Skip to main content
JAMA Network logoLink to JAMA Network
. 2021 Mar 29;4(3):e212530. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.2530

Association of Child and Family Attributes With Outcomes in Children With Autism

Peter Szatmari 1,2,, Katherine Tombeau Cost 1, Eric Duku 3, Teresa Bennett 3, Mayada Elsabbagh 4, Stelios Georgiades 3, Connor Kerns 4, Pat Mirenda 5, Isabel M Smith 6, Wendy J Ungar 1, Tracey Vaillancourt 7, Charlotte Waddell 8, Anat Zaidman-Zait 9, Lonnie Zwaigenbaum 10
PMCID: PMC8008283  PMID: 33779740

Key Points

Question

How prevalent is “doing well” in 5 developmental domains (communication, socialization, activities of daily living, internalizing, externalizing) as assessed by proficiency and growth in children with autism spectrum disorder in midchildhood, and what attributes of children and families are associated with it?

Findings

In this cohort study including 272 children, between 20% and 49% of children with autism spectrum disorder were proficient within the 5 developmental domains, while 13% to 34% of children demonstrated growth. Doing well was associated with preschool scores on that specific outcome domain, as well as early language skills, household income, and family functioning.

Meaning

These findings demonstrate the potential usefulness of taking a strengths-based approach to outcome assessments, while the importance of family income and functioning remind us that disabilities do not exist apart from a social context.


This cohort study of children with autism spectrum disorder in Canada assesses the prevalence of positive developmental outcomes using clinical metrics of proficiency and growth and examines associations between outcomes and individual- and family-level variables.

Abstract

Importance

The prevalence and attributes of positive outcomes (or doing well) among children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in midchildhood are not well known.

Objective

To estimate the prevalence of doing well according to metrics of proficiency and growth and to investigate the extent to which significant associations exist between child- and family-level variables and doing well.

Design, Setting, and Participants

This longitudinal cohort study included children with ASD from regional clinics across Canada. Participants were sampled 3 times between ages 2 and 4.9 years (T1) and twice in follow-up into middle childhood (T2). Data were analyzed March 2018 through January 2020.

Exposures

Language and IQ assessments at first sample; household income, parent coping, and family functioning.

Main Outcomes and Measures

Key outcome domains of developmental health included measures of socialization, communication, independent living skills, and measures of internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Thresholds for doing well in these domains by either proficiency or growth were established. The extent to which language, IQ, household income, parent coping, and family functioning were associated with assessed outcomes was determined by logistic regression. The association between outcomes and concurrent Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) classification scores was also estimated.

Results

In a total cohort of 272 children (234 [86.0%] boys; mean [SD] age, 10.76 [0.26] years), approximately 78.8% (95% CI, 73.2%-84.4%) of the sample were estimated to be doing well by either metric on at least 1 domain, and 23.6% (95% CI, 17.7%-29.4%) were doing well in 4 or 5 domains. It was possible to be doing well by either proficiency or growth and still meet ADOS criteria for ASD. For the growth metric, between 61.5% (95% CI, 40.7%-79.1%) and 79.6% (95% CI, 66.0%-88.9%) of participants had ADOS scores of 4 or greater; for the proficiency metric, between 63.8% (95% CI, 48.4%-76.9%) and 75.8% (95% CI, 63.0%-85.4%) had scores of 4 or greater. Doing well by either metric for all domains was associated with T1 scores on that outcome domain (eg, T1 daily living skills associated with doing well at T2 daily living by the proficiency metric as measured by the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition daily living skills scale [202 participants]: β = 0.07; OR, 1.07; 95% CI, 1.03-1.11; P < .001). Doing well in socialization by the growth metric was also associated with better T1 language skills scores (202 participants) (β = 0.04; OR, 1.04; 95% CI, 1.00-1.07, P = .04). Doing well in externalizing by the growth metric was also associated with higher household income at T1 (178 participants) (β = 0.10; OR, 1.10; 95% CI, 1.06-1.15; P < .001). Better family functioning at T1 was associated with doing well on both socialization and externalizing by proficiency metric and on internalizing by growth metric (socialization by proficiency [202 participants]: β = −1.01; OR, 0.36; 95% CI, 0.14-0.93; P = .04; externalizing by proficiency [178 participants]: β = 1.00; OR, 0.37; 95% CI, 0.16-0.82; P = .02; internalizing by growth [178 participants]: β = −1.03; OR, 0.36; 95% CI, 0.16-0.79; P = .01).

Conclusions and Relevance

This cohort study found that a substantial proportion of children with ASD were doing well by middle childhood in at least 1 key domain of developmental health, and that doing well was possible even in the context of continuing to meet criteria for ASD. These results support a strengths-based approach to treatment planning that should include robust support for families to increase the potential likelihood of doing well later in life.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a lifelong condition affecting multiple developmental domains.1 Pathways in ASD2 study researchers held conferences over several years with parents, youth, practitioners, policy makers, and researchers to formulate key evidence gaps that should be addressed by the field. Through these conferences, the group reached consensus that the most important evidence gap in ASD for a Canadian context was, “What factors are associated with good outcomes in ASD?”

This question, to some extent, has been addressed in systematic reviews and meta-analyses of adult outcomes.3,4 In primary studies, investigators used a common outcome tool5,6 combining metrics of attainment of emerging adult milestones in socialization, occupational status, and independent living into 1 score. Adults were stratified into groups ranging from good to poor outcomes. In a 2020 meta-analysis,7 the pooled estimate of a good adult outcome was 20% with substantial heterogeneity between studies.4

The definition of a good outcome for those with ASD at different developmental stages, such as late childhood or adolescence, is less clear. Recent longitudinal follow-up studies in childhood and adolescence have taken a person-centered approach to focus on individual differences in development in people with ASD8,9 or else have used intervention studies to measure relatively short-term outcomes.10 Indeed, much of the literature in this area takes a deficit-based measurement approach of good, best, or optimal outcomes that focuses on an absence of intellectual disability and no longer meeting criteria for ASD.11,12 While this approach has some face validity and has proved enlightening, a more nuanced and holistic approach might provide a more complete picture of outcomes generally (as argued by Mason et al7).

The definition of a good outcome is somewhat more ambiguous now than previously thought. The neurodiversity movement and involvement of those with lived experience13 in advocacy and research have provided much-needed perspectives. Given that ASD is multidimensional and heterogeneous, the developmental domains that should be considered as outcomes and the measurement tools that should be used to assess those domains is an evolving issue.14 Furthermore, consensus is still needed on criteria for defining a good outcome. For example, many individuals with lived experience15 would consider themselves as having a good outcome16,17 despite—or because of—retaining an ASD diagnosis. McCauley et al18 have adopted a novel approach in line with this view with their definition of different types of positive outcomes in adulthood depending on cognitive ability.

For this developmental stage, we prefer the phrase “doing well,” a less value-laden concept than having a good outcome. Specifying an outcome implies a final endpoint, whereas doing well relates to someone’s circumstances at a particular point in their life’s journey. A measurement framework for doing well that can be applied at multiple points in time is important. Taylor19 has outlined 2 possible ways to define doing well in ASD in general terms that address some of the above concerns: (1) “proficiency” sets a specific threshold of competence to be met, regardless of the starting point, whereas (2) “growth” sets a specific amount of improvement between 2 time points. In this sense, a child with ASD can be doing well compared with typically developing children by reaching a level of proficiency, or can be doing well by showing growth. Among other advantages, this approach can be flexibly applied to different developmental domains, measurement tools, developmental stages, and to children with diverse abilities.

Our objective with this study was to apply Taylor’s19 framework of growth and proficiency to children with ASD in middle childhood. Our primary research questions were: (1) What is the prevalence of doing well? and (2) What child- and family-level attributes are associated with doing well in midchildhood? We selected multiple outcome domains based on parent input and the literature. Given the paucity of research on family and contextual factors that might be associated with outcomes in ASD,20 we chose attributes based on the literature or on variables that were potentially modifiable.

Methods

Participants

Recruitment occurred through ASD clinics in Halifax, Montreal, Hamilton, Edmonton, and Vancouver, Canada. Each center provided diagnostic services to nearly all young children suspected of having ASD within administratively defined geographic regions, which included urban and rural communities surrounding those cities. An inception cohort of consecutive cases included children aged 2 to 4.9 years, diagnosed with ASD by agreement between clinical opinion, the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised21 (ADI-R), and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule22 (ADOS). Children included in the cohort had an estimated nonverbal mental age of at least 18 months (the minimum for the ADI-R). Exclusion criteria were genetic disorders, cerebral palsy, blindness, and deafness. One child per family was invited to participate. Children were enrolled23 at, or soon after, diagnosis (mean [SD] enrollment, 2.7 [2.5] months after diagnosis), at a mean (SD) age of 3.4 (0.8) years.

Information was drawn from 5 data points in the Pathways data set (see eFigure in the Supplement). Time 1 (T1) was composed of 3 data points during early childhood (mean [SD] ages of 3.4 [0.8], 4.0 [0.8], and 4.5 [0.8] years). Data collection for T1 began in May 2005 and ended in October 2012. Time 2 (T2) was composed of 2 data points during middle childhood (mean [SD] ages of 8.7 [0.2] and 10.8 [0.3] years). Data collection for T2 began in May 2009 and continued through March 2018. At T2, 64.6% of the original sample was retained and assessed. (Sample characteristics are available in Table 1.) Assessments typically took place in clinics and research centers, with some conducted in community settings and homes. The study was reviewed and approved by all local research ethics boards and informed consent was obtained in writing by all families. Data analyses were completed March 2018 through January 2020. We adhered to the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for cohort studies.

Table 1. Characteristics of Participants.

Characteristics T1 T2
Participants, No. (N = 272) Mean (SD) Participants, No. (N = 272) Mean (SD)
Age, mo 272 40.73 (8.95) 198 129.08 (3.10)
Boys, No. (%) 272 234 (86.0) 272 234 (86.0)
Girls, No. (%) 272 38 (14.0) 272 38 (14.0)
Developmental domain
VABS-II Communication 272 74.59 (16.26) 272 79.52 (16.39)
VABS-II Activities of daily living 272 76.08 (11.42) 272 77.26 (12.56)
VABS-II Socialization 272 72.20 (9.12) 272 73.86 (13.61)
CBCL Internalizing 255 60.29 (9.25) 228 53.96 (10.17)
CBCL Externalizing 255 56.52 (10.44) 228 51.06 (10.81)
Covariates
Merrill-Palmer 237 58.51 (25.85) NR NR
PLS-4 262 66.74 (19.50) NR NR
Ways of coping 262 0 (0.96) NR NR
ADOS 271 7.68 (1.68) 202 6.81 (2.62)
General family functioning 260 1.75 (0.47) NR NR
Socioeconomic, No. (%)
≥Bachelor’s degree attained by PMK 260 108 (41.6) 172 68 (39.6)
≥Bachelor’s degree attained by partner 248 91 (36.7) 154 48 (31.1)
Married or common law 262 242 (92.4) 172 146 (84.9)
Full-time employment of PMK 263 76 (28.9) 171 72 (42.1)
Full-time employment of partner 251 209 (83.3) 154 126 (81.9)
Income >$80 000 per year 256 102 (39.8) 169 86 (50.9)

Abbreviations: ADOS, Autistic Diagnostic Observation Schedule; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; NR, not reported; PLS-4, Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition; PMK, Person Most Knowledgeable; VABS-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition.

Procedure

Outcome Domain Selection

An advisory group of parents identified areas, such as peer relationships, communication, emotional health, and independent living skills, as outcomes important to them. These domains were very similar to domains identified in a comprehensive scoping review with stakeholder input of outcomes for younger children.24 We selected standardized measures from our assessment battery that matched these constructs and for which cutoffs could be derived to identify those who were doing well relative to a neurotypical population.

Instruments

Outcome Measures

Peer relationships, communication, and independent living skills were assessed using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition (VABS-II) socialization, communication, and daily living skills scales.25 The VABS-II is a semistructured interview measuring adaptive behavior with standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15). We defined proficiency as a standard score 85 or above (ie, 1 SD below the mean). This metric for “not impaired” was used in other optimal outcome studies26 and in the learning disability literature. We defined growth as improvement of at least 1 SD in standard scores (15 points) from T1 to T2. Improvement of 1 SD in clinical trials is considered a large effect size.27,28,29

Emotional health was assessed using the Child Behavior Checklist30 (CBCL). The CBCL for Ages 1.5-5 at T1 and the CBCL for Ages 6-18 at T2 was completed by a primary caregiver (usually the mother) based on observations of the child’s behavior in the 2 months prior. The CBCL yields 2 composite behavior scores: internalizing and externalizing. The CBCL has excellent psychometric properties30 and has been widely used in this population.31,32 Cut points of 70 or 60 or below may still represent subthreshold psychopathology in this population,33,34 so proficiency is defined as a T score of 50 or below (the population mean) for a more conservative estimate. We defined growth as improvement (ie, a T score decrease) of at least 1 SD (10 points) between T1 and T2.

T1 Attribute Measures

We selected T1 cognitive and language measures as traditional within-child attributes. General cognitive functioning was assessed with the Merrill-Palmer-Revised.35 We used the Developmental Index standard score, comprising cognitive, receptive language, and fine motor scales. Language skills at T1 were assessed with the Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition,36 a comprehensive language test reflecting receptive and expressive language abilities.

Based on previous work,37,38 we included 3 contextual variables completed by the primary caregiver: household income, parent coping, and family functioning. Household income was dichotomized as less than $80 000 per year and $80 000 per year or more based on a bimodal distribution in our sample. Parent disengaged emotion-focused coping was assessed with the Ways of Coping questionnaire.39 Disengaged emotion-focused coping comprises items related to parents’ attempts to distance themselves, avoid, or minimize a stressor and negative emotions associated with a stressor.37 Family functioning was assessed with the general family functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device.40 This tool assesses the characteristics of the family regarding communication, discipline, and support among family members.

ADOS Classification at T2

The ADOS22 is a semistructured, standardized observational measure that assesses social and communication behavior indicative of ASD, administered by research-reliable examiners at each site. We used the calibrated severity metric41 with scores ranging from 1 to 10 at T2. A score of 1 to 3 is classified as non-ASD, whereas a score of 4 or more is classified as consistent with ASD.

SRS Classification at T2

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) teacher report is a questionnaire completed by a child’s teacher that assesses social impairment indicative of ASD.42 A score of 60 or less is classified as non-ASD, whereas a score greater than 60 is classified as consistent with ASD.42

Statistical Analysis

Missing Data

We compared participants with missing data with those with complete data on the variables of interest. Differences between participants who completed only T1 measures compared with participants who completed both T1 and T2 measures are in eTable 1 in the Supplement.

Attribute and Outcome Variables

For variables that were collected more than once within the T1 or T2 spans, we used mean scores to obtain a more stable assessment (see eFigure in the Supplement). In all cases, identical measures used within either the T1 span (3 data collection points within T1) or the T2 span (2 data collection points within T2) were correlated (P < .001, r > .55; see eTable 2 in the Supplement). Furthermore, scores were stable within individuals based on z-transformed SDs across scores for each participant below an a priori threshold of 3 SDs (with all SDs being between 0.00 and 2.42), which was used to identify outlier participants with respect to change between combined time points.

Measurement Model

The growth and proficiency criteria for doing well were applied to each of the 5 outcome domains. To assess the measurement model, we determined the extent to which doing well was a multivariable concept composed of distinct metrics. Agreement between and within the proficiency and growth metrics was evaluated with Cohen κ. The threshold for distinct measures (ie, lack of agreement between measures) was 0.40, less than what is considered moderate to strong agreement.43

Prevalence of Doing Well by Domain

For each domain, we created a 4-level classification outcome variable of doing well based on: (1) only the growth criterion, (2) only the proficiency criterion, (3) both criteria, or (4) neither criterion. We estimated the prevalence and 95% confidence intervals of each of these outcome classifications for each domain. To estimate the extent to which doing well was similar across multiple domains within children, we calculated the number of domains in which a child was assessed to be doing well at T2 by any metric.

Association Between Doing Well and ADOS and SRS Classification

We determined the proportion of children identified as having growth or proficiency in the various domains who scored above and below the cutoff for ASD on the ADOS at T2 and those who scored above and below the cutoff on the teacher SRS at T2. The agreement between classification on the ADOS or SRS and doing well was estimated using Cohen κ; under 0.40 was considered weak agreement.

Attributes of Doing Well in Specific Domains

We hypothesized that better child cognitive and language skills, higher family income, better family functioning, and less use of emotion-focused coping would be associated with doing well in middle childhood. The analyses were done separately for each domain within each metric. Six attributes were entered into binary logistic regression, controlling for the T1 domain-specific ability. The Hosmer-Lemeshow test was used to estimate model fit. To control for type I error, we applied a Bonferroni correction across the 10 omnibus logistic regressions (α = .005). Given a significant omnibus test, 2-sided P < .05 determined significance and odds ratios (ORs) for the post hoc tests. Data were analyzed using SPSS version 24.0 (IBM) and R version 3.6 (R Project for Statistical Computing).

Results

The total sample of 272 children was composed of 234 boys (86.0%), and had a mean (SD) age of 3.39 (0.75) years at T1 and 10.76 (0.26) years at T2 (Table 1). Participants missing T2 data had lower VABS-II Communication and PLS scores at T1 than those with complete data and more frequently had a lone parent or parents living in a common-law relationship (eTable 1 in the Supplement). Sample size was not limited by T1 domain data. To reduce bias because of missing not at random in a regression model,44 we did not impute data.45 The sample size for each analysis is therefore provided with the results of each analysis.

Measurement Model

Agreement was only moderate between the different measures of doing well by the proficiency metrics within instruments: VABS-II (275 total respondents; κ = 0.30-0.57) and CBCL (224 total respondents; κ = 0.45-0.48; eTable 3 in the Supplement). Agreement between VABS-II and CBCL was subthreshold (213 total respondents; κ = 0.04-0.29). There was moderate agreement between metrics of growth and proficiency within a domain (κ = 0.29-0.55; eTable 3 in the Supplement). For the most part, then, the different measures of doing well are distinct.

Prevalence of Doing Well by Domain

By middle childhood, 20.2% of participants were proficient (ie, scoring in the not impaired range) in socialization (55 of 272 participants; 95% CI, 15.7%-25.6%), 24.3% in daily living skills (66 of 272 participants; 95% CI, 19.4%-29.9%), and 38.2% in communication (104 of 272 participants; 95% CI, 32.5%-44.3%). In CBCL domains, 35.9% were proficient for internalizing behavior (79 of 220 participants; 95% CI, 29.6%-42.7%), whereas 49.1% were proficient on externalizing problems (108 of 220 participants; 95% CI, 42.3%-55.9%). Using the growth criterion, doing well ranged from 12.5% in daily living skills (34 of 272 participants; 95% CI, 8.9%-17.2%) to 34.5% for internalizing problems (76 of 220 participants; 95% CI, 28.4%-41.3%) (Figure). A total of 78.8% (168 of 213 participants; 95% CI, 73.2%-84.4%) of the sample were doing well by either metric on at least 1 domain, and 23.6% (50 of 213 participants; 95% CI, 17.7%-29.4%) were doing well in 4 or 5 domains (Figure).

Figure. Outcomes Among Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder Measured by Proficiency and Growth.

Figure.

CBCL indicates Child Behavior Checklist; VABS-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition.

Prevalence of Doing Well Compared With ADOS or SRS Classification

Some children were doing well in specific domains and still scoring above the ASD cutoff on the ADOS (Table 2 and Table 3). For the growth metric, between 61.5% (95% CI, 40.7%-79.1%) and 79.6% (95% CI, 66.0%-88.9%) of participants had ADOS scores of 4 or greater; for the proficiency metric, between 63.8% (95% CI, 48.4%-76.9%) and 75.8% (95% CI, 63.0%-85.4%) had scores of 4 or greater. Cohen κ were consistently below the 0.40 threshold, suggesting little agreement between meeting ASD criteria on the ADOS and doing well by either metric in any domain. All Cohen κ were consistently below threshold, again indicating little agreement on meeting ASD criteria on the teacher-scored SRS and doing well by either metric (VABS-II domains, 139 participants; CBCL domains, 122 participants) (eTable 4 in the Supplement).

Table 2. Participants With ADOS Scores <4 or ≥4 for Growth Outcome Metric Within Each Domain.

Domain Growth No growth
No. ADOS <4, % (95% CI) ADOS ≥4, % (95% CI) No. ADOS <4, % (95% CI) ADOS ≥4, % (95% CI)
VABS-II (n = 202)
Communication 44 22.7 (12.0-38.2) 77.3 (61.8-88.0) 158 14.6 (9.7-21.3) 85.4 (78.8-90.3)
Socialization 26 38.5 (20.9-59.3) 61.5 (40.7-79.1) 176 13.1 (8.7-19.2) 86.9 (80.8-91.3)
Activities of daily living 23 30.4 (14.0-53.0) 69.6 (47.0-86.0) 179 14.5 (9.9-20.7) 85.5 (79.3-90.1)
CBCL (n = 170)
Internalizing 59 22.0 (12.7-35.0) 78.0 (65.0-87.3) 111 15.3 (9.4-23.7) 84.7 (76.3-90.6)
Externalizing 54 20.4 (11.1-34.0) 79.6 (66.0-88.9) 116 16.4 (10.4-24.7) 83.6 (75.3-89.6)

Abbreviations: ADOS, Autistic Diagnostic Observation Schedule; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; VABS-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition.

Table 3. Participants With ADOS Scores <4 or ≥4 for Proficiency Outcome Metric Within Each Domain.

Domain Proficient Not proficient
No. ADOS <4, % (95% CI) ADOS ≥4, % (95% CI) No. ADOS <4, % (95% CI) ADOS ≥4, % (95% CI)
VABS-II (n = 202)
Communication 83 27.7 (18.7-38.3) 72.3 (61.2-81.3) 119 8.4 (4.3-15.3) 91.6 (84.7-95.7)
Socialization 47 36.2 (23.1-51.6) 63.8 (48.4-76.9) 155 10.3 (6.2-16.5) 89.7 (83.5-93.8)
Activities of daily living 53 30.2 (18.7-44.5) 69.8 (55.5-81.3) 149 11.4 (7.0-17.9) 87.9 (81.3-92.5)
CBCL (n = 170)
Internalizing 62 24.2 (14.6-37.0) 75.8 (63.0-85.4) 108 13.9 (8.2-22.2) 86.1 (77.8-91.8)
Externalizing 83 25.3 (16.7-36.2) 74.7 (63.8-83.3) 87 10.3 (5.1-19.1) 89.7 (80.9-94.9)

Abbreviations: ADOS, Autistic Diagnostic Observation Schedule; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; VABS-II, Vineland Adaptive–Second Edition.

Attributes of Doing Well in Specific Domains

The omnibus test was significant for associations with proficiency in all domains except CBCL Internalizing (eTable 5 in the Supplement). In post hoc logistic regression for proficiency in each of these domains, the T1 score in the outcome domain was positively associated with the outcome. That is, a high T1 score on the VABS-II or a low T1 score on the CBCL was associated with a proficient outcome at T2 (T1 communication [202 participants]: β = 0.05; OR, 1.05; 95% CI, 1.01-1.09; P = .01; T1 daily living skills [202 participants]: β = 0.07; OR, 1.07; 95% CI, 1.03-1.11; P < .001; T1 socialization [202 participants]: β = 0.10; OR, 1.11; 95% CI, 1.06-1.16; P < .001; T1 externalizing [178 participants]: β, = –0.06; OR, 0.94; 95% CI, 0.91-0.98; P = .001). The only other variable that was significantly associated with doing well by the proficiency metric was general family functioning, for both externalizing (β = 1.00; OR, 0.37; 95% CI, 0.16-0.82; P = .02) and socialization (β = –1.01; OR, 0.36; 95% CI, 0.14-0.93; P = .04) (Table 4).

Table 4. T1 Attributes Associated With T2 Doing Well by the Proficient Metric in Each of the 5 Domains.

Characteristic OR (95% CI)
VABS-II Communication VABS-II Activities of daily living VABS-II Socialization CBCL Internalizinga CBCL Externalizing
T1 instrument scoreb 1.05 (1.01-1.09)c 1.07 (1.03-1.11)c 1.11 (1.06-1.16)c 0.96 (0.92-1.00) 0.94 (0.91-0.98)c
SESd 0.8 (0.40-1.58) 0.64 (0.32-1.28) 1.41 (0.64-3.10) 0.82 (0.42-1.58) 0.54 (0.28-1.08)
Sexe 0.86 (0.31-2.41) 1.76 (0.61-5.13) 1.22 (0.39-3.86) 0.64 (0.25-1.60) 1.8 (0.69-4.73)
Merrill Palmer 1.02 (0.99-1.04) 1 (0.97-1.02) 1.02 (0.99-1.04) 0.99 (0.96-1.01) 0.99 (0.96-1.01)
PLS-4 1.02 (0.98-1.05) 1.03 (1.00-1.06) 1.01 (0.98-1.04) 1.01 (0.98-1.04) 1.02 (0.99-1.06)
Ways of coping 0.38 (0.13-1.09) 0.52 (0.18-1.54) 0.68 (0.22-2.15) 1 (0.36-2.74) 0.38 (0.13-1.07)
General family functioning 0.84 (0.38-1.85) 0.59 (0.26-1.33) 0.36 (0.14-0.93)c 0.42 (0.19-0.93) 0.37 (0.16-0.82)c

Abbreviations: CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; OR, odds ratio; PLS-4, Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition; SES, socioeconomic status; T1, samples assessed between ages 2 and 4.9 years; T2, samples assessed in middle childhood; VABS-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition.

a

The omnibus test for this model was not significant and the results on variables associated with this outcome should not be interpreted (eTable 5 in the Supplement).

b

T1 instrument scores represent the score on the instrument at T1 in the domain being analyzed (ie, in the analysis on VABS-II Communication outcome at T2, the instrument score VABS-II Communication at T1 is included in analysis).

c

P < .05.

d

Estimated self-reported income stratified at $80 000, with 0 = <$80 000 per year household income, and 1 = ≥$80 000 per year household income.

e

Sex coded with 0 = male and 1 = female (ie, an OR > 0 indicates a female advantage).

In investigating associations between attributes and doing well by the growth metrics, the omnibus test was significant for growth in all domains except daily living skills (Table 5). In post hoc logistic regressions for growth in each of these domains, a lower VABS-II or higher CBCL score at T1 was associated with growth on that domain (T1 communication [202 participants]: β = –0.07; OR, 0.94; 95% CI, 0.90-0.97; P < .01; T1 socialization [202 participants]: β = –0.10; OR, 0.91; 95% CI, 0.85-0.96, P < .01; T1 internalizing [178 participants]: β = 0.09; OR, 1.09; 95% CI, 1.04-1.14, P < .001; T1 externalizing [178 participants]: β = 0.10; OR, 1.10; 95% CI, 1.06-1.15; P < .001). The PLS-4 was associated with growth in socialization (β = 0.04; OR, 1.04; 95% CI, 1.00-1.07; P = .04). General family functioning was also associated with growth in internalizing (β = –1.03; OR, 0.36; 95% CI, 0.16-0.79; P = 0.01) and higher household income was associated with growth in externalizing (β = –0.80; OR, 0.45; 95% CI, 0.18-1.00; P = .03). Previous work on the Pathways data set indicated that SES, child age at diagnosis, and mother age at consent have been associated with differences in rates of attrition.46 Sensitivity analysis including these variables in the logistic regression analyses did not change any of our results (eTable 6 in the Supplement).

Table 5. T1 Attributes Associated With T2 Doing Well by the Growth Metric in Each of the 5 Domains.

Characteristic OR (95% CI)
VABS-II Communication VABS-II Activities of daily livinga VABS-II Socialization CBCL Internalizing CBCL Externalizing
T1 instrument scoreb 0.94 (0.90-0.97)c 0.91 (0.86-0.97) 0.91 (0.85-0.96)c 1.09 (1.04-1.14)c 1.10 (1.06-1.15)c
SESd 1.08 (0.51-2.29) 0.5 (0.20-1.23) 0.79 (0.34-1.86) 0.99 (0.50-1.97) 0.45 (0.22-0.91)c
Sex 0.59 (0.22-1.59) 2.09 (0.43-10.13) 1.6 (0.46-5.54) 0.61 (0.24-1.54) 1.28 (0.47-3.51)
Merrill Palmer 1.02 (0.99-1.04) 1.01 (0.98-1.04) 1 (0.97-1.03) 0.98 (0.96-1.01) 1.00 (0.97-1.02)
PLS-4 0.99 (0.95-1.03) 1.01 (0.97-1.05) 1.04 (1.00-1.07)c 1.01 (0.98-1.04) 1.00 (0.97-1.03)
Ways of coping 0.42 (0.13-1.31) 0.74 (0.16-3.34) 0.36 (0.09-1.45) 1.46 (0.51-4.18) 0.41 (0.14-1.23)
General family functioning 0.81 (0.34-1.89) 1.07 (0.36-3.15) 0.66 (0.25-1.75) 0.36 (0.16-0.79)c 0.43 (0.18-1.00)

Abbreviations: CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; OR, odds ratio; PLS-4, Preschool Language Scale, Fourth Edition; SES, socioeconomic status; T1, samples assessed between ages 2 and 4.9 years; T2, samples assessed in middle childhood; VABS-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second Edition.

a

The omnibus test for this model was not significant and the results on variables associated with this outcome should not be interpreted (eTable 5 in the Supplement).

b

T1 instrument scores represent the score on the instrument at T1 in the domain being analyzed (ie, in the analysis on VABS-II Communication outcome at T2, the instrument score VABS-II Communication at T1 is included in analysis).

c

P < .05.

d

Estimated self-reported income stratified at $80 000, with 0 = <$80 000 per year household income, and 1 = ≥$80 000 per year household income.

Discussion

Our objective was to estimate the prevalence and attributes of doing well among children with ASD in middle childhood by applying Taylor’s19 framework of growth and proficiency. Our results suggest that doing well by either metric of growth or proficiency in at least 1 of the 5 developmental health domains was common (approximately 80%); 20% of children were doing well in 4 or more of the 5 domains evaluated. Children who did well according to the proficiency criterion were not necessarily those who did well according to the growth criterion. Doing well by either metric on measures of adaptive functioning or emotional health was not associated with scoring below the ASD cutoff on the ADOS and the SRS. The variable most associated with doing well was the initial score in that outcome domain at T1. Additional attributes of doing well were domain and metric specific, but in general included both child-specific characteristics, such as language, and contextual characteristics, such as household income and family functioning (controlling for baseline score in the outcome domain). Cognitive ability (at least as measured by the MPR) was not a significant factor in any outcome metric.

If different developmental domains in ASD have their own trajectories,2 it follows that doing well would be a multivariate construct. Given a measurement framework that incorporates multiple domains and metrics of growth and proficiency, it was encouraging to see that many children were doing well at age 8 to 10 years on at least a subset of domains. This provides support for incorporating a strengths-based approach to complement a deficit-based needs assessment in the ongoing care of children with ASD. A strengths-based perspective would support a more tailored and flexible approach to developing interventions, an approach that takes account of needs across a range of domains while at the same time making use of particular strengths that can be mobilized to improve the child-environment “fit.”47

It is perhaps not surprising that those with lower baseline scores on adaptive functioning (or more difficulties with emotional health) were more likely to show growth on those domains. Those with better adaptive functioning and emotional health were more likely to reach proficiency on those domains later on. These findings emphasize the potential importance of targeting early interventions to the domains considered key to doing well later in childhood. Interventions targeted only to symptoms of autism, or indeed cognitive ability, may not influence later outcomes in adaptive functioning or emotional health.10

The finding that family functioning is an important factor in several aspects of doing well suggests a new area of research focusing on the community and societal factors, whereby a well-functioning family might be able to improve outcomes for a child with ASD. Although we know that both proximal (ie, family functioning) and distal (socioeconomic disparities) contextual factors can influence typical child development, little research on psychosocial mediators and moderators of outcomes has been done in ASD. Our data support the need for such enquiries. One potential hypothesis to pursue is the possibility that higher income and good family functioning allow a family to access more resources or to apply developmentally appropriate and evidence-based interventions with greater fidelity. A research program embedded in a health services framework would be better positioned to address this issue in finer detail, especially given the results of the meta-analysis from Rogers et al10 of early interventions.

Because doing well is not a unitary construct, supporting children to do well will take a multipronged effort to address child, family, community, and societal factors. Systematically addressing the full breadth of modifiable factors during a child with ASD’s development may ensure that more children will do well over time.

Strengths and Limitations

The strengths of our study include having a longitudinal design in an inception cohort with a large sample size, its assessment of multiple developmental domains, its assessment of the impact of missing data, and its use of models testing a range of child-related and contextual factors. By using criteria for doing well that were independent of ASD symptoms and of cognitive ability, we were able to uncouple the assessment of doing well from the persistence of diagnostic indicators, providing a strengths-based framework that can be applied to the broader continuum of children with ASD whether or not they meet a threshold for a diagnosis.

We are aware of several limitations to our analyses. First, our definition of “doing well” would be strengthened if we had data on its predictive validity (its stability). Second, attrition is a limitation because children with missing data differ to a small extent from those with complete data in some T1 characteristics. Third, a single informant (often the mother) provided these data. Fourth, the CBCL may not be sufficiently sensitive to measure emotional health in ASD and the decision that a score below the typical population average (ie, a T score less than 50) may not be a sufficiently valid proxy for emotional health.33,34 Fifth, while we used measurement tools based on population norms, we do not know the prevalence of demonstrated growth among typical children on each of the outcome domains. Sixth, the measurement model also suggests that within an instrument and within a metric, our definitions of doing well share informant and method variance and may also lead to some overlap in the results on attributes associated with outcomes via a single informant.

Conclusions

This cohort study found that a substantial proportion of children with ASD were doing well by midchildhood according to at least 1 domain of developmental health. A crucial next step is for the ASD field—including young people with ASD, families, practitioners, researchers and policy makers—to come together and work toward consensus on what it means to do well in ASD at different developmental stages, as has been argued in recent articles.18 This discussion will include deciding which domains to incorporate, how elements of doing well should be operationalized within these domains, and whether any specific time points should be prioritized. Such consensus would support the development of a core outcome set for cohort studies, clinical trials in ASD,48 and routine clinical care. Some work in this area has already begun.24 An agreed-upon definition of doing well would also advance implementing a measurement-based care approach in ASD to monitor progress and to guide shared decision-making.49

Our findings on the importance of family income and family functioning remind us that disabilities do not exist apart from a social context. Children with ASD do well when, collectively, we create the social conditions for all people to participate and to experience their capabilities.50 Implementing this will require the active collaboration of all levels of government and policy makers as well as clinicians, researchers, family members, and people with ASD. We hope that the results of this study will encourage the ASD community writ large to also take a strengths-based approach to treatment planning as a way of supporting all children and families with ASD in doing well in every respect.

Supplement.

eTable 1. Missingness of T1 and T2 Characteristics of Participants

eTable 2. Spearman's Correlation Coefficients Between Outcome Measures (CBCL and VABS-II) and Predictor Measures (Ways of Coping, General Family Function) That Were Combined

eTable 3. Unweighted Cohen's Kappa for Agreement Between Binary Good Outcome Variables in the 5 Domains at T2 Within a Quantification Method at T2, and Agreement Between Quantification Methods

eTable 4. Percent with Teacher-Completed SRS Scores ≤ 60 (Below Cut-off for ASD) or > 60 (Above Cut-off for ASD) for Each Outcome Metric Within Each Domain

eTable 5. Omnibus Tests for Logistic Regression Models and Hosmer-Lemeshow for Suitability of Logistic Regression for Growth and Proficiency Metrics Across All Domains

eTable 6. Sensitivity Analysis to Control for Variables Associated With Attrition in the Larger Pathways Study of T1 Variables Associated With T2 Metrics Doing Well in Each of the Five Domains (Omnibus α = 0.005)

eFigure. Flow Chart of 5 Data Points in the Larger Pathways in ASD Study

References

  • 1.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Duku E, et al. ; Pathways in ASD Study Team . Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(3):276-283. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Howlin P, Magiati I. Autism spectrum disorder: outcomes in adulthood. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2017;30(2):69-76. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Steinhausen HC, Mohr Jensen C, Lauritsen MB. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the long-term overall outcome of autism spectrum disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2016;133(6):445-452. doi: 10.1111/acps.12559 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Eaves LC, Ho HH. Young adult outcome of autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(4):739-747. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0441-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(2):212-229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Mason D, Capp SJ, Stewart GR, et al. A meta-analysis of outcome studies of autistic adults: quantifying effect size, quality, and meta-regression. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;1-15. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04763-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Simonoff E, Kent R, Stringer D, et al. Trajectories in symptoms of autism and cognitive ability in autism from childhood to adult life: findings from a longitudinal epidemiological cohort. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;59(12):1342-1352. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.11.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Visser JC, Rommelse NNJ, Lappenschaar M, Servatius-Oosterling IJ, Greven CU, Buitelaar JK. Variation in the early trajectories of autism symptoms is related to the development of language, cognition, and behavior problems. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56(8):659-668. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.05.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Rodgers M, Marshall D, Simmonds M, et al. Interventions based on early intensive applied behaviour analysis for autistic children: a systematic review and cost-effectiveness analysis. Health Technol Assess. 2020;24(35):1-306. doi: 10.3310/hta24350 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Micheletti M, McCracken C, Constantino JN, Mandell D, Jones W, Klin A. Research review: outcomes of 24- to 36-month-old children with autism spectrum disorder vary by ascertainment strategy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020;61(1):4-17. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Zachor DA, Ben-Itzchak E. From toddlerhood to adolescence, trajectories and predictors of outcome: long-term follow-up study in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2020;13(7):1130-1143. doi: 10.1002/aur.2313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Baron-Cohen S. Editorial perspective: neurodiversity—a revolutionary concept for autism and psychiatry. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58(6):744-747. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Georgiades S, Kasari C. Reframing optimal outcomes in autism. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172(8):716-717. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Reid K, Flowers P, Larkin M.. Exploring lived experience. The Psychologist. 2005;18(1):20-23. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kapp SK, Gillespie-Lynch K, Sherman LE, Hutman T. Deficit, difference, or both? autism and neurodiversity. Dev Psychol. 2013;49(1):59-71. doi: 10.1037/a0028353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Teti M, Cheak-Zamora N, Lolli B, Maurer-Batjer A. Reframing autism: young adults with autism share their strengths through photo-stories. J Pediatr Nurs. 2016;31(6):619-629. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2016.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.McCauley JB, Pickles A, Huerta M, Lord C. Defining positive outcomes in more and less cognitively able autistic adults. Autism Res. 2020;13(9):1548-1560. doi: 10.1002/aur.2359 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lounds Taylor J. When is a good outcome actually good? Autism. 2017;21(8):918-919. doi: 10.1177/1362361317728821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Anderson KA, Roux AM, Kuo A, Shattuck PT. Social-ecological correlates in adult autism outcome studies: a scoping review. Pediatrics. 2018;141(suppl 4):S306-S317. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-4300H [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised. Western Psychological Services; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: ADOS. Western Psychological Services; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Zwaigenbaum L, Duku E, Fombonne E, et al. Developmental functioning and symptom severity influence age of diagnosis in Canadian preschool children with autism. Paediatr Child Health. 2019;24(1):e57-e65. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxy076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McConachie H, Parr JR, Glod M, et al. Systematic review of tools to measure outcomes for young children with autism spectrum disorder. Health Technol Assess. 2015;19(41):1-506. doi: 10.3310/hta19410 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Balla DA. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Vineland-II: Survey Forms Manual. Pearson; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fein D, Barton M, Eigsti IM, et al. Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(2):195-205. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Cohen J. Statistical power analysis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1992;1(3):98-101. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Henson RK. Effect-size measures and meta-analytic thinking in counseling psychology research. The Counseling Psychologist. 2006;34(5):601-629. doi: 10.1177/0011000005283558 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Chatham CH, Taylor KI, Charman T, et al. Adaptive behavior in autism: minimal clinically important differences on the Vineland-II. Autism Res. 2018;11(2):270-283. doi: 10.1002/aur.1874 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA Preschool Forms and Profiles. University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Guerrera S, Menghini D, Napoli E, Di Vara S, Valeri G, Vicari S. Assessment of psychopathological comorbidities in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder using the child behavior checklist. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10:535. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00535 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Pandolfi V, Magyar CI, Dill CA. An initial psychometric evaluation of the CBCL 6–18 in a sample of youth with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2012;6(1):96-108. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.03.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dovgan K, Mazurek MO, Hansen J. Measurement invariance of the child behavior checklist in children with autism spectrum disorder with and without intellectual disability: follow-up study. Res Autism Spectrum Disord. 2019;58:19-29. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2018.11.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Medeiros K, Mazurek MO, Kanne S. Investigating the factor structure of the Child Behavior Checklist in a large sample of children with autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectrum Disord. 2017;40:24-40. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2017.06.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Roid GH, Sampers JL. Merrill-Palmer-Revised: Scales of Development. Stoelting; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Zimmerman I, Steiner V, Pond R.. Preschool Language Scale. The Psychological Corporation; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Zaidman-Zait A, Mirenda P, Szatmari P, et al. ; Pathways in ASD Study Team . Profiles of social and coping resources in families of children with autism spectrum disorder: relations to parent and child outcomes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(6):2064-2076. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3467-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Woodman AC, Smith LE, Greenberg JS, Mailick MR. Contextual factors predict patterns of change in functioning over 10 years among adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(1):176-189. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2561-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Vitaliano PP, Russo J, Carr JE, Maiuro RD, Becker J. The ways of coping checklist: revision and psychometric properties. Multivariate Behav Res. 1985;20(1):3-26. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2001_1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Byles J, Byrne C, Boyle MH, Offord DR. Ontario Child Health Study: reliability and validity of the general functioning subscale of the McMaster Family Assessment Device. Fam Process. 1988;27(1):97-104. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.1988.00097.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(5):693-705. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Constantino JN, Lavesser PD, Zhang Y, Abbacchi AM, Gray T, Todd RD. Rapid quantitative assessment of autistic social impairment by classroom teachers. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;46(12):1668-1676. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e318157cb23 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Landis J, Koch G. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977;33(1):159–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hughes RA, Heron J, Sterne JAC, Tilling K. Accounting for missing data in statistical analyses: multiple imputation is not always the answer. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;48(4):1294-1304. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyz032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lewin A, Brondeel R, Benmarhnia T, Thomas F, Chaix B. Attrition bias related to missing outcome data. Epidemiol. 2018;29(1):87-95. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000755 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Richard AE, Smith IM, Duku E, et al. Predicting attrition in a longitudinal study of children with autism spectrum disorder. Paper presented at: International Society for Autism Research; May 11, 2018; Rotterdam, The Netherlands. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lai M-C, Anagnostou E, Wiznitzer M, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. Evidence-based support for autistic people across the lifespan: maximising potential, minimising barriers, and optimising the person-environment fit. Lancet Neurol. 2020;19(5):434-451. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(20)30034-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.COMET Initiative . Published 2019. Accessed April 04, 2019. https://www.comet-initiative.org/
  • 49.Peterson K, Anderson J, Bourne D. Evidence brief: use of patient reported outcome measures for measurement based care in mental health shared decision-making. US Department of Veterans Affairs . Published November 2018. Accessed February 19, 2021. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK536143/ [PubMed]
  • 50.Nussbaum MC. Creating Capabilities. Harvard University Press; 2011. doi: 10.4159/harvard.9780674061200 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplement.

eTable 1. Missingness of T1 and T2 Characteristics of Participants

eTable 2. Spearman's Correlation Coefficients Between Outcome Measures (CBCL and VABS-II) and Predictor Measures (Ways of Coping, General Family Function) That Were Combined

eTable 3. Unweighted Cohen's Kappa for Agreement Between Binary Good Outcome Variables in the 5 Domains at T2 Within a Quantification Method at T2, and Agreement Between Quantification Methods

eTable 4. Percent with Teacher-Completed SRS Scores ≤ 60 (Below Cut-off for ASD) or > 60 (Above Cut-off for ASD) for Each Outcome Metric Within Each Domain

eTable 5. Omnibus Tests for Logistic Regression Models and Hosmer-Lemeshow for Suitability of Logistic Regression for Growth and Proficiency Metrics Across All Domains

eTable 6. Sensitivity Analysis to Control for Variables Associated With Attrition in the Larger Pathways Study of T1 Variables Associated With T2 Metrics Doing Well in Each of the Five Domains (Omnibus α = 0.005)

eFigure. Flow Chart of 5 Data Points in the Larger Pathways in ASD Study


Articles from JAMA Network Open are provided here courtesy of American Medical Association

RESOURCES