Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2021 Jan 14;45(4):758–765. doi: 10.1038/s41366-020-00735-9

Appetite disinhibition rather than hunger explains genetic effects on adult BMI trajectory

Eric J Brunner 1,#, Koutatsu Maruyama 1,2,3,✉,#, Martin Shipley 1, Noriko Cable 1, Hiroyasu Iso 2, Ayako Hiyoshi 4, Daryth Stallone 5, Meena Kumari 1,6, Adam Tabak 1,7,8, Archana Singh-Manoux 1,9, John Wilson 10, Claudia Langenberg 11, Nick Wareham 11, David Boniface 1, Aroon Hingorani 12, Mika Kivimäki 1, Clare Llewellyn 1
PMCID: PMC8005371  EMSID: EMS114689  PMID: 33446837

Abstract

Background/objectives

The mediating role of eating behaviors in genetic susceptibility to weight gain during mid-adult life is not fully understood. This longitudinal study aims to help us understand contributions of genetic susceptibility and appetite to weight gain.

Subjects/methods

We followed the body-mass index (BMI) trajectories of 2464 adults from 45 to 65 years of age by measuring weight and height on four occasions at 5-year intervals. Genetic risk of obesity (gene risk score: GRS) was ascertained, comprising 92 BMI-associated single-nucleotide polymorphisms and split at a median (=high and low risk). At the baseline, the Eating Inventory was used to assess appetite-related traits of ‘disinhibition’, indicative of opportunistic eating or overeating and ‘hunger’ which is susceptibility to/ability to cope with the sensation of hunger. Roles of the GRS and two appetite-related scores for BMI trajectories were examined using a mixed model adjusted for the cohort effect and sex.

Results

Disinhibition was associated with higher BMI (β = 2.96; 95% CI: 2.66–3.25 kg/m2), and accounted for 34% of the genetically-linked BMI difference at age 45. Hunger was also associated with higher BMI (β = 1.20; 0.82–1.59 kg/m2) during mid-life and slightly steeper weight gain, but did not attenuate the effect of disinhibition.

Conclusions

Appetite disinhibition is most likely to be a defining characteristic of genetic susceptibility to obesity. High levels of appetite disinhibition, rather than hunger, may underlie genetic vulnerability to obesogenic environments in two-thirds of the population of European ancestry.

Subject terms: Lifestyle modification, Epidemiology

Introduction

Obesity is a global health problem [1, 2] and the modern environment contributes importantly to this epidemic [35]. Little is known about individual susceptibility to the obesogenic environment. Adiposity is partly heritable [6] and many adiposity-associated genetic variants have been identified [7]. High genetic risk of obesity is manifested among children, linked with large appetite, eating in the absence of hunger, [8] a weak sense of satiety [9, 10], and high energy intake [11]. Even in early infancy, very young children with a larger appetite gain weight faster [1214]. In adults, elevated genetic risk of obesity is also associated with appetite, blunted satiety [15], and high responsiveness to external food cues [1619]. However, it is not known whether genetic risk of weight gain continues to operate via appetite mechanisms during adulthood because there are no longitudinal studies of genetic risk and appetite-related phenotypes.

We conducted a longitudinal study to identify the roles of genetic risk of obesity, appetite-related traits for the 20-year weight gain during mid-life. We examined clinically assessed body-mass index (BMI) trajectories in men and women from age 45 to 65 with the UK civil service, i.e., Whitehall II cohort according to gene risk score, and two appetite-related traits, using a multi-item eating behavior questionnaire [20].

Subjects and methods

Study population

Individuals were recruited to the Whitehall II cohort study in 1985–1988 from civil servants working in the London offices of 20 Whitehall departments in London [21]. 10,308 individuals participated, with a response rate of 73%. In 1997–1999, the baseline for this study, 6551 individuals participated in the research clinic for medical check-up. At the 2003–2004 clinic, participants of European ancestry provided blood, which was used for genotyping BMI-associated SNPs. We analyzed 1896 men and 568 women aged 45–65 years at baseline with complete data available on genes, eating behaviors and BMI (collected at clinic phases 5, 7, 9, and 11, 1997–2013; see Fig. S1). The University College London Research Ethics Committee approved the study. Participants gave informed consent at each data collection.

Appetite-related trait assessment

Two appetite-related traits, hunger and disinhibition, were assessed through self-report responses to Stunkard and Messick’s Eating Inventory (EI), administered at baseline [20]. Hunger is defined as the internal physiological urge or drive to eat, relating to susceptibility to and ability to cope with the sensation of hunger [22]. Appetite disinhibition is defined as the opportunistic eating or overeating response to environmental and emotional cues. Disinhibition may involve several behavioral traits: high responsiveness to external food cues e.g., sight of attractive foods on display in a shop, smelling hot, freshly-prepared foods; eating in the absence of hunger; eating in an uncontrolled way (binge eating); over-eating in social situations. Emotionally cued eating concerns eating in response to low mood, depression or anxiety; eating as a response to loneliness.

The EI includes assessment of disinhibition and hunger with good reliability and validity (see Supplementary Text) [20]. Internal consistency of the disinhibition and hunger scales was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha in the present sample (Disinhibition: men 0.77, women 0.84; hunger: men 0.72, women 0.76). Disinhibition (16 items). Hunger (14 items). Up to one missing item response per subscale was allowed, scaling up the score proportionally. Among the 6551 individuals who participated at the clinic screening in 1997–1999, the EI was administered to 5308 (81%) (Fig. S1). Among those of European ethnicity (N = 4925), the EI was fully completed and appetite traits ascertained for 4794 (97%) participants.

Adiposity measurement

BMI was calculated as weight(kg)/height(m)2 based on clinic measurements following standard protocols [21]. BMI was assessed four times at 5-year intervals (1997–99, 2002–04, 2007–09 and 2012–2013). Among the 2464 participants, the percentage of participants who had BMI was 88% in 1997–99, 99% in 2002–04, 92% in 2007–09, and 84% in 2012–2013, respectively (Fig. S1). We also identified maximum BMI during follow-up, for the purpose of a sensitivity analysis (see Statistical Analysis section for detail).

Gene risk score

DNA was extracted from blood samples collected in the 2003–2004 phase 7 clinic using magnetic bead technology. Genotyping with the metabochip, a custom Illumina iSelect genotyping array, was successful for 5441 participants (78.7% of 6914 phase 7 participants) of whom 5067 reported European ethnicity [23]. We measured 92 of 97 independent obesity-related SNPs previously associated with BMI in GWAS [7]. The genotype frequencies did not deviate from Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (p > 0.05). We calculated a genetic risk score (GRS) for all available BMI-related loci on each individual, based on an additive model of genetic risk. We split the GRS at the median (high vs. low).

Covariates

Age and sex were reported by participants. For all analyses, age was centered at 60 years. Birth year was centered at 1940 to take account of the secular trend in BMI changes [24].

Study design overview

We used longitudinal modelling to analyze trajectories of weight gain between ages 45 and 65 years with BMI as outcome (dependent variable). After age 65, mean BMI plateaued and then tended to decline. Three risk factors were examined separately in relation to age 45 (baseline) BMI and BMI change: (1) gene risk score (2) hunger, and (3) disinhibition. For the second part of the study we conducted mediation analysis to compare the extent to which the two appetite traits (hunger, disinhibition) accounted statistically for the effect of gene risk score on baseline BMI and BMI trajectory.

Statistical analysis

Linear mixed effect models were fitted to examine heterogeneity in BMI trajectories by age, taking into account effects of sex, year of birth and linear and curvilinear (quadratic) effects over the follow-up. To examine BMI trajectories according to GRS and appetite-related traits we dichotomized the scores as either low or high according to the median values using the maximum number of participants available. The effect of GRS and appetite-related traits on BMI trajectories was examined by fitting the interaction of the linear term for age with each of the dichotomized exposures of interest. The estimates from these models were used to plot BMI trajectories for participants with low and high GRS and appetite-related traits.

