Skip to main content
Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Springer Nature - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2021 Apr 1;9(2):208–234. doi: 10.1007/s40489-021-00249-8

A Systematic Review of Family-Mediated Social Communication Interventions for Young Children with Autism

Cressida Pacia 1, Jennifer Holloway 1,, Ciara Gunning 1, Helena Lee 1
PMCID: PMC8012416  PMID: 33821200

Abstract

Social communication deficits are a core symptom of autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The present paper reviews 54 studies evaluating social communication interventions delivered by parents and siblings to children with ASD under 6 years old. Fifty studies evaluated parent-mediated intervention, and four studies evaluated sibling-mediated intervention. Fourteen studies evaluated interventions using telehealth. Treatment effects and research strength were variable across studies. Treatment modality, setting, and dosage had inconclusive impact on treatment effect. Parent-implemented intervention packages, Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT), Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), and Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation (JASPER), qualified as established evidence-based practice for this population. Most studies reported successful generalization of skills for some, but not all, children. Telehealth and sibling-mediated intervention are promising areas of further research and clinical practice.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder, Family-mediated interventions, Social communication, Telehealth


Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a lifelong neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by marked deficits in social communication along with restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Social communication deficits are strongly associated with negative long-term outcomes (Gillberg & Steffenburg, 1987; Howlin et al., 2004). Early intervention, starting as early as the second year of life, has been shown to have positive long-term outcomes (e.g., Pickles et al., 2016; Wetherby & Woods, 2006) and has been associated with greater developmental gains and reduction in ASD symptoms than intervention delivered later in life (Koegel et al., 2006). Interventions targeting social communication skills have demonstrated collateral benefits in other areas (Ledbetter-Cho et al., 2017). For instance, teaching children with ASD to make requests has resulted in concomitant decreases in problem behavior (Charlop-Christy et al., 2002; Gianoumis et al., 2012).

The recommended intervention for individuals with ASD is Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) (National Autism Centre, 2009, 2015). Recommended practice guidelines in early behavioral intervention for children with ASD emphasize building parents’ capacity to support their child’s behavior within the context of daily life (Division for Early Childhood, 2014; National Research Council, 2001). Parent training is an established intervention for increasing interpersonal and play skills and decreasing problem behavior in children with ASD (National Autism Centre, 2015). Parent training has also been shown to have benefits for the parent. Parents of children with ASD report higher levels of stress and affective symptoms than parents of typically developing children and parents of children with other disabilities and chronic illnesses (e.g., Abbeduto et al., 2004; Dumas et al., 1991; Smith et al., 2010). Parents who engage in parent training programs have reported decreases in stress (Keen et al., 2010), improvements in mental health symptoms such as anxiety and depression (Tonge et al., 2006), increases in the amount of parental leisure and recreation time (Koegel et al., 1982), and increased levels of parental self-efficacy (McConachie & Diggle, 2007) and optimism (Koegel et al., 1982). Participating in parent training has also been shown to have positive effects on parent-child interactions (Koegel et al., 1996).

Despite growing interest in parent training, few studies have examined the role of other family members (i.e., siblings) in intervention. A conceptually similar area of research is peer-mediated intervention (PMI), which involves typically developing peers in teaching a variety of skills to children with ASD. PMI is an established intervention for increasing communication and interpersonal skills with children with ASD (National Autism Centre, 2009, 2015). Benefits of PMI for individuals with ASD include increased opportunities to interact with social partners, improved social competence, and independence (Sperry et al., 2010). PMI has also been shown to have a positive impact on peer implementers. They were found to demonstrate academic gains, increased sensitivity to others, higher self-confidence, and expanded peer networks after participating as peer mediators for individuals with ASD (Carter et al., 2008). Criteria for successful peer candidates include attending the same school as the child with ASD, having developmentally appropriate cognitive and language abilities, a history of compliance, and social competence and enthusiasm (Gunning et al., 2019a).

Sibling training potentially combines the benefits of parent training and PMI, as siblings could support skill generalization for the child with ASD along with parents, and increase fun, reciprocal play, and learning opportunities comparable to peers. Indeed, a systematic review of sibling-mediated interventions found that results from these interventions were similar to results from PMIs (Shivers & Plavnick, 2015). Target skills included play skills, social skills, academic or functional skills, and physical fitness. Overall, the siblings learned the intervention procedures, and the children with ASD showed increases in skill acquisition and/or decreases in problematic behavior.

Given the high prevalence rates of ASD (1 in 59 children; Baio et al., 2018) and the importance of early intervention, attention has been called to innovating service delivery models that would allow clinicians to maximize their productive time and reach. Additionally, families who live in rural or remote areas often experience barriers in accessing evidence-based intervention (Kogan et al., 2008; Liptak et al., 2008; Mandell et al., 2010). Telehealth is a promising service delivery model in which clinicians consult and deliver treatment over a distance using communication technologies such as videoconferencing or interactive websites (Dudding, 2009). Telehealth has been used to deliver evidence-based intervention to individuals with ASD, including parent training, and has demonstrated positive results, although insufficient methodological rigor is a concern (Boisvert et al., 2010; Knutsen et al., 2016). Continued evaluation of remote parent training, employing stronger research methodologies, is ongoing (e.g., Ingersoll et al., 2016; Vismara et al., 2018).

A key advantage of involving family members in intervention delivery is the potential for improved generalization outcomes. Children with ASD often present with severe challenges in applying skills learned in treatment to everyday use, which is a major barrier to effective intervention (Vismara & Rogers, 2010). Generalization outcomes are improved when intervention addresses functional behaviors within the natural environment; natural consequences are used; training occurs across different settings, people, and stimuli; and mediation strategies such as problem-solving are taught (Chandler et al., 1992; Gunning et al., 2019b; Stokes & Baer, 1977; Stokes & Osnes, 1989). As family-mediated interventions are delivered by people closest to the child, it could be expected that increased learning opportunities would occur across contexts and interactions and continue to develop as the child grows. A review of generalization and maintenance of social communication skills in parent-mediated interventions found improved social communication across situations and over time, although not to the extent achieved during acute intervention (Hong et al., 2018a). Similarly, a meta-analysis of early intervention effects on social communication found the largest effect sizes in context-bound measures (i.e., outcomes measured with the same person and context as the treatment context), compared to semi-generalized and generalized measures (i.e., outcomes measured with a different person and/or in a different context) (Fuller & Kaiser, 2019).

Several recent systematic reviews have synthesized the literature on parent training as it relates to language and communication interventions (Akamoglu & Meadan, 2018), toddlers (Beaudoin et al., 2014), school-age children (Black & Therrien, 2018), and functional communication training (Gerow et al., 2018). Overall, parents were able to implement the intervention, and positive outcomes were noted for the parents and children. However, firm conclusions could not be reached given the paucity of studies with strong methodological rigor. A meta-analysis of 19 randomized clinical trials (RCT) of parent-mediated interventions for children with ASD found small improvements in symptom severity, socialization, and cognition, and trivial improvements in communication and language (Nevill et al., 2018). Although small effects are concerning, the reviewers noted that both the quantity and quality of research on parent-delivered interventions have been increasing. Additionally, more research is needed on parent and child variables that may explain the inconsistency in treatment outcomes. A systematic review on potentially influencing factors found mixed results on the impact of broad child factors (e.g., age, verbal ability), fine-grained child factors (e.g., vocal initiation, joint attention, imitation), and contextual child factors (e.g., service access, intervention hours), along with parent factors such as including demographics, intervention factors (e.g., adherence, involvement, fidelity), and contextual factors (e.g., therapist support, accessibility) (Trembath et al., 2019).

The present study aims to extend on these reviews and provide practical guidance for clinicians by synthesizing treatment effectiveness and research strength of family-mediated social communication interventions for children with ASD under 6 in terms of intervention characteristics such as intervention agents, modality, setting, dosage, focused intervention practices (FIP), and intervention packages. This is also the first review to combine and compare in situ and telehealth interventions along with parent- and sibling-mediated interventions.

Method

Search Procedures

A systematic search of the literature was conducted in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., 2009). The following databases were used: EBSCO (Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection), Education Resources Information Center (ERIC), PsycINFO, Scopus, and Web of Science. Combinations of the following terms were inputted into each database: (1) “autis*,” (2) “parent train*” or “parent mediat*” or “parent implement*” or “sibling train*” or “sibling mediat*” or “sibling implement*,” and (3) “social communication” or “social interaction” or “social skills” or “communication skills” or “play.” Initial online searches yielded 3682 results. A hand search of the reference sections of studies that met the criteria was also conducted to identify additional relevant studies. A total of 2574 records remained after duplicates were removed.

Inclusion Criteria

Studies were included if (a) at least one child had a confirmed diagnosis of ASD (in studies employing single-subject research design; SSRD) or all focal children had a confirmed diagnosis of ASD (in studies employing group designs); (b) at least one child with ASD was between 0 and 6 years old (in studies employing SSRD) or all focal children were between 0 and 6 years old (in studies employing group designs); (c) parent or sibling implemented 100% of the intervention; (d) the article presented an evaluation of an intervention to improve one or more social communication skills as defined in the Social Communication Checklist (SCC; Ingersoll & Dvortcsak, 2010); (e) the intervention employed an experimental design, i.e., either a single-subject research design (SSRD) or a group comparison design; (f) the intervention employed at least one evidence-based FIP as outlined by Wong et al. (2015); and (g) the study must have been published in a peer-reviewed journal. Only studies available electronically and published in English between 1980 and 2019 were included.

Data Extraction and Coding

Each article in the review was summarized in terms of participant and intervention agent characteristics, intervention characteristics, target skills, and collateral outcomes. Treatment effectiveness and research strength were then evaluated and synthesized by intervention agent, intervention characteristics, target skills, FIPs, and intervention packages. Generalization promotion strategies and outcomes were also coded.

Participant and Intervention Agent Characteristics

Information regarding the total number of participants, number of participants that met inclusion criteria, and age was noted. Only data for participants that met the criteria were extracted. For intervention agents, relationship to participant (i.e., mother, father, brother, sister) was recorded, along with age of sibling (if sibling was mediating the intervention). If additional characteristics such as sibling skills were reported, these variables were noted.

Intervention Characteristics

Intervention strategies used to teach target skills to the child with ASD were coded by FIP. Intervention packages that used combinations of these FIPs were also recorded. Protocols used to train intervention agents were coded as instructions, modelling, video modelling, role play, rehearsal, and/or feedback. Treatment modality (telehealth or in situ) and treatment setting (home, clinic, or mixed) were noted. Treatment dosage (i.e., duration and frequency of sessions and total hours of intervention) was also recorded. Total hours of treatment were coded into low-dose interventions (≤10 h), medium-dose interventions (11–20 h), high-dose interventions (21–30 h), and extended interventions (over 30 h).

Target Skills and Collateral Outcomes

Target skills were recorded as described by the study authors and then coded into one of the three social communication domains based on the SCC: social engagement, language and communication, and imitation and play. Collateral outcomes for the participant and intervention agents were recorded. If standardized measures were used, those were noted.

Treatment Effectiveness

In articles using SSRD, percentage of non-overlapping data (PND; Scruggs et al., 1987) was used to measure treatment effectiveness for each dependent variable. Advantages of PND include ease of calculation from graphical rather than raw data, high degree of inter-rater reliability, applicability to any SCD design type, and ease of interpretation (Campbell, 2013; Parker et al., 2007). PND has also been shown to be a more conservative measure of treatment effects compared to other nonoverlap methods (e.g., nonoverlap of all pairs (NAP) and percentage of all overlapping data (PAND)) and is widely applicable to data gathered for participants with ASD (Carr, 2015). PND is calculated by dividing the number of treatment data points that fall below the lowest baseline data point by the total number of data points in the treatment phase, multiplied by 100 (Scruggs et al., 1987).

PND scores range from 0 to 100%. An intervention with a PND score of 90% or higher is considered to be a very effective treatment, a PND score between 70 and 89% indicates effective treatment, a PND score between 51 and 69% is considered questionable treatment, and an intervention with a PND score of 50% or lower is classified as not effective treatment (Scruggs & Mastropieri, 1998). Where all baseline data points were at zero, this was noted. Graphs with fewer than three baseline data points and graphs that showed ceiling or floor effects at baseline were excluded from calculation (Scruggs et al., 1987; Schlosser et al., 2008).

In articles utilizing group designs, Cohen’s d was calculated for all primary dependent variables using means, standard deviations, and sample sizes of the treatment and control groups (Cohen, 1988). Cohen’s (1988) criteria were then used to determine effect sizes: trivial (<0.2), small (0.2–0.49), medium (0.5–0.79), or large (⩾0.8). Cohen’s d is commonly used to synthesize studies using group designs (Warner, 2013). Treatment effects were then synthesized by target skills, intervention agents and characteristics, FIPs, and intervention packages.

Research Strength and Determining Evidence-Based Practice

Studies were assessed for research quality using primary and secondary quality indicators and rated as strong, adequate, or weak using an operationalized evaluative method outlined by Reichow et al. (2008). Primary quality indicators for both SSRDs and group designs included information on participant characteristics and independent and dependent variables. Additional primary quality indicators for SSRDs included demonstration of a stable baseline condition, visual analysis, and experimental control, while additional primary quality indicators for group designs included use of a comparison condition, demonstration of the link between the research question and data analysis, and accurate statistical analysis. Primary quality indicators were rated as high, acceptable, or unacceptable quality. Secondary quality indicators for both SSRDs and group designs included evidence of interobserver agreement, blind raters, measurement of treatment fidelity and generalization and maintenance outcomes, and demonstration of social validity. Additional secondary quality indicators for SSRDs included calculation of the Kappa statistic, while additional secondary quality indicators for group designs included random assignment, details of participant attrition, and reporting of treatment effect sizes. Secondary quality indicators were rated as being present or absent.

To receive a strong rating, studies needed to receive high quality grades on all primary quality indicators and demonstrate evidence of at least three (for SSRDs) or four (for group designs) secondary quality indicators, while to receive an adequate rating, studies needed to receive high quality grades on at least four primary quality indicators and no unacceptable ratings on any primary quality indicators, along with demonstrating evidence of at least two secondary quality indicators. Studies that did not meet these criteria received a weak rating.

From this, Z scores were calculated to determine whether an intervention package could be rated as an evidence-based practice (EBP). The following formula was applied to calculate the Z score (“GroupS” equals the total number of group design studies with an overall “strong” rating, “GroupA” equals the total number of group design studies with an overall “adequate” rating, “SSEDS” equals the total number of participants in single-subject studies with a “strong” rating, and “SSEDA” equals the total number of participants in single-subject studies with an “adequate” rating):

GroupS30+GroupA15+SSEDS4+SSEDA2=Z

Z scores of ≥60 points indicate “established EBP” and >30 indicate “probable EBP.” Research strength and EBP status were then synthesized by target skills, intervention agents, intervention characteristics, FIPs, and intervention packages.

Generalization

The studies were assessed for generalization promotion strategies, generalization dimension, latency to maintenance probe, and generalization outcome. Generalization promotion strategies were divided into four categories: exploiting current functional contingencies, training diversely, incorporating functional mediators, and using sequential modification (Chandler et al., 1992; Gunning et al., 2019b; Neely et al., 2015; Stokes & Osnes, 1989; Swan et al., 2016). Generalization dimension was coded into settings, materials, people, and maintenance (i.e., generalization over time). Generalization outcome was coded as “complete” if all participants’ generalization scores were above baseline or intervention data, “partial” if some participants’ generalization scores were above baseline or intervention data, and “failure” if none of the participants’ generalization scores were above baseline or intervention data (Chandler et al., 1992; Gunning et al., 2019b).

Inter-rater Reliability

Inter-rater reliability (IRR) was calculated on at least 20% of the literature search, title and abstract screening, full-text screening, and data extraction procedures. All IRR scores in this review were calculated by dividing agreements between two independent raters by agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100. IRR was 100% for the literature search, 97% for title and abstract screening, 87.8% for full-text screening, and 82% for data extraction. If there was disagreement between the raters, the discrepancy was discussed until the raters came to a consensus.

IRR for PND and Cohen’s d was calculated on 20% of the included studies. An agreement was defined as both raters recording the same percentage of non-overlapping data per behavior. Overall agreement was determined by the following formula:

#of agreements=#agreements+disagreements×100=%

IRR for the calculation of PND and Cohen’s d was 90%. When calculations were considered inaccurate, the co-authors reached agreement through discussion. This process was repeated until 100% agreement was achieved.

Results

Fifty-four studies met inclusion criteria. None of the studies was published between 1980 and 999. Fourteen studies (25.9%) were published between 2000 and 2009, while 40 studies (74.1%) were published between 2010 and 2019. Two studies reported intervention outcomes for the same group of participants (Gengoux et al., 2015; Hardan et al., 2015). Data for these studies were consolidated where appropriate. Participant, intervention agent, and intervention characteristics are presented in Table 1, while dependent variables, treatment effects, and research design and strength are presented in Table 2.

Table 1.

Descriptive summary of included studies

Reference Participants
N (N eligible)
• Gender
• Age
Intervention agents
• Relationship
• Age (siblings)
Intervention characteristics
• FIPs*
• Intervention package
• Setting
• Dosage
Cardon (2012)

4 (4)

2 M; 2 F

24–42 months

4 mothers

PII, PP, R+, TAII, VM

Video Modelling Imitation Training (VMIT)

Home

<8 h

Coolican et al. (2010)

8 (8)

7 M; 1 F

28–56 months

5 mothers, 3 fathers

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Clinic and home

6 h

Ferraioli & Harris (2011)

4 (4)

3 M; 1 F

41–64 months

4 brothers

6–8 years

DTT, MD, PMII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

No program name

Home

3–12 h

Gengoux et al. (2015)

23 (23)

See Hardan et al. 2015

See Hardan et al. 2015 See Hardan et al. 2015
Gillett & Leblanc (2007)

3 (3)

3 M

4–5 years

3 mothers

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Natural Language Paradigm (NLP)

2 clinic, 1 home

1.5–2 h

Guðmundsdóttir et al. (2019)

3 (3)

3 M

47–55 months

3 mothers

PII, R+, TD

Sunny Starts; Decide Arrange Now Count Enjoy (Teaching DANCE)

Telehealth (plus one session at home)

14–29 h

Guðmundsdóttir et al. (2017)

2 (1)

1 M

58 months

1 mother

PII, R+, TD

Sunny Starts; Decide Arrange Now Count Enjoy (Teaching DANCE)

Telehealth

53 h

Hansen et al. (2018)

3 (2)

1 M; 1 F

36; 57 months

2 mothers

PII, PP, R+, TD

No program name

Clinic

Average 1.5 h

Hansen & Shillingsburg (2016)

2 (2)

2 M

32; 45 months

Parent (not specified)

PII, PP, R+, TD

Modified Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT)

Clinic

13–14 h

Hardan et al. (2015)

53 (25)

19 M; 6 F

4.1 (1.2) years

25 parents (not specified)

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment – Group (PRT-G)

Clinic

16 h

Hong et al., 2018b

2 (2)

1 M; 1 F

51 months; 57 months

2 mothers

PII, PP, R+, TD

No program name

Self-directed, telehealth

Total hours not calculated

Ingersoll & Gergans (2007)

3 (3)

2 M; 1 F

31–42 months

3 mothers

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT)

Clinic

10–13 h

Ingersoll & Wainer (2013)

8 (7)

6 M; 1 F

44–63 months

7 mothers

PII, PP, R+, TD

Project ImPACT

Clinic

12–24 h

Ingersoll et al. (2016)

28 (28)

20 M; 8 F

46.08 (13.18) SD; 41.57 (12.24) TA months

26 mothers, 2 fathers

PP, PII, R+, TD

Project imPACT

Self-directed, telehealth

27 h

Jones and Feeley (2009)

3 (2)

1 M; 1 F

45; 48 months

2 mothers, 1 father

DTT, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

No program name

Home

Total hours unclear

Jull & Mirenda (2011)

2 (2)

2 M

59; 65 months

2 mothers

1 sister, 1 cousin (not directly trained)

6; 4 years

PII, PP, PMI, R+, SPG

No program name

Home

Total hours unclear

Kaiser et al. (2000)

6 (3)

3 M

40–54 months

3 mothers

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Enhanced Milieu Teaching (EMT)

Clinic

18 h

Kasari et al. (2015)

86 (43)

35 M; 8 F

30.7 (3.5) months

43 mothers

MD, PII, PP, TD

Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation (JASPER)

Clinic

10 h

Kasari et al. (2010)

38 (19)

15 M; 4 F

30.35 (0.93) months

Parents (not specified)

MD, PII, PP, TD

No program name

Clinic

18 h

Kasari et al. (2014)

112 (60)

50 M; 10 F

41.9 (10.0) months

Parents (not specified)

MD, PII, PP, TD

Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation (JASPER)

Home

24 h

Kashinath et al. (2006)

5 (5)

4 M; 1 F

33–65 months

5 mothers

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

No program name

Home

40–60 h

Koegel et al. (2002)

5 (5)

2 M; 3 F

46–67 months

5 mothers, 3 fathers

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Clinic

25 h

Lane et al. (2016)

2 (2)

2 M

31–35 months

2 mothers

PII, PP, R+, TD

No program name

Clinic

3 h

Law et al. (2018)

3 (3)

2 M; 1 F

30–52 months

3 mothers

PII, PP, R+, TD

Map4speech (app); Project imPACT

Self-directed, telehealth

4.5–10 h

Loughrey et al. (2014)

2 (1)

1 M

3 years

1 mother

1 father (not directly trained)

PII, PP, R+

No program name

Clinic and home

Total hours unclear

Madzharova & Sturmey (2015)

3 (1)

1 M

5 years

1 mother

1 sister (not directly trained)

5 years

PII, PMI, PP, R+, SPG

No program name

Home

Total hours unclear

Mancil et al. (2009)

3 (2)

2 M

49; 58 months

2 mothers

FBA, FCT, PII, PP, R+, TD

Modified milieu therapy

Home

<5 h

Manohar et al. (2019)

50 (26)

22M 4F

42.6 (17.5) months

Parents (not specified)

MD, PII, PP, R+, SN

No program name

Clinic and home

5.25–6 h

McDuffie et al. (2013)

8 (8)

4 M; 4 F

27–69 months

8 mothers

MD, NCR, PP, PII

No program name

Telehealth

24 h

McGarry et al. (2019)

11 (11)

8M, 3F

20–46 months

Parents (8 mothers, 3 fathers)

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Telehealth

Total hours unclear

Meadan et al. (2016)

3 (3)

2 M; 1 F

2–4 years

3 mothers

MD, PII, TD

Internet-Based Parent-

Implemented Communication Strategies (i-PiCS)

Telehealth

Total hours unclear

Nefdt et al. (2010)

27 (13)

92.6% M

38.92 (14.57) months

Parents (not specified)

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Self-directed

Average 2 h

Nunes & Hanline (2007)

1 (1)

1 M

55 months

1 mother

MD, PII, PP, R+

No program name

Home

Average 1 h

Oppenheim-Leaf et al. (2012)

3 (2)

2 M

Both 4 years

1 brother, 1 sister

Both 5 years

PMII, PP, R+, TD

Program name not specified

Home

Total hours unclear

Park et al. (2011)

3 (3)

3 M

29–31 months

3 mothers

PECS, PII, PP, R+

Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS)

Home

2.5–4 h

Penney and Schwartz (2018)

3 (3)

2M, 1F

48–65 months

2 mothers, 1 father

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT)

Home and childhood center

< 7 h

Randolph et al. (2011)

3 (1)

1 M

5 years

1 father

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Clinic

7–9 h

Reagon & Higbee (2009)

3 (2)

2 M

35; 47 months

2 mothers

PII, PP, SC, R+, TAII

No program name

Home

Approximately 1 h

Rocha et al. (2007)

3 (3)

2 M; 1 F

26–42 months

2 mothers, 1 father

DTT, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

No program name

Clinic

17 h

Rogers et al. (2019)

45 (45)

31 M, 13 F

2.1 (0.4) years

Parents (not specified)

FBA, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Early Start Denver Model (ESDM)

Clinic and home

36 h

Rollins et al. (2016)

4 (4)

4 M

28–33 months

4 mothers

PII, R+, TD

Pathways Early Autism Intervention

Home

12–19.5 h

Simacek et al. (2017)

3 (2)

2 F

3.5; 4 years

2 mothers, 1 father

FBA, FCT, PII, PP, R+, TD

No program name

Telehealth

Total hours unclear

Stahmer & Gist (2001)

22 (22)

Gender not specified

19–50 months

Parents (not specified)

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Clinic

12 h

Symon (2005)

3 (3)

3 M

34–64 months

3 mothers

FBA, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Clinic

25 h

Tsao & Odom (2006)

4 (2)

2 M

41; 46 months

1 sister; 1 brother

11; 4 years

PMII, PP, R+

Stay-Play-Talk

Play Time/ Social Time

Getting Along with Others

Home

3 h

Vernon et al. (2012)

3 (3)

3 M

28–51 months

2 mothers; 1 father

MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT)

Home

16 h

Vismara et al. (2009)

8 (8)

Gender not specified

26–35 months

8 parents (not specified)

FBA, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Early Start Denver Model (ESDM)

Clinic

12 h

Vismara et al. (2018)

24 (14)

11 M; 3 F

31.9 (10.4) months

11 mothers, 3 fathers

FBA, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Parent Early Start Denver Model (P-ESDM)

Telehealth

18 h

Vismara et al. (2013)

8 (8)

Gender not reported

20–45 months

7 mothers, 1 father

FBA, MD, PII, PP, PRT, R+, TD

Parent Early Start Denver Model (P-ESDM)

Telehealth

22.5 h

Wainer & Ingersoll (2013)

3 (2)

2 M

26; 69 months

2 mothers

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT)

Self-directed, Telehealth

<2 h

Wainer & Ingersoll (2015)

5 (5)

Gender not reported

29–59 months

5 mothers

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT)

Telehealth

1.5 h

Walton & Ingersoll (2012)

4 (4)

4 M

45–57 months

2 brothers, 4 sisters

8–13 years

MD, PMII, PP, R+, TD

Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT)

Home

5–10 h

Wang (2017)

4 (4)

3 M; 1 F

30–50 months

2 mothers, 2 fathers

PII, TD, TAII, VM

No program name

Home

Total hours unclear

Zaghlawan & Ostrosky (2016)

2 (2)

2 M

37; 60 months

1 mother, 1 father

MD, PII, PP, R+, TD

Modified Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT)

Home

5.5–8 h

*All studies included naturalistic interventions (NI); therefore, NI was not listed

FIPs (Wong et al., 2015). DTT: Discrete Trial Training. FBA: Functional Behavior Assessment. FCT: Functional Communication Training. MD: Modelling. PII: Parent-implemented intervention. PMII: Peer-mediated instruction or intervention. PP: Prompting. PRT: Pivotal Response Treatment. R+: Reinforcement. SC: Scripting. TAII: Technology-aided instruction and intervention. TD: Time delay. VM: Video modelling.

Table 2.

Treatment effects and research strength

Reference Dependent variables
• Target skills (category of social communication)and treatment effects (PND or Cohen’s d)
• Secondary outcomes
• Parent/sibling outcomes
Research characteristics
• Design
• Strength
Cardon (2012)

Imitation of actions (IP): very effective;95.73% (1/4 with 0 baseline)

Secondary outcomes: MIS, PLS-5

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Strong

Coolican et al. (2010)

Functional verbal utterances (LC): questionable; 54.59%

Types of utterances (LC): no visual representation;calculated pre-post

Disruptive behavior (O): no visual representation;calculated pre-post

Secondary outcomes: PLS-4, PPVT-III

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, self-efficacy,social validity

SSRD

Weak

Ferraioli & Harris (2011)

Responding to tap/show (SE): effective; 74.98%

Responding to point/gaze (SE): very effective; 100%

Initiating behavioral requests (SE): very effective; 100%(3/4 excluded due to <3 baseline data points)

Initiating joint attention (SE): not effective; 50%(4/4 with 0 baselines)

Secondary outcomes: ESCS, imitation

Sibling outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Gengoux et al. (2015)

(3-month follow-up to Hardan et al., 2015)

Total utterances (LC): small; 0.48

Unintelligible (LC): small; 0.42

Imitative (LC): small; 0.49

Verbally prompted (LC): medium; 0.68

Nonverbally prompted (LC): large; 0.96

Spontaneous (LC): small; 0.37

Secondary outcomes: CDI, PLS-4, Vineland-II, SRS,CGI-S and CGI-I

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

Group

Strong

Gillett & Leblanc (2007)

Spontaneous vocalizations (LC): effective; 83.33%

Prompted vocalizations (LC): not effective; 45.4%

Secondary outcomes: mean length of utterance (MLU), appropriate play

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Guðmundsdóttir et al. (2019)

Requesting (LC): not effective; 24.7%

Social attending (SE): very effective; 91%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Guðmundsdóttir et al. (2017)

Requesting (LC): effective; 78.05%

Social attending (SE): effective; 88.89%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Adequate

Hansen et al. (2018)

Independent response to joint attention (SE): effective; 87.86%

Prompted response to joint attention (SE): very effective; 100%

Secondary outcomes: initiating JA

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Adequate

Hansen & Shillingsburg (2016)

Vocalizations (LC), compliance with instructions (O)

Could not calculate treatment effectiveness size for AB within-subject design

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Hardan et al. (2015)

Total utterances (LC): small; 0.42

Unintelligible (LC): trivial; 0.08

Imitative (LC): large; 1.06

Verbally prompted (LC): trivial; −0.09

Nonverbally prompted (LC): large; 0.94

Spontaneous (LC): small; 0.46

Secondary outcomes: CDI, PLS-4, Vineland-II, SRS,CGI-S and CGI-I

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

Group

Strong

Hong et al., 2018b

Verbal responding (LC): questionable; 52.4%

Use of different words (LC) (1/2 participants):very effective; 100%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Ingersoll & Gergans (2007)

Object imitation (IP): not effective; 48.17%

Gesture imitation (IP) (1/3 participants): effective; 75%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Ingersoll & Wainer (2013)

Spontaneous language (LC): not effective;27.15% (1/8 with 0 baseline)

Parent-selected language goals (LC): no visual representation; percent of goals achieved

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Adequate

Ingersoll et al. (2016)

Expressive language (LC): could not calculate effect size due to lack of no-treatment control group

Secondary outcomes: CDI, MSEL, VABS-II

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, sense of competence,family impact

Group

Strong

Jones and Feeley (2009)

Respond to joint attention (SE): questionable; 54.29%

Initiate joint attention (SE): effective; 73% (1/2 with 0 baseline)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Strong

Jull & Mirenda (2011)

Synchronous reciprocal interactions (SE): very effective; 97.53%

Secondary outcomes: affect

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Kaiser et al. (2000)

Frequency of communication target use (LC): questionable; 63.87

Spontaneous verbal utterances (LC), mean length of utterance (MLU; LC), diversity of utterances (LC): no visual representation; pre-post measures used

Secondary outcomes: SICD, PPVT-R, EOWPVT-R, SALT

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Strong

Kasari et al. (2015)

Joint engagement (SE): large; 1.42

Secondary outcomes: initiating joint attention, functional play, symbolic play, highest play level, RDLS, MSEL

Parent outcomes: Stress (PSI)

Group

Strong

Kasari et al., 2010

Joint engagement (SE): large; 0.86

Secondary outcomes: play diversity, play level, joint attention

Parent outcomes: quality of involvement, adherence and competence, treatment integrity

Group

Strong

Kasari et al. (2014)

Joint engagement (SE): medium; 0.47

Secondary outcomes: play diversity, joint attention (ESCS)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, caregiver quality of involvement, adherence and competence

Group

Strong

Kashinath et al. (2006)

Child-specific communication targets (LC): questionable; 60.49%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Koegel et al. (2002)

Functional verbal responses (LC): effective; 88.9% (2/5 excluded due to <3 baseline points)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, affect

SSRD

Adequate

Lane et al. (2016)

Vocal communicative responding (LC): questionable; 55.6% (2/2 with 0 baselines)

Initiation (LC): effective; 73.4% (2/2 with 0 baselines)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Adequate

Law et al. (2018)

Prompted utterances (LC): not effective; 1.47%

Spontaneous utterances (LC): not effective; 13.8%

Prompted pointing (SE): questionable;56.55% (2/2 with 0 baselines)

Spontaneous pointing (SE): not effective;14.65% (2/2 with 0 baselines)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Loughrey et al. (2014)

Prompted mands (LC): not effective; 0%

Spontaneous mands (LC): questionable; 53.85%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Madzharova & Sturmey (2015)

Independent mands to peer (LC): not effective; 0%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Mancil et al. (2009)

Communication with picture cards (LC): very effective; 100% (2/2 with 0 baselines)

Reduction in problem behaviors (O): very effective; 95.15%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Strong

Manohar et al. (2019)

Autism symptom severity (CARS; SE, LC, IP): small, 0.169

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, family interview of stress and coping (FISC), subjective distress, understanding of ASD and intervention techniques, and perceived competence to implement interventions

Group

Strong

McDuffie et al. (2013)

Verbal and nonverbal communication acts (LC)

Could not calculate PND due to lack of visual representation of child outcomes

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

McGarry et al. (2019)

Vocalizations (LC), eye contact (SE), positive affect (O): could not calculate effect size due to lack of control group

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

Group

Weak

Meadan et al. (2016)

Communication responses (LC): effective; 86.55% (1/3 excluded due to ceiling effects)

Communication initiation (LC): questionable; 52.6%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Nefdt et al. (2010)

Functional verbal utterances (LC): large; 0.953

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, observed confidence, DVD usage, social validity

Group

Strong

Nunes & Hanline (2007)

Communication turn (LC): questionable; 60%

Imitative response (LC): not effective; 6.25% (2/2 routines with 0 baseline)

AAC use (LC): questionable; 63.75 (1/2 routines with 0 baseline)

Verbalizations/vocalizations (LC): not effective; 25%

Gestures and manual signs (LC): not effective; 35%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Weak

Oppenheim-Leaf et al. (2012)

Playing with others (IP): effective; 75%

Sharing (IP): very effective;100% (1/2 excluded due to ceiling effects)

Choosing an activity (IP): not effective; 0%

Following instructions (IP): not effective;50% (1/1 with 0 baseline)

Sibling outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Park et al. (2011)

Independent picture exchanges (LC): very effective; 90.47%

Word vocalizations (LC): no visual representation

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Strong

Penney and Schwartz (2018)

Spontaneous imitation (IP): Questionable; 55%

Parent outcome: Treatment integrity

SSRD

Weak

Randolph et al. (2011)

Initiations (LC): not effective; 0%

Verbal responses (LC): very effective; 100% (1/1 with 0 baseline)

Nonverbal responses (LC): not effective; 50%

Appropriate play (IP): very effective; 90%

Varied play (IP): not effective; 0%

Secondary outcomes: VABS-II

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Reagon & Higbee (2009)

Unscripted verbal initiation in play (IP): effective; 78.13%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Adequate

Rocha et al. (2007)

Responding to joint attention (SE): questionable; 51.77%

Secondary outcomes: initiating joint attention,coordinated joint attention

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Rogers et al. (2019)

PATH Curriculum Checklist (SE, LC, IP): could not calculate effect size due to lack of no-treatment control group

Secondary outcomes: ADOS-T, MSEL, Vineland-II, CBCL, Child Intervention History

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

Group

Strong

Rollins et al. (2016)

Eye contact (SE): effective; 75%

Social engagement (SE): effective; 81.6%

Verbal reciprocity (SE): effective; 80.55%

Nonverbal turn-taking (SE): questionable; 62.15%

(LE stage not included in calculation)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Simacek et al. (2017)

AAC use (LC): effective; 72.39% (1/7 with 0 baseline)

Idiosyncratic communication (LC; for reduction): effective; 85.1% (3/7 excluded due to floor effects; 1/7 with 100 baseline)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Stahmer & Gist (2001)

Language use (LC) and communicative gestures (LC)

Could not calculate effect size due to lack of true control group

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

Groups

Weak

Symon (2005)

Functional verbal utterances (LC) and appropriate behavior (O): effective; 73.35% (Follow-up data used as no intervention data provided; 1/3 participants excluded due to <3 baseline points)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity of primaryand significant caregivers

SSRD

Weak

Tsao & Odom (2006)

Joint attention (SE): effective; 75%

Social behaviors (SE; verbal or nonverbal initiation or responses): not effective; 41.1%

Sibling outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity(interaction quality)

SSRD

Weak

Vernon et al. (2012)

Eye contact (SE): effective; 89.58%

Verbal initiations (LC): effective; 79.17% (1/3 with 0 baseline)

Positive affect (O): very effective; 93.75%

Synchronous engagement (SE): very effective;95.83% (2/3 with 0 baseline)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, positive affect

SSRD

Strong

Vismara et al. (2009)

Spontaneous verbal behavior (LC), imitation (IP), attentiveness (SE), initiation (SE)

Unable to calculate PND due to 8 data paths per graphs

Secondary outcomes: ADOS; MSEL

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity

SSRD

Weak

Vismara et al. (2018)

Imitation in play (IP): medium; 0.51

Joint attention (SE): no significant differences

Spontaneous functional verbal utterances (LC): not reported

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, website use, social validity

Group

Strong

Vismara et al. (2013)

Functional verbal utterances (LC): not effective; 16.66%

Joint attention initiations (SE): not effective; 15.64%

Secondary outcomes: CDI

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, website use, social validity, engagement style

SSRD

Weak

Wainer & Ingersoll (2013)

Imitation (IP): effective; 83.35%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, program utilization, knowledge of RIT, social validity

SSRD

Strong

Wainer & Ingersoll (2015)

Imitation (IP): not effective; 44%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, program engagement, parent knowledge, relationship between treatment integrity and imitation, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Walton & Ingersoll (2012)

Imitation (IP): not effective; 16.9% (3 excludeddue to ceiling effect)

Joint engagement (SE): not effective; 14.15%

Sibling outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity(satisfaction and engaging sibling, having fun, “typical” interaction via video ratings)

SSRD

Weak

Wang (2017)

Turn-taking (SE): questionable; 63.75%

Refusal (O; for reduction): questionable; 68.63%

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Adequate

Zaghlawan & Ostrosky (2016)

Object imitation (IP): not effective; 37,5%

Gestural imitation (IP): not effective; 50%

(Calculated when relevant strategy was introduced)

Parent outcomes: treatment integrity, social validity

SSRD

Weak

Category of social communication. SE, social engagement; LC, language and communication; IP, imitation and play; O, other

Standardized assessments. ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; CDI, MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories; CGI-S and CGI-I, Clinical Global Impression Scale severity and improvement subscales; EOWPVT-R, Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised; ESCS, Early Social-Communication Scales; MIS, Motor Imitation Scale; MSEL, Mullen Scales of Early Learning; PLS-4, Preschool Language Scales, 4th Edition; PLS-5, Preschool Language Scales, 5th Edition; PPVT-III, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 3rd edition; PPVT-R, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised; PSI, Parenting Stress Index; RDLS, Reynell Developmental Language scales; SALT, Systematic Analysis of Language Transcript program; SICD, Sequenced Inventory of Communication Development; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale; Vineland-II, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition

Research design. SSRD, single-subject research design

Participant Characteristics

The 54 included studies had a total of 653 participants. Twenty-three participants were included in two studies, a randomized controlled trial (Hardan et al., 2015) and its 3-month follow-up study (Gengoux et al., 2015). A total of 444 participants met inclusion criteria (not including participants who only participated in control conditions). A total of 316 (71.2%) participants were male, and 84 participants (18.9%) were female. Gender was not reported for 44 participants (9.9%). Participant age ranged from 19 to 69 months.

Intervention Agent Characteristics

Parents were trained to implement the intervention in 50 studies (92.6%), while siblings were trained in four studies (7.4%). None of the studies trained both parents and siblings together; however, two studies (3.7%) trained parents directly and involved siblings indirectly (Jull & Mirenda, 2011; Madzharova & Sturmey, 2015). None of the telehealth interventions involved siblings. A total of 193 mothers acted as intervention agents (43.8%), along with 24 fathers (5.4%), 8 brothers (1.8%), and 6 sisters (1.4%). A total of 220 intervention agents were labelled as “parent” and not specified as mother or father (49.9%).

Siblings ranged from 4 to 13 years old (M=7.86 years). One study reported that the siblings were verbal, exhibited strong interaction skills, and said that they would like to increase their interactions with their sibling with ASD (Oppenheim-Leaf et al., 2012). These siblings were reported to exhibit problem behavior. One sibling had undiagnosed sensory concerns as reported by his mother (Walton & Ingersoll, 2012). None of the siblings had cognitive or developmental concerns. No other sibling characteristics were noted in the articles.

Intervention Characteristics

Modality, Setting, and Dosage

Forty articles evaluated in situ interventions (74.1%), and 14 articles evaluated telehealth or self-directed interventions (25.9%). Of the in situ studies, 15 studies took place in a clinic (37.5%), while 18 studies took place in participants’ homes (45%). Six studies (15%) took place in multiple settings. Twenty-one studies (38.9%) were categorized as low-dose interventions, 12 studies (22.2%) were categorized as medium-dose interventions, seven studies (13%) were categorized as high-dose interventions, and three studies (5.6%) were categorized as an extended intervention. Total hours were unclear in ten studies (18.5%). Session duration ranged from 5 min to 5 h, and session frequency ranged from 3 per day to one per week. Intervention duration ranged from 5 days to 1 year.

Intervention Agent Training Methods

All studies (100%) used instructions to train the intervention agent, while 27 (50%) used modelling, 23 (42.6%) used video modelling, 15 (37.8%) used role play, and 34 studies (67%) used rehearsal and feedback. One study (1.9%) read a story with the sibling, and three studies (5.6%; 75% of sibling studies) used a reward system for participating. Of the 14 telehealth interventions, 4 (28.6%) involved partial in situ training, 10 (71.4%) involved live videoconferencing, and 11 (78.6%) involved self-directed materials (e.g., self-directed websites, apps, videos).

Focused Intervention Practices

Given the selection criteria for this review, all studies used naturalistic intervention (NI; 100%) along with either parent-implemented intervention (PII; n=50, 92.6%) or peer-mediated instruction and intervention (PMII; n=4, 7.4%). PMII was coded for both sibling and non-sibling peer involvement. In addition, 48 studies used prompting (88.9%), 47 studies used reinforcement (87%), 43 used time delay (79.6%), 32 used modelling (59.3%), 17 used PRT (31.5%), 7 used functional behavior assessment (FBA; 13%), 3 used Discrete Trial Teaching (DTT; 5.6%), 2 used video modelling (3.7%), 2 used functional communication training (FCT; 3.7%), 1 used picture exchange communications system (PECS; 1.9%), and 1 used scripting (1.9%). Prompting, reinforcement, modelling, and time delay were coded where it was outlined within the intervention method, as well as when it was inherent within other FIPs used in the study (e.g., PRT, FCT, PECS), even if it was not explicitly described in the procedure.

Intervention Packages

The most commonly studied intervention was Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT; Koegel et al., 1987). Ten studies (18.5%) used PRT as a standalone intervention package, including a follow-up study and a self-directed study. Seven additional studies (13%) combined PRT with other FIPs, four of which evaluated the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM; Vismara & Rogers, 2008) and three of which did not specify an intervention package. All PRT studies were delivered by parents. Six studies (11.1%) used Reciprocal Imitation Training (RIT; Ingersoll, 2008), including two telehealth studies and one sibling-mediated study. Four studies (7.4%) used ESDM, including two telehealth studies, and three studies (5.6%) used Project ImPACT, including two telehealth studies. Two studies (3.7%) used Milieu Teaching, including enhanced milieu teaching (EMT) and modified milieu teaching, and two studies (3.7%) used Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation (JASPER), all delivered by parents. One study each (1.9%) used Natural Language Paradigm (NLP), a modified Parent-Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT), Pathways, Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS), Video Modeling Imitation Training (VMIT), and a combination of Stay-Play-Talk, Play Time/Social Time, and Getting Along with Others. Other telehealth intervention packages include two studies (3.7%) that used Sunny Starts/Decide Arrange Now Count Enjoy (DANCE) and one study (1.9%) that used i-PiCS. All these studies were parent mediated. Seventeen studies (31.5%) used a combination of FIPs without labelling the intervention, including 3 sibling-mediated studies.

Dependent Variables and Collateral Outcomes

Dependent Variables

A total of 117 dependent variables were targeted as primary outcomes, and 6 dependent variables were measured as part of a 3-month follow-up (Gengoux et al., 2015). Primary outcomes were coded into social engagement skills (n=32; 28.3%), language and communication skills (n=55; 48.7%), and imitation and play skills (n=18; 15.9%). Two (1.8%) primary dependent variables were measured using only comprehensive assessments that included social communication skills across multiple domains. Six dependent variables (5.3%) not directly related to social communication were also measured, including adaptive skills, compliance with instructions, and increasing appropriate behaviors or decreasing problem behaviors.

Collateral Outcomes for Child with ASD

Seventeen studies (31.5%) measured collateral outcomes for the child with ASD. Nine studies (16.7%) evaluated collateral outcomes using standardized pre-post measures that assessed multiple developmental domains (see Table 2 for specific measures). Seven studies (13%) measured specific untargeted behaviors, including joint attention (n=6), play (n=3), imitation (n=1), and affect (n=1).

Family Outcomes

Of the 50 parent-mediated studies, parent outcomes included treatment integrity (n=49; 86%); social validity (n=30; 50%), program engagement, e.g., DVD or website use (n=5; 10%); adherence and/or competence (n=3; 6%); knowledge of intervention and/or ASD (n=3; 6%); self-efficacy (n=2; 4%); quality of involvement (n=2; 4%); parent affect (n=2; 4%); stress and/or coping (n=2; 4%); family impact (n=1; 2%); engagement style (n=1; 2%); subjective distress (n=1; 2%); and observed confidence (n=1; 2%). Of the 4 sibling-mediated studies, sibling outcomes included fidelity (n=4; 100%) and social validity (n=4; 100%), including 2 studies (50%) that assessed social validity through video ratings of sibling interactions such as interaction quality and fun. The most measured outcome for family members was treatment integrity, although a consistent method for assessing treatment integrity was not found. The next most measured outcome for family members was social validity. To assess social validity, 29 studies (54.2%) administered questionnaires, 4 studies (6.3%) conducted open-ended interviews, and 3 studies (6.3%) used video ratings. All participants (100%) who completed questionnaires and open-ended interviews reported being satisfied with the intervention. Nine out of 10 implementers (90%) who were part of social validity video ratings were rated as meeting the author’s criteria for social validity. Twenty studies (37%) did not explicitly measure social validity.

Treatment Effects and Research Strength

Treatment Effects

Treatment effects were calculated for each primary dependent variable. Where treatment effects could not be calculated, those studies were excluded from the analyses below. In studies that utilized SSRDs, treatment effectiveness could not be calculated for 3 studies. One study used an AB within-subject design (Hansen & Shillingsburg, 2016), one study did not provide visual representation of child outcomes (Mcduffie et al., 2013), and one study displayed 8 data paths per graph which made it impossible to calculate PND (Vismara et al., 2009). In studies that utilized group designs, effect sizes could not be calculated using Cohen’s d for four studies that did not utilize true control groups. One study compared a self-directed and therapist-assisted model of Project ImPACT (Ingersoll et al., 2016); one study compared a parent education group with a parent education plus parent support group, with both groups being trained in PRT methods (Stahmer & Gist, 2001); one study compared a basic and enhanced model of ESDM (Rogers et al., 2019); and one study used pre-post measures (Mcgarry et al., 2019). Additionally, in four studies, treatment effects could be calculated for some, but not all, dependent variables (e.g., PND could be calculated for one dependent variable but could not be calculated for the second dependent variable as the second dependent variable was only measured pre- and post-intervention).

Research Strength

Among all 54 studies, 17 studies (31.5%) received a “strong” rating, 18 studies (33.3%) received an “adequate” rating, and 19 studies (35.2%) received a “weak” rating (see Table 2). Among the 42 studies that utilized SSRD, 7 studies (16.7%) met criteria for a “strong” rating, 17 studies (40.5%) met criteria for an “adequate” rating, and 18 studies (42.9%) were given a “weak” rating. Among the 12 studies that utilized a group design, 10 studies (83.3%) met the criteria for a “strong” rating and two studies (16.7%) were given a “weak” rating. One group study that received a “weak” rating was given this rating only because it did not specify the gender of the participants.

Target Skills, Intervention Agents, and Intervention Characteristics

Treatment effects and research strength were synthesized by target skills, intervention agents, and intervention characteristics, i.e., modality, setting, and dosage (see Table 3). Target skills that were categorized into the social engagement domain showed a higher percentage of treatment effectiveness (60% very effective or effective; 75% large or medium effect size), as compared to target skills in the language and communication domain (37.2% very effective or effective; 50% large or medium effect size) and imitation and play domain (43.8% very effective or effective; 100% large or medium effect size, n=1). Additionally, social engagement skills were targeted in a larger proportion of studies rated as strong or adequate (70%), compared to language and communication skills (59.3%) and imitation and play skills (58.3%). In two studies (Manohar et al., 2019; Rogers et al., 2019), primary dependent variables were only measured using comprehensive assessments (Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; Scholpler et al., 1988); PATH Curriculum Checklist (PATH CC; Rogers et al., 2013)), which made it difficult to extract treatment effects for specific social communication categories. The comprehensive assessments measured skills from multiple domains but did not report specific data for each variable. Data from these studies were therefore not included in the calculations for treatment effects by social communication domains.

Table 3.

Treatment effectiveness, effect sizes, and research strength by intervention characteristics

Treatment effectiveness using PND* Effect sizes using Cohen’s d* Research strength**
Very effective Effective Questionable Not effective Large Medium Small Trivial or none Strong Adequate Weak
Target skills
SE 6 (24%) 9 (36%) 5 (20%) 5 (20%) 2 (50%) 1 (25%) 0 (0%) 1 (25%) 6 (30%) 8 (40%) 6 (30%)
LC 5 (14.3%) 8 (22.9%) 9 (25.7%) 13 (37.1%) 3 (50%) 0 (0%) 1 (16.7%) 2 (33.3%) 9 (28%) 10 (31.3%) 13 (40.6%)
IP 3 (18.8%) 4 (25%) 1 (6.3%) 8 (50%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 3 (25%) 4 (33.3%) 5 (41.7%)
Modality
TH 2 (11.1%) 6 (33.3%) 3 (16.7%) 7 (38.9%) 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 4 (28.6%) 4 (28.6%) 6 (42.9%)
In situ 13 (21.3%) 16 (26.2%) 14 (23%) 18 (29.5%) 4 (40%) 1 (10%) 3 (30%) 2 (20%) 12 (30.8%) 14 (35.9%) 13 (33.3%)
Setting
Home 10 (25.6%) 10 (25.6%) 8 (20.5%) 11 (28.2%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 6 (33.3%) 6 (33.3%) 6 (33.3%)
Clinic 3 (18.8%) 5 (31.2%) 3 (18.8%) 5 (31.2%) 4 (50%) 0 (0%) 2 (25%) 2 (25%) 4 (26.7%) 7 (46.7%) 4 (26.7%)
Mixed 0 (0%) 1 (16.7%) 3 (50%) 2 (33.3%) --- --- 1 (100%) --- 2 (33.3%) 1 (16.7%) 3 (50%)
Dosage
Low 9 (22%) 7 (17.1%) 6 (14.6%) 19 (46.3%) 2 (66.7%) 0 (0%) 1 (33.3%) 0 (0%) 7 (33.3%) 6 (28.6%) 8 (38.1%)
Medium 2 (15.4%) 6 (46.2%) 3 (23.1%) 2 (15.4%) 3 (37.5%) 1 (12.5%) 2 (25%) 2 (25%) 5 (41.7%) 4 (33.3%) 3 (25%)
High 1 (16.7%) 2 (33.3%) 0 (0%) 3 (50%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (28.6%) 2 (28.6%) 3 (50%)
Extended 0 (0%) 2 (66.7%) 1 (33.3%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 1 (33.3%) 2 (66.7%) 0 (0%)

*N target skills classified by level of effectiveness (percentage of target skills classified by level of effectiveness)

**N articles classified by research strength (percentage of articles classified by research strength)

SE, social engagement; LC, language and communication; IP, imitation and play; TH, telehealth

Parent-mediated interventions were coded as PII and sibling-mediated interventions were coded as PMII (see Table 4). Both types of intervention agents had similar proportions of very effective to effective treatment (46.3% for parents and 50% for siblings). However, parent-mediated studies (n=50) greatly outnumbered sibling-mediated studies (n=4), and parent-mediated studies had a higher proportion of studies rated as strong or adequate (68% compared to 25%).

Table 4.

Treatment effectiveness, effect sizes, and research strength by focused intervention practice (FIP)

Treatment effectiveness using PND* Effect sizes using Cohen’s d* Research strength**
Very effective Effective Questionable Not effective Large Medium Small Trivial or none Strong Adequate Weak
DTT 2 (25%) 3 (37.5%) 2 (25%) 1 (12.5%) --- --- --- --- 1 (33.3%) 1 (33.3%) 1 (33.3%)
FBA 2 (28.6%) 3 (42.9%) 0 (0%) 2 (28.6%) 0 (0%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%) 1 (50%) 3 (42.9%) 1 (14.3%) 3 (42.9%)
FCT 2 (50%) 2 (50%) 0(0%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%)
MD 6 (15%) 10 (25%) 9 (22.5%) 15 (37.5%) 6 (42.9%) 2 (14.3%) 3 (21.4%) 3 (21.4%) 12 (37.5%) 5 (15.6%) 15 (46.9%)
PECS 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
PII 12 (17.9%) 19 (28.4%) 16 (23.9%) 20 (29.9%) 5 (38.5%) 2 (15.4%) 3 (23.1%) 3 (23.1%) 17 (34%) 17 (34%) 16 (32%)
PMII 3 (25%) 3 (25%) 0 (0%) 6 (50%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 1 (25%) 3 (75%)
PP 13 (19.7%) 16 (24.2%) 12 (18.2%) 25 (37.9%) 5 (41.7%) 2 (16.7%) 3 (25%) 2 (16.7%) 16 (33.3%) 14 (29.2%) 18 (37.5%)
PRT 6 (28.6%) 6 (28.6%) 3 (14.3%) 6 (28.6%) 3 (33.3%) 1 (11.1%) 2 (22.2%) 3 (33.3%) 7 (41.2%) 3 (17.6%) 7 (41.2%)
R+ 15 (20.5%) 20 (27.4%) 13 (17.8%) 25 (34.2%) 2 (28.6%) 1 (14.3%) 2 (28.6%) 2 (28.6%) 13 (28.2%) 16 (34.8%) 17 (37%)
SC 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 1 0 (0%)
SN --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%)
TD 12 (18.5%) 20 (30.8%) 13 (20%) 20 (50.8%) 4 (44.4%) 2 (22.2%) 1 (11.1%) 2 (22.2%) 13 (31%) 15 (35.7%) 14 (33.3%)
VM 1 (33.3%) 0 (0%) 2 (66.7%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%)

*N target skills classified by level of effectiveness (percentage of target skills classified by level of effectiveness)

**N articles classified by research strength (percentage of articles classified by research strength)

Adapted from Wong et al., 2015. DTT, Discrete Trial Training; FBA, Functional Behavior Assessment; FCT, Functional Communication Training; MD, modelling; PII, parent-implemented intervention; PMII: peer-mediated instruction or intervention; PP, prompting; PRT, pivotal response treatment; R+, reinforcement; SC, scripting; SN, social narratives; TAII, technology-aided instruction and intervention; TD, time delay; VM, video modelling

Evaluating intervention modality, treatment effectiveness was similar in telehealth studies (44.4% very effective or effective) and in situ studies (47.5% very effective or effective). A larger proportion of effect sizes were in the large or medium range for telehealth studies (100%) than for in situ studies (50%), although there were more target skills measured in in situ group studies (n=10) compared to telehealth (n=2). Telehealth had a smaller proportion of studies rated as strong or adequate (57.2% for telehealth and 66.7% for in situ). Among in situ studies, there was no difference in treatment effectiveness for intervention delivered in home (51.2% very effective or effective) or clinic (50% very effective or effective). The proportion of large or medium effect sizes was higher in home (100%) compared to clinic (50%), but fewer skills were targeted in home (n=1) than clinic (n=8). Research strength was similar across home (66.6% strong or adequate) and clinic studies (73.4% strong or adequate). Interventions in mixed settings had the lowest treatment effectiveness (16.7% very effective or effective) and research strength (50% strong or adequate), although the single outcome measures in a group study showed a large effect (100%).

When considering dosage, the relationship between treatment effects and intervention hours is uncertain. Extended-dose intervention had the highest proportion of skills improved with very effective or effective intervention (66.7%), followed by medium-dose (61.6%), high-dose (50%), and low-dose (39.1%). However, only 3 studies evaluated an extended intervention (Guðmundsdóttir et al., 2017; Kashinath et al., 2006; Rogers et al., 2019). Examining effect sizes, low- and high-dose interventions showed 100% large or medium effect sizes, compared to 50% for medium-dose and 0% for extended-dose.

FIPs and Intervention Packages

Treatment effects and research strength were synthesized for each FIP (see Table 4) and intervention package (see Table 5). PRT is separately conceptualized here as an FIP (combined with other FIPs into intervention packages such as ESDM) and as a standalone intervention package (not combined with other FIPs). PRT as an FIP was very effective or effective for 57.2% of target skills and demonstrated large or medium treatment effects for 44.4% of target skills, while PRT as a standalone intervention package was very effective or effective for 66.7% of target skills and demonstrated large or medium effects for 42.9% of target skills.

Table 5.

Treatment effectiveness, effect sizes, and research strength by intervention package

Treatment effectiveness using PND* Effect sizes using Cohen’s d* Research strength**
Very effective Effective Questionable Not effective Large Medium Small Trivial or none Strong Adequate Weak Z score EBP
ESDM 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (100%) 0 (0%) 1 (50%) 0 (%) 1 (50%) 2 (50%) 0 (0%) 2 (50%) 6 Established
i-PiCS 0 (0%) 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 No
JASPER --- --- --- --- 1 (50%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 60 Established
Milieu training 2 (66.7%) 0 (0%) 1 (33.3%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 2 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 20 No
NLP 0 (0%) 1 (50%) 0 (0%) 1 (50%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 6 No
Pathways 0 (0%) 3 (75%) 1 (25%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 8 No
PCIT 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 No
PECS 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 12 No
PRT 4 (33.3%) 4 (33.3%) 1 (8.3%) 3 (25%) 3 (42.9%) 0 (0%) 2 (28.6%) 2 (28.6%) 4 (40%) 2 (20%) 4(40%) 98 Established
Project ImPACT 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (20%) 4 (80%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (66.6%) 1 (33.3%) 0 (0%) 50 Probable
RIT 0 (0%) 2 (22.2%) 1 (11.1%) 6 (66.7%) --- --- --- --- 1 (16.7%) 1 (16.7%) 4 (66.7%) 6 No
S-P-T 0 (0%) 1 (50%) 0(0%) 1 (50%) --- --- --- --- 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (100%) 0 No
Sunny Starts 1 (25%) 2 (50%) 0 (0%) 1 (25%) --- --- --- --- 0(0%) 2 (100%) 0 (0%) 4 No
VMIT 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) --- --- --- --- 1 (100%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 16 No

*N target skills classified by level of effectiveness (percentage of target skills classified by level of effectiveness)

**N articles classified by research strength (percentage of articles classified by research strength)

EBP, evidence-based practice; ESDM, Early Start Denver Model; JASPER, Joint Attention, Symbolic Play, Engagement & Regulation; NLP, Natural Language Paradigm; PCIT, Parent-Child Interaction Therapy; PECS, Picture Exchange Communication System; PRT, Pivotal Response Training; RIT, Reciprocal Imitation Training; S-P-T, Stay-Play-Talk; VMIT, Video Model Imitation Training

Most interventions combined at least two FIPs, making it difficult to attribute treatment effects to specific FIPs. All studies utilized naturalistic interventions. Given the inclusion criteria for this review, all studies utilized either parent-mediated intervention or peer-mediated intervention (i.e., sibling-mediated intervention). Unsurprisingly, prompting, reinforcement, modelling, and time delay were commonly utilized, as these practices are inherent to most behavioral interventions. Among the less utilized FIPs, FBA was very effective or effective for 71.5% of target skills and demonstrated large or medium effects for 50% of target skills, DTT was very effective or effective for 62.5% of target skills, and video modelling was very effective or effective for 33.3% of target skills. Three FIPs (FCT, PECS, and scripting) demonstrated 100% very effective or effective treatment; however, these FIPs were only used to target one (PECS and scripting) or two (FCT) skills.

After Z score calculations for intervention packages, PRT (Z=98), JASPER (Z=60), and ESDM (Z=60) qualified as established EBP, while Project ImPACT (Z=50) qualified as probable EBP. However, while treatment effects were promising for PRT (see above) and JASPER (100% large or medium effects), treatment effects were low for ESDM (0% very effective or effective; 50% large or medium effects) and Project ImPACT (0% very effective or effective; effect size could not be calculated due to lack of true control group). These four intervention packages were the only ones to be evaluated in group studies, which may have contributed to higher Z scores as group studies are weighted higher in calculation. PRT, Project ImPACT, and ESDM were all evaluated in both in situ and telehealth modalities, while JASPER was only evaluated in situ. None of these studies involved siblings.

Generalization

Generalization Promotion Strategies

Generalization promotion strategies are outlined in Table 6. In the category of exploiting current functional contingencies, 54 studies (100%) recruited natural contingencies and targeted functional behaviors, 40 studies (74.1%) involved contacting natural reinforcement, 14 studies (25.9%) involved modifying maladaptive consequences, and 12 studies (22.2%) specified reinforcement of occurrences of generalization. In the category of training diversely, 42 studies (77.8%) used sufficient stimulus exemplars, 34 studies (63%) used sufficient response exemplars, 9 studies (16.7%) made antecedents or consequences less discriminable, 37 studies (68.5%) occurred across settings or in natural settings, and 6 studies (11.1%) involved skills teaching across people. In the category of incorporating functional mediators, 54 studies (100%) programmed common stimuli and 5 studies (9.3%) incorporated self-mediated stimuli. Two studies (3.7%) used sequential modification.

Table 6.

Generalization measures and outcomes

Reference Generalization promotion strategies* Generalization dimension Generalization outcome Latency to maintenance probe
Cardon (2012) FB, RNC, SR, S, PCS Materials, maintenance Partial 1 week and 3 weeks
Coolican et al. (2010) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, SR, S, PCS Maintenance Partial 2–4 months
Ferraioli & Harris (2011) FB, CNC, RNC, S, PCS Setting and people, maintenance Partial 3 months
Gengoux et al. (2015) See Hardan et al. (2015) Maintenance (follow-up study to Hardan et al., 2015) Complete (groups design) 3 months
Gillett & Leblanc (2007) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, SR, S, PCS Setting and materials Partial None
Guðmundsdóttir et al. (2019) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS Setting, maintenance (2/3) Partial 1–3 months
Guðmundsdóttir et al. (2017) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS Maintenance Partial 1 month
Hansen et al. (2018) FB (joint attention), RNC, SS Maintenance Partial 3 weeks
Hansen & Shillingsburg (2016) FB, RNC, MMC, SS None N/A None
Hardan et al. (2015) FB, CNC, RNC, SR See Gengoux et al., 2015 See Gengoux et al., 2015 See Gengoux et al., 2015
Hong et al., 2018b FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS Maintenance Partial 1 month
Ingersoll & Gergans (2007) FB, RNC, SS, SR Setting and materials, maintenance Partial 1 month
Ingersoll & Wainer (2013) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS Setting and materials, maintenance Partial 1 month
Ingersoll et al. (2016) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS Maintenance Complete (group design) 3 months
Jones and Feeley (2009) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, LD, S, MA Materials Complete (data for one participant lost) None
Jull & Mirenda (2011) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, S, PCS None N/A None
Kaiser et al. (2000) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR Setting and materials, maintenance Partial Once a month for 6 months
Kasari et al. (2015) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, S, PCS Setting and people, maintenance Partial (group design) 6 months
Kasari et al. (2010) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS Maintenance Partial 12 months
Kasari et al. (2014) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, S Maintenance Complete (Group design) 3 months
Kashinath et al. (2006) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, S, PCS Setting and materials Partial None
Koegel et al. (2002) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, SR, S, MA Maintenance Complete Variable (3–12 months)
Lane et al. (2016) FB, CNC, RNC, SS Maintenance (1/2 participants) Complete 3 weeks
Law et al. (2018) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, SR, S, SM Setting Partial 1 month
Loughrey et al. (2014) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, LD, S, MA, PCS People, maintenance Partial 4–5 weeks
Madzharova & Sturmey (2015) FB, RNC, MMC, S Materials Complete (1 participant) None
Mancil et al. (2009) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, SS, S, SMS, PCS Setting and people, maintenance Complete 2 weeks
Manohar et al. (2019) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, SR, S, PCS None N/A N/A
McDuffie et al. (2013) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS None N/A None
McGarry et al. (2019) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS None N/A N/A
Meadan et al. (2016) FB, CNC, RNC, SR, S, PCS Setting, maintenance Partial Unclear
Nefdt et al. (2010) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, S, SR, PCS None N/A None
Nunes & Hanline (2007) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, S, SMS, PCS Setting and materials Partial None
Oppenheim-Leaf et al. (2012) FB, RNC, MMC, SS, S, PCS None for children with ASD (siblings were primary participants) N/A None
Park et al. (2011) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, SS, SR, LD, S, PCS, SMS People, maintenance Complete 1×/week for 1 month
Penney & Schwartz (2018) FB, RNC, SS, SR, S None N/A N/A
Randolph et al. (2011) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR Maintenance Partial 2 weeks (1–4 follow-up sessions)
Reagon & Higbee (2009) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, SR, LD, S, PCS, SMS Materials, maintenance Complete 2 weeks
Rocha et al. (2007) FB, CNC, RNC, RG, SS, LD, S Setting and materials, maintenance Partial Unclear
Rollins et al. (2016) FB, CNC, RNC, S, PCS None N/A None
Rogers et al. (2019) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, RG, SS, SR, LD, S, PCS None N/A N/A
Simacek et al. (2017) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, SS, S, MA, PCS, SMS None N/A None
Stahmer & Gist (2001) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR None N/A None
Symon (2005) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, RG, SS, SR, S, MA, PCS Maintenance Complete 2 weeks–3 months
Tsao & Odom (2006) FB, RNC, S, PCS Setting, maintenance Failure Unclear
Vernon et al. (2012) FB, CNC, RNC, SS, SR, S, PCS Setting, maintenance Partial 1 week
Vismara et al. (2009) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, SS, SR, LD, S, PCS People, maintenance Partial 2 weeks, 4 weeks, 3 months, and 4 months
Vismara et al. (2018) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, RG, SS, SR, LD, S, PCS Maintenance Unclear 12 weeks
Vismara et al. (2013) FB, CNC, RNC, MMC, RG, SS, SR, LD, S, PCS, SM Maintenance Partial 1×/month for 3 months
Wainer & Ingersoll (2013) FB, RNC, SS, SR, S None N/A None
Wainer & Ingersoll (2015) FB, RNC, SR, S, PCS Maintenance Partial 1 month and 3 months
Walton & Ingersoll (2012) FB, RNC, SS, SR, S, MA Setting, materials, and people, maintenance Partial 1 month
Wang (2017) FB, RNC, SS, SR, S People and materials, maintenance Partial 1 week and 1 month
Zaghlawan & Ostrosky (2016) FB, RNC, SS, SR, S None N/A None

*Adapted from Gunning et al. 2019b. FB, functional behaviors; CNC, contact natural consequences; RNC, recruit natural consequences; MMC, modify maladaptive consequences; RG, reinforce occurrences of generalization; SS, sufficient stimuli; SR, sufficient responses; LD, less discriminable; S, multiple or natural settings; MA, multiple agent; PCS, program common salient stimuli; SMS, incorporate self-mediated stimuli; SM, sequential modification

Generalization Dimension

Sixteen studies (29.6%) evaluated generalization across settings or situations, 13 studies (24.1%) evaluated generalization across materials, and 8 studies (14.8%) evaluated generalization across people. Thirty-three studies (61.1%) measured generalization across time (i.e., maintenance). Fourteen studies (25.9%) did not measure any generalization dimension.

Generalization Outcomes

Of the 40 studies that measured generalization, one study (2.5%) demonstrated failure to generalize, 26 studies (65%) demonstrated partial generalization, and 11 studies (27.5%) demonstrated complete generalization. Latency to maintenance probe ranged from 1 week to 12 months. Of the 33 studies that measured maintenance, four studies (12.1%) had unclear latency. Twenty-three studies (69.7%) measured maintenance once after treatment, 3 studies (9.1%) measured twice, and 5 studies (15.2%) measured 3 or more times. The maximum number of maintenance checks was 6.

Discussion

The purpose of this review was to synthesize and evaluate the literature on family-mediated interventions in order to identify the characteristics that impact treatment effectiveness along with generalization strategies and outcomes. Fifty-four studies were included in this review. Seventy-four percent of the included studies were published in or after 2010, corroborating the recent interest in family-mediated interventions and the relative infancy of the literature on this topic. The current review focused on interventions aiming to improve social communication skills in children with ASD. Social communication was categorized into social engagement, language and communication, and imitation and play. Treatment was evaluated as slightly more effective for social engagement skills when compared to language and communication skills and imitation and play skills, although not by a wide margin.

Consistent with the parent training literature, mothers were found to participate most often in family-mediated interventions. Only four studies investigated sibling-mediated interventions, with 75% of these studies rated as weak. Treatment was found to be very effective or effective for half of the target skills. The study that received an adequate rating (Oppenheim-Leaf et al., 2012) was found to be very effective at improving sharing behaviors and effective at improving play with others. The “weak” studies were shown to be very effective at increasing verbal initiation (Ferraioli & Harris, 2011) and very effective or effective at improving joint attention skills (Ferraioli & Harris, 2011; Tsao & Odom, 2006). Qualitatively, parents reported increased cooperative play, shared enjoyment, amount of time spent together, and improved interaction quality (Ferraioli & Harris, 2011; Oppenheim-Leaf et al., 2012). Taken together, the results tentatively point towards social engagement as a target skill area for researchers and clinicians interested in involving siblings in intervention. The current evidence for sibling-mediated intervention is still weak. However, given the small number of studies, the recent emergence of research interest, and the potential for positive outcomes for children with ASD and their siblings, more high-quality research in this area is recommended.

None of the included studies involved training both parents and siblings, although two studies trained parents directly and involved siblings indirectly. In one study, parents were trained to facilitate play sessions involving mutually reinforcing activities and cooperative arrangements; one of the parent participants arranged for the child’s older sister to act as his play partner (Jull & Mirenda, 2011). This intervention was very effective at increasing synchronous reciprocal interactions between siblings. The other study trained parents to teach peer-to-peer manding; one of the parent participants chose to involve the child’s younger sister as his peer partner (Madzharova & Sturmey, 2015). In this case, the intervention was not effective at improving the child’s independent requests to the sibling. Future research could examine child and family characteristics (e.g., developmental level, communication status, and areas of strength, challenge, and interest) that determine the most beneficial model of sibling-mediated intervention for each family unit (Wright & Benigno, 2019). For instance, researchers could compare horizontal, vertical, and pyramidal approaches, in which the practitioner trains the child and sibling on the same skills together, the practitioner trains the sibling to teach skills to the child, or the practitioner trains parents to teach one or both of their children.

Telehealth is a promising intervention modality due to the potential increased access to intervention for families living in areas where access to evidence-based intervention is scarce. These interventions could also increase access for families who experience barriers related to logistical challenges such as scheduling or childcare issues. Fourteen telehealth studies were evaluated in this review. Telehealth parent training methods included live videoconferencing, self-paced videos, websites, or apps, and training manuals. Training methods typically utilized in situ, including providing instructions, modelling (in the form of video modelling), rehearsal, and feedback, were also utilized in most telehealth interventions. Treatment effects were similar for telehealth and in situ interventions, suggesting that children were as able to make gains regardless of whether parents were trained remotely or face-to-face. This represents a promising opportunity for expanding the reach of evidence-based interventions, with minimal sacrifice in terms of outcome.

Surprisingly, of the in situ interventions, no difference was found in treatment effects between home and clinic settings. We hypothesized that interventions set in the family’s home would facilitate both family and child learning; however, this was not demonstrated in the current review. Additionally, it would be reasonable to assume that home-based interventions would increase access and adherence by reducing time and resource barriers for families. Given that clinic-based interventions allow clinicians to maximize their clinical time by eliminating commutes between families, the decision to provide home or clinic-based services may be best made on a case-by-case basis.

Greater intervention intensity has historically been associated with greater child gains (e.g., Eldevik et al., 2010; Magiati et al., 2007; Virues-Ortega et al., 2013). Most of the studies in this review provided low to medium hours of intervention (≤20 h), potentially because of the inherent dynamics of involving family members. Treatment effects demonstrated mixed results based on dose, with no apparent benefit to delivering additional hours of intervention. This is consistent with recent meta-analyses that did not find treatment effects of dosage in parent-mediated interventions (Nevill et al., 2018) and social-communication interventions (Fuller & Kaiser, 2019). There may be advantages to providing lower-dose interventions, such as increasing parents’ attendance and adherence (Carr et al., 2016). When involving family members in intervention, it is important to consider its potential effects on the family as a whole and weigh the costs and benefits of increased expectations on the family member delivering the intervention. For instance, in one of the included studies, three parents reported difficulty implementing the intervention at home due to comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and two parents reported difficulty due to personal distress and lack of social support (Manohar et al., 2019). Few studies measured family outcomes other than treatment integrity and social validity. Future studies should consider the collateral effects of intervention on the child and family unit.

Three intervention packages, PRT, ESDM, and JASPER, qualified as established EBPs, while Project ImPACT qualified as a probable EBP. However, Project ImPACT and ESDM did not show strong treatment effects. All these interventions were only evaluated with parents delivering the intervention; adapting these interventions for sibling involvement may provide further opportunities to improve outcomes for the child and family. Several other intervention packages were shown to be very effective or effective for at least two-thirds of the target skills, even though their Z scores were not yet high enough to qualify as established or probable EBP. These packages include milieu training, PECS, VMIT, Pathways, and Sunny Starts/DANCE. More research evaluating these intervention packages is recommended. Similarly, focused intervention practices such as FBA, DTT, FCT, PECS, and scripting showed promising treatment effects but were only utilized in a small number of studies. Incorporating these FIPs into other intervention packages could potentially improve outcomes, depending on child characteristics and target skills.

A major consideration of family-mediated intervention is the potential for generalization, which is often challenging for children with ASD (Vismara & Rogers, 2010). While it is discouraging that a quarter of studies did not evaluate any generalization dimension, it is assuring that among the studies that did evaluate at least one generalization dimension, over 90% demonstrated complete or partial generalization outcomes. Several strategies for promoting generalization are intrinsic to family-mediated intervention, such as recruiting natural contingencies and programming common stimuli. Potentially due to the primary role of family members in family-mediated interventions, addressing functional behaviors, contacting natural reinforcement, and conducting intervention in natural or multiple settings were also prevalent. Less utilized strategies included modifying maladaptive consequences, reinforcing occurrences of generalization, making antecedents or consequences less discriminable, and teaching skills across different people. These strategies are less inherent in family-mediated intervention and would require conscious effort on the part of the clinician to incorporate these strategies. The least utilized generalization promotion strategy was sequential modification, in which further strategies are employed to promote generalization if generalization results are not satisfactory. This could be due to the limitations of conducting research studies (e.g., time constraints) and may not reflect the reality occurring in clinical intervention. The lack of studies that take place in the clinical “real world” has been previously noted (Beaudoin et al., 2014).

Limitations

The articles included in this review were evaluated for quality using Reichow and colleagues’ (2008) operationalized rubric. It should be noted that this evaluative method has different criteria for SSRD and group comparison designs. Primary quality indicators for SSRD rely on strong research design as well as positive results (i.e., baseline, visual analysis, experimental control), while primary quality indicators for group comparison designs rely on strong research design alone, with no indicators related to results. Additionally, the stringent requirements for primary quality indicators lead to an “unacceptable” rating for baseline if even one out of several baselines is unstable, and an “unacceptable” rating for experimental control if even one out of several data paths did not demonstrate change in the dependent variable with introduction of the independent variable. This may explain the large proportion of “weak” ratings for studies that employed SSRD. Likewise, studies that employed a group design could obtain a “strong” rating even if results were negative or inconclusive. Previous reviews (e.g., Ferguson et al., 2018; Tomlinson et al., 2018) have noted similar issues using this evaluative method. In the current review, these potential discrepancies were partly accounted for by including additional evaluation of treatment effects using PND and Cohen’s d. Future reviews could evaluate methodological rigor using additional or alternative operationalized methods (e.g., What Works Clearinghouse, 2010).

Treatment effects were only calculated for primary outcome variables, i.e., target skills. Treatment effects were not calculated for secondary outcomes. Future studies could evaluate the relationship between target skills and collateral effects, as this could provide valuable information on which target skills would have the widest-reaching impact on the child and family unit. Collateral benefits have been shown when social communication skills are improved (Ledbetter-Cho et al., 2017); however, this has not been systematically evaluated in family-mediated interventions.

Summary

In the current review, parent-mediated PRT, ESDM, and JASPER were the only intervention packages that qualified as established EBP. Treatment was more effective for social engagement skills compared to language and communication skills and imitation and play skills. No major differences in treatment effects were found based on intervention agent, treatment modality, setting, or dosage. Generalization outcomes were encouraging, although more focus should be placed on incorporating additional generalization promotion strategies into family-mediated interventions. Sibling-mediated intervention, while still in its infancy in the literature, has the potential for meaningful impact when incorporated into clinical practice. A further avenue of research could be to explore interventions that involve the complete family unit by addressing motivational variables and intervention outcomes of parents, siblings, and children with ASD to promote integrated learning and benefits within the family. Telehealth is also a promising area of further research, not only during the current climate of social restrictions due to the COVID-19 pandemic, but also when moving forward to reach families who previously had limited access to evidence-based intervention.

Declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. Abbeduto L, Seltzer MM, Shattuck P, Krauss MW, Orsmond G, Murphy MM. Psychological well-being and coping in mothers of youth with autism, Down syndrome, or Fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Retardation. 2004;109(3):237. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2004)109&#x0003c;237:pwacim&#x0003e;2.0.co;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akamoglu Y, Meadan H. Parent-implemented language and communication interventions for children with developmental delays and disabilities: a scoping review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2018;5(3):294–309. doi: 10.1007/s40489-018-0140-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. (Fifth). American Psychiatric Association.
  4. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren Z, Kurzius-Spencer M, Zahorodny W, Rosenberg CR, White T, Durkin MS, Imm P, Nikolaou L, Yeargin-Allsopp M, Lee L, Harrington R, Lopez M, Fitzgerald RT, Hewitt A, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2014. MMWR. Surveillance Summaries. 2018;67(6):1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beaudoin, A. J., Sébire, G., & Couture, M. (2014). Parent training interventions for toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research and Treatment, 1–15. 10.1155/2014/839890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  6. Black ME, Therrien WJ. Parent training programs for school-age children with autism: a systematic review. Remedial and Special Education. 2018;39(4):243–256. doi: 10.1177/0741932517730645. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Boisvert M, Lang R, Andrianopoulos M, Boscardin ML. Telepractice in the assessment and treatment of individuals with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Developmental Neurorehabilitation. 2010;13(6):423–432. doi: 10.3109/17518423.2010.499889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Campbell JM. Commentary on PND at 25. Remedial and special education. 2013;34(1):20–25. doi: 10.1177/0741932512454725. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. *Cardon, T. A. (2012). Teaching caregivers to implement video modeling imitation training via iPad for their children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(4), 1389–1400. 10.1016/j.rasd.2012.06.002
  10. Carr ME. A sensitivity analysis of three nonparametric treatment effect scores for single-case research for participants with autism. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;2(1):67–78. doi: 10.1007/s40489-014-0037-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Carr T, Shih W, Lawton K, Lord C, King B, Kasari C. The relationship between treatment attendance, adherence, and outcome in a caregiver-mediated intervention for low-resourced families of young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2016;20(6):643–652. doi: 10.1177/1362361315598634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carter, E. W., Cushing, L. S., & Kennedy, C. H. (2008). Peer support strategies for improving all students’ social lives and learning. Paul H Brookes Publishing Company.
  13. Chandler LK, Lubeck RC, Fowler SA. Generalization and maintenance of preschool children’s social skills: a critical review and analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1992;25(2):415–428. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1992.25-415. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Charlop-Christy MH, Carpenter M, Le L, LeBlanc LA, Kellet K. Using the picture exchange communication system (PECS) with children with autism: assessment of PECS acquisition, speech, social-communicative behavior, and problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 2002;35(3):213–231. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2002.35-213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum.
  16. *Coolican, J., Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (2010). Brief parent training in pivotal response treatment for preschoolers with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(12), 1321–1330. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02326.x [DOI] [PubMed]
  17. Division for Early Childhood (2014). DEC recommended practices in early intervention/early childhood special education 2014. Division for Early Childhood. http://www.decsped.org/recommendedpractices
  18. Dudding CC. Digital videoconferencing: applications across the disciplines. Communication Disorders Quarterly. 2009;30(3):178–182. doi: 10.1177/1525740108327449. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dumas JE, Wolf LC, Fisman SN, Culligan A. Parenting stress, child behavior problems, and dysphoria in parents of children with autism, Down syndrome, behavior disorders, and normal development. Exceptionality. 1991;2(2):97–110. doi: 10.1080/09362839109524770. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Eldevik S, Hastings RP, Hughes JC, Jahr E, Eikeseth S, Cross S. Eti using participant data to extend the evidence base for intensive behavioral intervention for children with autism. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. 2010;115(5):381–405. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-115.5.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ferguson J, Craig EA, Dounavi K, Craig EA. Telehealth as a model for providing behaviour analytic interventions to individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2018;49:582–616. doi: 10.1007/s10803-018-3724-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. *Ferraioli, S. J., & Harris, S. L. (2011). Teaching joint attention to children with autism through a sibling-mediated behavioral intervention. Behavioral Interventions, 26(4), 261–281. 10.1002/bin.336
  23. Fuller EA, Kaiser AP. The effects of early intervention on social communication outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2019;50:1683–1700. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03927-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. *Gengoux, G. W., Berquist, K. L., Salzman, E., Schapp, S., Phillips, J. M., Frazier, T. W., Minjarez, M.B., Hardan, A. Y. (2015). Pivotal response treatment parent training for autism: findings from a 3-month follow-up evaluation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(9), 2889–2898. 10.1007/s10803-015-2452-3 [DOI] [PubMed]
  25. Gerow S, Hagan-Burke S, Rispoli M, Gregori E, Mason R, Ninci J. A systematic review of parent-implemented functional communication training for children with ASD. Behavior Modification. 2018;42(3):335–363. doi: 10.1177/0145445517740872. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gianoumis S, Seiverling L, Sturmey P. The effects of behavior skills training on correct teacher implementation of natural language paradigm teaching skills and child behavior. Behavioral Interventions. 2012;27(2):57–74. doi: 10.1002/bin.1334. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gillberg C, Steffenburg S. Outcome and prognostic factors in infantile autism and similar conditions: a population-based study of 46 cases followed through puberty. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1987;17(2):273–287. doi: 10.1007/BF01495061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. *Gillett, J. N., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2007). Parent-implemented natural language paradigm to increase language and play in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(3), 247–255. 10.1016/j.rasd.2006.09.003
  29. Guðmundsdóttir K, Sigurðardóttir ZG, Ala’i-Rosales S. Evaluation of caregiver training via telecommunication for rural Icelandic children with autism. Behavioral Development Bulletin. 2017;22(1):215–229. doi: 10.1037/bdb0000040. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. *Guðmundsdóttir, K., Ala’i-Rosales, S. & Sigurðardóttir, Z. G. (2019). Extending caregiver training via telecommunication for rural Icelandic children with autism. Rural Special Education Quarterly, 38(1) 26–42. 10.1177/8756870518783522
  31. Gunning C, Breathnach Ó, Holloway J, McTiernan A, Malone B. A systematic review of peer-mediated interventions for preschool children with autism spectrum disorder in inclusive settings. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2019;6(1):40–62. doi: 10.1007/s40489-018-0153-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gunning C, Holloway J, Fee B, Breathnach Ó, Bergin CM, Greene I, Ni Bheolain R. A systematic review of generalization and maintenance outcomes of social skills intervention for preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2019;6(2):172–199. doi: 10.1007/s40489-019-00162-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. *Hansen, B., & Shillingsburg, M. A. (2016). Using a modified parent-child interaction therapy to increase vocalizations in children with autism. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 38(4), 318–330. 10.1080/07317107.2016.1238692
  34. *Hansen, S. G., Raulston, T. J., Machalicek, W., & Frantz, R. (2018). Caregiver-mediated joint attention intervention. Behavioral Interventions, 33(2), 205–211. 10.1002/bin.1523
  35. *Hardan, A. Y., Gengoux, G. W., Berquist, K. L., Libove, R. A., Ardel, C. M., Phillips, J., Frazier, T.W., & Minjarez, M. B. (2015). A randomized controlled trial of pivotal response treatment group for parents of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 56(8), 884–892. 10.1111/jcpp.12354 [DOI] [PubMed]
  36. Hong ER, Neely L, Gerow S, Gann C. The effect of caregiver-delivered social-communication interventions on skill generalization and maintenance in ASD. Research in Developmental Disabilities. 2018;74(December 2017):57–71. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2018.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. *Hong, E.R., Gong, L., Ganz, J. B., & Neely, L. (2018b). Self-paced and video-based learning: parent training and language skills in Japanese children with ASD. Exceptionality Education International, 28, 1-19. Retrieved from https://ir.lib.uwo.ca/eei/vol28/iss2/1
  38. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(2):212–229. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Ingersoll B. The social role of imitation in autism: implications for the treatment of imitation deficits. Infants & Young Children. 2008;21(2):107–119. doi: 10.1097/01.IYC.0000314482.24087.14. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ingersoll, B., & Dvortcsak, A. (2010). Teaching social communication to children with autism: a practioners guide to parent training. Guilford Press.
  41. *Ingersoll, B., & Gergans, S. (2007). The effect of a parent-implemented imitation intervention on spontaneous imitation skills in young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(2), 163–175. 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed]
  42. *Ingersoll, B., & Wainer, A. (2013). Initial efficacy of project ImPACT: a parent-mediated social communication intervention for young children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(12), 2943–2952. 10.1007/s10803-013-1840-9 [DOI] [PubMed]
  43. *Ingersoll, B., Wainer, A. L., Berger, N. I., Pickard, K. E., & Bonter, N. (2016). Comparison of a self-directed and therapist-assisted telehealth parent-mediated intervention for children with ASD: a pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(7), 2275–2284. 10.1007/s10803-016-2755-z [DOI] [PubMed]
  44. *Jones, E. A., & Feeley, K. M. (2009). Parent implemented joint attention intervention for preschoolers with autism. The Journal of Speech and Language Pathology – Applied Behavior Analysis, 4(1), 74–89. 10.1037/h0100251
  45. *Jull, S., & Mirenda, P. (2011). Parents as play date facilitators for preschoolers with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 13(1), 17–30. 10.1177/1098300709358111
  46. *Kaiser, A. P., Hancock, T. B., & Nietfeld, J. P. (2000). The effects of parent-implemented enhanced milieu teaching on the social communication of children who have autism. Early Education & Development, 11(4), 423–446. 10.1207/s15566935eed1104_4
  47. *Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A. C., Wong, C., Kwon, S., & Locke, J. (2010). Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(9), 1045-1056. 10.1007/s10803-010-0955-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  48. *Kasari, C., Lawton, K., Shih, W., Barker, T. V, Landa, R., Lord, C., Orlich, F., King, B., Wetherby, A., & Senturk, D. (2014). Caregiver-mediated intervention for low-resourced preschoolers with autism: an RCT. Pediatrics, 134(1), e72–e79. 10.1542/peds.2013-3229 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  49. *Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A., Paparella, T., Hellemann, G., & Berry, K. (2015). Randomized comparative efficacy study of parent-mediated interventions for toddlers with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 83(3), 554–563. 10.1037/a0039080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  50. *Kashinath, S., Woods, J., & Goldstein, H. (2006). Enhancing generalized teaching strategy use in daily routines by parents of children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(3), 466–485. 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/036) [DOI] [PubMed]
  51. Keen D, Couzens D, Muspratt S, Rodger S. The effects of a parent-focused intervention for children with a recent diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder on parenting stress and competence. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders. 2010;4(2):229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2009.09.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  52. Knutsen J, Wolfe A, Burke BL, Hepburn S, Lindgren S, Coury D. A systematic review of telemedicine in autism spectrum disorders. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2016;3(4):330–344. doi: 10.1007/s40489-016-0086-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Koegel, R., Schreibman, L., Britten, K. R., Burke, J. C., & O’Neill, R. E. (1982). A comparison of parent training to direct child treatment. In R. L. Koegel, A. Rincover, & A. L. Egel (Eds.), Educating and understanding autistic children (pp. 260–279). College-Hill Press.
  54. Koegel RL, O’Dell MC, Koegel LK. A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1987;17(2):187–200. doi: 10.1007/BF01495055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Koegel RL, Bimbela A, Schreibman L. Collateral effects of parent training on family interactions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 1996;26(3):347–359. doi: 10.1007/BF02172479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. *Koegel, R. L., Symon, J. B., & Kern Koegel, L. (2002). Parent education for families of children with autism living in geographically distant areas. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4(2), 88-103. 10.1177/109830070200400204
  57. Koegel, R. L., Bruinsma, Y., & Koegel, L. K. (2006). Developmental trajectories in early intervention. In Pivotal Response Treatments for autism (pp. 131–140). Brookes.
  58. Kogan MD, Strickland BB, Blumberg SJ, Singh GK, Perrin JM, van Dyck PC. A national profile of the health care experiences and family impact of autism spectrum disorder among children in the United States, 2005-2006. Pediatrics. 2008;122(6):e1149–e1158. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. *Lane, J. D., Ledford, J. R., Shepley, C., Mataras, T. K., Ayres, K. M., & Davis, A. B. (2016). A brief coaching intervention for teaching naturalistic strategies to parents. Journal of Early Intervention, 38(3), 135–150. 10.1177/1053815116663178
  60. *Law, G. C., Neihart, M., & Dutt, A. (2018). The use of behavior modeling training in a mobile app parent training program to improve functional communication of young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 22(4), 424–439. 10.1177/1362361316683887 [DOI] [PubMed]
  61. Ledbetter-Cho K, Lang R, Watkins L, O’Reilly M, Zamora C. Systematic review of collateral effects of focused interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments. 2017;2:239694151773753. doi: 10.1177/2396941517737536. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Liptak GS, Benzoni LB, Mruzek DW, Nolan KW, Thingvoll MA, Wade CM, Fryer GE. Disparities in diagnosis and access to health services for children with autism: data from the National Survey of Children’s Health. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2008;29(3):152–160. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e318165c7a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. *Loughrey, T. O., Contreras, B. P., Majdalany, L. M., Rudy, N., Sinn, S., Teague, P., Marshall, G., McGreevy, P, & Harvey, A. C. (2014). Caregivers as interventionists and trainers: teaching mands to children with developmental disabilities. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 30(2), 128–140. 10.1007/s40616-014-0005-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  64. *Madzharova, M. S., & Sturmey, P. (2015). Effects of video modeling and feedback on mothers’ implementation of peer-to-peer manding. Behavioral Interventions, 30, 270–285. 10.1002/bin.1414
  65. Magiati I, Charman T, Howlin P. A two-year prospective follow-up study of community-based early intensive behavioural intervention and specialist nursery provision for children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 2007;48(8):803–813. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01756.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. *Mancil, G. R., Conroy, M. A., & Haydon, T. F. (2009). Effects of a modified milieu therapy intervention on the social communicative behaviors of young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 149–163. 10.1007/s10803-008-0613-3 [DOI] [PubMed]
  67. Mandell DS, Morales KH, Xie M, Lawer LJ, Stahmer AC, Marcus SC. Age of diagnosis among Medicaid-enrolled children with autism, 2001–2004. Psychiatric Services. 2010;61(8):822–829. doi: 10.1176/ps.2010.61.8.822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. *Manohar, H., Kandasamy, P., Chandrasekaran, V., & Philip, R. (2019). Brief parent-mediated intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder: a feasibility study from South India. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(8), 3146–3158. 10.1007/s10803-019-04032-x [DOI] [PubMed]
  69. McConachie H, Diggle T. Parent implemented early intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice. 2007;13(1):120–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2006.00674.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. *Mcduffie, A., Machalicek, W., Oakes, A., Haebig, E., Weismer, S. E., & Abbeduto, L. (2013). Distance video-teleconferencing in early intervention: pilot study of a naturalistic parent-implemented language intervention. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 33(3), 172–185. 10.1177/0271121413476348
  71. *Mcgarry, E., Vernon, T., & Baktha, A. (2019). Brief report: a pilot online pivotal response treatment training program for parents of toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 10.1007/s10803-019-04100-2 [DOI] [PubMed]
  72. *Meadan, H., Snodgrass, M. R., Meyer, L. E., Fisher, K. W., Chung, M. Y., & Halle, J. W. (2016). Internet-based parent-implemented intervention for young children with autism: a pilot study. Journal of Early Intervention, 38(1), 3–23. 10.1177/1053815116630327
  73. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine. 2009;6(7):1–6. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. National Autism Centre. (2009). National Standards Report. National Autism Centre.
  75. National Autism Centre. (2015). Findings and Conclusions: National Standards Project, Phase 2. National Autism Centre.
  76. National Research Council. (2001). Educating children with autism. The National Academies Press.
  77. Neely, L. C., Ganz, J. B., Davis, J. L., Boles, M. B., Hong, E. R., Ninci, J., & Gilliland,W. D. (2015). Generalization and maintenance of functional living skills for individuals with autism spectrum disorder: a review and meta-analysis. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3, 37–47. 10.1007/s40489-015-0064-7. [DOI] [PubMed]
  78. *Nefdt, N., Koegel, R., Singer, G., & Gerber, M. (2010). The use of a self-directed learning program to provide introductory training in pivotal response treatment to parents of children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 12(1), 23–32. 10.1177/1098300709334796
  79. Nevill RE, Lecavalier L, Stratis EA. Meta-analysis of parent-mediated interventions for young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2018;22(2):84–98. doi: 10.1177/1362361316677838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. *Nunes, D., & Hanline, M. F. (2007). Enhancing the alternative and augmentative communication use of a child with Autism through a parent-implemented naturalistic intervention. International Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 54(2), 177–197. 10.1080/10349120701330495
  81. *Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Leaf, J. B., Dozier, C., Sheldon, J. B., & Sherman, J. A. (2012). Teaching typically developing children to promote social play with their siblings with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 777–791. 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.10.010
  82. *Park, J. H., Alber-Morgan, S. R., & Cannella-Malone, H. (2011). Effects of mother-implemented Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS) training on independent communicative behaviors of young children with autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 31(1), 37–47. 10.1177/0271121410393750
  83. Parker RI, Hagan-Burke S, Vannest K. Percentage of All Non-Overlapping Data (PAND) The Journal of Special Education. 2007;40(4):194–204. doi: 10.1177/00224669070400040101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  84. *Penney, A., & Schwartz, I. (2019). Effects of coaching on the fidelity of parent implementation of reciprocal imitation training. Autism, 23(6), 1497-1507. 10.1177/1362361318816688 [DOI] [PubMed]
  85. Pickles A, Le Couteur A, Leadbitter K, Salomone E, Cole-Fletcher R, Tobin H, Gammer I, Lowry J, Vamvakas G, Byford S, Aldred C, Slonims V, McConachie H, Howlin P, Parr JR, Charman T, Green J. Parent-mediated social communication therapy for young children with autism (PACT): long-term follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet. 2016;388(10059):2501–2509. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31229-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. *Randolph, J. K., Stichter, J. P., Schmidt, C. T., & O’Connor, K. V. (2011). Fidelity and effectiveness of PRT implemented by caregivers without college degrees. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 26(4), 230–238. 10.1177/1088357611421503
  87. *Reagon, K. A., & Higbee, T. S. (2009). Parent-implemented script fading to promote play-based verbal initiations in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(3), 659–664. 10.1901/jaba.2009.42-659 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  88. Reichow B, Volkmar FR, Cicchetti DV. Development of the evaluative method for evaluating and determining evidence-based practices in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2008;38:1311–1319. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0517-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. *Rocha, M. L., Schreibman, L., & Stahmer, A. C. (2007). Effectiveness of training parents to teach joint attention in children with autism. Journal of Early Intervention, 29(2), 154–172. 10.1177/105381510702900207
  90. Rogers, S. J., Dawson, G., Zierhut, C., Winter, J., McCormick, C., & Holly, E. (2013). The PATH curriculum checklist for young children with autism. (unpublished document).
  91. *Rogers, S. J., Estes, A., Vismara, L., Munson, J., Zierhut, C., Greenson, J., Dawson, G., Rocha, M., Sugar, C., Senturk, D., Whelan, F., & Talbott, M. (2019). Enhancing low-intensity coaching in parent implemented Early Start Denver Model intervention for early autism: a randomized comparison treatment trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(2), 632–646. 10.1007/s10803-018-3740-5 [DOI] [PubMed]
  92. *Rollins, P. R., Campbell, M., Hoffman, R. T., & Self, K. (2016). A community-based early intervention program for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 20(2), 219–232. 10.1177/1362361315577217 [DOI] [PubMed]
  93. Schlosser RW, Lee DL, Wendt O. Application of the percentage of non-overlapping data (PND) in systematic reviews and meta-analyses: a systematic review of reporting characteristics. Evidence-Based Communication Assessment and Intervention. 2008;2(3):163–187. doi: 10.1080/17489530802505412. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Scholpler, E., Reichler, R., & Renner, B. (1988). The childhood autism rating scale. Western Psychological Services.
  95. Scruggs TE, Mastropieri MA. Summarizing single-subject research. Behavior Modification. 1998;22(3):221–242. doi: 10.1177/01454455980223001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Scruggs, T. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Casto, G. (1987). The quantitative synthesis of single-subject research: methodology and validation. Remedial and Special Education, 8(2), 24–33. 10.1177/074193258700800206.
  97. Shivers CM, Plavnick JB. Sibling involvement in interventions for indiciduals with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(3):685–696. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2222-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. *Simacek, J., Dimian, A. F., & McComas, J. J. (2017). Communication intervention for young children with severe neurodevelopmental disabilities via telehealth. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47(3), 744–767. 10.1007/s10803-016-3006-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  99. Smith LE, Hong J, Seltzer MM, Greenberg JS, Almeida DM, Bishop SL. Daily experiences among mothers of adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2010;40(2):167–178. doi: 10.1007/s10803-009-0844-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Sperry L, Neitzel J, Engelhardt-Wells K. Peer-mediated instruction and intervention strategies for students with autism spectrum disorders. Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for Children and Youth. 2010;54(4):256–264. doi: 10.1080/10459881003800529. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  101. *Stahmer, A. C., & Gist, K. (2001). The effects of an accelerated parent education program on tenchnique mastery and child outcome. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 3(2), 75–82. 10.1177/109830070100300203
  102. Stokes TF, Baer DM. An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. 1977;10(2):1311194. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1977.10-349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Stokes TF, Osnes PG. An operant pursuit of generalization. Behavior Therapy. 1989;20(3):337–355. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7894(89)80054-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Swan, A. J., Carper, M. M., & Kendall, P. C. (2016). In pursuit of generalization: an updated review. Behavior Therapy, 47, 733–746. 10.1016/j.beth.2015.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed]
  105. *Symon, J. B. (2005). Expanding interventions for children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7(3), 159–173. 10.1177/10983007050070030501
  106. Tomlinson SRL, Gore N, Mcgill P. Training individuals to implement applied behaviour analytic procedures vis telehealth: a systematic review of the literature. Journal of Behavioral Education. 2018;27:172–222. doi: 10.1007/s10864-018-9292-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  107. Tonge B, Brereton A, Kiomall M, Mackinnon A, King N, Rinehart N. Effects on parental mental health of an education and skills training program for parents of young children with autism: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 2006;45(5):561–569. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000205701.48324.26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Trembath D, Gurm M, Scheerer NE, Trevisan DA, Paynter J, Bohadana G, Roberts J, Iarocci G. Systematic review of factors that may influence the outcomes and generalizability of parent-mediated interventions for young children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research. 2019;12:1304–1321. doi: 10.1002/aur.2168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. *Tsao, L.-L., & Odom, S. L. (2006). Sibling-mediated social interaction intervention for young children with autism. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 26(2), 106–123. 10.1177/02711214060260020101
  110. *Vernon, T. W., Koegel, R. L., Dauterman, H., & Stolen, K. (2012). An early social engagement intervention for young children with autism and their parents. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2702–2717. 10.1007/s10803-012-1535-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  111. Virues-Ortega J, Rodríguez V, Yua CT. Prediction of treatment outcomes and longitudinal analysis in children with autism undergoing intensive behavioral intervention. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology. 2013;13(2):91–100. doi: 10.1016/S1697-2600(13)70012-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  112. Vismara LA, Rogers SJ. The Early Start Denver Model. Journal of Early Intervention. 2008;31(1):91–108. doi: 10.1177/1053815108325578. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  113. Vismara LA, Rogers SJ. Behavioral treatments in autism spectrum disorder: what do we know? Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2010;6(1):447–468. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. *Vismara, L. A., Colombi, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2009). Can one hour per week of therapy lead to lasting changes in young children with autism? Autism, 13(1), 93–115. 10.1177/1362361307098516 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  115. *Vismara, L. A., McCormick, C., Young, G. S., Nadhan, A., & Monlux, K. (2013). Preliminary findings of a telehealth approach to parent training in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(12), 2953–2969. 10.1007/s10803-013-1841-8 [DOI] [PubMed]
  116. *Vismara, L. A., McCormick, C. E. B., Wagner, A. L., Monlux, K., Nadhan, A., & Young, G. S. (2018). Telehealth parent training in the Early Start Denver Model: results from a randomized controlled study. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 33(2), 67–79. 10.1177/1088357616651064
  117. *Wainer, A. L., & Ingersoll, B. R. (2013). Disseminating ASD interventions: a pilot study of a distance learning program for parents and professionals. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(1), 11–24. 10.1007/s10803-012-1538-4 [DOI] [PubMed]
  118. *Wainer, A. L., & Ingersoll, B. R. (2015). Increasing access to an ASD imitation intervention vis a telehealth parent training program. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(12), 3877–3890. 10.1007/s10803-014-2186-7 [DOI] [PubMed]
  119. *Walton, K. M., & Ingersoll, B. R. (2012). Evaluation of a sibling-mediated imitation intervention for young children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 14(4), 241–253. 10.1177/1098300712437044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  120. *Wang, H. T. (2017). Utilizing primary tier intervention to enhance reciprocal turn-taking of children with autism in Taiwan. Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 52(1), 64–76. 10.1107/S1600536806016333
  121. Warner, R. M. (2013). Applied statistics: from bivariate through multivariate techniques (2nd.). SAGE Publications.
  122. Wetherby AM, Woods JJ. Early Social Interaction Project for children in the second year of life: a preliminary study. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education. 2006;26(2):67–82. doi: 10.1177/02711214060260020201. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  123. What Works Clearinghouse. (2010). What Works Clearinghouse Standards Handbook. What Works Clearinghouse, 0(July), 1–17. 10.1037/e578392011-004
  124. Wong C, Odom SL, Hume KA, Cox AW, Fettig A, Kucharczyk S, et al. Evidence-based practices for children, youth, and young adults with autism spectrum disorder: a comprehensive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2015;45(7):1951–1966. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2351-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Wright BM, Benigno JP. Autism spectrum disorder and sibling relationships: exploring implications for intervention using a family systems framework. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology. 2019;28(2):759–767. doi: 10.1044/2018_AJSLP-18-0088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. *Zaghlawan, H. Y., & Ostrosky, M. M. (2016). A parent-implemented intervention to improve imitation skills by children with autism: a pilot study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 44(6), 671–680. 10.1007/s10643-015-0753-y

Articles from Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES