Abstract
Long-lasting, romantic partnerships are a universal feature of human societies, but almost as ubiquitous is the risk of instability when one partner strays. Jealous response to the threat of infidelity is well studied, but most empirical work on the topic has focused on a proposed sex difference in the type of jealousy (sexual or emotional) that men and women find most upsetting, rather than on how jealous response varies1,2. This stems in part from the predominance of studies using student samples from industrialized populations, which represent a relatively homogenous group in terms of age, life history stage and social norms3,4. To better understand variation in jealous response, we conducted a 2-part study in 11 populations (1,048 individuals). In line with previous work, we find a robust sex difference in the classic forced-choice jealousy task. However, we also show substantial variation in jealous response across populations. Using parental investment theory, we derived several predictions about what might trigger such variation. We find that greater paternal investment and lower frequency of extramarital sex are associated with more severe jealous response. Thus, partner jealousy appears to be a facultative response, reflective of the variable risks and costs of men’s investment across societies.
One of the essential features of human mating is the prominence of stable, long-lasting partnerships, which in almost every society are socially enforced through the institution of marriage5,6. A widespread feature of marriage is the custom of sexual exclusivity. Despite the near ubiquity of this expectation, marriages are at risk of disruption by extramarital partnerships. Adultery is the most commonly cited reason for divorce across cultures7, and concurrent partnerships are often common8,9. In response to threats of infidelity, humans, like other species with stable partnerships, have evolved adaptations to protect against mate poaching and defection. Some of these behaviours, such as partner concealment, vigilance and sexual coercion, are shared with other species10–12, while others, such as foot binding and purdah, are culturally constructed and unique to humans13,14. Underlying these behaviours is a suite of psychological mechanisms, of which jealousy is one of the most important.
While jealousy itself is thought to be a universal human emotion, traditional evolutionary explanations predict that jealous response to cues of infidelity will differ in men and women, reflecting the unique adaptive problems they face1,13,14. Men face a risk of paternity uncertainty, which results in the loss of a fitness opportunity, but also in the potential misallocation of investment. Conversely, women risk the diversion of critical resources by their partner towards other women and their children. These sex differences in the potential costs of infidelity have led researchers to hypothesize that, when given a choice, men should report being more upset than women by a partner’s sexual infidelity, and that emotional infidelity will be more upsetting to women than to men1. This sex difference has been replicated in multiple studies of US student populations, several cross-cultural studies conducted with university students and a limited number of non-student populations from industrialized nations3,4.
Even with these robust findings, few replication attempts have been conducted outside of W.E.I.R.D. (western, educated, industrial, rich and democratic) societies15, with only one example from a small-scale society16. Conducted with Himba pastoralists in Namibia, this study found the predicted sex difference, but also found that Himba responses did not conform to W.E.I.R.D. standards. The majority of both men and women found sexual infidelity to be more upsetting in a forced-choice scenario, but both sexes also emphasized that infidelity was normatively permitted and that they would not be very upset by either type of infidelity. These findings suggest that jealousy is a facultative response, potentially responsive to local norms and socioecological conditions.
While the suggestion that partner jealousy is facultatively expressed has long been part of evolutionary discussions1,17,18, there is little empirical work attempting to explain cross-cultural variation. Where they exist, they almost always focus on individual-level differences, such as age, relationship experience and sexual orientation4. This is surprising given that the same evolutionary logic that was used to derive the prediction of a sex difference can be employed to generate predictions about variation. One clear example of this is that the level of paternal investment in a given society is expected to influence both men’s and women’s feelings about infidelity. For men, the more they are expected to invest in their wives’ children, the more concerned they should be with her having other partners. For women, a greater dependency on male resources increases the relative cost of diverted investment. Therefore, we expect that greater levels of paternal investment will be associated with more severe ratings of infidelity for both men and women.
Levels of paternal investment are intimately linked to other aspects of social structure and behaviour. Men tend to invest less in systems where the mating–parenting trade-off favours desertion, such as when the adult sex ratio (ASR) is female biased19. More generally, permissive sexual behaviour is more common when men’s investment is less obligatory (for example, in matrilineal systems) or when it is less critical for offspring survival20. Therefore, we expect that a female-biased ASR and social norms that are more permissive of extramarital sex will be associated with less severe jealous response.
We designed a 2-part survey, which was run in 11 populations (Fig. 1). Of these, eight are small-scale societies, spanning five continents and multiple modes of production. The other three populations are urban settings: Los Angeles (CA, USA), urban India (an online sample) and Okinawa, Japan. In part one, all respondents rated both same-sex and opposite-sex scenarios for sexual and emotional infidelity (a total of four ratings per respondent). A five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘very bad’ to ‘very good’ was used to rate each statement. In part two, we presented a replication of the forced-choice vignette originally designed by Buss and colleagues to assess differences in the type of jealousy (emotional and sexual) that is most distressing to men and women21. We tested the prediction that, when asked to think about both a hypothetical sexual and emotional infidelity, men, more than women, will find the sexual infidelity more distressing.
Fig. 1 |. Location of study populations.
Sample size for each population is presented in the boxes, broken out by sex (male/female) (n = 1,048).
To determine whether paternal investment and other related variables are important predictors of jealous response, each of the anthropologists in our study completed a survey about the population with whom they work. The anthropologists all conduct longterm projects, having worked at their sites for an average of 11.5 y (range = 6–17 y), excluding the researchers native to their sites in Japan and the United States. The survey included questions about the ASR, extramarital sex norms and measures of paternal investment. Questions about paternal investment were divided into two categories: (1) direct care, which measured the amount of time that men spend in contact with their children; and (2) provisioning, which measured male contribution to subsistence and resource transfers including marriage payments and inheritance.
The 4 severity ratings from part 1 provide a descriptive picture of how social norms about jealousy and infidelity differ cross-culturally (1,037 of 1,048 participants completed 4,148 ratings). Overall, we found that sexual infidelity, regardless of the sex of the unfaithful agent, is viewed more harshly than emotional infidelity (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 11). Another robust trend is that female infidelity is judged more harshly than male infidelity, regardless of type.
Fig. 2 |. Severity ratings and forced-choice responses by respondent sex and culture.
Rows 1-4 show the percent severity ratings according to Likert scale responses (very bad to very good) for each type of infidelity (n = 1,038). The final row shows the percent of respondents who were more upset by sexual than emotional infidelity when given a forced choice (n = 1,021). LA, Los Angeles.
A multilevel-ordered logit model allows us to evaluate the effects of both the sex of the respondent and the population to which they belong on their responses to the severity scales. Various iterations of the model highlight the explanatory power of culture (as a varying intercept) and sex (with fixed and varying effects by culture) in contributing to model fit (Table 1). The final model, including varying intercepts for culture, and varying slopes by sex, age and marital status by culture (model 5), shows substantially lower out-of-sample deviance than any other model and 100% of the model weight. However, the model that includes only culture (model 3) results in substantially lower widely applicable information criteria (WAIC) scores than do the models including sex as a fixed (model 2) or varying (model 4) effect, indicating that perceptions of infidelity are influenced more by what population a respondent belongs to than by the sex of the respondent. Variance estimates of severity responses indicate higher variance in ratings of sexual severity (male = 0.92; female = 1.01) relative to emotional severity (male = 0.35; female = 0.24) in the best-fit model (Supplementary Fig. 8).
Table 1 |.
Model comparison to predict severity ratings and forced-choice outcome
Model | Parametersa | Severity | Forced choice |
---|---|---|---|
1 | Intercept only | 6,806.3 (0) | 1,307.0 (0) |
2 | Fixed: sex | 6,798.2 (0) | 1,294.5 (0) |
3 | Varying intercept: culture | 6,613.8 (0) | 1,183.7 (0) |
4 | Varying intercept: culture Varying slope: sex |
6,618.0 (0) | 1,155.7 (0.85) |
5 | Varying intercept: culture Varying slope: sex, age and marital status |
6,591.1 (1) | 1,159.1 (0.15) |
Values indicate WAIC (and weight). Sex, age and marital status vary by culture when listed. Severity models include varying intercept by participant ID in all models to correct for repeated observations.
Population-level differences could result from many factors, including sampling variation, random and systematic measurement variation, and ecological or social processes not measured in our study. Here, we focus on three population-level variables: paternal investment, ASR and extramarital sex norms, which evolutionary theory predicts could contribute to the variation that we see here. We found that levels of paternal investment were strongly associated with jealous response to sexual infidelity, but had a smaller and more uncertain effect on respondent’s views of emotional infidelity (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Figs. 3–5). Both male and female sexual infidelity were viewed more harshly as direct care paternal investment scores increased (β = −0.62, 89% prediction interval (PI) = −1.15 to −0.11, β(probability< 0) = 97.2%; β = −0.46, 89% PI = −1.11 to 0.20, β(pr < 0) = 87.4%, respectively), although the distribution of the predictor on female sexual infidelity overlaps with zero. Increased levels of paternal provisioning were associated with harsher views of male sexual infidelity (β = −0.69, 89% PI = −1.24 to −0.16, β(pr < 0) = 97.9%), but not female infidelity (β = −0.30, 89% PI = −0.94 to 0.34, β(pr < 0) = 78.7%) where again the distribution of the predictor overlaps substantially with zero. Male, but not female, emotional infidelity was also viewed more harshly when direct paternal investment and provisioning were high (β = −0.25, 89% PI = −0.56 to 0.05, β(pr < 0) = 91.5%; β = −0.40, 89% PI = −0.74 to −0.05, β(pr < 0) = 96.3%, respectively).
Fig. 3 |. Influence of predictor variables on severity ratings.
Posterior distributions (posterior mean and 89% PI) for individually run models for key predictors of participant ratings on the severity scales. Where the majority of the distribution falls either below or above zero, the predictor is believed to have a meaningful impact. Negative scores indicate greater severity judgements. Both paternal investment and the frequency of extramarital sex are predictive of severity scores for sexual infidelity only (in opposite directions). Full model results are shown in the Supplementary Information.
Next, we modelled the effects of social norms that we expected would be linked to both the level of paternal investment and infidelity, to understand the broader social milieu affecting jealous response. Populations where extramarital sex was more common tended to rate male and female sexual infidelity less severely, as predicted (β = 0.78, 89% PI = 0.23–1.35, β(pr > 0) = 98.3%; β = 0.49, 89% PI = −0.13 to 1.10, β(pr > 0) = 90.1%, respectively; Supplementary Fig. 7). The effect of extramarital sex on perceptions of emotional infidelity had similar results (men: β=0.47, 89% PI = 0.16–0.79, β(pr > 0) = 99.1%; women: β = 0.18, 89% PI = −0.12 to 0.48, β(pr > 0) = 83.6%). In line with our predictions, and existing literature, we found that ASR was negatively correlated with the level of extramarital sex across populations (Pearson’s r = −0.59). Populations with more female-biased sex ratios had greater frequencies of extramarital sex. However, we did not find any meaningful effect of ASR on the severity ratings, although they trended in the expected direction (Supplementary Fig. 6).
When the three predictor variables were analysed together, we found that the effects of paternal investment were most robust (Supplementary Fig. 3), varying little across the models. The effect of extramarital sex appears to be mediated by the level of paternal investment. Similarly, ASR is a meaningful predictor only when all predictors are included. Model comparisons assessing model fit on combinations of these predictors further highlight the importance of paternal investment and the frequency of extramarital sex in predicting severity (Supplementary Table 6).
In part two of our study, the forced-choice vignette, we found strong and consistent support for the prediction that men are more upset by sexual infidelity than are women (Fig. 2). Nine of the 11 populations showed a sex difference in the expected direction. In Yasawa, the effect was reversed, with slightly more women than men stating that sexual infidelity was more upsetting. All of the Tsimane respondents stated that sexual infidelity was more upsetting than emotional infidelity. In addition, we found that, when differences in sample composition are accounted for via multilevel logistic regression, variation in the magnitude of the sex difference (which appears quite variable in the raw data; Fig. 2) is negligible (Supplementary Fig. 12).
When sex and culture are considered together in the forced-choice scenario, we see a more nuanced picture of jealous response. Posterior predictions from the best-fit model (in this case, one that includes both sex and culture; Table 1) illustrate the effects of both sex and sex-by-culture interactions on the probability of being more upset by sexual versus emotional infidelity. While in the majority of populations both men and women were more likely to report sexual infidelity as more upsetting, in others, both men and women were more upset by emotional infidelity (for example, Los Angeles and Mosuo) (Fig. 2).
These results make three important contributions to discussions of jealousy: (1) the sex difference in jealous response is robust and consistent across populations. (2) Sexual infidelity was of much greater concern and had more explanatory power than emotional infidelity in our study. (3) Sociocultural factors, such as the level of paternal investment and norms for extramarital sex, are important contributors to cross-cultural variation in jealous response.
In both the forced-choice and the severity questions, we see important sex differences. Respondents generally view infidelity committed by women more harshly than the same acts committed by men. In the forced choice, we find robust support for the notion of a universal sex difference, with men being more upset than women by sexual infidelity. Furthermore, the magnitude of the sex difference is relatively invariable (Supplementary Fig. 12). The inclusion in our sample of a broad range of populations, including many small-scale societies, leads us to conclude that this finding is not the result of a western bias about mating preferences, but rather reflects important sex-specific influences on partnership dynamics, most notably, the male risk of paternity uncertainty.
We also found variation in the relative importance of sexual and emotional infidelity across cultures. Whereas western conceptions of jealousy focus on the importance of ‘romantic love’, respondents of both sexes in our sample place more emphasis on sexual infidelity. In the forced choice, 7 out of 11 groups had both men and women reporting sexual infidelity as more upsetting, and sexual infidelity was uniformly viewed more harshly than emotional infidelity in the severity ratings (Fig. 2). This trend has been seen in other work, which shows that outside of the forced-choice paradigm, both sexes find sexual infidelity more distressing22. It is important to note that these findings should not be seen as dismissive of the cultural importance of romantic love in our sample. Even in the populations in our sample in which concurrent partnerships are common, there are strong notions of limerence, ‘love match’ marriages, and great value placed on long-lasting emotional bonds23–25.
In particular, the finding that women repeatedly choose sexual infidelity to be more upsetting contrasts with standard interpretations of jealous response in the evolutionary literature. Only 4 of the 11 populations had a majority of women choosing emotional infidelity as more upsetting in the forced choice, and 2 of these were Los Angeles and Okinawa. This indicates that the link between emotional infidelity and the diversion of resources, which has been presented as a ‘cardinal cue’ of resource loss2, could be an artefact of previous biases in studies concentrated on industrialized populations. Opportunistic free response data from two of our populations (Tsimane and Himba) provide examples of places where sexual rather than emotional infidelity is more tightly linked to resource diversion. Several Tsimane women reported that if a man has sex with another woman, his children with his wife would get sick because the man would no longer be caring for those children. They further noted that this would not happen if the husband only had an emotional connection to another woman. Himba women also noted that it was sex, more than love, that was likely to lead to diversion of resources and even divorce. Therefore, while sexual infidelity is likely to be a fairly ubiquitous cue of lost paternity, future researchers should pay more attention to the appropriate local cues of resource diversion when making predictions about jealous response.
Finally, we have identified the level of paternal investment as one critical component of the local socioecology that explains a substantial proportion of the variance in jealous response that we see across cultures. We find that the more that men invest in their children, the more severely people in that culture view infidelity. While men everywhere face the risk of losing biological paternity, the relative costs (to men and women) of extra-pair partnerships are greater when investment is higher. Our findings that higher frequencies of extramarital sex had a dampening effect on jealous response and that populations with female-biased sex ratios had higher levels of extramarital sex are similarly related. Where men’s investment is riskier, or the benefits of desertion are greater, paternal investment should be lower, and the need to enact strong jealous response reduced. Overall, we find that people’s responses to the threat of infidelity appear to reflect locally relevant risks and benefits.
There are several important limitations to doing cross-cultural research, to which our study is not immune. First, there is always a trade-off between internal and external validity in the design of study materials for projects that span multiple sites. While standardization allows for easier and clearer analysis, it is difficult to design a survey that has questions that are equally understandable and relevant in all cultures. In this case, we opted for a short, standardized survey with questions that were as unambiguous as possible. Similarly, we had to use questions about paternal investment that were relevant across cultures. Future studies that focus on a smaller number of populations or look at intra-community variation could utilize more fine-grained variables.
Likert scales and forced-choice questions were more familiar to some of our participants than to others. Ethnographers reported any obstacles they faced in administering the survey (see Supplementary Information), and we perceived no systematic biases that would lead to the results shown here. However, we cannot entirely discount the possibility that some of the cultural variation that we see is due to varying interpretations of the survey. Another potential problem with using Likert scales is the possibility that raters will be biased towards extreme responding. This would be problematic if some populations were more prone to this than others. Using Likert responses on other types of moral transgressions collected for another study in seven of the populations in our sample, we find no evidence that extreme responding systematically affected our results (Supplementary Fig. 19).
Third, while we aimed to include cultures that represent a wide range of practices relevant to jealous response, there were some ways in which our populations were similar. For example, only the Himba practice extensive polygyny. In addition, the fact that our sample spanned a broad range of parenting practices and obligations precluded a deeper investigation into the importance of particular types of paternal investment (for example, the amount of brideprice paid). These could be better studied by choosing a set of cultures that all share a particular system but vary in the amount of expected investment. Relatedly, our data are correlational and cannot determine the causal relationship between paternal investment and jealous response, nor do we believe that the directionality of these relationships will necessarily be uniform across populations. Our study was also framed in a heteronormative way, and we want to acknowledge the important work looking at the effects of gender identity and sexual orientation on jealous response26,27, which we did not address here.
Finally, while efforts were made to collect data on a broad swath of each population, we used convenience samples, and as such, they are not necessarily representative of the populations at large. Measured differences between cultures may be the result of local norms at work, including the socioecological predictors used here. In addition, variation could be the result of sample variation in characteristics such age or marital status. Our statistical approach attempts to correct for variance in sample sizes as well as underlying demographic characteristics, but error as a result of convenience sampling remains a concern.
Our results point to the importance of studying universals and variation in conjunction with one another. While we find a nearly ubiquitous sex difference in the type of jealousy that men and women find most upsetting, the rest of our results emphasize the importance of culture in producing and maintaining variation in jealous response. As opposed to the predominant emphasis on sex differences in the existing literature, we highlight the similarity between men and women within the same culture and emphasize the importance of between culture differences in norms about jealousy and infidelity. Evolutionary theory can help us to go beyond the simple finding that culture produces variation to generate predictions about the particular socioecological conditions that contribute to variation28. Here, we find strong support that the level of paternal investment (a reflection of differing mating—parenting trade-offs across societies) is one such variable.
Methods
All participants provided informed consent. In some cases, participants received small forms of compensation (details are included in the Supplementary Information). Ethical approval for this study was granted by the University of California, Los Angeles (no. 10-000238/#10-000253) covering work in Los Angeles, Namibia, China, Japan and India; California State University Fullerton for work in Ecuador (no. 2003505); University of Cincinnati (no. 2016-2377) for work in Nicaragua; Arizona State University (no. 00002770) for work in Fiji; University of New Mexico (no. 10-034) for work in Bolivia; University of Washington (no. 46690) for work in Indonesia; and University of Nevada Las Vegas (no. 783950-1) and the Commission for Science and Technology (COSTECH-2014-372-ER-2000-80) for work in Tanzania.
Sample.
In total, 1,048 individuals across 11 populations were surveyed (1,021 completed the forced-choice question, while 1,037 completed the severity component). Efforts were made to include populations that varied in their level of paternal investment and in their permissiveness towards extramarital sex. We also aimed to only include cultures that the anthropologist had substantial ethnographic knowledge about their population, as we relied on that knowledge to create culture-level variables. The sample from India was collected through an online Amazon Mechanical Turk survey; the ethnographer who implemented this survey and answered the culture-level variables has studied marriage and parental investment in South Asia (including urban South India) for 18 y. All other interviews were conducted in person by an anthropologist and/or, where necessary, local translators who worked as part of the research team. Most samples were evenly split between men and women and included a large proportion of married individuals (Fig. 1). Additional ethnographic and demographic details about each population can be found in the Supplementary Information.
Procedure.
Forced-choice vignette.
We used a standardized vignette with a single forced-choice response question to measure the relative importance of sexual versus emotional infidelity in men and women. In response to critiques that the original jealousy vignette developed by Buss and colleagues could exacerbate existing stereotypes and gendered beliefs about infidelity22, we used a variant of the original vignette, which posits both types of infidelity occurring and asks participants to choose which of the two is more upsetting. Previous studies have shown that this vignette produces results similar to the original while minimizing chances of the ‘double-shot’ effect21. Familiarity with forced-choice scenarios ranged across the populations, and in a few cases, participants were reluctant to make a choice. In total, 22 out of 1,048 respondents refused to answer this part of the survey.
Severity ratings.
Participants were asked to rate the severity of two types of hypothetical infidelity (sexual and emotional) committed by a male and female member of their own culture, for a total of four ratings per respondent. A five-point Likert scale ranging from ‘very bad’ to ‘very good’ was used to rate each statement. For example, the male sexual infidelity statement presented to Himba participants read, “If a Himba man has sex with someone other than his wife, how good or bad is this?”
Ethnographer culture scores.
To test predictions about the effects of norms of paternal investment and sociosexual behaviour on jealous response, each ethnographer completed a survey about their field site. Wherever possible, the survey utilized existing variables, mainly from the Standard Cross-Cultural Sample29. Two measures of paternal investment were used: direct care, which measures everyday care and interactions, and paternal provisioning, which included measures of male contribution to subsistence and other resource transfers. Details on the construction of these measures can be found in the Supplementary Information. Measures of the frequency of male and female extramarital sex were adapted from Broude and Greene30. ASR was measured using demographic data collected by the ethnographers, or in the cases of Yasawa and Los Angeles, where this was not available, regional census data. The survey also included questions about mode of production, marriage and inheritance systems, religiosity and market integration, which are presented in Supplementary Table 1, but were not included in our analyses. Complete survey responses can be found in Supplementary Table 3.
Statistical analysis.
Multilevel logistic regression was used to model the results of the forced-choice vignette (in which more upset by sexual infidelity = 1, emotional infidelity = 0). All severity results (4,148 observations from 1,037 participants) were assessed in an ordered logit model, with varying intercepts by participant, and dummy variables and interaction expressions to allow estimation of effects by severity type. To assess the effects of sex and culture on model fit, model comparisons were run using a null, intercept-only model, and then adding fixed or varying effects to assess model fit (see Table 1 for a description of each model). Culture was added as a varying intercept, and sex was added as both a fixed effect and as a varying slope by culture, to allow sex to have different effects on jealous outcomes in each culture. Finally, marital status and standardized age were added as varying slopes by culture. WAIC were calculated to compare models, which measures out of sample deviance, where the lowest WAIC values indicate the best fit and, therefore, the most accurate model. On the basis of these values, Akaike weights were also calculated, estimating the relative probability that a given model, out of a specified set, will make the best predictions of future data. Additional models were used to assess the effects of paternal investment, extramarital sex and ASR. Predictors were run individually as fixed effects, and then paternal investment variables, sexual infidelity and ASR were run in combination. Here, we report the results of predictors run individually, but results using a combination of predictors are shown in Supplementary Fig. 3. All models utilized regularizing priors and allowed for partial pooling to improve estimates by group, particularly in groups with smaller sample sizes. In addition, because the Indian sample was the only one conducted online, and for reason might be expected to be somewhat aberrant, all models were run excluding India. However, doing this did not cause any major changes to our results, so only the models with India included are presented in the main text. Models were fit to RStan31 using the rethinking package version 1.72 (ref. 32). Results presented here show 89% PI to avoid confusion with significance tests, which is standard with the statistical package. Full model details are available in the Supplementary Information.
Reporting Summary.
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank the communities that we work with for their contributions and continued good will. B.A.S. acknowledges support from a UCLA Faculty Research Grant and NSF-BCS-1534682, the latter of which also funded S.P.P. as a postdoctoral scholar. J.S. acknowledges support from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (ANR)—Labex IAST. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.
Footnotes
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Data availability
The variables used in this study are available at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/tgc95/?view_only=fb78b2eaae344efe95a6fd8b0d80739d.
Code availability
The R code used in our analyses is available at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/tgc95/?view_only=fb78b2eaae344efe95a6fd8b0d80739d.
Additional information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0654-y.
References
- 1.Buss DM, Larsen RJ, Westen D & Semmelroth J Sex differences in jealousy: evolution, physiology, and psychology. Psychol. Sci 3, 251–255 (1992). [Google Scholar]
- 2.Buss DM Sexual and emotional infidelity: evolved gender differences in jealousy prove robust and replicable. Perspect. Psychol. Sci 13, 155–160 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Sagarin BJ et al. Sex differences in jealousy: a meta-analytic examination. Evol. Hum. Behav 33, 595–614 (2012). [Google Scholar]
- 4.Edlund JE & Sagarin BJ Sex differences in jealousy: a 25-year retrospective. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol 55, 259–302 (2017). [Google Scholar]
- 5.Murdock GP Social Structure (Macmillan, 1949). [Google Scholar]
- 6.Walker RS, Hill KR, Flinn MV & Ellsworth RM Evolutionary history of hunter—gatherer marriage practices. PLoS ONE 6, e19066 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Betzig L Causes of conjugal dissolution: a cross-cultural study. Curr. Anthropol. 30, 654–676 (1989). [Google Scholar]
- 8.Broude GJ Extramarital sex norms in cross-cultural perspective. Cross Cult. Res 15, 181–218 (1980). [Google Scholar]
- 9.Scelza BA Choosy but not chaste: multiple mating in human females. Evol. Anthropol. Issues News Rev 22, 259–269 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Alcock J Postinsemination associations between males and females in insects: the mate-guarding hypothesis. Annu. Rev. Entomol 39, 1–21 (1994). [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kokko H & Morrell LJ Mate guarding, male attractiveness, and paternity under social monogamy. Behav. Ecol 16, 724–731 (2005). [Google Scholar]
- 12.Parker GA Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in the insects. Biol. Rev 45, 525–567 (1970). [Google Scholar]
- 13.Daly M, Wilson M & Weghorst SJ Male sexual jealousy. Ethol. Sociobiol 3, 11–27 (1982). [Google Scholar]
- 14.Symons D The Evolution of Human Sexuality. (Oxford Univ. Press: 1979. . [Google Scholar]
- 15.Henrich J, Heine SJ & Norenzayan A The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci 33, 61–83 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Scelza BA Jealousy in a small-scale, natural fertility population: the roles of paternity, investment and love in jealous response. Evol. Hum. Behav 35, 103–108 (2013). [Google Scholar]
- 17.Buunk B & Hupka RB Cross-cultural differences in the elicitation of sexual jealousy. J. Sex Res 23, 12–22 (1987). [Google Scholar]
- 18.Geary DC, Rumsey M, Bow-Thomas CC & Hoard MK Sexual jealousy as a facultative trait: evidence from the pattern of sex differences in adults from China and the United States. Ethol. Sociobiol 16, 355–383 (1995). [Google Scholar]
- 19.Schacht R & Mulder MB Sex ratio effects on reproductive strategies in humans. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2, 140402 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Geary DC Evolution and proximate expression of human paternal investment. Psychol. Bull 126, 55–77 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Buss DM et al. Jealousy and the nature of beliefs about infidelity: tests of competing hypotheses about sex differences in the United States, Korea, and Japan. Pers. Relatsh 6, 125–150 (1999). [Google Scholar]
- 22.DeSteno DA & Salovey P Evolutionary origins of sex differences in jealousy? Questioning the “fitness” of the model. Psychol. Sci 7, 367–372 (1996). [Google Scholar]
- 23.Mattison SM, Scelza B & Blumenfield T Paternal investment and the positive effects of fathers among the matrilineal Mosuo of southwest China. Am. Anthropol 116, 591–610 (2014). [Google Scholar]
- 24.Scelza BA & Prall SP Partner preferences in the context of concurrency: what Himba want in formal and informal partners. Evol. Hum. Behav 39, 212–219 (2018). [Google Scholar]
- 25.Stieglitz J, Gurven M, Kaplan H & Winking J Infidelity, jealousy, and wife abuse among Tsimane forager—farmers: testing evolutionary hypotheses of marital conflict. Evol. Hum. Behav 33, 438–448 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.de Souza AAL, Verderane MP, Taira JT & Otta E Emotional and sexual jealousy as a function of sex and sexual orientation in a Brazilian sample. Psychol. Rep 98, 529–535 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Frederick DA & Fales MR Upset over sexual versus emotional infidelity among gay, lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual adults. Arch. Sex. Behav 45, 175–191 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gurven MD Broadening horizons: sample diversity and socioecological theory are essential to the future of psychological science. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 11420–11427 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Murdock GP & White DR Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology 8, 329–369 (1969). [Google Scholar]
- 30.Broude GJ & Greene SJ Cross-cultural codes on twenty sexual attitudes and practices. Ethnology 15, 409–429 (1976). [Google Scholar]
- 31.Stan Development Team RStan: the R interface to Stan. R package version 2.14.1; (2016). [Google Scholar]
- 32.McElreath R Rethinking: statistical rethinking book package. R package version 1.72; (2016). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.