Abstract
Multisensory integration of auditory and tactile information occurs already at the level of the cochlear nucleus. Rodents use their whiskers for tactile perception to guide them in their exploration of the world. As nocturnal animals with relatively poor vision, audiotactile interactions are of great importance for this species. Here, the influence of whisker deflections on sound-evoked spiking in the cochlear nucleus was investigated in vivo in anesthetized mice. Multichannel, silicon-probe electrophysiological recordings were obtained from both the dorsal and ventral cochlear nucleus. Whisker deflections evoked an increased spiking activity in fusiform cells of the dorsal cochlear nucleus and t-stellate cells in ventral cochlear nucleus, whereas bushy cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus showed a more variable response. The response to broadband noise stimulation increased in fusiform cells and primary-like bushy cells when the sound stimulation was preceded (~ 20 ms) by whisker stimulation. Multi-sensory integration of auditory and whisker input can thus occur already in this early brainstem nucleus, emphasizing the importance of early integration of auditory and somatosensory information.
Subject terms: Auditory system, Sensory processing, Neuroscience, Whisker system
Introduction
In the auditory system, integration of information from the cochlea with information from other sensory modalities begins at the earliest processing stages in the cochlear nucleus1–6. Anatomical studies demonstrate that several somatosensory structures in the brainstem provide inputs to the cochlear nucleus (CN). These regions include the dorsal column nuclei, consisting of the gracile7 and cuneate8,9 nuclei that receive proprioceptive and somatosensory inputs from the lower and upper body, respectively. Projections to the cochlear nucleus also arise from the spinal trigeminal nucleus10–14 that conveys touch sensation from the face. Projections from these regions form mossy fibre inputs that synapse onto granule cells in the cochlear nucleus granule-cell domain, as well as onto dendrites of ventral cochlear nucleus (VCN) bushy cells15 and D-stellate cells16. The D-stellate cells inhibit fusiform cells in dorsal cochlear nucleus (DCN)17–19. The parallel fibre axons of granule cells in turn provide excitatory synaptic input to DCN cartwheel and fusiform cells. The presence of somatosensory inputs to a primary auditory nucleus such as the cochlear nucleus is intriguing in the context of the role of the pinnae and neck in generating sound localization cues16, as well as the suppression of self-generated signals20. Correct interpretation of this information requires integration of auditory signals with somatosensory and proprioceptive signals conveying information about head and pinna position. Studies in cats and guinea pigs6,21–23 have demonstrated that direct electrical stimulation of brainstem somatosensory nuclei evokes neuronal responses in the DCN and VCN15,21,24. These data suggest that activation of CN granule cells by these somatosensory inputs excites fusiform cells and provides feedforward inhibition to fusiform cells through the inhibitory interneurons, the cartwheel cells. The association between sound and whisker stimulation may be a consequence of similar encoding mechanisms: both senses process information that produces mechanical displacements of tissue (i.e., the basilar membrane for auditory and the skin for somatosensory) and are processed in frequency based codes in the cerebral cortex25. Rodents use a set of roughly 30 whiskers on each side of the snout, palpating surfaces through a 5–20 Hz forward‐backward motion known as “whisking”26,27. Whisker‐mediated object identification can thus be used as a model to learn more about the mechanisms of multisensory processing and the transformation of this processing to a behavioural output. In the present in vivo electrophysiology study, spiking activity was measured in spike-sorted single units, and audiotactile interaction in the mouse cochlear nucleus was investigated using whisker stimulation in combination with sound. The results suggest that whisker stimulation can modify the sound-evoked spiking activity in the cochlear nucleus.
Results
Cell type classification
Neurons in the mouse CN were classified based on their characteristic post-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) at best frequency (BF; as determined from a receptive-field analysis) 20 dB above threshold, as well as receptive-field types (Fig. 1) and the electrode location in DCN or VCN, verified by the electrode tract. Forty-five cells were classified as putative fusiform cells (pause/build-up temporal pattern) with type III and type I-III receptive fields in DCN (Fig. 1aa-ac) and 49 cells as putative VCN bushy cells (primary-like (n = 42; Fig. 1ba-bc) and primary-like with notch (n = 7; Fig. 1ca-cc)28). Furthermore, 10 cells in VCN were classified as putative t-stellate cells (5 transient choppers and 3 sustained choppers, 2 undefined, Fig. 1da-dc) based on their coefficients of variation29. Tone stimulation increased spiking in fusiform cells 4.1 ± 2.2 ms after stimulus onset, in bushy cells after 4.3 ± 1.6 ms and in t-stellate cells after 4.9 ± 1.8 ms.
Whisker-evoked responses in fusiform cells in DCN and bushy cells in VCN
The whiskers on the mouse whisker pad were deflected (1000 deflections at 5 Hz) using a magnetic stimulation system, and spiking activity was measured in the CN using multi-channel silicon probes. In DCN fusiform cells whisker stimulation evoked increased spiking activity above the spontaneous rate. The increase in spiking started 6.0 ± 1.8 ms after whisker movement. Calculated over the full spike-sorted cell population (n = 45 cells; 8 animals) whisker stimulation increased median spiking from 1.09 spikes/s [IQR: 0.55–3.40] to 4.56 spikes/s with whisker stimulation [IQR: 2.00–6.90] (p < 0.0001, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test; time window for calculating spikes was 50 ms/sweep; 1000 sweeps were recorded; Fig. 2a, b).
In primary-like bushy cells in VCN the spontaneous activity (n = 49 cells; 5 animals) did not change with whisker stimulation (Fig. 2c, d). The median spontaneous activity was 1.59 spikes/s [IQR: 1.03–3.16], and with whisker stimulation 1.89 [IQR: 1.18–3.14] (p = 0.1005, Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank). Splitting the cell population into primary-like (PL) and primary-like-with-notch (PLN) did not change the result. (PL (n = 42) spontaneous activity: 1.54 spikes/s [IQR: 0.99–2.66], whisker-evoked response (WER): 1.76 spikes/s [IQR: 1.34–2.58] p = 0.1181; PLN (n = 7) spontaneous activity: 11.75 spikes/s [IQR: 1.22–18.69], WER: 11.27 spikes/s [IQR: 1.89–18.04], p = 0.4688). It should be noted, however, that although when averaging over the entire sample there was no effect of whisker stimulation, there were individual PL cells where small effects were observed (Fig. 2d). The number of recorded t-stellate cells in VCN was comparatively low (n = 10 cells, from one animal, two different recording sites, 2 shank electrode), but there was an increase in spiking from 0.30 spikes/s [IQR: 0.13–0.57] to 1.20 spikes/s [IQR: 0.45–1.8] when whiskers were deflected (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank, p = 0.0020; IQR: 0.32–1.35) effect size: 0.63 (Fig. 2e, f). This increase could be detected 7.2 ± 2.3 ms after whisker movement.
Whisker stimulation increased the sound-evoked response in DCN fusiform cells and VCN bushy cells
Stimulation of the ipsilateral whiskers (ipsilateral to recording site in the CN) during acoustic stimulation increased the sound-driven responses of fusiform cells and bushy cells. The magnitude of the effect was dependent on the delay between whisker stimulation and sound stimulation onset. The whisker stimulation onset shifted from 50 ms before sound (+ 50), to 20 ms after onset of sound (−20); the protocols were performed in a random order (Supplementary Table S2).
In fusiform cells, the number of sound-evoked spikes increased for the + 20, + 10, + 5 and − 10 protocol (Fig. 3, Table 1 and Supplementary Fig. S1). The protocols where whisker stimulation preceded sound stimulation increased spiking with approx. 4 spikes/s (27 to 31 spikes/s) (average from the + 20, + 10, + 5 protocols). In the other protocols (+ 50, − 5, − 20) the bimodal stimulation did not significantly change the absolute evoked spikes compared to sound only stimulation. It remains to be investigated if small differences in timing, e.g. between the − 5 and − 10 protocol, relate to non-linear effects on encoding sensory information in the DCN. The bimodal response (calculated as BI; Fig. 6) was on average 9% smaller than the linear summation of the sound and whisker-evoked response (averaged over the + 20 and + 10, protocols; p < 0.001, Wilcoxon signed rank test, theoretical median zero).
Table 1.
Protocol | Median (spikes/s) | BI | IQR | p-value (Bonferroni corrected) | Effect size r | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sound | Whisker + sound | Sound | Whisker + sound | ||||
+ 50 | 25.82 | 25.31 | −19.81 | 14.16–44.13 | 12.84–47.30 | > 0.9999 | 0.04 |
+ 20 | 26.47 | 28.36 | −10.99 | 15.72–51.44 | 18.29–55.88 | 0.0007* | 0.50 |
+ 10 | 25.18 | 28.80 | −7.14 | 15.24–51.92 | 17.21–54.99 | 0.0042* | 0.35 |
+ 5 | 29.77 | 36.11 | −12.77 | 18.08–56.89 | 18.85–62.02 | 0.0021* | 0.37 |
−5 | 32.30 | 30.19 | −20.53 | 16.63–52.54 | 15.63–55.67 | 0.9999 | 0.02 |
−10 | 30.69 | 39.88 | −14.31 | 16.86–55.27 | 20.85–56.88 | 0.0007* | 0.44 |
−20 | 23.88 | 24.00 | −16.37 | 14.43–47.64 | 14.34–46.64 | 0.9999 | 0.11 |
In VCN, cells were classified based on the PSTH pattern as either primary-like (PL) or primary-like-with-notch bushy cells (PLN). In PL bushy cells, the + 20 and + 10 bimodal protocols significantly increased the number of sound evoked responses during whisker stimulation (Fig. 4, Table 2 and Supplementary Fig. S1) with approx. 5 spikes/s (48 to 53 spikes/s; average from the + 20, + 10 protocols). The bimodal response of the PL bushy cells was ~ 11% larger than the linear summation of the sound and whisker-evoked response (averaged over the + 20, + 10 protocols; p < 0.001, Wilcoxon signed rank test, theoretical median zero; Fig. 6). The + 50 protocol induced a response that was ~ 5% smaller than the linear summation of the sound and whisker-evoked response, indicating that although the bimodal response was slightly larger than only sound, it was smaller than the unimodal sum.
Table 2.
Protocol | Median (spikes/s) | BI | IQR | p-value (Bonferroni corrected)/effect size r | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
sound | Whisker + sound | Sound | Whisker + sound | ||||||||
PL | PLN | PL | PLN | PL | PL | PLN | PL | PLN | PL | PLN | |
+ 50 | 40.53 | 94.67 | 42.24 | 94.72 | −5.27 | 21.24–89.08 | 80.86–162.5 | 20.38–83.45 | 79.77–164.0 | 0.0056*/r = 0.35 | > 0.9999 |
+ 20 | 52.14 | 82.73 | 55.45 | 82.06 | 10.15 | 29.28–77.10 | 71.16–174.6 | 29.55–83.82 | 68.54–184.1 | 0.0007*/r = 0.48 | > 0.9999 |
+ 10 | 44.42 | 80.74 | 50.30 | 81.06 | 11.53 | 26.56–76.51 | 68.48–141.3 | 29.39–82.09 | 70.91–138.5 | 0.0007*/r = 0.48 | > 0.9999 |
+ 5 | 37.18 | 83.34 | 34.50 | 83.43 | −1.57 | 20.95–82.13 | 71.08–113.7 | 21.36–81.72 | 71.54–116.3 | 0.6986/r = 0.18 | 0.1092 |
−5 | 34.88 | 77.38 | 42.74 | 76.55 | 10.49 | 21.23–73.93 | 63.49–115.3 | 21.39–73.62 | 63.95–119.1 | 0.0238/r = 0.31 | > 0.9999 |
−10 | 31.85 | 79.89 | 30.82 | 79.49 | −2.42 | 19.94–75.51 | 65.07–108.4 | 20.60–75.52 | 65.27–105.3 | 0.3948/r = 0.15 | > 0.9999 |
−20 | 31.45 | 69.73 | 56.63 | 70.71 | 11.78 | 18.09–76.68 | 56.65–94.45 | 18.31–92.59 | 57.47–88.32 | > 0.9999/r = 0.14 | > 0.9999 |
t-stellate | |||||||||||
+ 50 | 24.38 | 25.97 | −1.41 | 19.63–28.70 | 20.93–29.86 | 0.4515/r = 0.42 | |||||
+ 20 | 28.11 | 30.13 | 2.21 | 21.78–32.18 | 21.98–35.04 | 0.0413/r = 0.58 | |||||
+ 10 | 26.12 | 25.56 | −3.13 | 20.48–30.25 | 19.88–31.55 | 0.9999/r = 0.24 | |||||
+ 5 | 19.74 | 24.84 | 14.09 | 15.79–25.21 | 19.20–29.52 | 0.0140/r = 0.63 | |||||
−5 | 16.29 | 18.65 | 0.32 | 14.48–21.91 | 14.76–23.08 | 0.3416/r = 0.44 | |||||
−10 | 15.03 | 25.39 | 39.07 | 12.41–19.36 | 17.10–29.55 | 0.0140/r = 0.63 | |||||
−20 | 18.03 | 28.69 | 29.29 | 13.34–20.39 | 15.77–32.15 | 0.0273/r = 0.60 |
In t-stellate cells (n = 10), the whisker alone stimulation induced a short-lasting (~ 20 ms) increased spiking (Fig. 2f), hence, when the whisker stimulation preceded the tone, the whisker stimulation did not influence the sound evoked response (Fig. 5, Table 2). Although the bimodal response was not significantly larger, when averaging over the sample, in some cells the brief burst of whisker-evoked activity could increase the bimodal response. This effect is corroborated by the relatively large BI (for the − 20 and − 10 protocols; Fig. 6).
Discussion
In the dorsal and ventral cochlear nucleus, the sensory-evoked spiking activity to sound, whisker deflections and bimodal (whisker and sound) stimulation was investigated using in vivo electrophysiology in anaesthetized mice.
Whisker deflections evoked increased spiking activity above spontaneous rates in the cochlear nucleus. The effect was most prominent in DCN fusiform cells, with a smaller, transient response in VCN t-stellate cells. Fusiform cells receive multisensory information via granule cells, whereas for t-stellate cells a multisensory input pathway remains to be investigated. In VCN bushy cells, however, the sample average did not increase significantly, although in some cells (Fig. 2d) there was a clear increase in spiking with whisker deflections.
The cochlear nucleus is primarily involved in sound processing, thus the pairing of whisker stimulation with sound was investigated. Pairing sound stimulation with whisker deflections increased the sound-evoked responses in fusiform cells. The effect was dependent on the temporal relationship between the whisker defection and the sound stimulation. An optimal range was found to be when the whisker deflection began 20 ms before the sound onset. The average bimodal increase was, however, smaller than the sum of the unimodal activity (sound plus whisker) (mean BI ≈ − 9 (average from the + 20 and + 10 protocols; Fig. 6). The variability in the bimodal effect could indicate that multisensory integration is more prominent in certain areas and/or that some cells were less activated by the stimulation parameters used. In VCN, cells were classified based on the PSTH pattern as either primary-like (PL) or primary-like-with-notch bushy cells (PLN), the former thus putatively being spherical bushy cells and the latter globular bushy cells30. Globular bushy cells are important for encoding interaural level differences, whereas the spherical bushy cells play a role in encoding interaural time and level differences31. Based on a limited sample (n = 7) in contrast to PL cells, the sound evoked response of PLN cells is not modulated by whisker deflections. Whereas in PL cells there was a bimodal enhancement (average bimodal increase, that was larger than the sum of the unimodal activity; mean BI ≈ 11 (average from + 20 & + 10 protocols); Fig. 6). In the t-stellate population, it’s noteworthy that the whisker deflection alone caused a transient initial burst (Fig. 2f), and when the whisker deflection preceded the tone, the bimodal activity was not significantly increased compared to only sound. Delaying the whisker deflection relative to the tone increased bimodal activity, because the whisker evoked burst was preserved in the bimodal response (Fig. 6b).
Anatomical data show that DCN receives input from the somatosensory system via dorsal column nuclei and the spinal trigeminal nuclei that project to the granule cell domain9,10,13,32. The DCN thus plays a central role in early multi-sensory integration of sound and tactile stimuli. The spinal trigeminal nuclei also mediate sensory information from the whiskers33. In rodents audiotactile interactions in the cochlear nucleus have been less examined34. The DCN is involved in auditory spatial perception in the vertical plane and suppression of self-generated signals20. The interactions of sound with the head creates different acoustic spectra depending on the angle of the incoming sound. These difference in the acoustic spectra enable the DCN to determine the sound location with respect to the ear35. Knowing the position of the ears also necessities integrating head position. Head position is determined by proprioceptive inputs from the neck. Breathing and sniffing, activities that self-generate sound21, are both linked to whisking36. It is thus possible that whisker deflection per se is not the most important input to DCN, but rather the information on head position inherent in the sensory signal evoked by whisker movements. Furthermore, sound produced when the whisker touches an object is another source of information that could be used for multisensory integration. Hence, whisker input might provide information on subtracting self-generated sounds20. In guinea pigs, fusiform cells have been shown to integrate auditory with somatosensory inputs evoked by stimulation of face and neck muscles3,16. This type of somatosensory stimulation induced D-stellate cell inhibition, with the degree of inhibition increasing or decreasing depending on the temporal relationship between the somatosensory and the auditory signal. In the present experiments, when whisker activation preceded the sound stimulation, the bimodal response was larger compared to only sound stimulation. Under this condition it is therefore possible, that the degree of D-stellate inhibition to fusiform cells was very small.
Interactions between the somatosensory and auditory systems has been shown in humans37–39, cats22 and guinea pigs15,40. It remains to be investigated to what degree similar multisensory integration is important in mice and rats for tactile information processing based on whisker inputs. One hypothesis is that, similar to that found in humans39, depending on the frequency band, sound can enhance or decrease the perception of surface textures. Depending on the texture discrimination task the performance might thus be better or worse when it is accompanied with sounds of different frequencies.
Materials and methods
The study was carried out in compliance with the ARRIVE guidelines and were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Michigan (IACUC; Protocol #009,202). We confirmed that all experiments were performed in accordance with IACUC guidelines and regulations.
Surgery Procedures
In vivo electrophysiology recordings in the DCN and VCN were performed in eight male C57/BL6 mice (20–24 g) (JAX stock #000,664, Jackson Laboratories, Bar Harbor, ME). Animals were anaesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of ketamine (97 mg/kg)/xylazine (16 mg/kg) and the body temperatures were kept constant (37 °C) using an automatically controlled heating pad. Additional anaesthetic (2–5% of the original dose) was administered approximately every hour after performing a pinch to check the paw reflex. After a local lidocaine (2% w/v) injection in the skin of the head, animals were placed in a stereotaxic frame secured with hollow ear-bars (Kopf Instruments) to deliver sound to the left ear. An incision was made in the skin, followed by a craniotomy over the left CN (anterior–posterior − 1.3 to − 1.6 mm (from lambda); lateral to midline 2.0 to 2.25 mm. Extracellular recordings were obtained with 32-channel silicon electrode arrays (A2 × 16–10 mm–50–500–177; NeuroNexus Technologies, MI, USA) in the DCN (~ − 1.3 mm anterior–posterior from lamda); ~ 2.1 mm lateral to midline; depth ~ 4750 µm (from skull)) and VCN; (~ − 1.5 mm from lamda); ~ 2.25 mm lateral to midline; depth ~ 5250 µm (from skull)) using the RZ2 multichannel acquisition system from Tucker-Davis Technologies (FL, USA). The raw signal (acquired at 25 kHz) was bandpass filtered between 300–5000 Hz and signals that exceeded the background noise with at least 4 SDs were detected as spikes. Recorded units were sorted offline into single-units with a customised, semi-automatic MATLAB algorithm via k-means clustering of n principal components (k and n were user-specified) of peak-amplitude aligned waveforms. Furthermore, spike-sorted units were analysed (Neuroexplorer, Nex Technologies, USA) with cross- and autocorrelation to ensure that they were separate units (Supplementary Fig. S2). Cross-correlation was done with spike-sorted units in adjacent channels. The refractory period in the autocorrelogram was estimated as the peak from the hazard function. The refractory period for fusiform cells in DCN was 9.8 ms ± 1.6 ms (mean ± SD; n = 45 spike sorted units). For bushy cells in VCN the refractory period was 8.4 ms ± 1.77 ms (mean ± SD; n = 49 spike sorted units; primary-like-with-notch (PLN): 8.3 ms ± 1.5 ms (7 units), primary like (PL): 8.4 ms ± 1.8 ms (42 units) and for t-stellate cells in VCN it was 9.0 ms ± 0.89 ms (mean ± SD; n = 10 spike sorted units). After the experiment mice were euthanized by i.p. injections of sodium pentobarbital (Med-Pharmex Inc., Pomona, CA, USA). The brains were removed and fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde. After dehydrating the tissue in 30% sucrose, the brains were sectioned in 30 μm slices, coverslipped and observed under epifluorescence (Leica, DMLB) to visualize the electrode tract.
Auditory Stimulation
Experiments were performed in a double-walled sound shielded chamber and acoustic signals were generated by the RX8 DSP hardware from Tucker-Davis Technologies (TDT). Sound stimulation was 50 ms broadband noise bursts (200 Hz–20 kHz) with 2 ms rise/fall times presented unilaterally through a closed, calibrated earphone to the left ears. 500 repetitions were presented at 80 dB SPL. The response latency was calculated as the timepoint when the spike rate in a 1 ms bin after stimulus onset was higher than the mean ± 2 SD calculated from 50 ms preceding the sound stimulation.
Somatosensory Stimulation
To stimulate the whiskers ipsilateral to the CN recording side a custom-built magnetic whisker stimulation system was used. This enabled whisker deflection in the absence of sound. The whiskers were lightly covered with magnetic paint (k03151000, Krylon) and an electromagnet coupled to an isolated electromagnetic driver was placed close to the whiskers. Signals to drive the magnet were generated using the TDT Synapse software. The whiskers were deflected (ramp and hold) 1000 times at 5 Hz with a 100 ms duration pulse. The whiskers were actively moved using the magnet, in the rostral direction (i.e. protraction). To ensure that sound-driven responses in the CN were caused by whisker movements, and not by other tactile or auditory cues, a control experiment with magnetic stimulation was performed by omitting the magnetic paint, and thus not eliciting movement of the whiskers when the magnet was activated. In this control experiment no effect of magnetic stimulation was observed on the spontaneous or sound-driven responses in fusiform cells (Supplementary Table S1). The time window used to calculate the spontaneous activity was 50 ms before the stimulus onset. The time window to calculate the whisker response was from stimulus onset to 50 ms (for the t-stellate cells it was only 25 ms; the t-stellate response was only on initial burst) after stimulus onset. The whiskers ipsilateral to the CN recording site (left side) were deflected. The response latency was calculated as the timepoint when the spike rate in a 1 ms bin after stimulus onset was higher than the mean ± 2 SD calculated from 50 ms preceding the whisker stimulation. The delay from magnet onset to whisker movement (~ 10 ms) was subtracted.
Bimodal Stimulation
To study multisensory integration in DCN and VCN the acoustic stimulation was combined with the whisker stimulation. A bimodal stimulation protocol was used based on previous in vitro41and in vivo42 studies on spike timing dependent plasticity in DCN. Seven different protocols with varying delays between the sound and whisker stimulation were used: + 50 ms, + 20 ms, + 10 ms, + 5 ms, − 5 ms, − 10 ms and − 20 ms. The prefix “ + ” means that the whisker stimulation starts before the auditory stimulation and the “−” that the whisker stimulation starts after the auditory stimulation. The different protocols were repeated 500 times and the spiking output compared to 500-times acoustic stimulation alone. The order of the protocol was randomized between experiments (Supplementary Table S2). The time window used to calculate the bimodal response was ~ 50 ms for protocols + 50, + 20, + 10 and + 5. When the whisker stimulation started after the sound the time window was reduced. For the − 5 protocol the time window was ~ 45 ms, for the − 10 protocol the time window was ~ 40 ms and for the − 20 protocol the time window was ~ 30 ms (the exact time varied 1–2 ms depending on the stimulus artefact). To compare the bimodal response to the unimodal responses (only sound or only whisker deflections) the percentage change was calculated as a bimodal integration index where Bi is bimodal, S is sound, and W is whisker response in spikes/s21,43. BI > zero, is called a bimodal enhancement, meaning that the bimodal response was larger than the sum of the unimodal response.
Statistics
The raw data (spikes/s) did not pass normality tests (neither the D'Agostino & Pearson omnibus normality test, nor the Shapiro–Wilk normality or the Kolmogorov–Smirnov normality test); the data was right-hand skewed–relatively few data points with a high spike rate and many with low spike rate. Furthermore, the sample variances in each group were not equal. Thus, the groups were compared with individual non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test, exact p-value), with p-values corrected for multiple testing using Bonferroni correction. There is one comparison per row/condition (sound vs. whisker deflection + sound); thus, the number of comparisons per family was seven, one for each protocol (+ 50, + 20 etc.). In the tables (Tables 1 and 2) the p-values from the individual Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed rank test were multiplied by seven. Unless stated otherwise the median and interquartile range (IQR) is plotted. The interquartile range is the difference between the third and first quartiles. Due to the expected variability in the response of different cells to bimodal stimulations, the experimental unit was each individual cell, rather than averaging cells from each animal. To reduce the Type 1 error rate, taking into account the large sample size when using the individual cells as the experimental units, the significance level was set at p < 0.01. The correlation coefficient r was calculated as a measure of the effect size: . Z is the z-score, n = number of single units, 2n = the total number of observations, including the cases where the difference is zero.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We thank Dileepkumar Deepak for designing and building the whisker stimulation system.
Author contributions
J.A. performed the experiments and analysed the data. C.W. provided analysis tools. P.K. conceived the original idea. J.A., C.W., S.E.S. and P.K. designed the study. J.A. and P.K. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. J.A. was sponsored by the Lore-Agnes Program at the Ruhr University Bochum.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-86236-9.
References
- 1.Young ED, Oertel D. The synaptic organization of the brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Basura GJ, Koehler SD, Shore SE. Multi-sensory integration in brainstem and auditory cortex. Brain Res. 2012;1485:95–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wu C, Stefanescu RA, Martel DT, Shore SE. Listening to another sense: somatosensory integration in the auditory system. Cell Tissue Res. 2015;361:233–250. doi: 10.1007/s00441-014-2074-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Burian M, Gstoettner W. Projection of primary vestibular afferent fibres to the cochlear nucleus in the guinea pig. Neurosci. Lett. 1988;84:13–17. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(88)90329-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Shore SE, Moore JK. Sources of input to the cochlear granule cell region in the guinea pig. Hear Res. 1998;116:33–42. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(97)00207-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Young ED, Nelken I, Conley RA. Somatosensory effects on neurons in dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 1995;73:743–765. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.743. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Weinberg RJ, Rustioni A. A cuneocochlear pathway in the rat. Neuroscience. 1987;20:209–219. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(87)90013-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wright DD, Ryugo DK. Mossy fiber projections from the cuneate nucleus to the cochlear nucleus in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 1996;365:159–172. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960129)365:1<159::AID-CNE12>3.0.CO;2-L. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Shore SE, Zhou J. Somatosensory influence on the cochlear nucleus and beyond. Hear Res. 2006;216–217:90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zhou J, Shore S. Projections from the trigeminal nuclear complex to the cochlear nuclei: a retrograde and anterograde tracing study in the guinea pig. J. Neurosci. Res. 2004;78:901–907. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zhou J, Nannapaneni N, Shore S. Vessicular glutamate transporters 1 and 2 are differentially associated with auditory nerve and spinal trigeminal inputs to the cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 2007;500:777–787. doi: 10.1002/cne.21208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Shore SE, Vass Z, Wys NL, Altschuler RA. Trigeminal ganglion innervates the auditory brainstem. J. Comp. Neurol. 2000;419:271–285. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000410)419:3<271::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-M. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Zhou J, Shore S. Convergence of spinal trigeminal and cochlear nucleus projections in the inferior colliculus of the guinea pig. J. Comp. Neurol. 2006;495:100–112. doi: 10.1002/cne.20863. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Itoh K, et al. Direct projections from the dorsal column nuclei and the spinal trigeminal nuclei to the cochlear nuclei in the cat. Brain Res. 1987;400:145–150. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90662-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Heeringa AN, Wu C, Shore SE. Multisensory integration enhances temporal coding in ventral cochlear nucleus bushy cells. J. Neurosci. 2018;38:2832–2843. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2244-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wu C, Shore SE. Multisensory activation of ventral cochlear nucleus D-stellate cells modulates dorsal cochlear nucleus principal cell spatial coding. J. Physiol. 2018;596:4537–4548. doi: 10.1113/JP276280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Smith PH, Rhode WS. Structural and functional properties distinguish two types of multipolar cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 1989;282:595–616. doi: 10.1002/cne.902820410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ferragamo MJ, Golding NL, Oertel D. Synaptic inputs to stellate cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 1998;79:51–63. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.1.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Oertel D, Wright S, Cao XJ, Ferragamo M, Bal R. The multiple functions of T stellate/multipolar/chopper cells in the ventral cochlear nucleus. Hear Res. 2011;276:61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2010.10.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Singla S, Dempsey C, Warren R, Enikolopov AG, Sawtell NB. A cerebellum-like circuit in the auditory system cancels responses to self-generated sounds. Nat. Neurosci. 2017;20:943–950. doi: 10.1038/nn.4567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Shore SE. Multisensory integration in the dorsal cochlear nucleus: unit responses to acoustic and trigeminal ganglion stimulation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2005;21:3334–3348. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04142.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kanold PO, Davis KA, Young ED. Somatosensory context alters auditory responses in the cochlear nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;105:1063–1070. doi: 10.1152/jn.00807.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kanold PO, Young ED. Proprioceptive information from the pinna provides somatosensory input to cat dorsal cochlear nucleus. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:7848–7858. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07848.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Koehler SD, Pradhan S, Manis PB, Shore SE. Somatosensory inputs modify auditory spike timing in dorsal cochlear nucleus principal cells. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011;33:409–420. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07547.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Soto-Faraco S, Deco G. Multisensory contributions to the perception of vibrotactile events. Behav. Brain Res. 2009;196:145–154. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Krieger P, Groh A. Sensorimotor integration in the whisker system. New York: Springer; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 27.de Kock CPJ, Luhmann HJ, Maravall M. Editorial. Neuroscience. 2018;368:1–2. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Rhode, W. S. & Greenberg, S. in The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neurophysiology (eds Arthur N. Popper & Richard R. Fay) 94–152 (Springer, New York, 1992).
- 29.Winter IM, Wiegrebe L, Patterson RD. The temporal representation of the delay of iterated rippled noise in the ventral cochlear nucleus of the guinea-pig. J. Physiol. 2001;537:553–566. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.00553.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rhode WS, Oertel D, Smith PH. Physiological response properties of cells labeled intracellularly with horseradish peroxidase in cat ventral cochlear nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 1983;213:448–463. doi: 10.1002/cne.902130408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Yin, T. C. T. in Integrative Functions in the Mammalian Auditory Pathway (eds Donata Oertel, Richard R. Fay, & Arthur N. Popper) 99–159 (Springer New York, 2002).
- 32.Li H, Mizuno N. Single neurons in the spinal trigeminal and dorsal column nuclei project to both the cochlear nucleus and the inferior colliculus by way of axon collaterals: a fluorescent retrograde double-labeling study in the rat. Neurosci. Res. 1997;29:135–142. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(97)00082-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bosman LW, et al. Anatomical pathways involved in generating and sensing rhythmic whisker movements. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2011;5:53. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Podachin VP, Naumova TS, Babichenko II, Kartseva OM. Interaction between auditory and trigeminal afferent volleys in the dorsal cochlear nucleus of rats. Neurophysiology. 1979;11:133–135. doi: 10.1007/BF01064524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Oertel D, Young ED. What's a cerebellar circuit doing in the auditory system? Trends Neurosci. 2004;27:104–110. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2003.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Deschenes M, et al. Inhibition, not excitation, drives rhythmic whisking. Neuron. 2016;90:374–387. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Occelli V, Spence C, Zampini M. Audiotactile interactions in temporal perception. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2011;18:429–454. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0070-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marks KL, et al. Auditory-somatosensory bimodal stimulation desynchronizes brain circuitry to reduce tinnitus in guinea pigs and humans. Sci. Transl. Med. 2018 doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.aal3175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ro T, Hsu J, Yasar NE, Elmore LC, Beauchamp MS. Sound enhances touch perception. Exp. Brain Res. 2009;195:135–143. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-1759-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Zeng C, Yang Z, Shreve L, Bledsoe S, Shore S. Somatosensory projections to cochlear nucleus are upregulated after unilateral deafness. J. Neurosci. 2012;32:15791–15801. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2598-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Tzounopoulos T, Kim Y, Oertel D, Trussell LO. Cell-specific, spike timing-dependent plasticities in the dorsal cochlear nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 2004;7:719–725. doi: 10.1038/nn1272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Koehler SD, Shore SE. Stimulus-timing dependent multisensory plasticity in the guinea pig dorsal cochlear nucleus. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e59828. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0059828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Populin LC, Yin TC. Bimodal interactions in the superior colliculus of the behaving cat. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:2826–2834. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-07-02826.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.