After that, the effects of the appetite-related traits on the association of GRS with BMI trajectory intercept were estimated using the equation ((β1 – β0)/β0) × 100%, where β0 is the coefficient for GRS in a model with covariates and β1 is the coefficient for GRS in a model with covariates + continuous appetite-related score. We calculated 95% CI for the attenuations, i.e., mediation using the bootstrap method with 2000 re-samplings to examine the role of appetite traits.

Further, we examined the joint effect of the two appetite traits by creating a variable with three levels according to the number of times (0, 1 or 2) participants were in the high category for the two appetite traits (Supplementary Text). Also, using just one observation per participant, we estimated the effects of appetite-related traits on the associations of GRS with the maximum BMI attained during follow-up using multiple regression and used the bootstrap method with 2000 re-samplings to calculate 95% CI for these attenuations as a sensitivity analysis (Supplementary files). Probability values for statistical tests were 2-tailed. All analyses were conducted using STATA 15.1 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

Genetic data were available, and GRS known on 2464 (51%) of the participants who completed the EI. A greater proportion of those with known, compared to unknown, GRS were men (77% vs. 68%) and they were 0.8 years younger (p < 0.001), but baseline BMI did not differ between groups (both 26.1 kg/m2, p = 0.65). Mean baseline BMI, disinhibition and hunger was 25.7 kg/m2, 4.38 and 3.37 in the low genetic risk group and 26.6 kg/m2, 4.95 and 3.62 in the high-risk group (Table 1). There were larger proportions of participants with high scores for disinhibition and hunger in the high (55.3%, 42.5%, respectively) compared to the low genetic risk group (48.3%, 38.2% respectively).

Table 1.

Baseline characteristics by gene risk score for adiposity.

Gene risk score
Low High P value
Number of participants 1329 1135
Men (%) 78.4 75.2 0.063
Age, Mean (SD) 55.4 (6.0) 55.5 (6.0) 0.82
BMI, Mean (SD) 25.7 (3.7) 26.6 (3.8) <0.0001
Gene risk score, Mean (SD) 84.5 (3.9) 94.1 (3.6) <0.0001
Disinhibition, Mean (SD) 4.38 (3.11) 4.95 (3.42) <0.0001
High disinhibition (%) 48.3 55.3 0.0005
Hunger, Mean (SD) 3.37 (2.59) 3.62 (2.73) 0.018
High hunger (%) 38.2 42.5 0.032

High categories defined as being above the median score in the whole cohort.

Table 2 and Fig. 1 show the BMI trajectories from age 45 to 65 years, predicted with linear mixed models of genetic risk and appetite traits. On average, BMI increased by 2.1 kg/m2 in men and 2.6 kg/m2 in women during the observation; 6.1 kg (13 lb) and 7.5 kg (17 lb) respectively for an adult 1.7 m (5′7′′) tall. BMI trajectories were curvilinear and increased in parallel to age 65 for high and low levels of genetic risk (Fig. 1A; age interaction p = 0.54).

Table 2.

Coefficients from linear mixed models of BMI trajectory comparing higha vs. low gene risk score and eating behaviors (disinhibition and hunger) and their interactions with age, for BMI measurements at ages 45–65 years. (N = 2464).

Coefficientb (95% CI) P value
High gene risk scorec 0.88 (0.56, 1.20) <0.0001
Interaction: High gene risk score × age 0.005 (−0.010, 0.019) 0.54
High disinhibitionc 2.96 (2.66, 3.25) <0.0001
Interaction: High disinhibition × age 0.005 (−0.009, 0.020) 0.49
High hungerc 1.20 (0.82, 1.59) <0.0001
Interaction: High hunger × age 0.023 (0.008, 0.038) 0.002

aHigh categories defined as being above the median score in the whole cohort.

bCoefficients are adjusted for sex, linear (age) and curvilinear (age squared) trend and birth cohort effect on BMI trajectories.

cEffects of high gene risk score, disinhibition and hunger shown for age 45.

Fig. 1. BMI trajectories to age 65, predictions from longitudinal model with gene risk score and each eating behavior. (N = 2464).

Fig. 1

Error bars show 95% CI around fixed effects. Orange dashed line indicates those with gene risk score or eating behavior above median; Green solid line indicates those with gene risk score or eating behavior below median. BMI values are adjusted for sex and birth cohort effects.

We found that high genetic risk was associated with higher average BMI from 45 to 65 years (difference 0.88 kg/m2). In regard to appetite traits, the high disinhibition group had substantially higher average BMI from 45 to 65 years (difference 2.96 kg/m2). BMI trajectories increased in parallel in the high and low disinhibition groups (Fig. 1B; age interaction p = 0.49). High hunger was also associated with higher BMI, but relatively smaller increase (difference 1.20 kg/m2) at age 45. BMI increased faster in the high hunger group (0.023 kg/m2 larger increase in BMI per year over follow-up) compared to the low hunger group, between ages 45 and 65 (Fig. 1C; age interaction p = 0.0021). The results were unchanged in a mutually adjusted model that included disinhibition and hunger scores together, and when using BMI measurements at all ages >45 years (Table S1). We examined the extent to which appetite-related traits accounted for the association between GRS and average BMI from 45 to 65 years of age, applying mediation analysis (Table S2). Disinhibition level accounted for 33.7% (95% confidence interval 29.1–42.9%) of the GRS-BMI association (Fig. 2). There was no further attenuation when hunger was added into the model, indicating that the association between genetic risk and average BMI in mid-life was mediated only by disinhibition.

Fig. 2. Mediation analysis of gene risk score difference in BMI by appetite-related traits in adults aged 45–65 years. (N = 2464).

Fig. 2

The proportion of BMI differences according to genetic risk score explained by disinhibition and hunger was estimated by adding each appetite-related trait to a model for BMI, separately and simultaneously. The 95% confidence intervals for the mediation proportions were estimated using a bootstrap procedure. The model was adjusted for sex, linear and curvilinear age (age2), birth cohort effect on BMI, and the eating behavior *age interaction. Disinhibition level accounted for 33.7% (95% confidence interval 29.1–42.9%) of the GRS-BMI association.

The joint effect of appetite traits model also confirmed that the combination of high hunger and high disinhibition (0, 1, 2) and BMI trajectories were associated with the highest and steepest BMI trajectories from age 45 to 65 (interaction between appetite combination group and age, p < 0.0001) (Fig. S2). Results in the sensitivity analyses were similar using measures of BMI at all ages >45 years (Table S3) and using maximum BMI, supporting the attenuating role of the appetite disinhibition in relation to GRS and BMI (Table S4).

Discussion

BMI increased during mid-life, with an average 20-year gain to age 65 of 2.1 and 2.6 kg/m2 in men and women respectively. The associations between genetic risk, disinhibition and BMI were established age 45. Disinhibited eating was a defining characteristic of adults with higher BMI throughout mid-life, and of those with higher genetic susceptibility to adiposity.

Among those scoring above the median on disinhibition, average BMI was nearly 3.0 units higher than those with low disinhibition, equating to a difference of 8.6 kg (or 19 lbs.) for an adult of average height (1.7 m). Disinhibition accounted for approximately one-third of the association between genetic susceptibility to obesity and average BMI during mid-life. We observed that high hunger was associated with higher BMI at age 45 and BMI increased faster in the high hunger group compared to the low hunger group. Nevertheless, hunger had no significant and independent explanatory effect in our mediation analysis. Based on our findings, we think appetite disinhibition—overeating in response to external food cues—is a focal behavior underpinning genetic variation in susceptibility to the obesogenic environment.

Findings in context

Disinhibited or uncontrolled eating is a characteristic of adults at higher genetic risk of obesity [1619]. Behavioral studies have shown aberrations in neurobehavioral markers of appetite, including the tendency to eat in the absence of hunger, and hyper-responsiveness to food cues among children [8, 11] and adults [25] according to genetic susceptibility to obesity. Gene expression studies support that many BMI-linked common genetic variants are expressed in brain areas with appetite regulation functions [7].

There is consistent longitudinal evidence from birth cohort studies that genetic risk of obesity is linked with weight gain throughout infancy and childhood [2628]. Previous findings showed that the length of the period of genetic effects in early adult life was likely subject to considerable individual variation [2931]. However, in our study, a difference in mean BMI of 0.88 kg/m2 between high and low genetic risk groups was established even after age 45 and it was stable at the age group level over the subsequent two decades.

Our statistical mediation models provide strongly that the level of disinhibition of appetite in response to environmental stimulus helps in understanding the nature of the global epidemic of obesity. The individual disinhibition score mediated around one-third of the genetic association with adiposity. On the other hand, the score on the internal physiological hunger trait scale, corresponding to the intrinsic urge to eat, accounted for none of that association in the mediation model.

Disinhibition is a tendency to want to eat in response to the sight, smell or taste of palatable food [20]. Its expression is likely strongly dependent on the food environment, and the current ubiquity of external food cues provides the ideal context for full expression of this trait. The link between appetite disinhibition and adiposity is congruent with findings in infants and children [1214], to which our findings offer extensive support. The disinhibition scale also measures the extent to which emotional reactions lead to overeating [20]. Several studies have shown obesity-related risk alleles were associated with emotional eating or emotional problems [32, 33], but the findings are controversial. Therefore, emotional eating might partly explain the association between obesity-related risk alleles and BMI trajectory.

Hunger, in contrast to the disinhibition component of appetite, is the drive to eat governed by internal neurobehavioral processes, as distinct from the desire to eat in response to external food cues [20, 22]. Eating behaviors exhibit long-term stability [34, 35]; we also observed that higher hunger scores were associated with higher BMI in mid-life and slightly greater weight gain equating to 0.5 BMI units over 20 years from age 45. Nevertheless, we did not find the significant role played by hunger compared to disinhibition. The declining rate of weight gain during later adulthood in our findings suggests that hunger may decline incrementally from mid-life to older ages.

Longitudinal data on genetic influences on clinically measured BMI trajectories from mid-life to older age are rare [2931, 36, 37], and there have been no longitudinal studies to date incorporating appetite-related traits in mid-life [3843]. The measured appetite traits, mainly disinhibition accounted moderately. Although appetite disinbition did not account fully for genetic susceptibility to high BMI, our findings point the potential relevance of appetite-related traits such as satiety sensitivity.

Strengths and limitations

We used a well-validated instrument to assess eating inventories in our study sample. BMI was measured on four occasions by a research nurse adhering to a standard protocol. With 20 years of longitudinal observation we were able to assess how gene risk score and eating inventories were independently and jointly related to BMI trajectory in a large sample of ethnically white adults. Among 2464 participants, the proportion with a BMI measurement was high at each phase (Fig S1). The high participation rate, with little drop-out over the two decades of the study, indicates that the BMI trajectories are subject to little health selection bias. However, the EI was administered only at baseline and we could not take behavioral change into account. It would be useful to extend this line of research to examine the dynamic associations between genetic risk, eating behavior, and weight change.

Genotyping was based on DNA collected at a later clinic phase than the study baseline (Fig S1) and the sample size was reduced because genotype data were missing. Potential selection bias was assessed by comparing cross-sectional correlations of disinhibition and hunger scores with BMI, and age trajectories of BMI, between those in the study sample and those excluded (Fig S3). The correlation coefficient between disinhibition and BMI was 0.46 in the study sample and r = 0.41 in those excluded (difference p = 0.08). The respective coefficients for hunger were 0.24 and 0.22 (difference p = 0.50). The lack of difference in these associations between those with and without GRS suggests the main associations of interest are not biased by the incomplete genotype data.

Whitehall II is an occupational cohort of British civil servants [22]. We consider that the associations between obesity-related genes and BMI trajectories, and the role of appetite traits in mediating the gene-BMI association is not substantially different from the general population. However, replication studies, including children and young adults as well as older adults, of the independent and joint effects of obesity-related genes and appetite disinhibition in relation to BMI trajectories would be useful.

Implications

Overweight and obesity can be a result from a combination of genetic susceptibility to overeating and exposure to an appetite facilitating food environment, i.e., food cues. Changes to the environment to reduce the frequency and intensity of food cues may have a positive impact in a population, shifting the distribution of BMI to a lower level. Among individuals, guidance about healthy food choices is important. It could be enhanced by emphasizing the particular value for disinhibited eaters of avoiding cues to overeat. Such cues may be direct, e.g., food on display, or indirect signals of food availability e.g., retail stores and product advertisements. Experimental studies show that such indirect cues increase motivation to actively seek out and consume food in the absence of hunger [44].

For individuals, the predictive value of genetic risk information for obesity is modest. Further, our findings suggest that appetite, especially disinhibition, is an important marker of obesity risk providing a pointer to tailored intervention. Measuring disinhibition and identifying problematic eating behaviors and environmental triggers could allow clinical advisors to support their patients to develop strategies to manage eating behavior and achieve weight-loss goals.

Conclusion

Appetite disinhibition appears to be a key characteristic, for adults at high genetic risk of obesity, given the epidemic of excessive weight gain. The trait of low appetite disinhibition may provide a clue to the elusive reason why some adults remain lean, despite exposure to obesogenic environments, to which so many are susceptible. Modification of the wider food environment is likely to be a valuable public health endeavor. Our findings suggest a potential clinical strategy which includes support for individuals with high appetite disinhibition to avoid food cues, in addition to their genetic risk status.

Supplementary information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION (115.4KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

We thank all of the participating civil service departments and their welfare, personnel, and establishment officers; the British Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the British Council of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in the Whitehall II study; and all members of the Whitehall II study team. The Whitehall II Study team comprises research scientists, statisticians, study coordinators, nurses, data managers, administrative assistants and data entry staff, who make the study possible.

Funding

The Whitehall II study has been supported by grants from the British Heart Foundation (PG/11/63/29011, RG/13/2/30098, RG/16/11/32334), UK Medical Research Council (K013351, R024227, S011676), US National Institute on Aging ((R01AG056477, R01AG034454) and US National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (R01HL036310). EB is supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ES/T014377/1). NW and CL are supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MC_UU_12015/1).

Author contributions

Conceptualization, EB, DS and MS; Methodology, EB, MS, AT, DB and MKi; Investigation, EB, DS, MKu and AH; Writing—Original Draft, EB, KM and CLl; Writing—Review & Editing, ASM, JW, CL, NW, AH, HI and MKi; Project Administration, NC, AHi; Funding Acquisition, EB, MKu, ASM, AH and MKi.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

These authors contributed equally: Eric J. Brunner, Koutatsu Maruyama

Change history

2/1/2021

A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1038/s41366-021-00770-0

Supplementary information

The online version of this article (10.1038/s41366-020-00735-9) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.

References

  • 1.Global BMI Mortality Collaboration. Di Angelantonio E, Bhupathiraju SHN, Wormser D, Gao P, Kaptoge S, Berrington de Gonzalez A, et al. Angelantonio and the Global BMI Mortality Collaboration. Body-mass index and all-cause mortality: individual-participant-data meta-analysis of 239 prospective studies in four continents. Lancet. 2016;388:776–86. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)30175-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Twig G, Yaniv G, Levine H, Leiba A, Goldberger N, Derazne E, et al. Body-Mass Index in 2.3 Million Adolescents and Cardiovascular Death in Adulthood. N Engl J Med. 2016;374:2430–40. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1503840. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hill JO, Peters JC. Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic. Science. 1998;280:1371–4. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5368.1371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rodgers A, Woodward A, Swinburn B, Dietz WH. Prevalence trends tell us what did not precipitate the US obesity epidemic. Lancet. Public Health. 2018;3:e162–e163. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(18)30021-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, et al. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. Lancet. 2011;378:804–14. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60813-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Elks CE, den Hoed M, Zhao JH, Sharp SJ, Wareham NJ, Loos RJ, et al. Variability in the heritability of body mass index: a systematic review and meta-regression. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) 2012;3:29. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2012.00029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Locke AE, Kahali B, Berndt SI, Justice AE, Pers TH, Day FR, et al. Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature. 2015;518:197–206. doi: 10.1038/nature14177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wardle J, Llewellyn C, Sanderson S, Plomin R. The FTO gene and measured food intake in children. Int J Obes (Lond) 2009;33:42–45. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Llewellyn CH, Trzaskowski M, van Jaarsveld CHM, Plomin R, Wardle J. Satiety mechanisms in genetic risk of obesity. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168:338–44. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.4944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Wardle J, Carnell S, Haworth CM, Farooqi IS, O’Rahilly S, Plomin R. Obesity associated genetic variation in FTO is associated with diminished satiety. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2008;93:3640–3. doi: 10.1210/jc.2008-0472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cecil JE, Tavendale R, Watt P, Hetherington MM, Palmer CN. An obesity-associated FTO gene variant and increased energy intake in children. N Engl J Med. 2008;359:2558–66. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0803839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Quah PL, Chan YH, Aris IM, Pang WW, Toh JY, Tint MT, et al. Prospective associations of appetitive traits at 3 and 12 months of age with body mass index and weight gain in the first 2 years of life. BMC Pediatr. 2015;15:153. doi: 10.1186/s12887-015-0467-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Steinsbekk S, Wichstrøm L. Predictors of Change in BMI From the Age of 4 to 8. J Pediatr Psychol. 2015;40:1056–64. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsv052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.van Jaarsveld CH, Boniface D, Llewellyn CH, Wardle J. Appetite and growth: a longitudinal sibling analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168:345–50. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.4951. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.den Hoed M, Smeets AJ, Veldhorst MA, Nieuwenhuizen AG, Bouwman FG, Heidema AG, et al. SNP analyses of postprandial responses in (an)orexigenic hormones and feelings of hunger reveal long-term physiological adaptations to facilitate homeostasis. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:1790–8. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cornelis MC, Rimm EB, Curhan GC, Kraft P, Hunter DJ, Hu FB, et al. Obesity susceptibility loci and uncontrolled eating, emotional eating and cognitive restraint behaviors in men and women. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2014;22:E135–41. doi: 10.1002/oby.20592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.de Lauzon-Guillain B, Clifton EA, Day FR, Clément K, Brage S, Forouhi NG, et al. Mediation and modification of genetic susceptibility to obesity by eating behaviors. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;106:996–1004. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.157396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jacob R, Drapeau V, Tremblay A, Provencher V, Bouchard C, Pérusse L. The role of eating behavior traits in mediating genetic susceptibility to obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2018;108:445–52. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Konttinen H, Llewellyn C, Wardle J, Silventoinen K, Joensuu A, Männistö S, et al. Appetitive traits as behavioural pathways in genetic susceptibility to obesity: a population-based cross-sectional study. Sci Rep. 2015;5:14726. doi: 10.1038/srep14726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Stunkard AJ, Messick S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res. 1985;29:71–83. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Marmot M, Brunner E. Cohort Profile: the Whitehall II study. Int J Epidemiol. 2005;34:251–6. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyh372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mayer J, Thomas DW. Regulation of food intake and obesity. Science. 1967;156:328–37. doi: 10.1126/science.156.3773.328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Talmud PJ, Drenos F, Shah S, Shah T, Palmen J, Verzilli C, et al. Gene-centric association signals for lipids and apolipoproteins identified via the HumanCVD BeadChip. Am J Hum Genet. 2009;85:628–42. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2009.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hulmán A, Tabák AG, Nyári TA, Vistisen D, Kivimäki M, Brunner EJ, et al. Effect of secular trends on age-related trajectories of cardiovascular risk factors: the Whitehall II longitudinal study 1985-2009. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:866–77. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Karra E, O’Daly OG, Choudhury AI, Yousseif A, Millership S, Neary MT, et al. A link between FTO, ghrelin, and impaired brain food-cue responsivity. J. Clin. Invest. 2013;123:3539–51. doi: 10.1172/JCI44403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Elks CE, Loos RJ, Sharp SJ, Langenberg C, Ring SM, Timpson NJ, et al. Genetic markers of adult obesity risk are associated with greater early infancy weight gain and growth. PLoS Med. 2010;7:e1000284. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Elks CE, Loos RJ, Hardy R, Wills AK, Wong A, Wareham NJ, et al. Adult obesity susceptibility variants are associated with greater childhood weight gain and a faster tempo of growth: the 1946 British Birth Cohort Study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;95:1150–6. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.027870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dahl AK, Reynolds CA, Fall T, Magnusson PK, Pedersen NL. Multifactorial analysis of changes in body mass index across the adult life course: a study with 65 years of follow-up. Int J Obes (Lond) 2014;38:1133–41. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2013.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Song M, Zheng Y, Qi L, Hu FB, Chan AT, Giovannucci EL. Longitudinal Analysis of Genetic Susceptibility and BMI Throughout Adult Life. Diabetes. 2018;67:248–55. doi: 10.2337/db17-1156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hertel JK, Johansson S, Sonestedt E, Jonsson A, Lie RT, Platou CG, et al. FTO, type 2 diabetes, and weight gain throughout adult life: a meta-analysis of 41,504 subjects from the Scandinavian HUNT, MDC, and MPP studies. Diabetes. 2011;60:1637–44. doi: 10.2337/db10-1340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sandholt CH, Allin KH, Toft U, Borglykke A, Ribel-Madsen R, Sparso T, et al. The effect of GWAS identified BMI loci on changes in body weight among middle-aged Danes during a five-year period. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2014;22:901–8. doi: 10.1002/oby.20540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Velders FP, De Wit JE, Jansen PW, Jaddoe VW, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, et al. FTO at rs9939609, food responsive-ness, emotional control and symptoms of ADHD in preschool children. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e49131. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Choquette AC, Lemieux S, Tremblay A, Drapeau V, Bouchard C, Vohl MC, et al. GAD2 gene sequence variations are associated with eating behaviors and weight gain in women from the Quebec family study. Physiol Behav. 2009;98:505–10. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ashcroft J, Semmler C, Carnell S, van Jaarsveld CH, Wardle J. Continuity and stability of eating behaviour traits in children. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2008;62:985–90. doi: 10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Craigie AM, Lake AA, Kelly SA, Adamson AJ, Mathers JC. Tracking of obesity-related behaviours from childhood to adulthood: A systematic review. Maturitas. 2011;70:266–84. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2011.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Rukh G, Ahmad S, Ericson U, Hindy G, Stocks T, Renström F, et al. Inverse relationship between a genetic risk score of 31 BMI loci and weight change before and after reaching middle age. Int J Obes (Lond) 2016;40:252–9. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Vimaleswaran KS, Ängquist L, Hansen RD, van der A DL, Bouatia-Naji N, Holst C, et al. Association between FTO variant and change in body weight and its interaction with dietary factors: the DiOGenes study. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2012;20:1669–74. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.de Lauzon-Guillain B, Basdevant A, Romon M, Karlsson J, Borys JM, Charles MA, FLVS Study Group. Is restrained eating a risk factor for weight gain in a general population. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83:132–8. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/83.1.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Drapeau V, Provencher V, Lemieux S, Després JP, Bouchard C, Tremblay A. Do 6-y changes in eating behaviors predict changes in body weight? Results from the Québec Family Study. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2003;27:808–14. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hays NP, Bathalon GP, Roubenoff R, McCrory MA, Roberts SB. Eating behavior and weight change in healthy postmenopausal women: results of a 4-year longitudinal study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2006;61:608–15. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.6.608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.van Strien T, Konttinen H, Homberg JR, Engels RC, Winkens LH. Emotional eating as a mediator between depression and weight gain. Appetite. 2016;100:216–24. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Chaput JP, Leblanc C, Pérusse L, Després JP, Bouchard C, Tremblay A. Risk factors for adult overweight and obesity in the Quebec Family Study: have we been barking up the wrong tree. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2009;17:1964–70. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Koenders PG, van Strien T. Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. J Occup Environ Med. 2011;53:1287–93. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31823078a2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Colagiuri B, Lovibond PF. How food cues can enhance and inhibit motivation to obtain and consume food. Appetite. 2015;84:79–87. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION (115.4KB, docx)

Articles from International Journal of Obesity (2005) are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES