Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Mar 25;16(3):e0248919. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248919

Are adversities and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic related to sleep quality? Longitudinal analyses of 46,000 UK adults

Liam Wright 1,2,*, Andrew Steptoe 1, Daisy Fancourt 1
Editor: Kristin Vickers3
PMCID: PMC7993810  PMID: 33765097

Abstract

Background

There are concerns that both the experience of adversities during the COVID-19 pandemic and worries about experiencing adversities will have substantial and lasting effects on mental health. One pathway through which both experience of and worries about adversity may impact health is through effects on sleep.

Methods

We used data from 46,284 UK adults in the COVID-19 Social Study assessed weekly from 01/04/2020-12/05/2020 to study the association between adversities and sleep quality. We studied six categories of adversity including both worries and experiences of: illness with COVID-19, financial difficulty, loss of paid work, difficulties acquiring medication, difficulties accessing food, and threats to personal safety. We used random-effect within-between models to account for all time-invariant confounders.

Results

Both the total number of adversity experiences and total number of adversity worries were associated with lower quality sleep. Each additional experience was associated with a 1.16 (95% CI = 1.10, 1.22) times higher odds of poor quality sleep while each additional worry was associated with a 1.20 (95% CI = 1.17, 1.22) times higher odds of poor quality sleep. When considering specific experiences and worries, all worries and experiences were significantly related to poorer quality sleep except experiences relating to employment and finances. Having a larger social network offered some buffering effects on associations but there was limited further evidence of moderation by other social or psychiatric factors.

Conclusion

Poor sleep may be a mechanism by which COVID-19 adversities are affecting mental health. This highlights the importance of interventions that support adaptive coping strategies during the pandemic.

Introduction

The pandemic of coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) has disrupted lives across the globe. There have been sharp increases in the experience of adversities, both arising from the virus itself (i.e. infection, illness, and possibly death from the disease) and resulting from efforts to contain the disease, such as financial shocks following the loss of employment and income, challenges in accessing food, medication or accommodation, and adverse domestic experiences such as abuse [17]. Adversities have followed previous epidemics [815], but the scale and length of the COVID-19 pandemic are causing concern that we face manifold public health crises in the years to come [2, 16, 17].

More specifically, there are concerns that adversities experienced during the pandemic will have persisting impacts on physical and mental health [17, 18]. Studies suggest that intimate partner violence [19] and socio-economic adversities such as poverty [20], job loss [21], economic recession [22, 23], and job insecurity [24], can have lasting impacts on mortality and physical and mental health outcomes. Moreover, it is not just the experience of these stressors, but also worries about the potential experience of these stressors, that can affect health, increasing levels of stress and impacting depression and wellbeing [25, 26], as well as affecting physical outcomes such as cardiovascular health [27]. Specifically during COVID-19, we have shown in recent work that there is a relationship between worries and experiences of adversities and poorer mental health [28]. But it is important to understand the biobehavioural pathways through which this relationship exists.

One pathway through which both experience of and worries about adversity may impact health is through effects on sleep [29]. Both worries about adversities and experience of adversities are types of stressors [30]. The stress of experiencing adversities has been shown to impair sleep [3133], while the stress of worrying about life events has been associated with shorter sleep length and greater sleep disturbance [34, 35]. Numerous biological studies have focused on the pathways underlying these effects, including disruption of HPA axis activity, increased cortisol production, and bidirectional changes between hormonal variation and circadian rhythm [36, 37]. Inadequate sleep may also reinforce the impact of stressors by reducing individual’s ability to respond effectively, leading to a maladaptive psychophysiological cycle [3841]. Impaired sleep is in turn related to worsened physical health outcomes, such as cardiovascular disease, weight gain, and mortality [42, 43], and poorer mental health outcomes, such as anxiety and depression [44]. It is therefore essential to understand whether experience of worries about adversities during the COVID-19 are leading to sleep problems.

While adversity may be related to poorer sleep quality on average, there are several factors that could protect against such effects. First, social support may buffer against stress through the provision of informational or tangible assistance or emotional support [45]. A large body of literature shows that social support is associated with better sleep [46] and with improved physical and mental health outcomes, including lower mortality rates [47]. Further, improved sleep has been identified as a pathway through which social support may affect health [48]. However, decreased face-to-face contact and the increasing prevalence of adversity throughout populations may have reduced the availability and quality of social support during the pandemic [7]. Further, the novel nature of several adversities faced may have reduced the efficacy of informational or tangible assistance aspects of social support. Therefore, an unresolved question is whether social support buffers the association between adversity and sleep quality during lockdown.

A second factor that may be important for the link between adversity and sleep is existing mental health. Studies show that individuals with pre-existing mental health issues may be disproportionately affected psychologically by stressful events. For example, anxiety and depression can predispose individuals (especially men) to greater stress reactivity [49], while anxiety sensitivity can moderate the relationship between exposure to traumatic events and post-traumatic stress [50]. Further, in previous studies of epidemics, there has been some indication that pre-existing psychiatric conditions are a risk factor for poorer psychological outcomes [8]. However, when considering the link between psychological experiences and sleep, it is possible that individuals with existing mental health conditions may already have poorer sleep, leading to a ceiling effect, such that adversity does not have any further material detrimental effect on sleep [38, 51, 52].

To explore these issues further, the present study used data from a large, longitudinal study of the experiences of adults during the early weeks of the lockdown due to COVID-19 in the UK to explore the time-varying longitudinal relationship between (i) worries about adversity, and (ii) experience of adversity and quality of sleep. Further, it sought to ascertain whether the relationship between adversity and sleep quality was moderated by social support and existing mental health diagnoses.

Materials and methods

Participants

We use data from the COVID-19 Social Study; a large panel study of the psychological and social experiences of over 50,000 adults (aged 18+) in the UK during the COVID-19 pandemic. The study commenced on 21 March 2020 and involves online weekly data collection from participants for the duration of the pandemic in the UK. Recruitment into the study is ongoing. The study is not random but does contain a heterogeneous sample. Participants were recruited using three primary approaches. First, snowballing was used, including promoting the study through existing networks and mailing lists (including large databases of adults who had previously consented to be involved in health research across the UK), print and digital media coverage, and social media. Second, more targeted recruitment was undertaken focusing on (i) individuals from a low-income background, (ii) individuals with no or few educational qualifications, and (iii) individuals who were unemployed. Third, the study was promoted via partnerships with third sector organisations to vulnerable groups, including adults with pre-existing mental health conditions, older adults, carers, and people experiencing domestic violence or abuse. The study was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee [12467/005] and all participants gave informed consent. The study protocol and user guide (which includes full details on recruitment, retention, data cleaning, weighting and sample demographics) are available at www.covidsocialstudy.org.

Our questions asked about experiences of adversity in the last week, so we focused on data from 1st April 2020 (one week after lockdown commenced) to 12th May 2020, limiting our analysis to participants with two or more waves of data during this period (n = 48,723, observations = 208,057; 80.2% of individuals surveyed between 1 April– 12 May). We used complete case data and excluded participants with complete data in fewer than two data collections (n = 2,439; 5% of eligible participants). This provided a final analytical sample of 46,284 participants (197,372 observations). Note, the sample overlaps that used in our previous work looking at adversities and mental health [28]. Differences in the sample are limited to the small proportion of participants who have missing data on the variables used in this or the previous study.

Measures

Adversities

We study six categories of adversity, each measured weekly (see Table 1). We constructed weekly total adversity worries and total adversity experiences measures by summing the number of adversities present in a given week (range 0–6). For adversity experiences that are likely to be continuing (i.e. once experienced in one week, their effects would likely last into future weeks), we counted them on subsequent waves after they had first occurred. This applied to experiencing suspected/diagnosed COVID-19, loss of paid work, major cut in household income, and abuse victimisation. We considered worries to be one-off events and counted them only in the weeks they were reported.

Table 1. Questions on adversities.
Type of adversity Adversity worries Adversity experiences
COVID-19 illness Worried about catching COVID-19 Currently have or previously had suspected or diagnosed COVID-19
Financial difficulty Worried about finances Experienced a major cut in household income
Loss of paid work Worried about losing your job/unemployment Lost one’s job or been unable to do paid work
Difficulties accessing food Worried about getting food Unable to access sufficient food
Difficulties acquiring medication Worried about getting medication Unable to access required medication
Threats to personal safety Worried about personal safety/security Experienced being physically harmed or hurt by somebody else or being bullied, controlled, intimidate or psychologically hurt by someone else

Sleep

Sleep quality was elicited using a single item on sleep: “Over the past week, how has your sleep been?” The item had five response categories: very good, good, average, not good, very poor. Similar single item sleep scales have been shown to be highly correlated with responses to lengthier sleep questionnaires and are widely used in research [53, 54]. To distinguish between minor variations in individual reporting and focus instead on levels of poor sleep quality that are likely to have larger consequences for health, we dichotomised this into a binary variable for not good or poor vs average or better sleep.

Social support

We measured social support at first data collection using four separate variables for loneliness, perceived social support, social network size, and living alone. Loneliness was measured using the 3-item UCLA-3 loneliness, a short form of the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (UCLA-R). Each item is rated with a 3-point rating scale, ranging from “never” to “often”, with higher scores indicating greater loneliness. We used the sum score measure (range 3–9).

Perceived social support was measured using an adapted version of the six-item short form of Perceived Social Support Questionnaire (F-SozU K-6). Each item is rated on a 5-point scale from “not true at all” to “very true”, with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived social support. We used the sum score measure (range 6–30). Minor adaptations were made to the language in the scale to make it relevant to experiences during COVID-19 (see S1 Table for a comparison of changes). Social network size was measured as number of close friends, with numbers capped at 10+. We included this as a continuous variable.

Psychiatric illness

We defined psychiatric illness as reporting a clinically diagnosed mental health problem (“clinically-diagnosed depression”, “clinically-diagnosed anxiety”, or “another clinically-diagnosed mental health problem”) at first data collection.

Analysis

We used random-effect within-between (REWB) logit models [55] (also known as hybrid models [56]) to explore the association between within-person change in adversity experiences and adversity worries and the likelihood of poor quality sleep. Our basic model can be expressed as follows:

P(BadSleepit=1)=logit1(β0t+β1Eikt+β2E¯ik+β3Wikt+β4W¯ik+βLXt+αi+εit)

where Bad Sleepit is an indicator for whether individual i reported bad quality sleep at time t. E¯ik is the person-specific mean level of adversity experience k across time periods for individual i, while W¯ik is the corresponding figure for adversity worries. Eikt and Wikt are the deviations from the person-specific mean values of adversity experiences k and adversity worries k for individual i at time t. Xt is a vector of control variables defined below. αi is the random intercept for individual i, which we model as distributed ~N(0,σα2). εit is the observation-specific residual error (~N(0,σε2)). We ran the models once with “adversity experiences” and “adversity worries” entered separately into the models, so as to ascertain if there was any initial association with sleep, and then re-ran the models with both sets of factors together, to see if results were maintained when mutually adjusting for one another.

Our interest was the sign and size of the coefficients β1 and β3, which represent the association between within-person change in adversity experiences and adversity worries and the likelihood of poor sleep. We focused on within-person change rather than cross-sectional variation as cross-sectional associations are likely to be confounded by factors such as socio-economic class or personality, which are related to the prevalence of adversity and to sleep. When looking at within-person changes, these characteristics should be fixed, and so associations should not be biased due the influence of these omitted variables on sleep. In fact, in non-linear models such as the logistic model, the coefficients β1 and β3 are unbiased by time-invariant heterogeneity if the random intercept, αi, is a linear function of the level-2 (between-person) predictors. However, simulations have shown that the extent of bias due to violations of this assumption are limited in practice [55]. Nevertheless, results can still be biased if exposure to new adversities or worries is related to other unobserved changes occurring for the individual.

We estimated several models. In Model 1, we regressed sleep quality on the total number of adversity experiences and total number of adversity worries, both (a) separately and (b) jointly, using the REWB estimator to account for time-invariant heterogeneity across participants. (Variance Inflation Factors suggest multicollinearity was not a problem in this model.) In Model 2, we regressed sleep quality on adversity experiences and adversity worries separately for each category of adversity in turn (finances, personal safety, etc.). In Model 3, we repeated Model 1a including interactions between adversity measures and each social support variable, for each social support variable in turn. In Model 4, we repeated Model 1a including interactions between adversity measures and mental health diagnosis. We adjusted for day of week (categorical) and days since lockdown commenced (continuous) in each regression (person-specific means and deviations from these means). To account for the non-random nature of the sample, all data were weighted to the proportions of gender, age, ethnicity, education and country of living obtained from the Office for National Statistics [57].

We carried out several sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our results. First, we re-estimated Model 3 using inability to pay bills, rather than major cut in household income, as our measure of experienced financial adversity to differentiate between a change in wealth and a change in wealth that impacts on core financial activity. Second, we repeated each analysis using the sleep item as a continuous variable to test whether results were robust to variable measurement. For these regressions, we used the linear fixed effects estimator which controls for time-invariant confounding by design. Third, we repeated regressions using both the linear probability fixed effect estimator and the fixed effects logit estimators. We did not use the fixed effects logit estimator in the main analysis as the estimator uses information from those whose sleep quality changes only, which may exclude those whose sleep is least responsive to adversity. Fourth, we repeated our main REWB model for the subset of individuals whose sleep quality changed and compared results against those from the fixed effect logit estimator to assess the possibility of confounding due to time invariant heterogeneity in our main analysis. Analyses were carried out in Stata version 16.0 [58] and R version 3.6.3 [59].

Due to conditions set out by the ethics committee, the data will be made available at the end of 2021. Replication code is available at https://osf.io/4epqm/.

Results

Demographics

Descriptive statistics for the exposures and outcomes are shown in Table 2. There was within-variation in each of the measures suggesting REWB was a valid approach. S2 and S3 Tables in the supplementary material displays descriptive statistics for baseline demographic, social support, and mental health diagnosis variables. The weighted sample was 51.3% female, 9.6% from Black and minority ethnic backgrounds, and 19.0% of the sample were aged 18–34, 24.8% aged 35–49, 31.4% aged 50–64 and 24.8% aged 65 and above. Individuals with diagnosed mental illness or with lower social support had worse sleep, on average. S4 Table in the supplementary information displays the sample size by week of data collection. Attrition was less than 10% each week. The average number of follow-ups was 4.26.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics.

Weighted figures. Between SD is the standard deviation in participants’ average responses. Within SD is the standard deviations in an individual’s report, averaged across participants.

Variable Overall Mean Overall SD Between SD Within SD
Sleep quality (range 1–5) 3.12 1.08 0.95 0.51
Bad Sleep (binary) 0.29 0.45 0.37 0.26
Experiences Total number of adversity experiences (range 0–6) 0.59 0.84 0.79 0.28
Lost work (binary) 0.10 0.30 0.29 0.08
Cut in income (binary) 0.19 0.39 0.37 0.12
Unable to access sufficient food (binary) 0.04 0.20 0.15 0.13
Unable to access required medication (binary) 0.03 0.16 0.12 0.11
Suspected or diagnosed COVID-19 (binary) 0.13 0.34 0.33 0.08
Physically or psychologically harmed (binary) 0.09 0.29 0.27 0.11
Worries Total number of adversity worries (range 0–6) 1.30 1.32 1.15 0.66
Losing job/unemployment (binary) 0.13 0.34 0.28 0.18
Finances (binary) 0.31 0.46 0.39 0.24
Getting food (binary) 0.20 0.40 0.30 0.26
Getting medication (binary) 0.12 0.32 0.25 0.20
Catching COVID-19 (binary) 0.42 0.49 0.40 0.29
Personal safety (binary) 0.13 0.34 0.26 0.22

Participants reported worry about catching COVID-19 in only 42% of observations overall (S2 Table). We explored the characteristics of those who reported worry about catching COVID-19 at any point during follow-up using bivariate and multivariate logistic models (S1 Fig). When adjusting for all factors simultaneously, older participants, females, individuals with diagnosed physical or mental health conditions, individuals higher in neuroticism, and participants from Northern Ireland were more likely to report worry about catching COVID-19. Conversely, single participants and those with university degrees and high incomes were less likely to report worry about catching COVID-19.

Associations between adversities and sleep

Both the total number of adversity experiences and total number of adversity worries were associated with lower quality sleep (Fig 1). The inclusion of experiences and worries in the same model slightly reduced the effect size of experiences and had little effect on the effect size of worries. In models including both experiences and worries, each additional experience was associated with a 1.16 (95% CI = 1.10, 1.22) times higher odds of poor quality sleep while each additional worry was associated with a 1.20 (95% CI = 1.17, 1.22) times higher odds of poor quality sleep.

Fig 1. Associations (with 95% confidence intervals) between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and odds of bad quality sleep.

Fig 1

Derived from REWB models. Note: Models either added experiences and worries separately or simultaneously (i.e. so mutually adjusted for one another). Analyses were further adjusted for day of the week and time since lockdown began.

When considering specific experiences and worries, worries were significantly related to poorer quality sleep in every category of adversity (Fig 2). There was some heterogeneity in effect sizes, with the largest effects found for worries about personal safety (OR = 1.43 [1.35, 1.53]), followed by access to medication (OR = 1.39 [1.30, 1.49]), employment (OR = 1.25 [1.16, 1.35]), access to food (OR = 1.24 [1.17, 1.32]), finances (OR = 1.19 [1.12, 1.26]), and catching COVID-19 (OR = 1.18 [1.12, 1.25]).

Fig 2. Associations (with 95% confidence intervals) between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and odds of poor sleep.

Fig 2

Derived from REWB models. Experiences and worries were entered into separate models, for each category of adversity in turn. Analyses were further adjusted for day of the week and time since lockdown began.

For experiences, the largest effects were found for access to medication (OR = 1.42 [1.25, 1.61]) and difficulty in accessing food (OR = 1.31 [1.17, 1.47]). Experiencing adversities relating to personal safety such as abuse were also related to poor quality sleep (OR = 1.29 [1.14, 1.47]), as was catching COVID-19 (OR = 1.30 [1.08, 1.54]) (although the confidence intervals were wide potentially indicating heterogeneity in responses). There was some evidence of a relationship between losing work and poor sleep (OR = 1.14 [0.95, 1.38]), but no evidence of a relationship with experiencing a cut in income (OR = 0.95 [0.84, 1.07]).

Moderators

There was little clear evidence that social support moderated the relationship between sleep quality and adversity experiences (Fig 3; see S5 Table in the supplementary information for interaction term coefficients). For adversity worries (Fig 3), there was evidence that the association between poor quality sleep and adversity worries was weaker among those with more close friends (OR = 0.97 [0.957, 0.99]). For adversity experiences (Fig 3), there was evidence that the association between poor quality sleep and adversity experiences was weaker among those who were lonelier (OR = 0.95 [0.91, 1.00]). For other measures, associations were more tentative (S3 Table).

Fig 3. Associations (with 95% confidence intervals) between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and odds of poor quality sleep according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

Fig 3

Estimates are from REWB models, with experiences and worries entered into separate models. Analyses were further adjusted for day of the week and time since lockdown began.

There was also no evidence of differences in the relationship between worries and sleep quality in people with and without a diagnosed mental illness (Fig 4). There was limited evidence of moderation by mental health for adversity experiences, with larger effects found among those with diagnosed psychiatric conditions (OR = 1.10 [0.99, 1.24]).

Fig 4. Associations (with 95% confidence intervals) between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and odds of poor quality sleep according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

Fig 4

Note: Estimates are from REWB models, with experiences and worries entered into separate models. Analyses were further adjusted for day of the week and time since lockdown began.

Sensitivity analysis

The results from sensitivity analyses are displayed in the Supplementary Information. Point estimates suggest that inability to pay bills was more highly related to poor sleep quality than reporting a major cut in household income (S2 Fig).

Results using the fixed effects linear probability estimator were qualitatively similar to those from REWB models (S3S6 Figs). An increase in adversity experiences or adversity worries was association with a ~2% point increase in the probability of poor sleep (S3 Fig). Results using the fixed effects logit estimator, which, as noted above, only uses data from those whose sleep quality changed, were also qualitatively similar to those from REWB models, but produced stronger effect sizes (S7S10 Figs). An increase in adversity experiences or adversity worries was association with a ~ 4–5% point increase in the probability of poor sleep (S7 Fig). Moderation analyses produced similar effect sizes to those from REWB models, though evidence of moderation according to loneliness was weaker (S9 and S10 Figs and S3 Table). When limiting analyses to individuals whose sleep quality changed, similar results were produced by the REWB and fixed effects logit estimators (S11 Fig), suggesting our main results are not biased due to time invariant heterogeneity.

When analysing sleep quality as a continuous measure, the main findings were qualitatively also similar, with both experiences and worries related to poorer sleep (S12S15 Figs). However, there was no clear evidence of a moderating role of social support in the association between adversities experiences or worries and sleep (S14 Fig). There was still a moderating role of mental health in the association between adversity experiences and sleep quality (S15 Fig and S3 Table).

One possible issue with our results is our use of a single item sleep quality measure. We compared a similar measure from Wave 4 of the United Kingdom Household Longitudinal Study (“During the past month, how would you rate your sleep quality overall?”: 1 = Very good– 4 = Very bad) with other self-report sleep measures collected in that survey (S16 Fig). Lower quality sleep was related to shorter sleep duration, higher wakefulness during the night, greater use of sleep medicines, and greater difficulties getting to sleep.

Discussion

In this study, we explored the relationship between worries and experience of adversities and quality of sleep during the first lockdown due to COVID-19 in the UK. Cumulative number of worries and experience of adversities were both related to lower quality sleep. When considering specific types of adversities, all types of worries explored were associated with poorer sleep quality, while only specific experiences such as abuse, inabilities to pay bills, access food or medication, and catching COVID-19 showed clear associations with poorer sleep. Effects sizes were small: additional adversity experience or worries were related to approximately a 2%-point higher likelihood of poor quality sleep, on average. Having more close friends helped to moderate the relationship between worries and sleep but there was weaker evidence that other social factors had any clear protective buffering effects.

This study supports findings from emerging research on COVID-19, which has suggested that sleep is being adversely affected amongst people during the pandemic [60]. The clear relationship between both specific and cumulative worries and poor sleep echoes findings about the adverse effects of stress on sleep from a number of previous studies [3133]. However, it is notable that only specific experiences were related to poor sleep. These related specifically to difficulties in accessing food and medication, experience of abuse, and contracting COVID-19. In particular, experience of domestic violence has previously been well-researched in relation to sleep, with studies notably suggesting that fear of future abuse and nightmares can disrupt sleep [61]. There has also been increasing research focus on the neuropsychiatric effects of coronavirus infections, with suggestions that sleep disturbance can follow from infection [62]. This could explain the findings showing a relationship between having COVID-19 and impaired sleep. Notably, we didn’t find a clear relationship between experiencing loss of work or cuts in household income and impaired sleep, although worry about these things was associated with poorer sleep. It is possible that consequences may take time to arise. For instance, loss of paid work or cuts in income may impact sleep only following repeated rejections during job search or when reduced incomes begin to impact living standards [63, 64]. Financial adversities may also have been anticipated such that effects were felt in anticipation of the financial adversities, and high strain work may itself have adversely impacted sleep [33]. The effect of job loss on stress may also have been counterbalanced by increased leisure time [65].

Our results also found only limited evidence of buffering of these associations by social factors, in perhaps contradictory direction. Having more close friends appeared to buffer the association between adversity worries and sleep. This is notable given that social contact with friends was not permitted during the period follow due to lockdowns. It could suggest that having a circle of friends provides reassurance even if their support is not explicitly drawn on. But it is notable that other similar factors, such as social support, did not act as moderators. Cross-sectional studies during the pandemic have founds associations between social support and better sleep quality [66, 67]. However, these studies did not look at the relationship between social support and adversity worries or experiences, suggesting that while social support may be linked with sleep, it may not go so far as to moderate the relationship between sleep and other factors. There has been some previous research finding that social support can moderate the relationship between occupational stress and sleep outside of pandemic settings [68]. But social support at work (available on a daily basis alongside one’s occupational stressors) may play a more meaningful role than during a pandemic when one may know one has social support available if needed but may not be necessarily drawing on that support, thereby rendering it a hypothetical rather than realised type of social support.

There was also some evidence that loneliness buffered the association with adversity experiences. However, this was non-robust when testing it in sensitivity analyses, so remains to be explored further. For other social factors there was only limited evidence of any moderating effect. It is possible that decreased social interaction or limited face-to-face contact with social networks may have reduced any protective effects [7].

Further, it is interesting that there was only limited evidence of moderation by mental illness. Anxiety and depression can predispose individuals to greater stress reactivity [49], and our results suggested there could be slightly larger effects amongst those diagnosed psychiatric conditions. But results were not clear, and both those with and without psychiatric conditions are at risk of poor sleep as a result of adversities. This echoes other research showing how adversities and stresses affect not just those at high risk but also the broader population [7].

This study has a number of strengths. We used data from a large, heterogeneous sample, which was weighted to UK population proportions according to major socio-demographic characteristics. The data covered six weeks of lockdown in the UK, providing rich longitudinal data that allowed us to estimate the relationship between adversity and change in sleep within individuals, rather than rely on cross-sectional variation, which would likely be confounded by time-invariant heterogeneity across individuals.

The study has several limitations, however. While we were about to account for confounding through time-invariant heterogeneity, time-varying factors that remain unobserved may explain results. Further, whilst is appears logical that poor sleep itself cannot cause adverse experiences, there is likely a bidirectional relationship between worries and poor sleep, and worries may pre-date experiences. Nevertheless, our analyses suggest that both worries and experiences are independently associated with poor sleep. Another limitation was our use of a single item five-category self-report measure of sleep quality. This did not provide detail on which aspect of sleep was most affected (e.g. duration, onset, interruption etc) and may have lacked sufficient variation and validity to accurately estimate effects. Indeed, self-reports of sleep have been found to be worse in psychiatric patients, which may also have biased responses [69]. It is possible that individuals experiencing worries or adversities may have perceived their sleep to be worse, but without substantial variation in the core qualitative parameters of sleep. However, single item sleep scales have been shown to be highly correlated with responses to lengthier sleep questionnaires and are widely used in research [53]. Further, we also show that a similar item collected in a representative UK household panel survey is related to aspects of sleep such as sleep duration and use of sleep medicines.

The sampling in the COVID-19 Social Study was not random. While we deliberately sampled from groups such as individuals of low socio-economic position and individuals with existing mental illness, individuals with particularly extreme experiences may not have been adequately captured in the study. Further, individuals experiencing extreme adversity during the lockdown may have been more likely to withdraw from the study. While our statistical approach allowed for an unbalanced panel, non-random attrition could have biased results. Social support was measured at first data collection, which for many was after lockdown began. Responses to these questions could have been affected by adversities experienced already. We also focused on just six types of adversities, but other types of adversity were not included in the study, notably those relating to interpersonal relationships and bereavement. Finally, our study only followed individuals up over a period of weeks. It remains for future studies to assess how experience of adversities during the COVID-19 pandemic relates to sleep–and to health–in the long-term.

Previous studies have shown that experience and worries about adversities during COVID-19 are associated with poorer mental health. The results presented here suggest that poor sleep may be a mechanism by which such adversities are affecting mental health. Worries about adversities were related to poorer quality sleep over time, as was cumulative load of adverse experiences was also associated with poorer quality sleep. But only specific adversities such as those relating to personal safety, catching COVID-19, or challenges in accessing food and medication showed clear associations with poor sleep on their own. These results were relatively consistent amongst those with and without a diagnosed mental illness. Having a larger social network had some protective effects, but other social factors had more limited moderating effects on the relationship. These results may be generalisable to non-pandemic settings, showing how two different types of stressors (experiences and worries) are similarly related to sleep. Further, many of the measures of stressors we focused on (including both the worries about and experiences of adversities) can be experienced in daily life. However, the results also have an immediate relevance to supporting individuals during the current pandemic. They suggest the importance of interventions that seek to reassure individuals and enable adaptive coping strategies. Given the challenges in providing face-to-face mental health support to individuals during lockdowns due to COVID-19, these findings highlight the importance of developing online and remote interventions that could provide such support, both as COVID-19 continues and in preparation for future pandemics.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Comparison of items in the original and revised perceived social support questionnaire (F-SozU K-6).

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Descriptive statistics, social support and psychiatric diagnosis.

Weighted figures.

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Sample descriptive statistics.

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Sample size by week.

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Estimated interaction effects between social support and diagnosed psychiatric illness and deviation in total adversity experiences and adversity worries.

(DOCX)

S1 Fig. Association between demographic characteristics and reporting worry about catching COVID-19 during any data collection.

Derived from bivariate and multivariate survey weighted logistic regression models. Multivariate models include adjustment for each factor simultaneously. Big-Five personality traits are scaled (mean = 0, SD = 1).

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Associations between (i) cut in income and (ii) inability to pay bills and odds of bad quality sleep.

Derived from REWB models.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of bad quality sleep.

Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and probability of poor sleep.

Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S5 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S6 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S7 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of bad quality sleep.

Derived from fixed effects logit models.

(TIF)

S8 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and probability of poor sleep.

Derived from fixed effects logit models.

(TIF)

S9 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

Derived from fixed effects logit models.

(TIF)

S10 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

Derived from fixed effects logit models.

(TIF)

S11 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of all/specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about all/specific types of adversities and probability of poor sleep.

Derived from fixed effects logit models and REWB models, where sample is those whose sleep quality changed during follow-up period.

(TIF)

S12 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and (continuous) sleep quality.

Derived from fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S13 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and (continuous) sleep quality.

Derived from fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S14 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and (continuous) sleep quality according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

Derived from fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S15 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and (continuous) quality sleep quality according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

Derived from fixed effects models.

(TIF)

S16 Fig. Convergent validity of a single item sleep quality measure from Wave 4 of the United Kingdom household longitudinal study with other measures of sleep from the same survey.

Sleep quality: “During the past month, how would you rate your sleep quality overall?”. Sleep hours: “How many hours of actual sleep did you usually get per night during the last month?”. 30+ minutes to sleep: “During the past month, how often have you had trouble sleeping because you… cannot get to sleep within 30 minutes?”. Waking frequency: “(During the past month, how often have you had trouble sleeping because you…) wake up in the middle of the night or early in the morning?”. Sleep medicine: “During the past month, how often have you taken medicine (prescribed or "over the counter") to help you sleep?”. The correlation between sleep quality and sleep time is 0.51.

(TIF)

S1 File. STROBE checklist for observational studies.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The researchers are grateful for the support of a number of organisations with their recruitment efforts including: the UKRI Mental Health Networks, Find Out Now, UCL BioResource, SEO Works, FieldworkHub, and Optimal Workshop.

Patient and public involvement

The research questions in the UCL COVID-19 Social Study built on patient and public involvement as part of the UKRI MARCH Mental Health Research Network, which focuses on social, cultural and community engagement and mental health. This highlighted priority research questions and measures for this study. Patients and the public were additionally involved in the recruitment of participants to the study and are actively involved in plans for the dissemination of findings from the study.

Data Availability

The code to produce each of the analyses in this paper is available at https://osf.io/4epqm/. A link to the other data files will be added on that website when they become available.

Funding Statement

The Covid-19 Social Study was funded by the Nuffield Foundation [WEL/FR-000022583], but the views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily the Foundation (https://www.nuffieldfoundation.org/). The study was also supported by the MARCH Mental Health Network (https://www.marchnetwork.org/) funded by the Cross-Disciplinary Mental Health Network Plus initiative supported by UK Research and Innovation [ES/S002588/1] (https://www.ukri.org/), and by the Wellcome Trust [221400/Z/20/Z] (https://wellcome.org/). DF was funded by the Wellcome Trust [205407/Z/16/Z]. LW is funded by the Economic and Social Research Council (https://esrc.ukri.org/) through the UCL, Bloomsbury and East London Doctoral Training Partnership [ES/P000592/1] (https://ubel-dtp.ac.uk/). The study was also supported by HealthWise Wales, the Health and Car Research Wales initiative, which is led by Cardiff University in collaboration with SAIL, Swansea University. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. All researchers listed as authors are independent from the funders and all final decisions about the research were taken by the investigators and were unrestricted.

References

  • 1.van Dorn A, Cooney RE, Sabin ML. COVID-19 exacerbating inequalities in the US. The Lancet 2020;395:1243–4. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30893-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Nassif-Pires L, Xavier L de L, Masterson T, et al. Pandemic of Inequality. Levy Economics Institute 2020. https://ideas.repec.org/p/lev/levppb/ppb_149.html (accessed 22 Apr 2020).
  • 3.Chung RY-N, Dong D, Li MM. Socioeconomic gradient in health and the covid-19 outbreak. BMJ 2020;369. 10.1136/bmj.m1329 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Lancet T. Redefining vulnerability in the era of COVID-19. The Lancet 2020;395:1089. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30757-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Usher K, Bhullar N, Durkin J, et al. Family violence and COVID-19: Increased vulnerability and reduced options for support. International Journal of Mental Health Nursing; n/a. 10.1111/inm.12735 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.COVID-19 and violence against women: What the health sector/system can do. World Health Organisation 2020. http://www.who.int/reproductivehealth/publications/vaw-covid-19/en/ (accessed 29 Apr 2020).
  • 7.Wright L, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Are we all in this together? Longitudinal assessment of cumulative adversities by socio-economic position in the first 3 weeks of lockdown in the UK. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Jeong H, Yim HW, Song Y-J, et al. Mental health status of people isolated due to Middle East Respiratory Syndrome. Epidemiol Health 2016;38. 10.4178/epih.e2016048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pellecchia U, Crestani R, Decroo T, et al. Social Consequences of Ebola Containment Measures in Liberia. PLoS One 2015;10. 10.1371/journal.pone.0143036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mihashi M, Otsubo Y, Yinjuan X, et al. Predictive factors of psychological disorder development during recovery following SARS outbreak. Health Psychology 2009;28:91–100. 10.1037/a0013674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Taylor MR, Agho KE, Stevens GJ, et al. Factors influencing psychological distress during a disease epidemic: Data from Australia’s first outbreak of equine influenza. BMC Public Health 2008;8:347. 10.1186/1471-2458-8-347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Desclaux A, Badji D, Ndione AG, et al. Accepted monitoring or endured quarantine? Ebola contacts’ perceptions in Senegal. Social Science & Medicine 2017;178:38–45. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.02.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hawryluck L, Gold WL, Robinson S, et al. SARS Control and Psychological Effects of Quarantine, Toronto, Canada. Emerg Infect Dis 2004;10:1206–12. 10.3201/eid1007.030703 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Blendon RJ, Benson JM, DesRoches CM, et al. The Public’s Response to Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome in Toronto and the United States. Clin Infect Dis 2004;38:925–31. 10.1086/382355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Wilken JA, Pordell P, Goode B, et al. Knowledge, Attitudes, and Practices among Members of Households Actively Monitored or Quarantined to Prevent Transmission of Ebola Virus Disease—Margibi County, Liberia: February-March 2015. Prehospital and Disaster Medicine 2017;32:673–8. 10.1017/S1049023X17006720 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Anderson RM, Heesterbeek H, Klinkenberg D, et al. How will country-based mitigation measures influence the course of the COVID-19 epidemic? The Lancet 2020;395:931–4. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30567-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.McKee M, Stuckler D. If the world fails to protect the economy, COVID-19 will damage health not just now but also in the future. Nat Med 2020;26:640–2. 10.1038/s41591-020-0863-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Holmes EA, O’Connor RC, Perry VH, et al. Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science. The Lancet Psychiatry 2020;:S2215036620301681. 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ellsberg M, Jansen HA, Heise L, et al. Intimate partner violence and women’s physical and mental health in the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic violence: an observational study. The Lancet 2008;371:1165–72. 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60522-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Clark AE, D’Ambrosio C, Ghislandi S. Poverty Profiles and Well-Being: Panel Evidence from Germany. In: Garner TI, Short KS, eds. Research on Economic Inequality. Emerald Group Publishing Limited 2015. 1–22.
  • 21.Sullivan D, von Wachter T. Job Displacement and Mortality: An Analysis Using Administrative Data *. Quarterly Journal of Economics 2009;124:1265–306. 10.1162/qjec.2009.124.3.1265 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Cutler DM, Huang W, Lleras-Muney A. Economic Conditions and Mortality: Evidence from 200 Years of Data. Cambridge, MA: National Bureau of Economic Research; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cutler DM, Huang W, Lleras-Muney A. When does education matter? The protective effect of education for cohorts graduating in bad times. Social Science and Medicine 2015;127:63–73. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.07.056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Barrech A, Baumert J, Emeny RT, et al. Mid-life job insecurity associated with subjective well-being in old age: Results from the population-based MONICA/KORA study. Scandinavian Journal of Work, Environment and Health, Supplement 2011;37:170–4. 10.5271/sjweh.3546 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Szabó M. The emotional experience associated with worrying: anxiety, depression, or stress? Anxiety, Stress, & Coping 2011;24:91–105. 10.1080/10615801003653430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rief W, Glaesmer H, Baehr V, et al. The relationship of modern health worries to depression, symptom reporting and quality of life in a general population survey. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 2012;72:318–20. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Kubzansky Laura D., Kawachi Ichiro, Spiro Avron, et al. Is Worrying Bad for Your Heart? Circulation 1997;95:818–24. 10.1161/01.cir.95.4.818 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wright L, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. Does thinking make it so? Differential associations between adversity worries and experiences and mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. J Epidemiol Community Health 2021;:jech-2020-215598. 10.1136/jech-2020-215598 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Matthews KA, Gallo LC. Psychological Perspectives on Pathways Linking Socioeconomic Status and Physical Health. Annual Review of Psychology 2011;62:501–30. 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130711 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Chen E, Miller GE. Socioeconomic Status and Health: Mediating and Moderating Factors. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 2013;9:723–49. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185634 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.El-Sheikh M, Keiley M, Bagley EJ, et al. Socioeconomic Adversity and Women’s Sleep: Stress and Chaos as Mediators. Behavioral Sleep Medicine 2015;13:506–23. 10.1080/15402002.2014.940110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kim E-J, Dimsdale JE. The Effect of Psychosocial Stress on Sleep: A Review of Polysomnographic Evidence. Behavioral Sleep Medicine 2007;5:256–78. 10.1080/15402000701557383 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.De Lange AH, Kompier MAJ, Taris TW, et al. A hard day’s night: a longitudinal study on the relationships among job demands and job control, sleep quality and fatigue. Journal of Sleep Research 2009;18:374–83. 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00735.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kelly WE. Worry and Sleep Length Revisited: Worry, Sleep Length, and Sleep Disturbance Ascribed to Worry. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 2002;163:296–304. 10.1080/00221320209598685 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Talamini LM, Bringmann LF, de Boer M, et al. Sleeping Worries Away or Worrying Away Sleep? Physiological Evidence on Sleep-Emotion Interactions. PLoS One 2013;8. 10.1371/journal.pone.0062480 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Buckley TM, Schatzberg AF. On the Interactions of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) Axis and Sleep: Normal HPA Axis Activity and Circadian Rhythm, Exemplary Sleep Disorders. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 2005;90:3106–14. 10.1210/jc.2004-1056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Irwin MR, Olmstead R, Carroll JE. Sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and inflammation: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies and experimental sleep deprivation. Biological psychiatry 2016;80:40–52. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Germain A. Sleep Disturbances as the Hallmark of PTSD: Where Are We Now? AJP 2013;170:372–82. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12040432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Yoo S-S, Gujar N, Hu P, et al. The human emotional brain without sleep—a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Current Biology 2007;17:R877–8. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Prather AA, Bogdan R, Hariri AR. Impact of Sleep Quality on Amygdala Reactivity, Negative Affect, and Perceived Stress: Psychosomatic Medicine 2013;75:350–8. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31828ef15b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Goldstein AN, Walker MP. The Role of Sleep in Emotional Brain Function. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2014;10:679–708. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032813-153716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Buysse DJ. Sleep Health: Can We Define It? Does It Matter? Sleep 2014;37:9–17. 10.5665/sleep.3298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Patel SR, Hu FB. Short Sleep Duration and Weight Gain: A Systematic Review. Obesity 2008;16:643–53. 10.1038/oby.2007.118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Madrid-Valero JJ, Ronald A, Shakeshaft N, et al. Sleep quality, insomnia, and internalizing difficulties in adolescents: insights from a twin study. Sleep 2020;43. 10.1093/sleep/zsz229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Thoits PA. Mechanisms Linking Social Ties and Support to Physical and Mental Health. J Health Soc Behav 2011;52:145–61. 10.1177/0022146510395592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kent de Grey RG, Uchino BN, Trettevik R, et al. Social support and sleep: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology 2018;37:787–98. 10.1037/hea0000628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Uchino BN, Bowen K, Kent de Grey R, et al. Social Support and Physical Health: Models, Mechanisms, and Opportunities. In: Fisher EB, Cameron LD, Christensen AJ, et al., eds. Principles and Concepts of Behavioral Medicine. New York, NY: Springer New York; 2018. 341–72. 10.1007/978-0-387-93826-4_12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nordin M, Knutsson A, Sundbom E. Is Disturbed Sleep a Mediator in the Association between Social Support and Myocardial Infarction? J Health Psychol 2008;13:55–64. 10.1177/1359105307084312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Zorn JV, Schür RR, Boks MP, et al. Cortisol stress reactivity across psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2017;77:25–36. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.11.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Feldner MT, Lewis SF, Leen-Feldner EW, et al. Anxiety Sensitivity as a Moderator of the Relation Between Trauma Exposure Frequency and Posttraumatic Stress Symptomatology. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy 2006;20:201–13. 10.1891/jcop.20.2.201 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tsuno N, Besset A, Ritchie K. Sleep and Depression. J Clin Psychiatry 2005;66:1254–69. 10.4088/jcp.v66n1008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Alvaro PK, Roberts RM, Harris JK. A Systematic Review Assessing Bidirectionality between Sleep Disturbances, Anxiety, and Depression. Sleep 2013;36:1059–68. 10.5665/sleep.2810 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Snyder E, Cai B, DeMuro C, et al. A New Single-Item Sleep Quality Scale: Results of Psychometric Evaluation in Patients With Chronic Primary Insomnia and Depression. J Clin Sleep Med 2018;14:1849–57. 10.5664/jcsm.7478 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Carpenter JS, Andrykowski MA. Psychometric evaluation of the pittsburgh sleep quality index. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 1998;45:5–13. 10.1016/s0022-3999(97)00298-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Bell A, Fairbrother M, Jones K. Fixed and random effects models: making an informed choice. Qual Quant 2019;53:1051–74. 10.1007/s11135-018-0802-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Allison P. Fixed Effects Regression Models. 2455 Teller Road, Thousand Oaks California 91320 United States of America: SAGE Publications, Inc. 2009.
  • 57.Overview of the UK population: November 2018. Office for National Statistics 2018. https://www.ons.gov.uk/releases/overviewoftheukpopulationnovember2018 (accessed 7 May 2020).
  • 58.StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 16. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC 2019.
  • 59.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020. https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Xue Z, Lin L, Zhang S, et al. Sleep problems and medical isolation during the SARS-CoV-2 outbreak. Sleep Med 2020;70:112–5. 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.04.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Humphreys C, Lowe P, Williams S. Sleep disruption and domestic violence: exploring the interconnections between mothers and children. Child & Family Social Work 2009;14:6–14. 10.1111/j.1365-2206.2008.00575.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Rogers JP, Chesney E, Oliver D, et al. Psychiatric and neuropsychiatric presentations associated with severe coronavirus infections: a systematic review and meta-analysis with comparison to the COVID-19 pandemic. The Lancet Psychiatry 2020;0. 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30203-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wanberg CR, Zhu J, Kanfer R, et al. After the Pink Slip: Applying Dynamic Motivation Frameworks to the Job Search Experience. AMJ 2012;55:261–84. 10.5465/amj.2010.0157 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Wanberg CR, Basbug G, Van Hooft EAJ, et al. Navigating the Black Hole: Explicating Layers of Job Search Context and Adaptational Responses. Personnel Psychology 2012;65:887–926. 10.1111/peps.12005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Aguiar M, Hurst E, Karabarbounis L. Time Use During the Great Recession. American Economic Review 2013;103:1664–96. 10.1257/aer.103.5.1664 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Grey I, Arora T, Thomas J, et al. The role of perceived social support on depression and sleep during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Research 2020;293:113452. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113452 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Li D-J, Ko N-Y, Chen Y-L, et al. COVID-19-Related Factors Associated with Sleep Disturbance and Suicidal Thoughts among the Taiwanese Public: A Facebook Survey. IJERPH 2020;17:4479. 10.3390/ijerph17124479 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Pow J, King DB, Stephenson E, et al. Does social support buffer the effects of occupational stress on sleep quality among paramedics? A daily diary study. Journal of occupational health psychology 2017;22:71. 10.1037/a0040107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hartmann JA, Carney CE, Lachowski A, et al. Exploring the construct of subjective sleep quality in patients with insomnia. J Clin Psychiatry 2015;76:e768–773. 10.4088/JCP.14m09066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Kristin Vickers

21 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-32170

Are adversities and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic related to sleep quality? Longitudinal analyses of 46,000 UK adults

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Wright,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by February 20, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Kristin Vickers, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide the following information in your Methods section relating to the inter-views conducted in the study. In this case, please detail 1) the training and background of the inter-viewer, 2) where the interviews took place, 3) how long each interview lasted.

3.We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. Please see http://www.bmj.com/content/340/bmj.c181.long for guidelines on how to de-identify and prepare clinical data for publication. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. We note you have included a table to which you do not refer in the text of your manuscript. Please ensure that you refer to Table 2 in your text; if accepted, production will need this reference to link the reader to the Table.

5. We noticed you have some minor occurrence of overlapping text with the following preprint, of which you are an author, which needs to be addressed:

- https://www.medrxiv.org/content/10.1101/2020.05.14.20101717v2

In your revision ensure you cite all your sources (including your own works), and quote or rephrase any duplicated text outside the methods section. Further consideration is dependent on these concerns being addressed.

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

Dear Dr. Wright,

Thank you for submitting your interesting research to PLOS One. I apologize for the delay in our response; several reviewers who agreed initially were not able to complete their reviews. In order to get a decision to you as soon as possible, we are proceeding with two reviews, one from a content expert and one from a statistical expert. The content expert (reviewer one) recommends a major revision, and I agree with this decision. In particular, I ask you to consider whether more substantive content might be added to the Discussion section (reviewer one). Both reviewers provide other thoughtful and important feedback, and I will not repeat the comments here, but ask you to action these (or if you choose not to action a particular suggestion, please explain why). Of especial importance, both reviewers note that there is a paper that may have an overlapping sample (meaning that these manuscripts are related). PLOS ONE has guidelines when different publications are related. If there is no overlap between this manuscript and the other publication, please let me know that. If there is any overlap, please follow the PLOS ONE guidelines at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-related-manuscripts. Specifically, upon your submission of your revised manuscript, please indicate whether there are any related manuscripts under consideration or published elsewhere. If related work has been submitted or published elsewhere, please include a copy of it with your revised manuscript and describe its relation to the submitted work. I will also ask that that the authors make it clear in the revised manuscript that results from a related dataset have been published previously or are under consideration for publication, if applicable.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This manuscript examines how adverse events and worries affect sleep in these days of the COVID pandemic. The paper is obviously timely, and the large stratified sample (~46,000) from the UK is a strength of the study. Both worries and adverse events had a small deleterious effect on sleep, with worries being slightly more impactful. Interestingly, social network had relatively little impact on this effect.

The paper is heavy on statistical analysis and light on discussion. A different reviewer would be better to comment on that statistical analysis.

The authors have an overlapping paper (their reference number 28) which used the same sample in an effort to understand the impact of adversities and worries on mental health. Slicing their analyses so finely, makes it difficult to examine the interplay of sleep and mental health.

There is a typo on Table 1. The rows “difficulties acquiring medication” and “difficulties accessing food” are reversed.

There is a glitch in the manuscript citation in line 1 of the demographics results section.

It is intriguing that so few people were worried about “catching COVID-19” (see table following line 210). That sample would be interesting in its own right. What sorts of life situations contribute to such worries or conversely make some people oblivious to the worries?

I am not sure that the figures add that much value to the paper.

Reviewer #2: Important note: This review pertains only to ‘statistical aspects’ of the study and so ‘clinical aspects’ [like medical importance, relevance of the study, ‘clinical significance and implication(s)’ of the whole study, etc.] are to be evaluated [should be assessed] separately/independently. Further please note that any ‘statistical review’ is generally done under the assumption that (such) study specific methodological [as well as execution] issues are perfectly taken care of by the investigator(s). This review is not an exception to that and so does not cover clinical aspects {however, seldom comments are made only if those issues are intimately / scientifically related & intermingle with ‘statistical aspects’ of the study}. Agreed that ‘statistical methods’ are used as just tools here, however, they are vital part of methodology [and so should be given due importance].

COMMENTS: Your ABSTRACT is well drafted but assay type. Please note that it is preferable [refer to item 1b of CONSORT checklist 2010: Structured summary of trial design, methods, results, and conclusions] to divide the ABSTRACT with small sections like ‘Objective(s)’, ‘Methods’, ‘Results’, ‘Conclusions’, etc. which is an accepted practice of most good/standard journals [including PLOS-ONE]. It will definitely be more informative then, I guess, whatever the article type may be.

I find lot many things common in this article with that of reference number 28 [Wright L, Steptoe A, Fancourt D. How are adversities during COVID-19 affecting mental health? Differential associations for worries and experiences and implications for policy. medRxiv. 2020 May 19;2020.05.14.20101717] given here (though the focus now is on pathway through effects on quality of sleep). It is natural as the data-base is probably same and the authors are same (in same sequence), however (though that is alright), some comments on or summary of overlap is desirable to appear in this article in brief. Although the topic [Are adversities and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic related to sleep quality?] dealt in this article is interesting, at least few things, if not many, here are ‘obvious’ in my opinion.

What exactly you mean by “The study is not random but does contain a well-stratified sample” (lines 93-4)? [no stratified sampling done then is this conclusion/finding/observation based on post-stratification?] Rest of the methodology and statistical methods used are correct [very good]. It is appreciated that ‘logit’ is used in hybrid models {though ‘logit’ is the only appropriate choice, often mistakenly ‘linear’ model(s) are used}.

Nothing more could be suggested/highlighted. Minor revision is recommended.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: renamed_aaf3b.docx

PLoS One. 2021 Mar 25;16(3):e0248919. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248919.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


20 Jan 2021

Dear Dr Vickers,

We hope you had a restful Christmas break.

Thank you to you and the reviewers for accepting our work for publication with revisions. We have uploaded an amended manuscript based on your comments. In the attached file we respond to each of the points raised directly.

Best wishes,

Daisy

Attachment

Submitted filename: reviewer_comments_2020_01_18.docx

Decision Letter 1

Kristin Vickers

18 Feb 2021

PONE-D-20-32170R1

Are adversities and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic related to sleep quality? Longitudinal analyses of 46,000 UK adults

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Wright,

Thank you for submitting your revised manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The decision on this revision is Minor Revision. Please address the comment of the reviewer. Also, please consider whether you can expand on how this manuscript is related to the pre-print (now accepted for publication; congratulations). I see one sentence that references your prior work (reference 28). “Specifically during COVID-19, we have shown in recent work that there is a relationship between worries and experiences of adversities and poorer mental health [28].”  Please consider if there is other information you could add to clarify how these studies are related (was the same sample used, for example; were the 41,909 UK adults in the pre-print also included in the 46,284 participants in this work). I look forward to receiving your minor revision of this manuscript.   

Please submit your revised manuscript by April 2, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Kristin Vickers, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: COMMENTS: Please note that the meaning of the terms “well stratified” and “heterogeneous” are not same Therefore, your response/answer to my comment/question regarding stratification that “We meant that the sample was heterogeneous in the demographics represented” is not acceptable {and even liked} as what you meant cannot be understood [how do expect that?] automatically. Remember that this is a scientific/academic document and so all details should be accurately/clearly/correctly communicated, and terms used carefully. Agreed that English is not my mother tongue however, I never knew that the term “well stratified” is alternative to “heterogeneous”.

It is well known that the evidence from secondary data analyses of survey data is/are considered as very low level research evidence, however, importance of study on ‘COVID-19 adversities and sleep quality’ is of enough value and so this view was not taken by this reviewer. I guess, what I said on earlier draft [that “Nothing more could be suggested/highlighted” after making just two very minor comments] is a clear indication. Does not that so?

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Mar 25;16(3):e0248919. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248919.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


18 Feb 2021

Dear Dr Vickers,

Please see the attached files for our response to your and Reviewer #2's comments.

With best wishes,

Liam

Attachment

Submitted filename: reviewer_comments_2021_02_18.docx

Decision Letter 2

Kristin Vickers

9 Mar 2021

Are adversities and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic related to sleep quality? Longitudinal analyses of 46,000 UK adults

PONE-D-20-32170R2

Dear Dr. Wright,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Kristin Vickers, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: COMMENTS: All the comments made on earlier draft(s) by me (and hopefully by other respected reviewers also) were/are attended positively/adequately, I had already expressed satisfaction and the manuscript is improved a lot. I recommend acceptance.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: Yes: Dr. Sanjeev Sarmukaddam

Acceptance letter

Kristin Vickers

15 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-32170R2

Are adversities and worries during the COVID-19 pandemic related to sleep quality? Longitudinal analyses of 46,000 UK adults

Dear Dr. Wright:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Kristin Vickers

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Comparison of items in the original and revised perceived social support questionnaire (F-SozU K-6).

    (DOCX)

    S2 Table. Descriptive statistics, social support and psychiatric diagnosis.

    Weighted figures.

    (DOCX)

    S3 Table. Sample descriptive statistics.

    (DOCX)

    S4 Table. Sample size by week.

    (DOCX)

    S5 Table. Estimated interaction effects between social support and diagnosed psychiatric illness and deviation in total adversity experiences and adversity worries.

    (DOCX)

    S1 Fig. Association between demographic characteristics and reporting worry about catching COVID-19 during any data collection.

    Derived from bivariate and multivariate survey weighted logistic regression models. Multivariate models include adjustment for each factor simultaneously. Big-Five personality traits are scaled (mean = 0, SD = 1).

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Associations between (i) cut in income and (ii) inability to pay bills and odds of bad quality sleep.

    Derived from REWB models.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of bad quality sleep.

    Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and probability of poor sleep.

    Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S5 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

    Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S6 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

    Derived from linear probability fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S7 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of bad quality sleep.

    Derived from fixed effects logit models.

    (TIF)

    S8 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and probability of poor sleep.

    Derived from fixed effects logit models.

    (TIF)

    S9 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

    Derived from fixed effects logit models.

    (TIF)

    S10 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and probability of poor quality sleep according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

    Derived from fixed effects logit models.

    (TIF)

    S11 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of all/specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about all/specific types of adversities and probability of poor sleep.

    Derived from fixed effects logit models and REWB models, where sample is those whose sleep quality changed during follow-up period.

    (TIF)

    S12 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and (continuous) sleep quality.

    Derived from fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S13 Fig. Associations between (i) change in experience of specific types of adversities or (ii) worries about specific types of adversities and (continuous) sleep quality.

    Derived from fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S14 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and (continuous) sleep quality according to (a) living arrangement, (b) social network size), (c) loneliness, and (d) perceived social support at baseline data collection.

    Derived from fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S15 Fig. Associations between (i) change in total number of adversity experiences and (ii) total number of adversity worries and (continuous) quality sleep quality according to mental health diagnosis at baseline data collection.

    Derived from fixed effects models.

    (TIF)

    S16 Fig. Convergent validity of a single item sleep quality measure from Wave 4 of the United Kingdom household longitudinal study with other measures of sleep from the same survey.

    Sleep quality: “During the past month, how would you rate your sleep quality overall?”. Sleep hours: “How many hours of actual sleep did you usually get per night during the last month?”. 30+ minutes to sleep: “During the past month, how often have you had trouble sleeping because you… cannot get to sleep within 30 minutes?”. Waking frequency: “(During the past month, how often have you had trouble sleeping because you…) wake up in the middle of the night or early in the morning?”. Sleep medicine: “During the past month, how often have you taken medicine (prescribed or "over the counter") to help you sleep?”. The correlation between sleep quality and sleep time is 0.51.

    (TIF)

    S1 File. STROBE checklist for observational studies.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: renamed_aaf3b.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: reviewer_comments_2020_01_18.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: reviewer_comments_2021_02_18.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The code to produce each of the analyses in this paper is available at https://osf.io/4epqm/. A link to the other data files will be added on that website when they become available.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES