. This is in part because the crisis hit the female-dominated sectors particularly hard, and, within these industries, women have been more likely than men to lose their jobs [4].

Policy responses from all over the world aim to reduce social contacts and limit contagion, but the compliance with non-pharmaceutical interventions can vary across different groups. Insights from social and behavioural sciences can help to understand the gap between the recommendations of experts and actual human behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic [5]. Gender is perceived as one of the factors associated with this compliance. Previous research has found that women are more likely than men to perceive the COVID-19 pandemic as a very serious health problem [6] and to engage in preventive behaviours [7]. Women more often follow the public health recommendations, such as wearing face masks [8], especially when these are not compulsory [9]. Also during the past respiratory epidemics, women were found to be 50% more likely than men to adopt non-pharmaceutical protective behaviours [10].

Overall, women appear to be more concerned with the pandemic. An important question that arises in this context is whether these potential gender differences in perceptions of COVID-19 risk affect the labor market participation of men and women.

In this paper, we contribute to the literature on the gender dimension of the COVID-19 pandemic by studying the gender gap in aversion to pandemic exposure. We use a natural experiment of the US Open tennis tournament held in New York City between August 31 and September 13, 2020. It was the first major tournament that was organized after the tennis season had been put on hiatus due to COVID-19 concerns. We analyze the factors associated with the voluntary withdrawals of players who were eligible to play. The Grand Slam tennis tournaments, like the US Open, constitute a useful setting for studying gender differences in decision-making and performance, as the conditions for participation, the structure of the tournaments, and the prize money amounts are identical for men and women. In 2020, the health and safety protocols at the US Open were also the same for both genders. Hence, the gender differences in the propensity to voluntarily withdraw from the US Open can be interpreted as having been driven by gender differences in aversion to pandemic exposure, especially given that the tournament took place in the country that had the highest numbers of COVID-19 cases and deaths.

We find that female players were significantly more likely to have voluntarily withdrawn from the tournament, which was organized in an epidemiologically risky setting. Higher-ranked players and older players were also more likely to have withdrawn. Our findings are robust to controlling for cross-country differences in cultural preferences. While players from countries characterized by higher levels of patience and trust and by lower levels of risk-taking were also significantly more likely to have withdrawn, the gender gap remains significant even after we control for these factors. It is also found to be particularly large among players ranked in the top 50, who are richer than lower ranked players.

Gender differences in risk-taking have been cited as being among the main causes of gender gaps in employment outcomes, including the gender pay gap and the underrepresentation of women in top-tier jobs [11] Experimental studies have suggested that females are more averse to risk and tend to shy away from competitive settings [12], although the estimated sizes of the gender differences in risk attitudes vary depending on the method used and the context in which decisions are made [13] The real-life studies tend to be focused on gender differences in investment choices in financial markets or pension funds [14]. However, grasping the differences in risk-taking behavior is harder in real-life situations than in lab experiments because outside of the laboratory, the available options are usually not restricted to a well-defined set of choices. Professional sports create opportunities to tackle this challenge [15]. Sports involve rules that aim to ensure that the players have symmetrical information, and to define a set of options that can often be ranked by their level of risk. Incentive structures and rewards are linked to performance, which can be precisely measured. Individual sports such as tennis, are particularly suitable to study strategic behavior [16, 17]. Psychological factors, such as risk preferences, play an important role [18, 19]. We contribute to this strand of literature by studying gender differences in reactions to a specific type of risk related to exposure to a pandemic. While we do not study the actual performance of players in the tournament, we use its identical treatment of men and women to assess gender differences in the willingness to participate in a professional activity perceived as risky and inconvenient due the COVID-19 pandemic.

Data and methodology

The professional tennis season was paused on March 12, 2020, due to concerns about COVID-19. The female tour resumed on August 3, 2020, with tournaments in Palermo and Prague, followed by the Western & Southern Open and the US Open tournaments played at the same venue in New York City. The male tour resumed on September 22, 2020, with the Western & Southern Open tournament, followed by the US Open, which were played at the same venue, and parallel to the female tournaments. During the tournaments in NYC, a “bubble” was created and the same health and safety rules were applied to all male and female players: (i) each player’s entourage could include up to three people, and (ii) the players had to stay in one of the two pre-approved hotels. The players were not allowed to leave the bubble, or to have contact with any people outside of it. Audiences were not allowed in the stadiums.

The Grand Slam tournaments such as the US Open have an identical structure for male and female competitions: 128 players are included in each draw, and the prize money is the same for men and women. In general, players ranked in the top 100 before a Grand Slam tournament are automatically eligible to play, while the remaining places are awarded to players who are successful in a qualifying tournament, and players who receive the so-called wildcards. In 2020, the qualifying tournament was not held, and entry lists were created based on rankings. However, we restricted our study to the top 100 male and female players, as these players could have expected long in advance that they would be eligible to play. To collect information about rankings, we scraped data from websites of Association of Tennis Professionals (www.atptour.com) and Women Tennis Association (www.wtatennis.com), and we merged it with the entry list to the US Open (www.usopen.org).

In 2020, 36 out of the top 100 male and female players did not participate in the US Open. Of these players, 25 (19 women and six men) withdrew due to COVID-19 concerns (Fig 1). Another 11 players withdrew for other reasons. For each player, we specified the reason for his/her withdrawal using his/her social media posts and/or media interviews (see S1 and S2 Tables, sources available upon request). Our final sample includes 189 players from 48 countries who were eligible and fit to play.

Fig 1. Structure of top 100 players by the US Open status.

Fig 1

Source: Own elaboration based on webscraped data.

In order to analyze the differences between female and male players in the propensity to withdraw from performing in a pandemic environment, we estimated logistic (1) regressions:

Pr(withdrawfromUSOpenjc=1)=F(β0+β1Xj+β2λc+εjc) (1)

where F(Z)=eZ1+eZ, j stands for individual, and c for country; Xj is a vector of personal characteristics (sex, age (log), ranking (log)); and λc is a vector of country-level controls.

As noted by Van Bavel et al. [5], some differences in the response to the pandemic may be described as cultural. Bargain and Aminjonov [20] showed that European regions with higher levels of political trust recorded significantly larger mobility reductions, and more pronounced effects of the containment policy stringency, so trust appears to be related to compliance. In order to account for cultural factors, we controlled for country-level measures of patience, risk-taking, altruism, and trust based on the Global Preference Survey conducted in 76 countries [21] to account for the general willingness to take risks, as well as in other preferences observed differences in preferences that may affect attitudes toward the pandemic and compliance with containment policies. The GPS data are not available for the following countries that include a total of 21 top 100 male or female players who were eligible and fit to participate: Belgium, Belarus, Latvia, Montenegro, Norway, Puerto Rico, Slovenia, Slovakia, Taipei, Tunisia, and Uruguay. We also controlled for GDP per capita (in purchasing power parity, log, from the World Development Indicators database), as gender differences in preferences may be expressed more frequently in more developed than in less developed countries [22]. We standardized all country-level variables, ages, and rankings.

We estimated two variants of model (1). First, we controlled for individual characteristics only. Second, we added country-level controls. We also re-estimated our model on a subsample of non-US players, for whom the perceived risk of participation may be higher because they have to undertake international travel and may be unfamiliar with the local health system; as well as for subsamples of players ranked in the top 50 and in places 51–100. In order to test whether our findings are robust to changes in estimation method, we also estimated probit and linear probability models instead of a logit model.

Finally, in order to assess the relative role of gender and other factors in the probability of withdrawing from the 2020 US Open, we used the Shapley decomposition proposed by Shorrocks [23]. For each factor, the marginal impact on the withdrawal probability is calculated as the factors are eliminated in succession. Next, these marginal effects are averaged over all the possible elimination sequences. The contributions sum to the total probability explained by a model, and they can be interpreted as expected marginal effects. This method originated with poverty decompositions, but can be applied to any econometric specification [23].

Results

We begin with descriptive evidence. In 2020, the total number of players who withdrew from the US Open (36) was more than double the number in 2019 (17). In 2019, 17 out of the top 100 male and female players withdrew from the US Open because of injuries or for personal reasons. In 2020, the number of injured players was lower than it was in 2019, as there was no competition between March and August which reduced the risk of injury. Thus, the increase in the total number of withdrawals was driven by voluntary withdrawals which suggests that the pandemic increased the propensity of players to avoid performing.

The share of women among the top 100 players who voluntarily withdrew from the 2020 US Open was noticeably higher (76%) than it was among the players who participated (46%, Table 1). Moreover, the players who withdrew were, on average, older and higher ranked than the players who participated. These players also came disproportionately from countries that exhibit higher levels of patience, lower levels of risk-taking, higher levels of altruism and trust, and lower levels of GDP per capita.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

Female Age Ranking Patience Risk taking Altruism Trust GDP per capita
Participant 46.3% 26.9 53 -0.02 0.03 0.00 -0.04 0.00
Withdrawn 76.0% 29.1 34 0.21 -0.04 0.11 0.45 -0.16

Note: All country-level variables- patience, risk taking, altruism, trust and GDP per capita are standardized in our sample.

Source: Own calculations based on data web scraped from US Open, ATP and WTA websites, GPS, and WDI data.

Next, we present our econometric results. We find that female players were significantly more likely (by 15.3 percentage points on average) to decide against participating in the US Open than men (column 1 of Table 2). Older players, and higher ranked players were also more likely to withdraw. The probable mechanism behind the effect associated with ranking is that higher ranked players are more affluent and earn higher incomes from endorsements, and thus may have been more willing to forego prize money from a tournament perceived as risky. The size of the effect associated with gender is found to be slightly smaller (12.3 pp) in a subsample of players for which the Global Preference Survey data are available (column 2 of Table 2). While controlling for country-level covariates reduces the size of the effects associated with gender, it remains highly significant and large (10.2 pp, column 3 of Table 2). Moreover, players from countries characterized by higher levels of patience and by lower levels of risk-taking were significantly more likely to have decided against participating in the 2020 US Open (column 3 of Table 2). This means that part of the differences in the rate of withdrawals among female and male players can be attributed to the fact that women players happen to more often come from countries characterized by stronger preferences conducive to avoiding exposure, such as higher patience and lower risk aversion. However, female players were significantly more likely to withdraw even if these differences in country-level preferences are factored in.

Table 2. The correlates of voluntary withdrawal from the 2020 US Open due to COVID-19 concerns (marginal effects).

Player controls Player controls (sample w/ GPS data) Player and country controls Player and country controls, no US players Player controls, players ranked in top 50 Player and country controls, players ranked in top 50 Player controls, players ranked 51–100 Player and country controls, players ranked 51–100 Player and country controls (probit) Player and country controls (OLS)
(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10)
Female 0.153*** 0.123** 0.102** 0.151*** 0.180** 0.148* 0.064 0.054* 0.092** 0.101**
(0.051) (0.050) (0.048) (0.044) (0.079) (0.079) (0.053) (0.030) (0.044) (0.046)
Age (log) 0.069** 0.071** 0.067** 0.101*** 0.110** 0.101** 0.035 0.044 0.068*** 0.070**
(0.028) (0.029) (0.030) (0.032) (0.048) (0.050) (0.029) (0.031) (0.026) (0.027)
Ranking (log) -0.067*** -0.069*** -0.068*** -0.082*** -0.075** -0.076** -0.012 -0.018 -0.071*** -0.103***
(0.018) (0.018) (0.016) (0.016) (0.033) (0.031) (0.102) (0.102) (0.017) (0.031)
Patience 0.092** 0.101*** 0.109* 0.070 0.092*** 0.089**
(0.037) (0.033) (0.062) (0.055) (0.035) (0.035)
Risk- taking -0.065** -0.049* -0.095* -0.048 -0.059** -0.042**
(0.031) (0.028) (0.057) (0.034) (0.029) (0.021)
Altruism -0.005 0.073** -0.007 0.000 -0.007 -0.000
(0.023) (0.034) (0.044) (0.014) (0.023) (0.022)
Trust 0.042* 0.008 0.046 0.046** 0.043** 0.052*
(0.022) (0.028) (0.046) (0.020) (0.022) (0.027)
GDP per capita (PPP, log) -0.061*** -0.035 -0.054 -0.043** -0.061*** -0.090**
(0.023) (0.026) (0.054) (0.022) (0.023) (0.042)
Adj. R2 / R2 0.185 0.215 0.308 0.412 0.189 0.241 0.103 0.472 0.315 0.233
No. of obs. 189 168 168 142 86 86 82 82 168 168

Note: All parameters are presented as average marginal effects. All models include constant (not presented). R2 shown in the case of linear probability model (8). Robust standard errors in parentheses.

*** p<0.01

** p<0.05

* p<0.1.

Source: Own estimation based on data web scraped from US Open, ATP and WTA websites, GPS, and WDI data

Importantly, in a subsample of non-US players, the effect associated with gender is shown to be noticeably larger (15.1 pp) than in the full sample (column 4 of Table 2). At the same time, the effects associated with other factors at both the individual and the country level are found to be similar to those estimated in the full sample. This suggests that the female players may have been particularly more concerned about exposure associated with travel than the male players.

Finally, the female players were significantly more likely to have voluntarily withdrawn than the male players among the top 50 players (columns 5 and 6 of Table 2), but among the players ranked 51–100 the effect is not significant (columns 7 and 8 of Table 2, respectively). This difference in the effect associated with gender is likely to be related to the income and wealth discrepancies between higher and lower ranked players. Professional tennis exhibits rather large concentration of incomes among those at the top. The top 50 players (who constitute 1% of all players) earn more than 50% of all prize money, and the average earnings of top 50 players are about five times the average earnings of players ranked 51–100, both among women and men [24]. Hence, the top 50 players may be more prepared to forego income from a given tournament perceived as risky. We find that the gender gap in aversion to pandemic exposure is manifested to a larger extent among players with higher ranking and incomes. It is consistent with Falk and Hermle [22] who show that gender differences in preferences are more likely to be revealed when incomes are higher.

Our findings are robust to changes in the econometric methodology, as the probit and linear probability models (columns 9 and 10 of Table 2, respectively) deliver similar estimates as the logit model (column 3 of Table 2).

Having established the statistical significance of particular variables, we move to the decomposition analysis which allows us to assess the magnitudes of various factors in explaining the probability of withdrawal from the 2020 US Open. To this aim, we use the Shapley decomposition proposed by Shorrocks [23]. Overall, our models are able to explain 20–30% of the overall variance of withdrawal probability in different samples (Table 3). It’s a relatively high share considering that the decision about participation may be affected by individual factors and past experiences [25], but our data allow controlling only for sex, age, and ranking, and country-level variables.

Table 3. The Shapley decomposition of the probability of voluntary withdrawal from the 2020 US Open.

Gender Age Ranking Preferences (country-level) GDP per capita Total
All players
Contribution 0.028 0.039 0.100 0.047 0.019 0.233
% of explained variance 12.0% 16.9% 42.8% 20.0% 8.3% 100.0%
Non-US players
Contribution 0.053 0.052 0.108 0.086 0.010 0.310
% of explained variance 17.2% 16.8% 35.0% 27.8% 3.3% 100.0%
Top 50 players
Contribution 0.039 0.066 0.061 0.047 0.008 0.221
% of explained variance 17.8% 29.7% 27.6% 21.2% 3.8% 100.0%
Players ranked 51–100
Contribution 0.018 0.022 0.001 0.098 0.051 0.191
% of explained variance 9.6% 11.6% 0.5% 51.6% 26.7% 100.0%

Note: Shapley decomposition based on linear probability models. Contribution of preferences is a sum of contributions of patience, risk- taking, altruism, and trust.

Source: Own estimation based on data web scraped from US Open, ATP and WTA websites, GPS, and WDI data.

We find that about 15% of explained variance in the probability of voluntary withdrawal can be associated to gender (Table 3). The contribution of gender is the largest (almost 20%) in the sample of non-US players, and in the sample of top 50 players. It is the smallest in the sample of players ranked 51-100(10%).

Overall, ranking is the factor with the largest contribution (30–40% of explained variance), except for the players ranked 51–100 (0.5%). The contribution of age is particularly high among the top 50 players (30%). Thus, the decision to voluntarily withdraw from a tournament in a country badly affected by the pandemic could have been related to the ability to forego income from this tournament, presumably higher among higher ranked players.

The contribution of country-level cultural preferences is the highest among the players ranked 51–100 (50%)–in this group of players for whom the tournament could have been a key source of income, country-level preferences are by far the most important factor associated with voluntary withdrawals. The contribution of preferences is also high among the non-US players (28%). Our results suggest that differences in preferences may translate into differences in attitudes to the pandemic exposure.

Discussion and conclusions

In this paper, we have studied the gender differences in aversion to COVID-19 exposure. To do so, we used a natural experiment of the professional tennis US Open tournament, which was the first major tournament organized after the tennis season had paused for six months due to the pandemic. It was held in the country with the highest numbers of COVID-19 cases and deaths, and 14% of eligible and fit players declined to participate. As the conditions and rules for participation, as well as the prize money amounts, were identical for men and women, we have argued that the differences found in the propensity to voluntarily withdraw reflect gender differences in aversion to pandemic exposure.

Our results show that female players were significantly more likely to have withdrawn from the 2020 US Open because of COVID-19 concerns. Players from countries characterized by higher levels of trust and patience and lower levels of risk-taking were more likely to have withdrawn. However, the female players exhibited significantly higher levels of aversion to pandemic exposure than male players, even if cross-country differences in preferences are accounted for. About 15% of the withdrawal probability explained by our model can be attributed to gender, namely to higher propensity to withdraw among women.

Importantly, the pattern of COVID-19 infections among professional tennis players is in line with our findings: between February and October, 2020, 10 of the top 100 male players, and none of the top 100 female players tested positive. Only one of these players (Benoit Paire) tested positive in the US Open bubble, and none of his contacts have tested positive [26]. This suggests that women players were more likely to engage in preventive behaviors, including withdrawing from a tournament in the US. Previous studies showed that past experiences of negative shocks can increased risk aversion [25] and lower risk taking [27]. However, we think it is unlikely that this experience will affect future risk taking of players, especially of women players who appear more risk-averse, because no outbreaks emerged.

Our study has limitations, in particular related to sample size. However, the US Open is the only natural experiment in tennis that we can study. To our knowledge, no other sport has held parallel competitions with identical rules and prize money amounts for men and women, especially competitions requiring two to three-week long stay at the same venue which amplifies potential pandemic exposure. Moreover, the participation of fit players in the tennis tournaments in Europe, organized after the US Open, was likely affected by the fact that there was no COVID-19 outbreak at the US Open. Indeed, only four players–two Australian (one male, one female) and two Chinese (both female) players–did not participate due to COVID-19 concerns in Roland Garros, the next Grand Slam tournament taking place in Paris on September 27-October, 2020. Finally, our sample size is similar to the one used in past studies of top tennis players [28, 29].

Of course, there may be other hypotheses that can potentially explain gender differences in the propensity to withdraw, career length being one of them. If women had longer professional tennis careers than men, they could perhaps feel they can better accommodate skipping a tournament. In general, women reach their highest career level earlier than men, which is consistent with their more precocious biological development [30]. Indeed, in our sample the average age of top 100 female players (26.9 years) is lower than that of top 100 male players (27.8). Moreover, the average career length of female and male players is not significantly different [30]. Overall, it is unlikely that differences in career life cycle of female and male players can explain different attitudes towards performing during the pandemic.

Our findings have implications for sport competitions as well as for labor markets. Women appear to be more concerned about the pandemic, more willing to comply with preventive measure [6, 7], and more likely to voluntarily avoid performing in a setting perceived as risky. Organizers of sporting competitions should perhaps take these differences into account and adapt the rules and preventive measures to reflect higher level of concern among women. Ignoring these differences may discourage women from performing, which may have negative repercussions for career development, especially among younger players, and may impact negatively on performance if participation is associated with higher level of stress.

Professional tennis players are a relatively well-paid group of people who can afford to avoid participating in tournaments if doing so is perceived as risky, even though because of their young age they face a low risk of severe illness from COVID-19 [1]. In the general labor market, however, women are more exposed to contagion than men because of sectoral and occupational segmentation [3], and can rarely shield themselves from this exposure [4]. However, standard labor market data do not allow studying whether the aversion to pandemic affects willingness to work because most people are not able to freely choose whether they participate in a given work task. Our findings based on a natural experiment suggest that as women have higher levels of aversion to exposure, working women who cannot avoid exposure may experience additional hardships, which may contribute to their reported worse mental health outcomes during the pandemic [31]. Our results also suggest that focusing on gender differences in labor market outcomes may underestimate the true gender impact of the COVID-19 pandemic in terms of wellbeing. Labour, social and public health policies could account for these gender differences in the design of programs aimed at shielding people from the exposure to infection, as well as mental, social and economic consequences of the pandemic.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Players who withdrew from the 2020 US Open because of COVID-19.

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Players who withdrew from the 2020 US Open for other reasons.

(DOCX)

S1 File

(DTA)

Acknowledgments

We thank Jan Gromadzki for help with data scraping. The usual disclaimers apply. All errors are ours.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Williamson EJ, Walker AJ, Bhaskaran K, Bacon S, Bates C, Morton CE, et al. Factors associated with COVID-19-related death using OpenSAFELY. Nature. 2020;584: 430–436. 10.1038/s41586-020-2521-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Mossong J, Hens N, Jit M, Beutels P, Auranen K, Mikolajczyk R, et al. Social Contacts and Mixing Patterns Relevant to the Spread of Infectious Diseases. PLOS Medicine. 2008;5: e74. 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Lewandowski P, Lipowska K, Magda I. The gender dimension of occupational exposure to contagion in Europe. Feminist Economics. 2021. 10.1080/13545701.2021.1880016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Adams-Prassl A, Boneva T, Golin M, Rauh C. Inequality in the impact of the coronavirus shock: Evidence from real time surveys. Journal of Public Economics. 2020;189: 104245. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104245 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Bavel JJV. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour. 2020;4: 12. 10.1038/s41562-019-0780-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Galasso V, Pons V, Profeta P, Becher M, Brouard S, Foucault M. Gender differences in COVID-19 attitudes and behavior: Panel evidence from eight countries. PNAS. 2020. [cited 17 Oct 2020]. 10.1073/pnas.2012520117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Clark C, Davila A, Regis M, Kraus S. Predictors of COVID-19 voluntary compliance behaviors: An international investigation. Glob Transit. 2020/06/26 ed. 2020;2: 76–82. 10.1016/j.glt.2020.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Haischer MH, Beilfuss R, Hart MR, Opielinski L, Wrucke D, Zirgaitis G, et al. Who is wearing a mask? Gender-, age-, and location-related differences during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLOS ONE. 2020;15: e0240785. 10.1371/journal.pone.0240785 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Capraro V, Barcelo H. The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. PsyArXiv; 2020. May. 10.31234/osf.io/tg7vz [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Moran KR, Valle SYD. A Meta-Analysis of the Association between Gender and Protective Behaviors in Response to Respiratory Epidemics and Pandemics. PLOS ONE. 2016;11: e0164541. 10.1371/journal.pone.0164541 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Booth AL, Nolen P. Gender Differences in Risk Behaviour: Does Nurture Matter? The Economic Journal. 2012;122: F56–F78. 10.1111/j.1468-0297.2011.02480.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Croson R, Gneezy U. Gender Differences in Preferences. Journal of Economic Literature. 2009;47: 448–474. 10.1257/jel.47.2.448 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Filippin A. Gender differences in risk attitudes. IZA World of Labor. 2016. [cited 14 Sep 2020]. 10.15185/izawol.100 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Eckel CC, Füllbrunn SC. Thar SHE Blows? Gender, Competition, and Bubbles in Experimental Asset Markets. American Economic Review. 2015;105: 906–920. 10.1257/aer.20130683 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Balafoutas L, Chowdhury SM, Plessner H. Applications of sports data to study decision making. Journal of Economic Psychology. 2019;75: 102153. 10.1016/j.joep.2019.02.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Walker M, Wooders J. Minimax Play at Wimbledon. American Economic Review. 2001;91: 1521–1538. 10.1257/aer.91.5.1521 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ely J, Gauriot R, Page L. Do agents maximise? Risk taking on first and second serves in tennis. Journal of Economic Psychology. 2017;63: 135–142. 10.1016/j.joep.2017.02.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cohen-Zada D, Krumer A, Rosenboim M, Shapir OM. Choking under pressure and gender: Evidence from professional tennis. Journal of Economic Psychology. 2017;61: 176–190. 10.1016/j.joep.2017.04.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Paserman MD. Gender Differences in Performance in Competitive Environments: Evidence from Professional Tennis Players. 2007. Report No.: No. 2834. Available: http://www.ssrn.com/abstract=997269
  • 20.Bargain O, Aminjonov U. Trust and compliance to public health policies in times of COVID-19. Journal of Public Economics. 2020;192: 104316. 10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104316 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Falk A, Becker A, Dohmen T, Enke B, Huffman D, Sunde U. Global Evidence on Economic Preferences. Q J Econ. 2018;133: 1645–1692. 10.1093/qje/qjy013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Falk A, Hermle J. Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality. Science. 2018;362: eaas9899. 10.1126/science.aas9899 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Shorrocks AF. Decomposition procedures for distributional analysis: a unified framework based on the Shapley value. J Econ Inequal. 2013;11: 99–126. 10.1007/s10888-011-9214-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.ITF. ITF pro circuit review. International Tennis Federation; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bu D, Hanspal T, Liao Y, Liu Y. Risk Taking, Preferences, and Beliefs: Evidence from Wuhan. Rochester, NY: Social Science Research Network; 2020 Dec. Report No.: ID 3559870. 10.2139/ssrn.3559870 [DOI]
  • 26.Fitzgerald M. “I’m breaking”: Paire retests COVID-19 positive, still plays Hamburg. In: Tennis.com [Internet]. 2020 [cited 27 Sep 2020]. Available: /pro-game/2020/09/im-breaking-benoit-paire-retests-positive-covid-19-still-plays-hamburg-ruud/90915/
  • 27.Malmendier U, Nagel S. Depression Babies: Do Macroeconomic Experiences Affect Risk Taking? The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2011;126: 373–416. 10.1093/qje/qjq004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Moss B, O’Donoghue P. Momentum in US Open men’s singles tennis. International Journal of Performance Analysis in Sport. 2015;15: 884–896. 10.1080/24748668.2015.11868838 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Maquirriain J, Baglione R, Cardey M. Male professional tennis players maintain constant serve speed and accuracy over long matches on grass courts. European Journal of Sport Science. 2016;16: 845–849. 10.1080/17461391.2016.1156163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Guillaume M, Len S, Tafflet M, Quinquis L, Montalvan B, Schaal K, et al. Success and decline: top 10 tennis players follow a biphasic course. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011;43: 2148–2154. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31821eb533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Oreffice S, Quintana-Domeque C. Gender Inequality in COVID-19 Times: Evidence from UK Prolific Participants. 2020 p. 42. Report No.: No. 13463.

Decision Letter 0

Valerio Capraro

12 Jan 2021

PONE-D-20-38751

The gender gap in aversion to COVID-19 exposure: evidence from professional tennis

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Lewandowski,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please find below the reviewers' comments, as well as those of mine.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Feb 25 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

I have now collected two reviews from two experts in the field. The reviewers like the paper but suggest a major revision. Therefore, I would like to invite you to revise your paper following the reviewer's comments. Besides these, I would like to add two more comments, regarding the literature review, which I found to be incomplete. (i) The "perspective article" on what social and behavioural science can do to support pandemic response, published by Van Bavel et al. in Nature Human Behaviour can be a useful introductory reference. (ii) regarding gender differences in pandemic response, I have recently published a paper on gender differences in wearing a face mask and in perceived likelihood to get infected by covid-19, whose results seem to be in line with your results (Capraro & Barcelo, 2020). Of course, it is not a requirement to cite these papers, but I am mentioning them, because they seem very related to yours.

I am looking forward for the revision.

References

Capraro, V., & Barcelo, H. (2020). The effect of messaging and gender on intentions to wear a face covering to slow down COVID-19 transmission. Journal of Behavioral Economics for Policy, 4, Special Issue 2, 45-55.

Van Bavel, J. J., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 460-471.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

3. We note that your paper includes detailed descriptions of individual patients/participants. As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: General Comments: The current study uses analysis of participation in the 2020 US Open tennis tournament to investigate gender differences in aversion to COVID-19 exposure. The authors are applauded for their novel approach in using the tournament as a natural experiment and the results align with previous literature indicating that women are more likely to engage in protective behaviors (e.g. withdrawing from the tournament). While this work has interesting results, I challenge the authors to consider the implications of their findings more rigorously. Assuming that aversion to exposure to COVID-19 is still relevant as we are still in the throws of the pandemic, how might these results relate to sporting competition at lower levels? Is gender differences in aversion to exposure something that needs to be addressed to facilitate increased participation among females? The authors note that tournaments after the US Open did not see a similar number of withdrawals but this may still be an issue at lower levels of sporting competition. Additionally, the overall flow of the manuscript can be significantly improved to better facilitate readability and comprehension of the material. As noted in my comments, there are multiple paragraphs that seem out of place. Even so, with the necessary revisions I believe this manuscript will make a valuable addition to the COVID-19 literature as it continues to underscore gender differences in the approach to managing the pandemic.

Major Concerns:

• Lines 50-56: As this paragraph involves discussion of the results, it seems it should be moved out of the introduction of the manuscript

• Lines 124-125: Please provide further detail for readers on the Shapley decomposition method

• Lines 160-163; 165-168: Please clarify how the percentage of explained variance is obtained or calculated. Can this be discerned from Figure 2? In the figure 2 caption, please provide greater detail and clarify what the x-axis represents.

• Discussion: The last two paragraphs in the discussion section seem like they should be swapped. Ending the manuscript by discussing the limitations minimizes the impact of the work so I would suggest addressing limitations and then finishing with the concluding paragraph.

Minor Concerns:

• Line 51: “Higher-ranked”

• Line 85: Word missing? “…study (to) the…”

• Line 103 and throughout: I believe text concerning the methodology should be in past tense (i.e. “…we estimated logistic…)

• Line 123: Please briefly clarify for readers why probit and linear probability models were estimated in addition to a logit model.

• Line 133: Please write out what “pp” indicates before using the abbreviation

• Lines 170-172: Seems like a word or two is missing in this sentence as it is unclear what is being discussed (“…while the contribution of country-level preferences…”)

• Lines 186-187: Please discuss further the result that 15% of the withdrawal probability explained can be attributed to gender. This seems low, especially given the disparity in withdrawals between males and females. What could explain the other 85%? Is the low percentage explained a limitation and does it hinder the ability to draw strong conclusions about gender differences from these data?

• Do females typically have longer professional careers than males? If so, they may feel they are better able to accommodate skipping a tournament. Just a thought.

Reviewer #2: Referee report on:

“The Gender Gap in Aversion to COVID-19 Exposure: Evidence from Professional Tennis”

Manuscript: PONE-D-38751

PLOS One

Summary and General Assessment:

This paper studies gender differences in aversion to risks related to the COVID-19 Pandemic using the setting of the 2020 US Open. The authors find that female players are more likely to withdraw from the tournament compared to men, even after controlling for country-characteristics. About 15% of the withdrawal rate is explained by gender. .

In general, there are many things to like about this paper, the setting is novel and well suited to study this question, the paper is written in excellent English, and the analysis is competently executed.

Main Comments:

1. I would like to see a larger discussion about the weaknesses of the study, most centrally about the statistical inference given the small sample size. I know that this is mentioned briefly in the last paragraph, however the authors should think carefully about if there is evidence that would support these findings given a larger dataset. Additionally, if there is a larger sample which could be used as a robustness check, this would greatly strengthen the analysis.

2. The introduction and beginning of the paper should include a larger discussion about changes in risk taking throughout the COVID pandemic. A number of papers now examine how preferences and beliefs over risk change due to exposure to the COVID pandemic (e.g., Bu et al 2020). I think that this study needs to link to that larger literature and perhaps a brief discussion more fundamentally if the authors observations are driven by changing preferences/beliefs about risk due to exposure, or if their effects are more driven by innate gender differences towards risk.

3. The authors may also consider gender differences in expectations and beliefs in other domains, and how this may motivate their study, for example, D’Acunto et al (2019).

References

D’Acunto, F., Malmendier, U., Ospina, J. and Weber, M., 2019. Exposure to daily price changes and inflation expectations. Forthcoming Journal of Political Economy

Bu, Di and Hanspal, Tobin and Liao, Yin and Liu, Yong. 2020. Risk Taking, Preferences, and Beliefs: Evidence from Wuhan. Working Paper.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Mar 25;16(3):e0249045. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249045.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


23 Feb 2021

Response to editor:

Thank you for your consideration of our manuscript titled titled “The gender gap in aversion to COVID-19 exposure: evidence from professional tennis” for consideration by the PLOS ONE, and for the reviews of our paper.

We have revised the paper accordingly and we believe that the paper has been improved. In line with the editor and reviewers’ suggestions, we have edited the motivation and conclusions of the study to locate it in a broader context of gender differences in risk aversion and expectations, and of lessons from social and behavioural science for pandemic response. We have added a discussion of alternative explanations to which the reviewers hinted to, and weaknesses of our study. We have edited the entire draft to improve the clarity of presentation and readability, and elaborated the implications of our findings. We have also replaced Figure2, which was found confusing by one of the reviewers, by a table that conveys more information.

We hope that you would find the revised draft worth publishing in PLOS ONE.

Response to Reviewer 1:

Thank you for your comments and suggestions which we have accounted for in the revised draft. We have elaborated the implications of our findings in the “conclusions” section. We have also reorganized the manuscript and edited the presentation of our results and findings in order to facilitate readability. Below we respond to each of your main comments.

• Lines 50-56: As this paragraph involves discussion of the results, it seems it should be moved out of the introduction of the manuscript

We would prefer to retain this paragraph as it helps us to show our contribution.

• Lines 124-125: Please provide further detail for readers on the Shapley decomposition method

We have explained the method in more detail.

• Lines 160-163; 165-168: Please clarify how the percentage of explained variance is obtained or calculated. Can this be discerned from Figure 2? In the figure 2 caption, please provide greater detail and clarify what the x-axis represents.

We have replaced figure 2 with a table (Table 3) that includes the contributions of particular factors to overall variance, as well as their shares in total explained variance which were discussed in the text but not shown explicitly in figure 2. In the current draft, all data cited in the text are shown in Table 3.

• Discussion: The last two paragraphs in the discussion section seem like they should be swapped. Ending the manuscript by discussing the limitations minimizes the impact of the work so I would suggest addressing limitations and then finishing with the concluding paragraph.

We have swapped these paragraphs and elaborated the (now final) paragraph on broader implications of our findings.

• Line 51: “Higher-ranked”

• Line 85: Word missing? “…study (to) the…”

• Line 103 and throughout: I believe text concerning the methodology should be in past tense (i.e. “…we estimated logistic…)

• Line 133: Please write out what “pp” indicates before using the abbreviation

• Lines 170-172: Seems like a word or two is missing in this sentence as it is unclear what is being discussed (“…while the contribution of country-level preferences…”)

We have edited the text accordingly.

• Line 123: Please briefly clarify for readers why probit and linear probability models were estimated in addition to a logit model.

We have added an explanation: we do it in order to test whether our findings are robust to changes in estimation method. We find that our results do not depend on choice of the estimation method.

• Lines 186-187: Please discuss further the result that 15% of the withdrawal probability explained can be attributed to gender. This seems low, especially given the disparity in withdrawals between males and females. What could explain the other 85%? Is the low percentage explained a limitation and does it hinder the ability to draw strong conclusions about gender differences from these data?

We have elaborated this discussion. Overall, our models explain 20-30% of the overall variance in the total sample and various subsamples. We think it’s a decent share considering that the decision about participation may be affected by individual factors and past experiences, but the data allows to control only for sex, age, and ranking, and country-level variables (cultural preferences, level of development).

• Do females typically have longer professional careers than males? If so, they may feel they are better able to accommodate skipping a tournament. Just a thought.

In general, women reach their highest career level earlier than men, which is consistent with their more precocious biological development. Indeed, female players in our sample are on average younger than male players by about a year. However, the average career length of female players is not significantly different than that of male players (Guillaume et al., 2011). It is therefore unlikely that the differences in career life cycle can explain gender differences in the willingness to perform during the pandemic. We have added the discussion of this potential explanation to the “conclusions and discussion” section.

References:

Guillaume M, et al. Success and decline: top 10 tennis players follow a biphasic course. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011 Nov 43(11):2148–54.

Response to Reviewer 2:

Thank you for your comments and suggestions which we have accounted for in the revised draft. Below we respond to each of your main comments.

1. I would like to see a larger discussion about the weaknesses of the study, most centrally about the statistical inference given the small sample size. I know that this is mentioned briefly in the last paragraph, however the authors should think carefully about if there is evidence that would support these findings given a larger dataset. Additionally, if there is a larger sample which could be used as a robustness check, this would greatly strengthen the analysis.

We are aware that expanding our dataset with other tournaments would improve the analysis. However, the US Open was a unique natural experiment and there is no larger sample that can be used as a robustness check. The number of players eligible to participate was constrained. We are not aware of any other main, international sport competitions with identical rules and prize money for men and women and held in a country severely affected by the pandemic, such as the US. Therefore, it is not possible to find a larger data set that would support our hypothesis that the gender differences in the perception of COVID-19 were crucial in deciding whether to participate in the tournament.

We have expanded the relevant discussion in the conclusions section. We think that the differences in the number of COVID cases among top 100 male (10) and female (0) players is a suggestive evidence of the gender differences in risk aversion which we identify in our study. Also, studies of top professional tennis players tend to be based on relatively small samples (200-250 individuals, Guillaume et al., 2011), because of the nature of the sport.

2. The introduction and beginning of the paper should include a larger discussion about changes in risk taking throughout the COVID pandemic. A number of papers now examine how preferences and beliefs over risk change due to exposure to the COVID pandemic (e.g., Bu et al 2020). I think that this study needs to link to that larger literature and perhaps a brief discussion more fundamentally if the authors observations are driven by changing preferences/beliefs about risk due to exposure, or if their effects are more driven by innate gender differences towards risk.

We added a disclaimer that the decision about participation may be affected by individual factors and, most of all, personal experience, as noted by Bu et al (2020). However, our data does not allow controlling for personal exposure to the virus or any changes in preferences over time. Moreover, because the tournament that constitutes a natural experiment in our study was the first major tournament after the season was paused due to COVID-19 concerns, we cannot assess if the risk preferences of players have changed over the course of the pandemic. However, as our findings are consistent with previous evidence of higher risk aversion among women, we tend to interpret the effects we find as driven by innated gender differences towards risk, in particular to a specific type of risk related to pandemic exposure. Finally, because no outbreak occurred, we think that this experience is unlikely to have a long-term impact on risk aversion – because such effects are found for individuals affected by negative shocks and experiences (e.g. Malmendier and Nagel, 2011).

3. The authors may also consider gender differences in expectations and beliefs in other domains, and how this may motivate their study, for example, D’Acunto et al (2019).

Thank you for suggesting these studies. In the introduction, we expanded the discussion of research on gender differences in risk taking in various settings and contexts. We have also added more references, including the study by Bu et al. (2020), in the discussion of our results, which have expanded to discuss broader implications of our study.

References

Bu, Di and Hanspal, Tobin and Liao, Yin and Liu, Yong. 2020. Risk Taking, Preferences, and Beliefs: Evidence from Wuhan. Working Paper.

Guillaume M, et al. Success and decline: top 10 tennis players follow a biphasic course. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2011 Nov 43(11):2148–54.

Malmendier U, Nagel S. Depression Babies: Do Macroeconomic Experiences Affect Risk Taking? The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 2011 Feb 1;126(1):373–416.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewer 1.docx

Decision Letter 1

Valerio Capraro

10 Mar 2021

The gender gap in aversion to COVID-19 exposure: evidence from professional tennis

PONE-D-20-38751R1

Dear Dr. Lewandowski,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors did a nice job incorporating suggestions that has lead to a greatly improved manuscript in terms of the implications and context of the results and overall readability. While I still feel like the introduction paragraph that discusses results (lines 55-61) belongs later in the manuscript, I can appreciate the authors rationale for including earlier and do not feel like it detracts from the paper. Thus, I am recommending acceptance of this manuscript.

Reviewer #2: I think the authors did an excellent job incorporating the feedback from the reviewers into the revised manuscript.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Valerio Capraro

12 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-38751R1

The gender gap in aversion to COVID-19 exposure: evidence from professional tennis

Dear Dr. Lewandowski:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Valerio Capraro

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Players who withdrew from the 2020 US Open because of COVID-19.

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Players who withdrew from the 2020 US Open for other reasons.

    (DOCX)

    S1 File

    (DTA)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewer 1.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES