Skip to main content
Wiley - PMC COVID-19 Collection logoLink to Wiley - PMC COVID-19 Collection
. 2021 Jan 16;20(3):545–571. doi: 10.1111/1745-9133.12535

What does the public want police to do during pandemics? A national experiment

Justin Nix 1,, Stefan Ivanov 2, Justin T Pickett 3
PMCID: PMC8013863  PMID: 33821153

Abstract

Research Summary

We administered a survey experiment to a national sample of 1068 U.S. adults in April 2020 to determine the factors that shape support for various policing tactics in the midst of the COVID‐19 pandemic. Respondents were sharply divided in their views about pandemic policing tactics and were least supportive of policies that might limit public access to officers or reduce crime deterrence. Information about the health risks to officers, but not to inmates, significantly increased support for “precautionary” policing, but not for “social distance” policing. The information effect was modest, but may be larger if the information came from official sources and/or was communicated on multiple occasions. Other factors that are associated with attitudes toward pandemic policing include perceptions of procedural justice, altruistic fear, racial resentment, and authoritarianism.

Policy Implications

When considered together with other evidence, one clear takeaway from our study is that the public values police patrols and wants officers on call, even during pandemics. Another is that people who believe the police are procedurally just are more willing to trust officers in times of crisis and to empower them to enforce new laws, such as social distancing ordinances. Our results thus support continued procedural justice training for officers. A third takeaway is that agencies must proactively communicate with the public about the risks their officers face when responding to public health crises or natural disasters, in addition to how they propose to mitigate those risks. They must also be amenable to adjusting in response to community feedback.

Keywords: COVID‐19, deterrence, legitimacy, policing, public health, public opinion

1. INTRODUCTION

In January 2020, the World Health Organization identified a new coronavirus, COVID‐19, which has since spread rapidly throughout the world. By March, they had classified it as a global pandemic. At the time of this writing, the virus has infected over 29 million people worldwide and claimed the lives of over 930,000. The United States has been hit especially hard, with over 6.5 million confirmed cases and 195,000 deaths (CDC, 2020). In the absence of a vaccine, the virus has caused significant societal disruptions to those not infected—including the cancellation of sporting events, festivals, and concerts; the suspension of in‐person K‐12 and college classes; closure of nonessential businesses; and in many places, orders from government officials to stay at home and practice “social distancing.” The shutdowns have had dire economic consequences. In May, the U.S. Labor Department reported that 36.5 million Americans had filed for unemployment in the previous eight weeks. Such abrupt changes to citizens’ daily lives have major consequences for the criminal justice system (Jennings & Perez, 2020; Lum, Maupin, & Stoltz, 2020).

The police, in particular, are faced with three primary concerns. The first is that, like everyone else, officers are at risk of contracting COVID‐19. In New York City, a hotspot for the virus, over 5000 members of the NYPD had tested positive, and 41 had died, as of early May (Rummler, 2020). The Detroit Police Department was similarly ravaged by the virus: 6% of uniformed officers tested positive—including the Chief—and nearly 36% were quarantined at some point (Eligon & MacFarquhar, 2020). Such alarming statistics, coupled with a shortage of personal protective equipment (PPE) 1 , suggest a need for precautionary policing—that is, cutting back on self‐initiated activities and enforcement of low‐level crimes. Indeed, a survey of approximately 1000 police departments conducted in March revealed that 61% had “adopted formal policies to reduce or limit proactive traffic or pedestrian stops” (Lum et al., 2020). For example, the South Burlington (VT) Police Department instructed officers to limit proactive traffic stops to “more serious violations.” Meanwhile, Gloucester Township (NJ) police officers were told “pedestrian contacts and stops will only be conducted when necessary for public safety or caretaking purposes.” 2 In theory, deemphasizing these sorts of enforcement activities should protect both officers and citizens by providing fewer opportunities for the virus to be transmitted.

A second, related concern for the police is reducing jail populations. Many states have made conscious efforts to reduce jail and prison populations to avoid the spread of COVID‐19 among inmates (Nowotny, Bailey, Omori, & Brinkley‐Rubinstein, 2020; Saloner, Parish, Ward, DiLaura, & Dolovich, 2020). Here again, precautionary policing is one obvious way to do this. As but one example, the Rockford (IL) Police Department ordered its officers to issue Notices to Appear, rather than making custodial arrests, for misdemeanor crimes in the interest of reducing the jail population. Even where police departments have not instructed their officers to minimize self‐initiated contact with citizens, the courts might nevertheless induce officers to cut back on arrests by changing bail requirements or refusing to prosecute certain offenses. Similarly, correctional facilities may adopt policies limiting the types of arrestees they will admit. According to Lum et al. (2020, p. 1), 65% of responding agencies “noted that the jail or correctional facility that intakes agency arrestees had already restricted the types of arrestees it would receive because of COVID‐19 (e.g., misdemeanors or those who seemed ill).” Thus, whether due to departmental guidance or in response to policies adopted by the courts and/or correctional system, police officers find themselves using more discretion about who they arrest for low‐level offenses in the midst of the pandemic.

The third concern is quite different from the previous two. In some places, such as New York City, police were directed to enforce social distancing ordinances (e.g., stay at home orders, prohibitions against large gatherings). Though in the interest of public health, it was fiercely opposed by the Police Benevolent Association (the union representing NYPD officers), which called social distancing enforcement “untenable” and warned that “police officers should be allowed to focus on our core public safety mission,” or “the city will fall apart before our eyes” (Axelrod, 2020). Additionally, social distancing enforcement gives officers discretion to police new types of offenses (e.g., gathering in public, not wearing a mask), which carries the potential for racial disparities. In the Brooklyn Borough, officers arrested 40 people for social distancing violations from March 17th to May 4th—35 of whom were Black (Southall, 2020; see also Kaste, 2020). Social distancing enforcement is also opposed by some segments of the general population, who see it as government interference with their individual liberties, and who have begun protesting and filing lawsuits in response (Hoskin, 2020; Mervosh, Lu, & Swales, 2020).

Several critical but as yet unanswered questions remain. One is simply: What sort of policing does the public want during the pandemic? It is the public, after all, who empower the police to do their jobs (Sunshine & Tyler, 2003), whose trust is necessary for effective policing (Peyton, Sierra‐Arévalo, & Rand, 2019), and whose attitudes determine the sustainability of criminal justice policy and practice (Pickett, 2019). In fact, it appears that public pressure may be deterring officers from enforcing social distancing in some areas (Philips, 2020). If the public is informed about the health risks for officers of policing during a pandemic, will they support pulling back on traditional enforcement? Would information about the health risks to jail and prison inmates have similar effects? Or, will the public be less able to “empathetically identify” (Unnever & Cullen, 2009, p. 284) with inmates than officers? How will different types of health‐risk information affect views about social distance policing?

Another important question is: Who is most supportive of precautionary and social distance policing? Prior research provides clues about how people might perceive precautionary policing and social distancing enforcement. For example, because social distance policing may give officers more discretion to enforce new offenses, whites and those who perceive police as procedurally fair may be more supportive (Weitzer & Tuch, 2006; Wolfe, Nix, Kaminski, & Rojek, 2016). We might also expect that factors that increase punitiveness in general, such as racial resentment, authoritarianism, and conservativism, will also foster opposition to allowing police to “pull back” in the name of public health. Indeed, general punitiveness is a strong predictor of support for aggressive policing (Johnson & Kuhn, 2009; Pickett, 2016; Silver & Pickett, 2015). Alternatively, for both rational and altruistic reasons, those who are more fearful of contracting the virus, or concerned about others getting sick, might be more supportive of precautionary and social distance policing. We know that both personal fear and altruistic fear influence precautionary behaviors in other contexts (Drakulich, 2015; Warr & Ellison, 2000).

To begin answering some of these questions, we conducted an experiment with a nationwide survey of 1068 U.S. adults in April 2020, soon after most states issued lockdown orders and police agencies began grappling with how to enforce social distancing ordinances. Respondents were randomly assigned to either receive no relevant information (control group) or to receive information about COVID‐19′s threat to police health or inmate health. They were then asked a series of questions about how the police should function during the COVID‐19 pandemic. We also measured theoretical predictors. Results suggest that the public is split on how officers should police pandemics, that information about officer health risks increases support for pulling back on traditional enforcement, and that two consistent and strong predictors are racial resentment and altruistic fear. Racially resentful respondents tend to oppose pandemic‐based changes in policing, whereas altruistically fearful respondents tend to support changes.

2. HOW TO POLICE PANDEMICS: AN OPEN QUESTION

The function of police during a pandemic—just as in the absence of a pandemic—is to protect the community (Bittner, 1970), but the pandemic adds a new threat to public safety besides crime and disorder: a deadly virus. The world has not experienced a pandemic like COVID‐19 since the 1918 Spanish Flu—long before sociological inquiries of policing were common. However, the disaster response literature is rife with examples of how local police departments adapt to natural disasters (Drabek, 1985; Drabek & McEntire, 2002; Rojek & Smith, 2007). In their review of the literature, Bonkiewicz and Ruback (2012, p. 144) observed:

Law enforcement agents may change their style of policing in disasters to respond not only to the physical impact of the event but also the social impact, including the perceptions, beliefs, and behaviors of citizens. Put simply, how citizens respond to disasters…affects officers’ style of policing.

Specifically, they noted three disaster stages—approach, onset, and recovery—and proposed that citizen priorities and behaviors during each of these stages dictate which “style” of policing is most appropriate (i.e., Wilson's [1978] Legalistic, Watchman, and Service styles). The “Watchman” style of policing is best suited for the onset of a disaster, they contended, because of its emphasis on order maintenance, increased officer discretion, and enforcement flexibility. All of these components of Watchman policing, however, may concern people who are worried about police discrimination and may create police–community tensions.

During the onset of the COVID‐19 pandemic, consistent with the recommendation to use Watchman policing in disasters, officers in many U.S. jurisdictions were given the discretion to enforce new types of public health offenses (see, e.g., Phillips, 2020) while simultaneously being instructed to pull back from enforcing traditional offenses (Lum et al., 2020). Importantly, criminal activity did not simply cease to be a problem once many Americans were under stay‐at‐home orders (Ashby, 2020; Campedelli, Aziani, & Favarin, 2020; Mohler et al., 2020). In fact, homicides increased in 25 major cities (Abt, Rosenfeld, & Lopez, 2020). As a result, police departments were forced to balance new responsibilities with traditional activities (e.g., traffic enforcement, foot patrols, criminal investigations), all while their officers were at risk of contracting the virus from community members. Notably, they juggled these responsibilities in the absence of knowledge concerning community expectations of them during the onset of COVID‐19.

Therefore, a key question is whether the public supports either precautionary or social distance policing during pandemics. As Meares and Tyler (2020) emphasize, the first question in policing must always be: What does the public want? An equally important question is: What would the public want if it were informed? In general, the public has very little knowledge about the criminal justice system (Pickett, Mancini, Mears, & Gertz, 2015; Roberts & Stalans, 1997) or about the conditions in prisons and jails (Roberts & Hough, 2005; Travis, Western, & Redburn, 2014; Wozniak, 2014). This is because, lacking relevant direct or vicarious experience, most citizens get criminal justice information from news media depictions (Shi, Lu, & Pickett, 2020), which are notoriously inaccurate (Dorfman & Schiraldi, 2001; Roberts & Stalans, 1997; Surette, 2011). However, the public often does change its attitudes after acquiring accurate information (Indermaur, Roberts, Spiranovic, Mackenzie, & Gelb, 2012; Larsen & Olsen, 2020; Roberts, Hough, Jackson, & Gerber, 2012). This suggests that exposure to reliable information about pandemics may affect how citizens want officers to police them.

In the context of the COVID‐19 pandemic, some media sources provided accurate information about the virus, but others did not (Bridgman et al., 2020; Jamieson & Albarracin, 2020). However, much less information was available about the virus's criminal justice consequences. As the toll on the criminal justice system became deadlier and more pressing, some news outlets began to cover the pandemic's effects on police and inmates (e.g., Bates, 2020; Elinson, 2020; Giorgis, 2020; NYT Editorial Board, 2020). Although it is unlikely that most Americans would have anticipated the virus's effects on police departments and correctional facilities, information about those effects, through empathetic identification with officers and inmates, may change their attitudes about pandemic policing.

3. ADDITIONAL SOURCES OF ATTITUDES TOWARD PANDEMIC POLICING

Beyond accurate information about the criminal justice consequences of COVID‐19, there are other theoretical sources of attitudes toward pandemic policing styles. These include factors affecting views about the police generally, about crime generally, and about the importance of controlling the spread of the virus. We discuss each below.

3.1. Perceived procedural justice

People want to be treated justly by legal authorities, and their perceptions about whether they receive fair treatment strongly influence their attitudes toward those authorities, toward the law as a whole, and toward specific criminal justice policies and practices (Tyler, 2004; Tyler & Jackson, 2014). In the policing context, people express more satisfaction and trust in police officers who are procedurally just—that is, those who explain their decisions, listen to citizens, are unbiased and fair, and show genuine care and concern about people (Tyler, 2006). When individuals perceive greater police procedural justice, they are more likely to accept officers’ decisions, cooperate with the police, and bestow legitimacy on the broader institution of policing (Hinds & Murphy, 2007; Jackson, Bradford, Stanko, & Hohl, 2012; Maguire, Lowrey, & Johnson, 2017; Mazerolle, Antrobus, Bennett, & Tyler, 2013; Tankebe, Reisig, & Wang, 2016; Wolfe et al., 2016).

Pandemic policing puts faith in officers to use their enormous discretion both to enforce new, potentially controversial and ambiguous types of offenses, such as congregating in large groups or refusing to wear a mask in public, and to pull back on enforcing certain traditional offenses (Jouvenal & Brice‐Saddler, 2020). Agencies in some cities faced backlash when evidence emerged that officers were disproportionately enforcing social distancing against poor and minority citizens (Southall, 2020). On a few occasions, officers were observed using force on citizens who were not abiding by social distance ordinances (Moore & Lapin, 2020; Stelloh, 2020). Giving officers discretion to enforce these new ordinances clearly invites the opportunity for unfair treatment and abuse of power, as does allowing them to decide not to enforce other traditional offenses, which they may choose to continue enforcing only against certain types of people. Nevertheless, these policies may be deemed necessary in the interest of public health. Past research shows that people who perceive the police as procedurally just, and thus as more legitimate, are more willing to empower them to engage in invasive or coercive tactics (Fox, Moule, Jaynes, & Parry, 2020; Moule, Burruss, Parry, & Fox, 2019; Sunshine & Tyler, 2003).

3.2. Racial resentment and authoritarianism

Precautionary policing and social distance policing both entail a shift of enforcement away from traditional crime‐control: precautionary policing entails less enforcement while social distance policing entails focusing on a new type of offense that is separate from street crime. Factors that increase concern about street crime (e.g., theft, drugs, violence) and punitiveness toward criminals might increase opposition to pandemic policing if individuals perceive changes in enforcement as facilitating crime by passively condoning it, as with precautionary policing, or by diverting resources from its prevention, as with social distance enforcement. Theoretically, two factors are particularly applicable here: racial resentment and authoritarianism.

Racial resentment is a modern form of racism that centers on a denial of discrimination and a conviction that Black citizens are undeserving of special government assistance (Henry & Sears, 2002; Kinder & Sanders, 1996). It is one of the most consistent and strongest predictors of punitive attitudes (Brown & Socia, 2017; Pickett & Baker, 2014; Unnever & Cullen, 2010). This association reflects the “racial typification” of crime, which draws a nexus between street crime and minorities—particularly young, Black men—in the minds of American citizens (Chiricos, Welch, & Gertz, 2004; Muhammad, 2010). Insofar as support for aggressive policing is akin to punitiveness, as it appears to be (Johnson & Kuhn, 2009; Pickett, 2016; Silver & Pickett, 2015), racial resentment should reduce support for pandemic policing styles that involve officers either pulling back from enforcement or shifting their focus to new, nonstreet crime offenses.

Authoritarianism may also reduce support for pandemic policing. Authoritarians are concerned with enforcing the traditional moral order and ensuring obedience to authorities (Gerber & Jackson, 2013; Tyler & Boeckman, 1997) and tend to be more punitive (Brown & Socia, 2017; Unnever & Cullen, 2010). Moral foundations theory (MFT) provides a useful conceptualization of authoritarianism (Haidt, 2012) and helps explain its relevance to criminal justice attitudes (Silver, 2017). Broadly, MFT suggests that individuals possess different domains of moral intuitions, called moral foundations, which have their basis in evolution, but which are strengthened and shaped though socialization (Graham, Haidt, & Nosek, 2009; Haidt, 2012). These moral foundations lead individuals to perceive certain behaviors as either virtuous or transgressive (Graham et al., 2009), and each foundation increases concern about specific types of deviance (Silver, 2017). The authority/respect foundation “promotes intuitions that obedience and deference to authority, social hierarchies, and social customs are virtuous whereas disrespect and disobedience are moral transgressions” (Silver, 2017, p. 416). This foundation should promote opposition to policing styles that divert attention and resources away from enforcement of traditional threats to the moral order. 3

3.3. Personal and altruistic fear of COVID‐19

Many Americans are personally afraid of being infected with COVID‐19, so afraid that they have suffered mental health consequences (Fitzpatrick, Harris, & Drawve, 2020). Many more are altruistically afraid—that is, they are worried about other people, such as their family members and friends, becoming infected with COVID‐19 (Sloan et al., 2020). Graham et al. (2020) found that personal, but not altruistic, fear was associated with social distancing intentions. Other research shows that both personal and altruistic fear can lead to defensive and protective behaviors (Drakulich, 2015; Warr & Ellison, 2000). Additionally, many studies have found that fear of various public safety threats, such as crime, terrorism, and social breakdown, influence views about policing and social control (Huddy, Feldman, Taber, & Lahav, 2005; Jackson & Bradford, 2009; Pickett, 2016).

4. THE CURRENT STUDY

To provide a preliminary understanding of public preferences for policing during the COVID‐19 pandemic, as well as to assess the role of information exposure on these preferences, we surveyed members of the American public about their attitudes toward various policing practices that have been central to discussions of policing in the context of the novel coronavirus. While we discuss the specifics of our survey experiment in more detail below, we developed the following hypotheses based on our review of the relevant literatures:

  • H1:

    Information about COVID‐19′s negative effects on officers and inmates will increase public support for precautionary and social distance policing.

  • H2:

    Those who perceive the police as procedurally just will also express greater support for both precautionary and social distance policing.

  • H3:

    Racially resentful people will be less supportive of both precautionary and social distance policing.

  • H4:

    Authoritarianism will reduce support for both precautionary and social distance policing.

  • H5:

    Personal and altruistic fear of infection will increase support for both precautionary and social distance policing.

5. DATA AND METHODS

To test our hypotheses, we embedded an experiment in an online survey that was administered to a nationwide sample of adult U.S. citizens (N = 1068) between April 15, 2020 and April 19, 2020. Researchers now use online samples regularly to examine public attitudes toward the police (Celestin & Kruschke, 2019; Hamm, Trinkner, & Carr, 2017; Mummolo, 2018; Pickett, Nix, & Roche, 2018), toward procedural justice (Pedersen, Stritch, & Taggart, 2017; Tyler, Mentovich, & Satyavada, 2014), and toward criminal justice policies (Gerber & Jackson, 2013; Gottlieb, 2017; Silver, 2017; Vaughan, Holleran, & Silver, 2019). Many online samples come from Amazon's Mechanical Turk (MTurk) (e.g., Gottlieb, 2017; Hamm et al., 2017; Mummolo, 2018; Silver, 2017), and for good reason. Studies have found that MTurk respondents provide high quality self‐reports (Anson, 2018; Weinberg, Freese, & McElhattan, 2014), and that experimental findings from MTurk samples almost always generalize to the U.S. population (Coppock, 2019; Mullinix, Leeper, Druckman, & Freese, 2015; Weinberg et al., 2014). 4 For these reasons, we used MTurk to field our survey.

Following the current best practices to ensure data quality, we restricted our survey to MTurk workers who had an approval rating on prior human intelligence tasks (HITs) of at least 95% (Peer, Vosgerau, & Acquisti, 2014). Additionally, we required that respondents had completed at least 50 HITs, so that they had a reliable approval rating, and were residents of the United States. We also included two attention checks in the questionnaire, which were passed by the vast majority of respondents. We ran supplemental analyses of our main models excluding those who failed one or both of the attention checks (reported below). Table 1 provides the sample descriptive statistics, by experimental condition. Compared with the U.S. population, the respondents in each experimental condition are whiter, younger, better educated, and more likely to be male, which is normal in MTurk surveys (Levay, Freese, & Druckman, 2016; Thompson & Pickett, 2020). Across the experimental conditions, respondents’ characteristics are similar (see Table 1). 5

TABLE 1.

Descriptive statistics

Control condition Incarceration condition Policing condition
Variable Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD
Authoritarian morality 3.507 0.873 3.574 0.805 3.499 0.831
Racial resentment 2.783 1.057 2.768 1.050 2.723 1.009
Procedural justice 3.444 0.964 3.533 0.969 3.544 1.009
Personal fear 2.749 0.863 2.802 0.875 2.815 0.873
Altruistic fear 3.289 0.938 3.398 1.036 3.412 1.012
White 0.787 0.789 0.763
Latino 0.120 0.138 0.117
Male 0.554 0.523 0.537
Age 39.633 13.333 40.012 13.411 40.172 12.673
Married 0.569 0.569 0.572
Education 3.599 1.077 3.669 1.076 3.491 1.192
Income 3.111 1.251 3.196 1.224 3.150 1.356
Employed 0.686 0.717 0.677
Republicanism 2.862 1.210 2.745 1.197 2.710 1.146
Family victim 0.192 0.238 0.207
Attention to COVID 3.304 0.707 3.329 0.759 3.380 0.745

5.1. Experimental procedure

We randomly assigned respondents to one of three experimental conditions: control, policing, or incarceration. All participants received the following introductory text: “The CORONAVIRUS (COVID‐19) is a highly contagious virus that first infected people in China in 2019 and has since spread to the United States and other countries around the world, resulting in a worldwide pandemic.” Those in the control condition received no further information. Those in the policing and incarceration conditions received additional information. They were asked to “read a brief summary from the following article about the impact of the CORONAVIRUS pandemic [on POLICING/in PRISONS AND JAILS] in the United States.”

To emulate a realistic scenario in which the public receives information from a veritable news source, respondents in the treatment conditions were shown a picture of the title section of an online news article about the impact of COVID‐19 on policing or in prisons and jails (see Appendix A for reproduced images), followed by a brief synopsis of the information in the respective articles. The policing article came from Time magazine (Bates, 2020) and was titled: “Police Departments, Sheriff's Offices Across the U.S. Grapple with COVID‐19′s Impact on Public Safety—And Their Own.” The summary read:

Police departments across the country have been struggling to adapt to changes due to the coronavirus pandemic. Police officers often have to interact with citizens in public places, which puts them at higher risk of contracting the coronavirus. The result is that many officers are getting the virus. In some places, as many as one in six officers is out sick or in quarantine.

The incarceration article came from the New York Times (Williams, Weiser, & Rashbaum, 2020) and was titled: “‘Jails Are Petri Dishes’: Inmates Freed as the Virus Spreads Behind Bars,” with the subtitle, “Some jails are releasing people to stem outbreaks, but critics say it is not happening quickly enough to save lives and resources.” The summary read:

Many scholars are concerned that inmates are in danger during the coronavirus pandemic. Prisons and jails are often crowded and unsanitary. Many inmates are old, come from backgrounds of poverty, and have a history of poor healthcare. Some inmates suffer from respiratory problems and heart conditions. Practices to slow the spread of the virus—avoiding crowds, frequent handwashing, disinfecting clothing—are nearly impossible inside prisons and jails.

5.2. Dependent variables

The dependent variables measure respondents’ attitudes toward two types of pandemic‐related policing policies (see Lum et al., 2020). We presented respondents with eight “proposals for POLICING during the CORONAVIRUS (COVID‐19) PANDEMIC,” and asked them to indicate how much they supported or opposed each (1 = strongly oppose, 5 = strongly support). The proposals covered the two dimensions of pandemic policing. Six entailed precautionary policing: (1) “Cutting back on police foot patrols”; (2) “Cutting back on policing of traffic offenses (e.g., speeding)”; (3) “Cutting back on policing minor drug offenses (e.g., possession)”; (4) “Cutting back on policing of minor property offenses (e.g., petty theft)”; (5) “Having police only respond to emergency (911) calls for assistance”; (6) “Having police focus their enforcement efforts mainly on violent crimes.” Two involved social distance policing: (1) “Having police increase their enforcement of social distancing”; (2) “Having police break up large parties and large social gatherings.” Table 2 provides the pairwise correlations between each of the items. An exploratory factor analysis with promax rotation revealed two factors, corresponding to precautionary policing (loadings: 0.551–0.696) and social distance policing (loadings: 0.727 and 0.748). The factor loadings were well above the 0.32 cutoff proposed by Tabachnick and Fidell (2001). Accordingly, we averaged the responses to the respective items to create two standardized mean indices: precautionary policing (α = 0.811) and social distance policing (α = 0.781).

TABLE 2.

Pairwise correlations for pandemic policing items

Item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8
1. Less foot patrol
2. Fewer traffic stops 0.470
3. Less drug enforcement 0.413 0.495
4. Less property enforcement 0.467 0.460 0.470
5. Only 911 calls 0.455 0.342 0.274 0.457
6. Focus on violence 0.364 0.467 0.440 0.338 0.355
7. Enforce social distancing 0.212 0.242 0.205 0.247 0.260 0.250
8. Break up gatherings 0.148 0.185 0.221 0.189 0.150 0.250 0.641

Note: All correlations statistically significant at the 0.001 level.

5.3. Independent variables

We included several independent variables that are of theoretical interest to explaining public support for modifications to policing during the COVID‐19 pandemic: perceived procedural justice; authoritarian morality; racial resentment; and personal and altruistic fear of the coronavirus. Procedural justice is a mean index (factor loadings: 0.824–0.874, α = 0.947) based on responses to seven items (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) asking how police behave in interactions with civilians (e.g., “Treat people fairly”; “Clearly explain the reasons for their actions”; see Appendix B). We adapted these items from previous research examining procedural justice (Mazerolle et al., 2013; Pickett et al., 2018).

Authoritarian morality is a mean index (factor loadings: 0.559–0.678, α = 0.748) based on four items (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) asking about the importance of obedience to authority and traditions (e.g., “Respect for authority is something all children need to learn”; “When the government makes laws, those laws should always respect the traditions and heritage of the country”; see Appendix B). The items are adapted from Graham et al. (2009), where they were used to measure the authority foundation and are similar to those used in other studies to measure this dimension of morality (Silver, 2017).

Racial resentment is a mean index (factor loadings: 0.690–0.822, α = 0.876) based on five items (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) adapted from Henry and Sears's (2002) “Symbolic Racism 2000″ scale. Examples of the items include: “Irish, Italians, Jewish, and many other minorities overcame prejudice and worked their way up. Blacks should do the same without any special favors”; “It's really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if Blacks would only try harder they could be just as well off as Whites” (see Appendix B). All of the items have been used in other studies to measure racial resentment (Schutten et al., 2020; Silver, 2017).

To measure personal fear and altruistic fear of COVID, we adapted questions from Graham et al. (2020). The question for personal fear was “How worried are you that you will become sick from the coronavirus?” The response options were 1 = Not worried at all, 2 = not too worried, 3 = somewhat worried, and 4 = very worried. Altruistic fear is a mean index (factor loadings: 0.765–0.827, α = 0.909) based on six items that asked how often (1 = very rarely, 5 = very often) respondents worried about different groups of people becoming sick from the coronavirus: (1) “First responders, police, and correctional officers”; (2) “Your family members”; (3) “Your neighbors”; (4) “Doctors and nurses”; (5) “People across America”; (6) “Your friends.”

5.4. Control variables

We also controlled for a host of additional variables that prior research suggests may influence attitudes about criminal justice in the United States (Brown & Socia, 2017; Silver & Pickett, 2015; Unnever & Cullen, 2010). These items include race (1 = White); ethnicity (1 = Hispanic); gender (1 = male); age in years; marital status (1 = married); education (1 = high school or less, 5 = graduate degree); annual household income (1 = $0–24,999, 5 = $100,000 or more); employment status (1 = employed full time); political ideology (1 = strong Democrat, 5 = strong Republican); and family victimization in past five years (1 = yes). Finally, due to the widespread news coverage of the COVID‐19 pandemic, and evidence that news media consumption is linked to attitudes about crime and punishment (Baranauskas & Drakulich, 2018; Shi et al., 2020), we asked respondents how closely they had followed news about the pandemic (1 = Not at all closely, 4 = Very closely).

5.5. Analytic strategy

First, we examine the level of support for each of the proposed policies. We then estimate multivariate models analyzing the main effects of the experimental manipulations as well as the independent and control variables. Given that both outcomes are continuous, we use ordinary least squares (OLS) regression to estimate the models. In keeping with recommendations in the experimental literature (Freedman, 2008; Lin, 2013), we first estimate models for both outcomes with only the experimental manipulations included as predictors, and then estimate models including the other variables. Due to listwise deletion of cases with missing data on key variables, our analytic sample ranges between n = 987 and n = 1015.

6. RESULTS

Figure 1 displays the percentage of respondents who support or strongly support each pandemic policing style. Turning first to precautionary policing, a slight majority of the sample expresses support for having the police focus on violence (60%) and deemphasize drug (54%) and traffic enforcement (53%). However, only a minority of the sample expresses support for having officers do less foot patrol (37%), respond only to 911 calls (36%), and deemphasize property crime enforcement (30%). The most accurate interpretation, then, is that the respondents are lukewarm at best about precautionary policing—they are split on some policies, but opposed to others. The findings are similar for social distance policing. A slight majority of the sample supports having officers shut down large gatherings (55%) and enforce social distancing ordinances (52%). In other words, the respondents are sharply divided about whether officers should engage in social distancing enforcement. The next portion of our analysis sheds light on the factors explaining this divide.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Percentage of Respondents Supporting Pandemic Policing Styles

Note: Numbers displayed reflect the % of respondents who support or strongly support each style

Table 3 presents the results of OLS models that regressed each of the outcomes onto the experimental manipulations, without and then with the covariates. Model 1, the baseline model for precautionary policing, shows that informing respondents about the health risks of COVID‐19 for police officers has a statistically significant and positive effect on their support for having officers pull back from enforcement of traditional offenses (= 0.203, = .001). The effect is significant even using a Bonferroni‐corrected alpha of 0.0125 to account for the fact that we tested two manipulations with two experimental outcomes. The effect remains significant at conventional levels in Model 2, which includes the covariates (= 0.170, = .021). By contrast, providing respondents information about the risks of COVID‐19 for inmates does not have a statistically significant effect on support for precautionary policing, either in the baseline (Model 1) or full model (Model 2), and in both cases the coefficient is much smaller than that for the information about officer health risks. Apparently, then, concern for officers’ but not inmates’ safety causes members of the public to become more supportive of limiting traditional law enforcement activities, as they learn more about the criminal justice consequences of COVID‐19.

TABLE 3.

Regressions predicting support for precautionary and social distancing policing during COVID‐19 crisis

DV = Precautionary policing DV = Social distance policing
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Variables b SE b SE b SE b SE Z
Experimental manipulation
Control (reference)
Inmate health prime 0.025 0.077 0.045 0.073 0.069 0.077 0.037 0.069 0.080
Police health prime 0.203** 0.076 0.170* 0.073 0.133 0.075 0.077 0.070 0.920
Authoritarian morality ‒0.152*** 0.046 0.080 0.048 3.490***
Racial resentment ‒0.144*** 0.039 ‒0.139*** 0.039 0.091
Procedural justice 0.057 0.037 0.113** 0.036 1.085
Personal fear ‒0.134*** 0.040 0.054 0.040 3.323***
Altruistic fear 0.220*** 0.039 0.303*** 0.037 1.544
White 0.181* 0.078 ‒0.141 0.072 3.033**
Latino 0.031 0.104 ‒0.031 0.098 0.434
Male 0.023 0.062 ‒0.100 0.059 1.437
Age ‒0.013*** 0.002 ‒0.005* 0.002 2.828**
Married 0.072 0.066 0.041 0.063 0.340
Education ‒0.043 0.030 ‒0.004 0.028 0.950
Income ‒0.040 0.026 0.029 0.025 0.305
Employed ‒0.074 0.068 ‒0.030 0.064 0.471
Republicanism 0.023 0.030 ‒0.057* 0.029 1.917
Family victim ‒0.045 0.075 ‒0.215** 0.072 1.635
Attention to COVID news 0.051 0.042 0.051 0.044 0.000
R 2 0.008 0.139 0.003 0.219
N 1,015 987 1,015 987

Note: Both dependent variables are standardized mean indices.

Abbreviations: b = unstandardized coefficient; SE = robust standard error, Z = slope‐difference test for full models.

* p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001 (two‐tailed).

Turning to the covariates in Model 2, we find support for some of our hypotheses but not others. First, contrary to our expectations, perceived procedural justice is not significantly related to support for precautionary policing. As hypothesized, however, respondents who are more authoritarian (= –0.152, p < .001) and racially resentful (b = –0.144, < .001) tend to be significantly less supportive of precautionary policing. Personal fear of being infected with COVID‐19 is also significantly and negatively associated with support for precautionary policing (= –0.134, < .001), which is contrary to our expectations. However, those who expressed more altruistic fear about others being infected were significantly more supportive of such policing (= 0.220, < .001), as we hypothesized. Finally, two control variables are significantly related to support for precautionary policing: White respondents tended to be more supportive (= 0.181, = .021) and older respondents tended to be less supportive (= –0.013, < .001).

Models 3 and 4 in Table 3 present the results from regressing social distance policing onto the experimental manipulations, with and without the covariates. Unlike in the case of precautionary policing, we find no evidence that information about the health risks of COVID‐19 for officers or inmates significantly affects attitudes toward social distance policing. Similarly, we again find mixed support for our hypotheses. Several of the covariates have significant associations with this outcome. As hypothesized, perceived procedural justice is significantly and positively associated with support for social distance policing (b = 0.113, p = .002), as is altruistic fear (b = 0.303, p < .001). Thus, respondents who believe police are fair and who are afraid for others’ safety tend to be more willing to grant police discretion to enforce social distancing ordinances. Also as hypothesized, racial resentment is significantly and negatively associated with support for social distance policing (= –0.139, < .001). By contrast, neither authoritarianism nor personal fear have statistically significant associations with support for social distance policing, contrary to our hypotheses. Finally, three control variables are significantly associated with support for social distance policing: support tends to be lower among older respondents (= –0.005, = .015), Republicans (= –0.057, = .047), and those with crime victims in their family (= –0.215, = .003).

The final column in Table 3 provides z‐scores for equality‐of‐coefficient tests, per the Paternoster, Brame, Mazerolle, and Piquero (1998) formula. These tests provide evidence about which variables have effects on the two outcomes that differ significantly across models. Among the key independent variables, the most noteworthy findings are that authoritarianism and personal fear are both much more strongly related to support for precautionary policing than to attitudes toward social distancing enforcement. Those who are more authoritarian and afraid for their own safety tend to oppose allowing police to pull back from traditional enforcement, but do not differ significantly in their views about whether police should enforce social distancing ordinances.

6.1. Supplementary analyses

We included two attention checks in the survey, which were passed by 78% and 91% of respondents, which is consistent with or better than passage rates in prior studies (Anson, 2018; Berinsky, Margolis, & Sances, 2014). Paying attention in surveys is necessary for receiving experimental treatments, but attention checks measure attention with error (r = 0.458 for responses to the two checks in our survey), and full‐sample findings are intent‐to‐treat effects (Aronow, Baron, & Pinson, 2019). Nonetheless, it is best practice is to stratify on attention. When we exclude respondents who failed one or both of the attention checks, the police‐health manipulation has a similar effect (b = 0.170 vs. 0.123–0.179) on support for precautionary policing in the full model, but the significance level varies (p = .021 vs. 0.017–0.129), because the sample size changes. The findings for the inmate health manipulation are unchanged.

7. DISCUSSION

How do Americans define public safety? How do they want their communities policed? The nature of American policing should hinge on the answers to these questions (Meares & Tyler, 2020), because police effectiveness and the quality of police–community relations certainly do. In response to the COVID‐19 pandemic, police agencies across the country redefined public safety to include social distancing, and pulled back on enforcement of traditional offenses (Lum et al., 2020). Officers made numerous arrests and issued thousands of citations to enforce social distancing (Jouvenal & Brice‐Saddler, 2020), evidence of racial discrimination quickly accumulated (Ross, 2020), dozens of lawsuits were filed challenging the constitutionality of stay‐at‐home orders (Harris, 2020), and antilockdown protests broke out across the country (Hoskin, 2020). All of this happened mostly in the dark as to whether any of the pandemic‐related changes in policing were consistent with public expectations. Our study addressed this research void, providing the first evidence about public attitudes toward precautionary and social distance policing during the pandemic.

Americans who responded to our survey were sharply divided in terms of their support for both precautionary and social distance policing. Slight majorities support social distancing policies. However, only a minority supported precautionary policies that would reduce police patrols—a key component of deterrence (Lum & Nagin, 2017; Nagin, Solow, & Lum, 2015)—or limit officers’ responsiveness to civilians’ calls for assistance. Prior studies have shown that Americans have great faith in the crime‐control benefits of preventive, patrol‐based policing (Metcalfe & Pickett, 2018). Additionally, policing scholars have increasingly called for police to focus less on apprehension and more on crime prevention through focused patrol and service (Lum & Nagin, 2017). Our findings are consistent with this general body of scholarship, suggesting that in pandemics, the public is willing to accept cuts in some types of enforcement, but not in preventative patrols or service. A fundamental American belief, it appears then, is that the police should always be “out there” on watch and on call.

Turning to our experimental findings, four are notable. First, brief exposure to information about the health risks of COVID‐19 for police officers significantly increased support for precautionary policing. Second, this information did not significantly affect support for social distance policing. This indicates that the public may see enforcement of social distancing ordinances as involving a more difficult trade‐off between officer safety and the public health benefits of reducing virus transmission between civilians. Third, exposure to the inmate health prime did not significantly influence support for either pandemic policing style, suggesting that the public empathetically identifies less with criminal suspects than with officers, which is unsurprising. Fourth, all of the information effects in our study, whether statistically significant or not, were modest in size, suggesting that views about pandemic policing are largely rooted in more general attitudes—about policing, about the virus, about crime, and about race.

Our observational findings provide support for the police empowerment hypothesis (Fox et al., 2020; Sunshine & Tyler, 2003). Regardless of the experimental prime they viewed, respondents who perceived greater police procedural justice were more supportive of giving officers discretion to enforce social distancing requirements. Agencies will need this support moving forward if the continued spread of the virus necessitates more aggressive social distancing policies. Indeed, in many cities, police leaders have expressed concern that “punishing people for not wearing masks … would put officers at the center of yet another fraught controversy” (Phillips, 2020, para. 6). In practical terms, this underscores the need to increase officers’ use of procedural justice in their day‐to‐day interactions with the public (Nagin & Telep, 2020), which are increasingly likely to be captured on camera and disseminated through social media (Parry, Moule, & Dario, 2019). Quasi‐experimental studies of one‐day training programs in Chicago (Skogan, Van Craen, & Hennessy, 2015) and Louisville (Schaefer & Hughes, 2016) suggest improvement in officers’ attitudes toward treating people with procedural justice. A two‐site randomized‐controlled trial of T3 (tact, tactics, and trust) training delivered over several months in Tucson (Arizona) and Fayetteville (North Carolina) showed similar promise (McLean, Rojek, Wolfe, Alpert, & Smith, 2020). Beyond the immediate impact of such training—including improved attitudes and in some cases, behaviors (see Owens, Weisburd, Amendola, & Alpert, 2018; Wood, Tyler, & Papachristos, 2020) —agencies might also find that community perceptions improve in the long run. What our findings suggest is that the reservoir of public trust accumulated during ordinary times through procedurally just policing may be critical in times of crisis. A critical direction for future research, then, as Nagin and Telep (2020) emphasize, is examining the long‐term impacts of police procedural justice.

We also found support for the altruistic fear perspective, which holds that fear for others is not only more common than personal fear but also tends to be a stronger motivator of defensive and precautionary behavior (Drakulich, 2015; Warr & Ellison, 2000). In our study, altruistic fear of others getting sick with COVID‐19 was significantly and positively associated with support for both precautionary and social distance policing. In fact, the standardized coefficients (not shown) indicated that altruistic fear was by far the strongest predictor of support for both types of pandemic policing. To our knowledge, no previous study has examined the role of any type of altruistic fear—of COVID‐19, of crime, or of police violence—in shaping views about police policies or practices. Our findings suggest this is a regrettable oversight. It appears that concern for others, including for police officers, is an important factor influencing public attitudes about policing in America.

Additionally, we found that racial resentment has a sizable, consistent, and negative association with support for pandemic policing. Racially resentful respondents are more likely to oppose both precautionary and social distance policing. In other words, racists want police to stick with enforcing street crimes—the kinds of crimes stereotypically associated with Black people. This finding is consistent with prior work that suggests support for aggressive, crime‐control‐focused policing tends to reflect racialized punitiveness (Johnson & Kuhn, 2009; Pickett, 2016; Silver & Pickett, 2015). In other words, for racists, the importance of police as guardians of the racial hierarchy (Weitzer & Tuch, 2006) does not diminish just because there is a pandemic threatening officer health and public safety.

Although modest in size, the information effect documented in our experiment provides valuable insight into public opinion regarding the activities police departments should be prioritizing during the pandemic. We provided respondents with only a short summary of a single news article. It is likely that providing them with greater information, especially from police sources, over a prolonged period of time would have a much larger effect. Thus, communicating the risks officers face is a promising strategy for increasing community support for alternative, precautionary approaches such as deemphasizing traffic and pedestrian stops, property crime enforcement, and/or drug enforcement. It may even increase support among members of the community who might otherwise be opposed to such approaches (e.g., those who score higher on authoritarian morality and racial resentment).

Such support will be critical in the months ahead, as municipalities all over the United States brace for another wave of infections (Coyle & Spencer, 2020). Agencies would be wise to leverage their relationships with the local media to push out information, as prior work suggests television news consumption increases confidence in and willingness to empower the police (Callanan & Rosenberger, 2011; Roche, Pickett, & Gertz, 2016). They should also consider regularly posting information updates on various social media platforms (Intravia, Wolff, & Piquero, 2018). According to a Gallup poll conducted around the same time as our survey, 70% of Americans find public officials’ social media posts about COVID‐19 “moderately” or “very helpful” (Ritter, 2020). Data collected by the Pew Research Center suggests agencies can reach the biggest audience by posting on Facebook, though notably, Instagram and Snapchat are popular alternatives frequented by adults in the 18–29 age group (Perrin & Anderson, 2019).

As they proactively push out information about the risks officers face and steps being taken to mitigate them, agencies will need to be mindful of the kinds of information they share, and remain open to adjusting in response to community feedback. In Louisville (Kentucky), for example, Chief Steve Conrad issued a special order in mid‐March which stated officers would no longer respond to most nonviolent crimes requiring a police report (e.g., disorderly conduct, intoxicated persons, and certain burglaries; see Riley & Ragsdale, 2020). Less than 1 month later, he reversed the special order, citing community concerns and “criminals taking advantage of the pandemic” (Vogt, 2020). Such is the challenge that comes with being transparent.

In the years ahead, there will also be a pressing need for research examining the long‐term effects of the COVID‐19 pandemic on officers, agencies, and the communities they serve. For example, the disaster response literature suggests officers working during the COVID‐19 pandemic might be at heightened risk of experiencing posttraumatic stress disorder (Faust & Vander Ven, 2014). Likewise, there is reason to be concerned that enforcement of social distancing ordinances could have harmful effects on the mental health of community members, especially if officers disproportionately target minority communities (Geller, Fagan, Tyler, & Link, 2014, 2017), as initial stories suggest (Ross, 2020). At the organizational level, agencies will undoubtedly see their recruitment and retention efforts hampered by budget constraints in the wake of COVID‐19 (as well as public pressure to “defund the police” following the killing of George Floyd in Minneapolis). This means we could see fewer police officers patrolling the streets of our cities in the coming years. Researchers must rise to the challenge of rigorously evaluating the relationship between police presence, policing tactics, and crime in communities.

7.1. Limitations and additional directions for future research

Several limitations of our study deserve further discussion. To begin, we surveyed a convenience sample of MTurk workers, our survey was cross‐sectional, and we used news articles as the information sources. Studies have shown that experiments with MTurk typically are generalizable to the U.S. population (Coppock, 2019; Mullinix et al., 2015; Weinberg et al., 2014). Additionally, as Mutz (2011) emphasizes, one‐shot treatments in experiments like ours are assumed to persist in the real world because of recurring exposure to relevant information. It is also the case that much of the information the public receives does come from the media. Nevertheless, future studies should attempt to replicate our work with probability samples and should examine the duration of treatment effects as well as how they vary with the information source (as respondents may vary in terms of how much they trust mainstream media outlets) and the frequency of exposure. Future work should also examine regional‐ or state‐level variation in public attitudes, and consider alternative analytic strategies such as using a psychometric approach to construct response scales, collecting political spectrum data (to avoid the limitations of impression management in self‐labeling), and/or using a multilabel classification approach to analyses. 6

Another limitation of our study is that we measured personal and altruistic fear of contracting COVID‐19 (Graham et al., 2020), but not fear of criminal victimization, which conceivably would also influence respondents’ perceptions of what police should focus on during the pandemic. In fact, respondents would likely balance these fears against one another when thinking about what they want from the police. Note, however, that we did control for whether respondents or their family members had been the victim of a crime in the past 5 years, which prior research suggests is a strong predictor of fear of victimization (see, e.g., Hale, 1996). In our sample, those who had been criminally victimized—or whose family members had been criminally victimized—were significantly less supportive of social distance policing. However, criminal victimization was not significantly related to support for precautionary policing. This is preliminary evidence that the relationship between fear of criminal victimization and preferences for pandemic policing may be complex and may vary depending on the specific policy in question. Accordingly, future studies in this area should measure fear of criminal victimization in addition to pandemic‐related fears.

Our inmate health prime did not directly communicate any implications for policing, which is one potential explanation for its null effects on respondents’ attitudes toward precautionary and social distance policing. Future studies attempting to build on our work should ensure respondents are provided information about how efforts to reduce jail populations can affect policing and public health (e.g., the probability that some of those released will recidivate or that community fear of crime—or contracting COVID‐19—could increase). Finally, by the time we administered our survey in mid‐April, the general public was acutely aware of COVID‐19 and its implications for societal norms (although many Americans and some politicians continued to believe conspiracy theories and/or downplay the virus's health risks; see Graham et al., 2020). What this means is that respondents’ baseline knowledge may have diminished the impact of our police and inmate health primes. Though it seems unlikely that the public had a thorough understanding of the unique risks posed to police officers or inmates, this could be yet another explanation for some of our null findings.

7.2. Conclusion

The COVID‐19 pandemic has been and continues to be an unprecedented challenge for American policing. Yet, perhaps their most pressing task is to come up with an answer to the same fundamental question with which they have always grappled: What does the public want them to do? Our study uncovered modest support for social distance policing, and mixed support for various tactics which we have referred to as precautionary policing. More importantly, we found that providing the public with information about the health risks faced by officers during this pandemic can increase their support for precautionary policing. In a moment of intense public scrutiny of law enforcement, we hope that our findings will be useful for agencies to engender public support for policies that promote the safety and well‐being of police officers as they continue to operate in the dangerous and uncertain context of the COVID‐19 pandemic.

“Few doubt that major epidemics and pandemics will strike again and few would argue that the world is adequately prepared,” Fan, Jamison, and Summers (2018, p. 129) explain. If the United States does experience another pandemic after the current one abates, we hope our findings will be useful for considering the policing implications of those public health crises. Hopefully, however, the current COVID‐19 pandemic will prove to be a once‐in‐a‐lifetime event. If so, we hope our study will nevertheless serve as a preliminary evidentiary base for how the public wants police to respond to natural disasters and public health crises, and for how such preferences may diverge among different subgroups of Americans.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

The authors confirm that they have no conflict of interest to declare.

APPENDIX A. ONLINE NEWS ARTICLE TITLES SHOWN TO TREATMENT GROUPS

A.1. Policing condition

A.1.

A.2. Incarceration condition

A.2.

APPENDIX B. SURVEY ITEMS FOR PROCEDURAL JUSTICE, AUTHORITARIAN MORALITY, AND RACIAL RESENTMENT

B.1. Procedural justice (α = 0.947)

How much do you agree or disagree that the POLICE in your community …?

  1. Treat people with dignity and respect.

  2. Clearly explain the reasons for their actions.

  3. Treat everyone equally.

  4. Treat people fairly.

  5. Show genuine care and concern about the well‐being of people in my community.

  6. Take time to listen to people before making decisions about how to handle a case.

  7. Respect people's rights.

B.2. Authoritarian morality (α = 0.748)

  1. Respect for authority is something all children need to learn.

  2. Men and women each have different roles to play in society.

  3. When the government makes laws, those laws should always respect the traditions and heritage of the country.

  4. If I were a soldier and disagreed with my commanding officer's orders, I would obey anyway because that is my duty.

B.3. Racial resentment (α = 0.876)

  1. Irish, Italians, Jewish, and many other minorities overcame prejudice and worked their way up. Blacks should do the same without any special favors.

  2. Generations of slavery and discrimination have created conditions that make it difficult for Blacks to work their way out of the lower class (reverse coded).

  3. Over the past few years, Blacks have gotten less than they deserve (reverse coded).

  4. It's really a matter of some people not trying hard enough; if Blacks would only try harder they could be just as well off as Whites.

Nix J, Ivanov S, Pickett JT. What does the public want police to do during pandemics? A national experiment. Criminol Public Policy. 2021;20:545–571. 10.1111/1745-9133.12535

ENDNOTES

3

If there was a consensus among U.S. authorities about social distancing, authoritarians might view it in moral terms. No such consensus exists, however. The director of the Office of National Intelligence, for example, put a photo of the Bill of Rights on social media and wrote: “Signed Permission Slip to Leave Your House” (Sommer, Branco, & Stein, 2020). President Trump also has refused to wear a mask, has held large gatherings (e.g., rallies), has praised protests of lockdown orders, and has called for reopening the country.

4

That said, MTurk can break down in observational studies that fail to measure (and control for) the factors influencing selection. These include age, gender, race and ethnicity, income, education, marital status, religion, ideology, and partisanship (Levay, Freese, & Druckman, 2016). Experimental studies like ours are less affected by this (Mullinix, Leeper, Druckman, & Freese, 2015; Weinberg, Freese, & McElhattan, 2014), because random assignment ensures internal validity (on expectation), and selection variables do not appear to moderate treatment effects to a substantial degree (Coppock, 2019). As Coppock, Leeper, and Mullinix (2018), p. 12441) explain, there tends to be “low treatment effect heterogeneity in these types of standard social science survey experiments.”

5

An anonymous reviewer correctly pointed out that access to technology can influence results from online surveys. In our case, however, it is not clear that differences in access to technology would result in selection bias, given that we measured and controlled for factors such as age, race, and income. In other words, under the assumption that technology usage mediates the effects of demographic factors on sample selection, the use of an Internet sample would not create a backdoor path between the included predictors and our outcome variables, as long as the model controls for those demographic factors that influence technology access. Levay et al. (2016, p. 1, emphasis added) made a similar point when noting that “MTurk respondents do not appear to differ fundamentally from population‐based respondents in unmeasurable ways. This suggests that MTurk data can be used … if researchers measure and account for a range of political and demographic variables as needed.” In our study, we account for most of the selection variables identified by Levay et al. (2016).

6

Our thanks to an anonymous reviewer for suggesting some of these potential avenues for future research.

REFERENCES

  1. Abt, T. , Rosenfeld, R. , & Lopez, E. (2020). COVID‐19 and homicide: Final report to Arnold Ventures . Retrieved from https://craftmediabucket.s3.amazonaws.com/uploads/COVID-19-Homicide_061520_Final.pdf
  2. Anson, I. G. (2018). Taking the time? Explaining effortful participation among low‐cost online survey participants. Research and Politics, 5(3), 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  3. Aronow, P. M. , Baron, J. , & Pinson, L. (2019). A note on dropping experimental subjects who fail a manipulation check. Political Analysis, 27, 57–589. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashby, M. (2020). Initial evidence on the relationship between the coronavirus pandemic and crime in the United States. Crime Science, 9(6), 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Axelrod, T. (2020). NY police union warns city will “fall apart” if enforcement of social distancing does not end. The Hill. Retrieved from https://thehill.com/homenews/state-watch/496221-ny-police-union-warns-city-will-fall-apart-if-enforcement-of-social
  6. Baranauskas, A. J. , & Drakulich, K. M. (2018). Media construction of crime revisited: Media types, consumer contexts, and frames of crime and justice. Criminology, 56(4), 679–714. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bates, J. (2020). Police departments, sheriffs' offices across the U.S. grapple with COVID‐19's impact on public safety—And their own. Time Magazine. Retrieved from https://time.com/5812833/coronavirus-police-departments/
  8. Berinsky, A. J. , Margolis, M. F. , & Sances, M. W. (2014). Separating the shirkers from the workers? Making sure respondents pay attention on self‐administered surveys. American Journal of Political Science, 58(3), 739–753. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bittner, E. (1970). The functions of the police in modern society: A review of background factors, current practices, and possible role models (No. 2059) . National Institute of Mental Health, Center for Studies of Crime and Delinquency.
  10. Bonkiewicz, L. , & Ruback, R. B. (2012). The role of the police in evacuations: Responding to the social impact of a disaster. Police Quarterly, 15(2), 137–156. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bridgman, A. , Merkley, E. , Loewen, P. J. , Owen, T. , Ruths, D. , Teichmann, L. , & Zhilin, O. (2020). The causes and consequences of COVID‐19 misperceptions: Understanding the role of news and social media. The Harvard Kennedy School of Misinformation Review, 1(3), 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  12. Brown, E. K. , & Socia, K. M. (2017). Twenty‐first century punitiveness: Social sources of punitive American views reconsidered. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 33(4), 935–959. [Google Scholar]
  13. Callanan, V. J. , & Rosenberger, J. S. (2011). Media and public perceptions of the police: Examining the impact of race and personal experience. Policing and Society, 21(2), 167–189. [Google Scholar]
  14. Campedelli, G. M. , Aziani, A. , & Favarin, S. (2020). Exploring the effect of 2019‐nCoV containment policies on crime: The case of Los Angele s [Preprint]. Open Science Framework. 10.31219/osf.io/gcpq8 [DOI]
  15. Celestin, B. D. , & Kruschke, J. K. (2019). Lay evaluations of police and civilian use of force: Action severity scales. Law & Human Behavior, 3(3), 290–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . (2020). Cases in the U.S. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/cases-updates/cases-in-us.html
  17. Chiricos, T. , Welch, K. , & Gertz, M. (2004). Racial typification of crime and support for punitive measures. Criminology, 42(2), 358–390. [Google Scholar]
  18. Coppock, A. (2019). Generalizing from survey experiments conducted on Mechanical Turk: A replication approach. Political Science Research Methods, 7(3), 613–628. [Google Scholar]
  19. Coppock, A. , Leeper, T. J. , & Mullinix, K. J. (2018). Generalizability of heterogenous treatment effect estimates across samples. PNAS, 115(49), 12441–12446. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Coyle, J. , & Spencer, T. (2020, July 2). Confirmed coronavirus cases are rising in 40 of 50 states. Associated Press. Retrieved from https://apnews.com/0468a60b64947879926d2a16e45c00ee
  21. Dorfman, L. , & Schiraldi, V. (2001). Off balance: Youth, race, and crime in the news. Washington, DC: Building Blocks for Youth. [Google Scholar]
  22. Drabek, T. E. (1985). Managing the emergency response. Public Administration Review, 45, 85–92. [Google Scholar]
  23. Drabek, T. E. , & McEntire, D. A. (2002). Emergent phenomena and multiorganizational coordination in disasters: Lessons from the research literature. International Journal of Mass Emergencies and Disasters, 20, 197–224. [Google Scholar]
  24. Drakulich, K. M. (2015). Concerns for self or family? Sources of and responses to altruistic fear. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 30(7), 1168–1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Editorial Board . (2020, April 23). No one deserves to die of Covid‐19 in Jail. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/23/opinion/coronavirus-prisons.html
  26. Eligon, J. , & MacFarquhar, N. (2020, May 13). Coronavirus devastates Detroit Police, from the chief on down. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/20/us/coronavirus-detroit-police.html
  27. Elinson, Z. (2020, May 7). Coronavirus has changed everything for police across the country. The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved from https://www.wsj.com/articles/coronavirus-has-changed-everything-for-police-across-the-country-11588843801
  28. Fan, V. Y. , Jamison, D. T. , & Summers, L. H. (2018). Pandemic risk: How large are the expected losses? Bulletin of the World Health Organization, 96, 129–134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Faust, K. L. , & Vander Ven, T. (2014). Policing disaster: An analytical review of the literature on policing, disaster, and post‐traumatic stress disorder. Sociology Compass, 8(6), 614–626. [Google Scholar]
  30. Fitzpatrick K. M., Harris C., Drawve G. (2020) Fear of COVID‐19 and the mental health consequences in America. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 12(S1), S17–S21. 10.1037/tra0000924. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fox, B. , Moule, R. K. , Jaynes, C. M. , & Parry, M. M. (2020). Are the effects of legitimacy and its components invariant? Operationalization and the generality of Sunshine and Tyler's empowerment hypothesis. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, Advance online publication. 10.1177/2F0022427820926228. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Freedman, D. A. (2008). On regression adjustments in experiments with several treatments. The Annals of Applied Statistics, 2(1), 176–196. [Google Scholar]
  33. Geller, A. , Fagan, J. , Tyler, T. , & Link, B. G. (2014). Aggressive policing and the mental health of young urban men. American Journal of Public Health, 104(12), 2321–2327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Geller, A. , Fagan, J. , & Tyler, T. (2017). Police contact and mental health. Columbia Public Law Research Paper, 14–571. https://scholarship.law.columbia.edu/faculty_scholarship/2078. [Google Scholar]
  35. Gerber, M. M. , & Jackson, J. (2013). Retribution as revenge and retribution as just deserts. Social Justice Research, 26(1), 61–80. [Google Scholar]
  36. Gottlieb, A. (2017). The effect of message frames on public attitudes toward criminal justice reform for nonviolent offenses. Crime & Delinquency, 63(5), 636–656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Giorgis, H. (2020, April 28). Quarantine could change how Americans think of incarceration. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/culture/archive/2020/04/quarantine-could-change-how-americans-think-incarceration/610831/
  38. Graham A., Cullen F. T., Pickett J. T., Jonson C. L., Haner M., Sloan M. M. (2020). Faith in Trump, moral foundations, and social distancing defiance during the Coronavirus Pandemic. Socius: Sociological Research for a Dynamic World, 6, 1–23. 10.1177/2378023120956815. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Graham, J. , Haidt, J. , & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. London: Vintage. [Google Scholar]
  41. Hale, C. (1996). Fear of crime: A review of the literature. International Review of Victimology, 4, 79–150. [Google Scholar]
  42. Hamm, J. A. , Trinkner, R. , & Carr, J. D. (2017). Fair process, trust, and cooperation: Moving toward an integrated framework of police legitimacy. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 44(9), 1183–1212. [Google Scholar]
  43. Harris, J. (2020, May 14). US lockdown protests fueled by distrust of government, experts. Courthouse News. Retrieved from https://www.courthousenews.com/us-lockdown-protests-fueled-by-distrust-of-government-experts/
  44. Henry, P. J. , & Sears, D. O. (2002). The symbolic racism 2000 scale. Political Psychology, 23(2), 253–283. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hinds, L. , & Murphy, K. (2007). Public satisfaction with police: Using procedural justice to improve police legitimacy. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 40(1), 27–42. [Google Scholar]
  46. Hoskin, M. N. (2020, April 25). The whiteness of anti‐lockdown protests. Vox. Retrieved from https://www.vox.com/first-person/2020/4/25/21234774/coronavirus-covid-19-protest-anti-lockdown
  47. Huddy, L. , Feldman, S. , Taber, C. , & Lahav, G. (2005). Threat, anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science, 49(3), 593–608. [Google Scholar]
  48. Indermaur, D. , Roberts, L. , Spiranovic, C. , Mackenzie, G. , & Gelb, K. (2012). A matter of judgment: The effect of information and deliberation on public attitudes to punishment. Punishment & Society, 14(2), 147–165. [Google Scholar]
  49. Intravia, J. , Wolff, K. T. , & Piquero, A. R. (2018). Investigating the effects of media consumption on attitudes toward police legitimacy. Deviant Behavior, 39(8), 963–980. [Google Scholar]
  50. Jackson, J. , & Bradford, B. (2009). Crime, policing and social order: On the expressive nature of public confidence in policing. British Journal of Sociology, 60(3), 493–521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Jackson, J. , Bradford, B. , Stanko, B. , & Hohl, K. (2012). Just authority? Trust in the police in England and Wales. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  52. Jamieson, K. H. , & Albarracin, D. (2020). The relation between media consumption and misinformation at the outset of the SARS‐COV‐2 pandemic in the US. The Harvard Kennedy School of Misinformation Review, 1(3), 1–20. [Google Scholar]
  53. Jennings W. G., Perez N. M. (2020). The immediate impact of COVID‐19 on law enforcement in the United States. American Journal of Criminal Justice, 45(4), 690–701. 10.1007/s12103-020-09536-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Johnson, D. , & Kuhn, J. B. (2009). Striking out: Race and support for police use of force. Justice Quarterly, 26(3), 592–623. [Google Scholar]
  55. Jouvenal, J. , & Brice‐Saddler, M. (2020, May 10). Social distancing enforcement is ramping up. So is concern that black and Latino residents may face harsher treatment. The Washington Post. Retrieved from https://www.washingtonpost.com/local/public-safety/social-distancing-enforcement-is-ramping-up-so-is-concern-that-black-and-latino-residents-may-face-harsher-treatment/2020/05/10/b1bcf490-8fbd-11ea-9e23-6914ee410a5f_story.html
  56. Kaste, M. (2020, May 15). Police back off from social distancing enforcement. NPR. Retrieved from https://www.npr.org/2020/05/15/857144397/police-back-off-from-social-distancing-enforcement
  57. Kinder, D. R. , & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color:Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  58. Larsen, M. V. , & Olsen, A. L. (2020). Reducing bias in citizens’ perceptions of crime rates: Evidence from a field experiment on burglary prevalence. Journal of Politics, 82(2), 747–752. [Google Scholar]
  59. Levay, K. E. , Freese, J. , & Druckman, J. N. (2016). The demographic and political composition of Mechanical Turk samples. SAGE Open, 6(1), 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  60. Lin, W. (2013). Agnostic notes on regression adjustments to experimental data: Reexamining Freedman's critique. The Annals of Applied Statistics, 7(1), 295–318. [Google Scholar]
  61. Lum, C. , & Nagin, D. S. (2017). Reinventing American policing: A seven‐point blueprint for the 21st century. Crime and Justice, 46(1), 339–393. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lum, C. , Maupin, C. , & Stoltz, M. (2020). The Impact of COVID‐19 on Law Enforcement Agencies (Wave 1) . A joint report of the International Association of Chiefs of Police and the Center for Evidence‐Based Crime Policy, George Mason University.
  63. Maguire, E. R. , Lowrey, B. V. , & Johnson, D. (2017). Evaluating the relative impact of positive and negative encounters with police: A randomized experiment. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 13(3), 367–391. [Google Scholar]
  64. Mazerolle, L. , Antrobus, E. , Bennett, S. , & Tyler, T. R. (2013). Shaping citizen perceptions of police legitimacy: A randomized field trial of procedural justice. Criminology, 51(1), 33–63. [Google Scholar]
  65. McLean K., Wolfe S. E., Rojek J., Alpert G. P., Smith M. R. (2020). Randomized controlled trial of social interaction police training. Criminology & Public Policy, 19(3), 805–832. 10.1111/1745-9133.12506. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  66. Meares, T. L. , & Tyler, T. R. (2020, June 8). The first step is figuring out what police are for. The Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/ideas/archive/2020/06/first-step-figuring-out-what-police-are/612793/
  67. Mervosh, S. , Lu, D. , & Swales, V. (2020, April 20). See which states and cities have told residents to stay home. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2020/us/coronavirus-stay-at-home-order.html
  68. Metcalfe, C. , & Pickett, J. T. (2018). The extent and correlates of public support for deterrence reforms and hot spots policing. Law & Society Review, 52(2), 471–502. [Google Scholar]
  69. Mohler, G. , Bertozzi, A. L. , Carter, J. , Short, M. B. , Sledge, D. , Tita, G. E. , … Brantingham, P. J. (2020). Impact of social distancing during COVID‐19 pandemic on crime in Los Angeles and Indianapolis. Journal of Criminal Justice, 68, 101692. 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2020.101692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Moule, R. K. , Burruss, G. W. , Parry, M. M. , & Fox, B. (2019). Assessing the direct and indirect effects of legitimacy on public empowerment of police: A study of public support for police militarization in America. Law & Society Review, 53(1), 77–107. [Google Scholar]
  71. Moore, T. , & Lapin, T. (2020). May 4). Video shows NYPD using force during social distancing arrest in Brooklyn. New York Post. Retrieved from https://nypost.com/2020/05/04/video-shows-nypd-using-force-during-social-distancing-arrest/
  72. Muhammad, K. G. (2010). The condemnation of blackness: Race, crime, and the making of modern urban America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  73. Mullinix, K. J. , Leeper, T. J. , Druckman, J. N. , & Freese, J. (2015). The generalizability of survey experiments. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 2(2), 109–138. [Google Scholar]
  74. Mummolo, J. (2018). Militarization fails to enhance police safety or reduce crime but may harm police reputation. PNAS, 115(37), 9181–9186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Mutz, D. C. (2011). Population‐based survey experiments. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  76. Nagin, D. S. , Solow, R. M. , & Lum, C. (2015). Deterrence, criminal opportunities, and the police. Criminology, 53, 74–100. [Google Scholar]
  77. Nagin D. S., Telep C. W. (2020). Procedural justice and legal compliance: A revisionist perspective. Criminology & Public Policy, 19(3), 761–786. 10.1111/1745-9133.12499. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  78. Nowotny, K. , Bailey, Z. , Omori, M. , & Brinkley‐Rubinstein, L. (2020). COVID‐19 exposes need for progressive criminal justice reform. American Journal of Public Health, 110(7), e1–e2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Owens, E. , Weisburd, D. , Amendola, K. L. , & Alpert, G. P. (2018). Can you build a better cop? Experimental evidence on supervision, training, and policing in the community. Criminology & Public Policy, 17(1), 41–87. [Google Scholar]
  80. Parry, M. M. , Moule, R. K. , & Dario, L. M. (2019). Technology‐mediated exposure to police–citizen encounters: A quasi‐experimental assessment of consequences for citizen perceptions. Justice Quarterly, 36(3), 412–436. [Google Scholar]
  81. Paternoster, R. , Brame, R. , Mazerolle, P. , & Piquero, A. (1998). Using the correct statistical test for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology, 36(4), 859–866. [Google Scholar]
  82. Pedersen, M. J. , Stritch, J. M. , & Taggart, G. (2017). Citizen perceptions of procedural fairness and the moderating roles of ‘belief in a just world’ and ‘public service motivation’ in public hiring. Public Administration, 95(4), 874–894. [Google Scholar]
  83. Peer, E. , Vosgerau, J. , & Acquisti, A. (2014). Reputation as a sufficient condition for data quality on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 1023–1031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Perrin, A. , & Anderson, M. (2019, April 10). Share of U.S. adults using social media, including Facebook, is mostly unchanged since 2018. Pew Research Center. Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2019/04/10/share-of-u-s-adults-using-social-media-including-facebook-is-mostly-unchanged-since-2018/
  85. Peyton, K. , Sierra‐Arévalo, M. , & Rand, D. G. (2019). A field experiment on community policing and police legitimacy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 19894–19898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Phillips, K. (2020). Many face mask mandates go unenforced as police feel political, economic pressure. USA Today. Retrieved from https://www.usatoday.com/story/news/politics/2020/09/16/covid-19-face-mask-mandates-go-unenforced-police-under-pressure/5714736002/
  87. Pickett, J. T. (2016). On the social foundations for crimmigration: Latino threat and support for expanded police powers. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 32, 103–132. [Google Scholar]
  88. Pickett, J. T. (2019). Public opinion and criminal justice policy: Theory and research. Annual Review of Criminology, 2, 405–428. [Google Scholar]
  89. Pickett, J. T. , & Baker, T. (2014). The pragmatic American: Empirical reality or methodological artifact. Criminology, 52(2), 195–222. [Google Scholar]
  90. Pickett, J. T. , Mancini, C. , Mears, D. P. , & Gertz, M. (2015). Public (mis)understanding of crime policy: The effects of criminal justice experience and media reliance. Criminal Justice Policy Review, 26(5), 500–522. [Google Scholar]
  91. Pickett, J. T. , Nix, J. , & Roche, S. P. (2018). Testing a social schematic model of police procedural justice. Social Psychology Quarterly, 81(2), 97–125. [Google Scholar]
  92. Riley, J. , & Ragsdale, T. (2020, March 17). LMPD to no longer respond to hit and run wrecks, other incidents. WDRB News. Retrieved from https://www.wdrb.com/in-depth/lmpd-to-no-longer-respond-to-hit-and-run-wrecks-other-incidents/article_3fa29722-686a-11ea-9ae7-1fcf2d6fb288.html
  93. Ritter, Z. (2020, May 21). Americans use social media for COVID‐19 info, connection. Gallup. Retrieved from https://news.gallup.com/poll/311360/americans-social-media-covid-information-connection.aspx
  94. Roberts, J. V. , & Hough, M. (2005). The state of the prisons: Exploring public knowledge and opinion. The Howard Journal of Criminal Justice, 44(3), 286–306. [Google Scholar]
  95. Roberts, J. V. , Hough, M. , Jackson, J. , & Gerber, M. M. (2012). Public opinion towards the lay magistracy and the sentencing council guidelines: The effects of information on attitudes. British Journal of Criminology, 52(6), 1072–1091. [Google Scholar]
  96. Roberts, J. V. , & Stalans, L. J. (1997). Public opinion, crime, and criminal justice. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. [Google Scholar]
  97. Roche, S. P. , Pickett, J. T. , & Gertz, M. (2016). The scary world of online news? Internet news exposure and public attitudes toward crime and justice. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 32(2), 215–236. [Google Scholar]
  98. Rojek, J. , & Smith, M. R. (2007). Law enforcement lessons learned from Hurricane Katrina. Review of Policy Research, 24(6), 589–608. [Google Scholar]
  99. Ross, J. (2020, May 28). Pattern of uneven social distancing enforcement coming into view, civil rights experts say. NBC News. Retrieved from https://www.nbcnews.com/news/nbcblk/pattern-uneven-social-distancing-enforcement-coming-view-civil-rights-experts-n1216506
  100. Rummler, O. (2020, May 21). Over 5,500 NYPD officers return to work after testing positive for coronavirus. Axios. Retrieved from https://www.axios.com/coronavirus-nypd-police-19bca402-58df-498d-aeca-e09e14d7a1df.html
  101. Saloner B., Parish K., Ward J. A., DiLaura G., Dolovich S. (2020). COVID‐19 cases and deaths in federal and state prisons. JAMA, 324(6), 602–603. 10.1001/jama.2020.12528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Schaefer, B. , & Hughes, T. (2016). Honing Interpersonal Necessary Tactics (H.I.N.T.): An evaluation of procedural justice training. Louisville, KY: Southern Police Institute, University of Louisville. [Google Scholar]
  103. Schutten N. M., Pickett J. T., Burton A. L., Cullen F. T., Jonson C. L., Burton V. S. (2020). Punishing rampage: Public opinion on sanctions for school shooters. Justice Quarterly, 1–24. 10.1080/07418825.2019.1707857. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  104. Shi L., Lu Y., Pickett J. T. (2020). The public salience of crime, 1960–2014: Age–period–cohort and time–series analyses †. Criminology, 58(3), 568–593. 10.1111/1745-9125.12248. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  105. Silver, J. R. (2017). Moral foundations, intuitions of justice, and the intricacies of punitive sentiment. Law & Society Review, 51(2), 413–449. [Google Scholar]
  106. Silver, J. , & Pickett, J. T. (2015). Toward a better understanding of politicized policing attitudes: Conflicted conservatism and support for police use of force. Criminology, 53(4), 650–676. [Google Scholar]
  107. Skogan, W. G. , Van Craen, M. , & Hennessy, C. (2015). Training police for procedural justice. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 11(3), 319–334. [Google Scholar]
  108. Sloan, M. , Haner, M. , Graham, A. , Cullen, F. T. , Pickett, J. , & Jonson, C. L. (2020). Pandemic emotions: The extent, correlates, and mental health consequences of personal and altruistic fear of COVID‐19. SocArXiv. 10.31235/osf.io/txqb6 [DOI]
  109. Sommer, W. , Branco, E. , & Stein, S. (2020, April 15). Trumpists urging people to leave their homes to own the libs. The Daily Beast. Retrieved from https://www.thedailybeast.com/trump-loyalists-and-allies-urge-defiance-of-coronavirus-safety-measures
  110. Southall, A. (2020, May 7). Scrutiny of social‐distance policing as 35 of 40 arrested are black. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2020/05/07/nyregion/nypd-social-distancing-race-coronavirus.html
  111. Stelloh, T. (2020, May 4). Video shows NYPD officer punching man after alleged social distancing violation. NBC News. Retrieved from https://www.nbcnews.com/news/us-news/video-shows-nypd-officer-punching-man-after-alleged-social-distancing-n1199141
  112. Sunshine, J. , & Tyler, T. R. (2003). The role of procedural justice and legitimacy in shaping public support for policing. Law & Society Review, 37(3), 513–548. [Google Scholar]
  113. Surette, R. (2011). Media, crime, and criminal justice: Images, realities, and politics (4 ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. [Google Scholar]
  114. Tabachnick, B. G. , & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon. [Google Scholar]
  115. Tankebe, J. , Reisig, M. D. , & Wang, X. (2016). A multidimensional model of police legitimacy: A cross‐cultural assessment. Law and Human Behavior, 40(1), 11–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Thompson, A. J. , & Pickett, J. T. (2020). Are relational inferences from crowdsourced and opt‐in samples generalizable? Comparing criminal justice attitudes in the GSS and five online samples. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 36, 907–932. 10.1007/s10940-019-09436-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  117. Travis, J. , Western, B. , & Redburn, F. S. (2014). The growth of incarceration in the United States: Exploring causes and consequences. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. [Google Scholar]
  118. Tyler, T. R. (2004). Enhancing police legitimacy. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 593, 84–99. [Google Scholar]
  119. Tyler, T. R. (2006). Why people obey the law. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
  120. Tyler, T. R. , & Boeckmann, R. J. (1997). Three strikes and you are out, but why? The psychology of public support for punishing rule breakers. Law and Society Review, 31(2), 237–266. [Google Scholar]
  121. Tyler, T. R. , & Jackson, J. (2014). Popular legitimacy and the exercise of legal authority: Motivating compliance, cooperation, and engagement. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 20(1), 78–95. [Google Scholar]
  122. Tyler, T. R. , Mentovich, A. , & Satyavada, S. (2014). What motivates adherence to medical recommendations? The procedural justice approach to gaining deference in the medical arena. Regulation & Governance, 8(3), 350–370. [Google Scholar]
  123. Unnever, J. D. , & Cullen, F. T. (2009). Empathetic identification and punitiveness: A middle‐range theory of individual differences. Theoretical Criminology, 13, 283–312. [Google Scholar]
  124. Unnever, J. D. , & Cullen, F. T. (2010). The social sources of Americans’ punitiveness: A test of three competing models. Criminology, 48(1), 99–129. [Google Scholar]
  125. Vaughan, T. J. , Holleran, L. B. , & Silver, J. R. (2019). Applying moral foundations theory to the explanation of capital jurors’ sentencing decisions. Justice Quarterly, 36(7), 1176–1205. [Google Scholar]
  126. Vogt, D. (2020, April 14). LMPD responding to burglary alarms again after revising in‐person service responses. Wave 3 News. Retrieved from https://www.wave3.com/2020/04/14/lmpd-responding-burglary-alarms-again-after-revising-in-person-service-responses/
  127. Warr, M. , & Ellison, C. G. (2000). Rethinking social reactions to crime: Personal and altruistic fear in family households. American Journal of Sociology, 106(3), 551–578. [Google Scholar]
  128. Weinberg, J. D. , Freese, J. , & McElhattan, D. (2014). Comparing data characteristics and results of an online factorial survey between a population‐based and a crowdsource‐recruited sample. Sociological Science, 1, 292–310. [Google Scholar]
  129. Weitzer, R. , & Tuch, S. A. (2006). Race and policing in America: Conflict and reform. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  130. Williams, T. , Weiser, B. , & Rashbaum, W. K. (2020, May 20). Jails are Petri dishes’: Inmates freed as the virus spreads behind bars. The New York Times. Retrieved from https://www.nytimes.com/2020/03/30/us/coronavirus-prisons-jails.html
  131. Wilson, J. Q. (1978). Varieties of police behavior: The Management of law and order in eight communities. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  132. Wolfe, S. E. , Nix, J. , Kaminski, R. , & Rojek, J. (2016). Is the effect of procedural justice on police legitimacy invariant? Testing the generality of procedural justice and competing antecedents of legitimacy. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 32, 253–282. [Google Scholar]
  133. Wood, G. , Tyler, T. R. , & Papachristos, A. V. (2020). Procedural justice training reduces police use of force and complaints against officers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(18), 9815–9821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Wozniak, K. H. (2014). American public opinion about prisons. Criminal Justice Review, 39(3), 305–324. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Criminology & Public Policy are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES