Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2021 Apr 6;16(4):e0247855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247855

Links between problem gambling and spending on booster packs in collectible card games: A conceptual replication of research on loot boxes

David Zendle 1,*, Lukasz Walasek 2, Paul Cairns 1, Rachel Meyer 1, Aaron Drummond 3
Editor: Stefano Triberti4
PMCID: PMC8023484  PMID: 33822788

Abstract

Loot boxes are digital containers of randomised rewards present in some video games which are often purchasable for real world money. Recently, concerns have been raised that loot boxes might approximate traditional gambling activities, and that people with gambling problems have been shown to spend more on loot boxes than peers without gambling problems. Some argue that the regulation of loot boxes as gambling-like mechanics is inappropriate because similar activities which also bear striking similarities to traditional forms of gambling, such as collectable card games, are not subject to such regulations. Players of collectible card games often buy sealed physical packs of cards, and these ‘booster packs’ share many formal similarities with loot boxes. However, not everything which appears similar to gambling requires regulation. Here, in a large sample of collectible card game players (n = 726), we show no statistically significant link between in real-world store spending on physical booster and problem gambling (p = 0.110, η2 = 0.004), and a trivial in magnitude relationship between spending on booster packs in online stores and problem gambling (p = 0.035, η2 = 0.008). Follow-up equivalence tests using the TOST procedure rejected the hypothesis that either of these effects was of practical importance (η2 > 0.04). Thus, although collectable card game booster packs, like loot boxes, share structural similarities with gambling, it appears that they may not be linked to problem gambling in the same way as loot boxes. We discuss potential reasons for these differences. Decisions regarding regulation of activities which share structural features with traditional forms of gambling should be made on the basis of definitional criteria as well as whether people with gambling problems purchase such items at a higher rate than peers with no gambling problems. Our research suggests that there is currently little evidence to support the regulation of collectable card games.

Introduction

Recently, there has been a surge in public policy interest in the incorporation of gambling-like mechanics in video games [1,2]. In particular, the concerns have focused upon loot boxes–digital containers of randomised rewards often purchasable for real world money. Many loot boxes appear to meet the psychological (Drummond & Sauer, 2018), and legal [3] definitional characteristics of traditional gambling practices. As such, many policymakers have regulated (e.g., Belgium, Japan, the Netherlands) or considered regulating (e.g., Australia, New Zealand, UK, US) loot boxes in some way. One argument that has been put forward by industry representatives is that many existing activities, such as Collectable Card Games (CCGs), utilise the same mechanics as loot boxes and are not currently subject to such regulation [4]. Researchers have certainly raised concerns about the possibility that CCGs might constitute a form of gambling (e.g., [5]). However, just because something appears to resemble gambling does not necessarily mean it is a form of gambling. Chief among the key differences between the case for regulating loot boxes and the case for regulating CCGs is the mounting evidence that players with higher problem gambling symptomology spend more on loot boxes than players with fewer problem gambling symptoms, suggesting that people with gambling problems may engage with them in a similar manner to traditional gambling activities [2,613]. No such evidence currently exists for CCGs. Here, we investigate whether people with higher problem gambling symptomology also spend more on CCGs than people with fewer problem gambling symptoms. If they do, this may imply that a discussion about whether CCGs require regulation is needed. If they are not purchased disproportionately by people with gambling problems, then this may imply that CCGs are not engaged with in a similar manner to traditional forms of gambling, and that regulators should not treat CCGs as equivalent to loot boxes.

What are loot boxes?

Loot boxes are items in video games that may be bought for real-world money but contain randomised contents. The value of any given loot boxes content is also uncertain at the time of purchase. For example, players of the football game FIFA Ultimate Team may pay real-world money to purchase digital ‘player packs’ in-game. These virtual packs contain new footballers that gamers may use when playing FIFA. The contents of a player pack may be both desirable and valuable–or they may not be. One may pay to open a pack and receive a high-powered and helpful player such as Arsenal’s Pierre-Emerick Aubameyang. Or one may instead receive a less powerful and useful player such as Oxford United’s Shandon Baptiste.

Loot box mechanics are prevalent on both mobile and desktop games. Indeed, the majority of top-grossing mobile games appear to contain loot boxes [14]. Furthermore, rates of exposure to loot boxes appear to have been high for several years. Analysis of publicly-available daily player counts from the online gaming platform Steam suggests that the majority of desktop play sessions have taken place in games that feature loot boxes since as early as 2015 [15].

Loot boxes and gambling

The randomisation of rewards present in loot boxes has led to comparisons between them and gambling. Consider placing a bet on a roulette wheel and opening a loot box. In both cases, individuals are staking real-world money on the chance outcome of a future event. These similarities have led to suggestions that some loot boxes may be “psychologically akin” to gambling [1]. These similarities might result in greater engagement in gambling amongst vulnerable groups, and in turn, lead to the development of problem gambling amongst this group [16]. Problem gambling is commonly considered to be extremely harmful. It refers to an excessive and disordered engagement with gambling activities that is typically outside of the gambler’s volitional control and leads to severe problems in their personal, financial, and professional lives [17,18].

Recent research has established the existence of a reliable link between loot box spending and problem gambling: The more that a gamer spends on loot boxes, the more they also appear to be at risk of gambling problems [6,8,9,19,20]. It is unclear what this link represents. It may, as suggested above, represent a situation in which engagement with loot boxes literally causes problem gambling. Alternatively, it may be that people with gambling problems find loot boxes particularly attractive, and spend disproportionally more money on them than other players. However, in either case, academics have argued that links between problem gambling and loot box spending may be an important potential source of real-world harm [7,10,12,21].

What are booster packs?

In collectible card games players are often able to buy sealed physical packs of cards, known as booster packs. The contents of booster packs are typically determined in a random manner. Thus, the value of any given booster pack is of uncertain value to the player at the point of purchase, and they are thus similar to loot boxes in video games. For example, players of the strategic battle card game Magic: The Gathering may spend money to purchase packs containing additional cards to use when playing the game. The contents of these packs vary in desirability and value. A player may open a booster pack and receive a Mox Opal card, for example, which is extremely useful and hence can fetch more than $100 on a resale market. However, sometimes the contents of a booster pack are neither desirable nor valuable. Players may open a booster pack and merely receive cards like Memoricide, Steel Hellkite, and Tunnel Ignus, which are less useful and fetch only a few cents on resale markets. When purchasing booster packs, players of collectible card games therefore cannot know the value of what they will receive in return for their money.

Gambling and booster packs

In [22], Griffiths defines a set of criteria by which gambling activities may be differentiated from other forms of behaviour:

  1. Gambling involves an exchange of money or other valuable goods

  2. This exchange is determined by a future event, whose outcome is unknown when the bet is made

  3. This event’s result is determined (at least in part) by chance

  4. Winners gain at the sole expense of losers

  5. Losses can be avoided by not taking part in the gamble

Drummond and Sauer [1] note that a number of loot boxes appear to meet these criteria, and hence could be considered psychologically akin to gambling. By extension, these criteria may be also seen to apply to booster packs in CCGs. When purchasing a booster pack, players are involved in an exchange of money or valuable goods: A booster pack for the Pokemon collectible card game, for example, currently costs approximately £3.99 and contains 11 randomly-assorted cards. Similarly, the success of this exchange is determined by a future event (the opening of the booster) whose outcome is unknown when the booster is purchased. Drummond and Sauer argue that just as with loot boxes, winners gain at the expense of losers in that those who receive useful cards in their booster packs gain an in-game competitive advantage over those who do not receive useful and rare cards. Finally, a loss of money can be avoided simply by not buying a booster pack.

Indeed, some similarities between physical card collections and digital loot boxes are so great that some video game loot boxes take the appearance of ‘virtual’ card packs. For example, FIFA Ultimate Team’s ‘player packs’ are presented in-game in a way that mirrors the opening of physical packs of playing cards. Even more strikingly, some video games adopt overarching in-game metaphors that directly mimic traditional physical collectible card games, and incorporate loot boxes into these games in the form of digital card packs. Hearthstone, for example, is a computer game in which players collect and battle using virtual cards in a similar manner to physical card games such as Magic: The Gathering and the Pokemon collectible card games. Players of Hearthstone may pay real world money to purchase virtual ‘packs’ of digital cards. However, crucially, this monetisation technique involves disbursing virtual and not physical goods, and takes place within the context of a video game: whilst it may have graphics which seek to mimic real-world booster packs, it nonetheless constitutes a video game loot box under out definitions, due to its virtual content and presence within a video game rather than a booster pack which distributes physical cards for a physical card game.

The similarities outlined above have led to comparisons between collectible card games and loot boxes by members of the video game industry [23]. These similarities have led to significant policy interest in whether the effects of loot boxes and booster packs are fundamentally equivalent: The terms of reference of a recent evidence call by the UK government, for example, requested information regarding “Whether any harms identified [to be associated with loot boxes] also apply to offline equivalents of chance mechanisms, such as buying packs of trading cards” [24].

Differences between booster packs and loot boxes

Booster packs therefore share many formal similarities with both loot boxes and more traditional gambling activities. However, it is important to note that loot boxes and booster packs, whilst similar, are not identical. A key distinction between booster packs and loot boxes lies in the fact that loot boxes are purchased and opened in an entirely virtual environment. There is no such thing as a physical player pack for FIFA or a physical weapon case for Counter-Strike: Global Offensive. Both payment for these items, acquisition of these items, and the opening of these items take place online in a digitally-mediated fashion. By contrast, the opening of booster packs necessarily takes place in a physical space. Whilst booster packs may be bought either in a physical store or via an online storefront, they must always be opened in the real world. Because of this qualitative difference between loot boxes and physical collectible card games, there may be important real-world differences in the experience and effects of engagement with these items.

First, the digital nature of loot boxes means that companies can present the opening of them in creative ways that would not be possible in the real world. Often, these methods are designed to increase the engagement of the player, using a range of salient visual and auditory cues. For example, when opening loot boxes in Counter-Strike: Global Offensive, players are presented with a slot machine-like interface which displays to them a revolving reel of potential prizes, eventually settling on the item which is inside the box that they are opening. When opening a player pack in FIFA, the in-game interface is similarly exciting in nature, and presents players with a spectacular light-show if a rare player is uncovered during an opening [25]. By contrast, opening a physical booster pack involves tearing open a physical pack of cards–whilst such packs may contain exciting foil covers, the audio-visual experience associated with this event is much more constrained.

The importance of visual and auditory cues in promotion of gambling products has been extensively studied. Both types of cues contribute to the subjective arousal experienced by a gambler, in addition to the arousal elicited by the prospect of uncertain gains and losses [26,27]. In fact, overlapping neural substrates responsible for processing rewards, emotional responses, and habit formation appear to be reactive to cues (e.g. visual) associated with gambling [28,29]. Casinos appear to strategically choose the background music and sounds produced by the gambling machines to encourage the development and maintenance of gambling behaviour [30]. It is therefore possible that the visual and auditory cues associated with loot box openings contribute to their appeal for people with gambling problems.

Second, the digital nature of loot boxes allows players to buy, acquire, and open these products very rapidly. Videos posted to video streaming sites like Youtube, for example, demonstrate that games are set up to allow gamers to purchase, receive, and open thousands of dollars’ worth of loot boxes over just a few minutes time [31,32]. By contrast, booster packs take more time to be opened. It is necessarily slower to go to a storefront, find the booster pack for the game of one’s choice, purchase said booster pack, find a suitable environment to open it in, and then open it. By comparison, the environment in which a loot box is opened is custom-built to ensure that the experience of spending is as rapid and smooth as possible. In recognition of the danger posed by the possibility of gambling away ones money very rapidly, recent gambling regulation in the UK imposed the £2 limits on fixed odds betting machines [33].

From the perspective of behavioural science, the ease with which loot boxes are purchased and opened is particularly relevant to problem gambling. It is well known that when facing a choice between amounts of money that could be received at different points in time, people typically demand a premium to accept a reward that will be received in a more distant future. In other words, people discount future rewards and are willing to accept less money with the immediate effect. In numerous studies, steeper discount rates of future rewards have been associated with various forms of addiction (alcohol, cigarettes, cocaine) as well as financial mismanagement [34]. People with gambling problems also appear to be more impulsive and have higher discounting rates of future rewards than non-gamblers, which suggest that more rapid discounting of future rewards may be a route to problem gambling [35,36]. The instantaneous nature of loot boxes in today’s video games seems to lift barriers that could otherwise prevent those at risk of developing a gambling problem from engaging in games of chance.

Furthermore, it is important to note that the way that cards are played and collected amongst players of physical collectible card games may differ from how loot box contents are accrued by players of video games. For instance, anecdotal reports suggest that players of collectible card games may end up owning large number of cards through means other than buying. For example, the tangible nature of physical card games mean that players may gift or trade cards with each other. In a video game, rules for the transfer of property between players are controlled by the architects of the game, and may not allow such transfers to take place. For example, loot box contents in the video game Last Shelter: Survival are locked to a player’s account, and may not be traded between players [37]. Conversely, winning packs, cards, or decks by participating in formal and informal tournaments is thought to be a common occurrence amongst players of physical card games. Such bonuses may not translate to all loot box implementations.

Finally, it is important to note that specific technological differences between loot boxes and booster packs may be responsible for different relationships between these things and problem gambling. For example, many video games do not disclose the odds of loot boxes, and sometimes even algorithmically adjust the odds of receiving different rewards as people spend money [37]: This kind of manipulation is not possible when buying booster packs. In a similar vein, aggressive or otherwise misleading advertising regarding microtransactions are reported by players of video games [38]: Such advertising may differ in its form and prevalence within the context of physical collectible card games.

Summary

On the face of it, there appear to be both substantial similarities and substantial differences between CCG booster packs and loot boxes. However, there is no evidence regarding whether the correlates of booster pack spending mirror the correlates of loot box spending: while people with gambling problems have repeatedly been shown to spend more on loot boxes than their peers without gambling problems, presently no such evidence exists for CCGs.

Some stakeholders argue that loot boxes and booster packs are fundamentally equivalent activities. If this were the case, one would expect that effects observed in the context of loot boxes may replicate in the context of booster packs. However, another perspective might suggest that fundamental differences between loot boxes and booster packs mean that their correlates (and potential effects) differ in important ways.

In short, although CCGs appear to be similar to loot boxes, it is important to establish whether they are associated with problem gambling symptomology in the same way as loot boxes. If no link between problem gambling symptomology and CCGs exist, then narratives regarding the identicality of loot boxes and CCGs may be unhelpful to understanding the effects of loot boxes. Here, we investigate both the size and importance of links between spending on booster packs and problem gambling to provide empirical evidence on whether problem gambling symptomology is linked to spending on CCG booster packs.

Hypotheses

Based on the fact that CCG booster packs structurally resemble loot boxes, we predicted that there would be a relationship between problem gambling symptoms and CCG booster pack purchasing (H1-H2). We also investigated (H3-H4) whether the relationship between problem gambling and spending on booster packs was greater than or less than an η2 of 0.04, which is commonly defined as being a threshold for effects of clinical significance [39]. Specifically, we predicted

  • H1. The more money an individual spends on booster packs in real-world stores, the more severe their problem gambling is.

  • H2. The more money an individual spends on booster packs in digital stores, the more severe their problem gambling is.

  • H3. The strength of the relationship between spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores and problem gambling will be equivalent to magnitude η2 = 0.04 or greater.

  • H4. The strength of the relationship between spending on physical booster packs in digital stores and problem gambling will be equivalent to magnitude η2 = 0.04 or greater.

Method

Ethics

Ethical approval for this research was granted by the York St. John University Cross-Departmental Ethics Board. Informed consent was gathered from each participant.

Design

We conducted a cross-sectional online survey with a sample of players of collectible card games aged 18+. Participants were recruited via posts on reddit (www.reddit.com), a popular internet bulletin board. Posts were made to a variety of ‘subreddits’, or specialist interest bulletin boards, for a variety of collectible card games. The specific subreddits that the survey was posted to are as follows: r/wixoss; r/cardfightvanguard; r/FoWtcg; r/CardWarsTCG; r/PokemonTCG; r/TCG; r/pkmntcg; r/yugioh. Posts mentioned that the survey is about “physical card games, and more specifically, spending preferences in physical card games” but does not mention loot boxes. It invites individuals to take part in “a *very short* questionnaire (5 minutes max) that asks a couple of questions about spending on card games”. No reimbursement was given to participants in return for their participation in this study.

In total, 731 responses were collected. Four participants with duplicate IP addresses were removed from the sample. One participant listed their age as 100, and their gender as ‘cat’. They were removed from the sample as non-serious. This left a total of 726 responses.

Participant gender was recorded via an open-response text entry box. 655 participants (90%) described themselves as ‘Male’ or ‘M’. 49 described themselves as ‘Female’ or ‘F’ (6%). The remaining 22 participants identified as other categories (e.g., Non bigendered, genderfluid).

123 participants (16.9%) were aged 18–19; 520 (71.6%) 20–29; 70 (9.6%) 30–39, and 13 (1.8%) 40+.

The majority of the sample (n = 434) indicated that their residence was within the USA, the UK (n = 67), and Canada (n = 52). However, the sample was highly international, with 32 participants coming from Germany, 24 from Australia, and the remaining 117 participants coming from countries ranging from Italy to Indonesia.

Measurements

Spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores was measured by asking participants the following question, where $CURRENCY was replaced dynamically by the currency in the participant’s home country:

  • During the last month, how much money in $CURRENCY would you say that you have spent on booster packs in *real-world stores* (such as your local Walmart)?

  • Please not that we are only interested in spending on *physical* booster packs—not virtual or digital ones. Cards that you can hold in your hand.

  • If you have not bought a booster pack in a real-world store during the past month, just enter ’0’.

Spending on physical booster packs in digital stores was measured by asking participants the following question, where $CURRENCY was replaced dynamically by the currency in the participant’s home country:

  • During the last month, how much money in $CURRENCY would you say that you have spent on booster packs in *digital stores* (such as a website that sells boosters and posts them to you)?

  • Please note that we are only interested in spending on *physical* booster packs—not virtual or digital ones. Cards that you can hold in your hand.

  • If you have not bought a booster pack in a digital store during the past month, just enter ’0’.

All spending data was converted into US Dollars and rank-transformed prior to analysis. Studies that investigate loot box spending (e.g. [19]) have routinely found extreme outliers in spending data. Rank transformation was therefore applied prior to all analysis, in an identical fashion to as in [16].

Problem gambling was measured via the Problem Gambling Severity Index [40]. This is a 9-item instrument that asks individuals a series of questions about the gambling related behaviours. For example, one item asks “Have you borrowed money or sold anything to get money to gamble?”. In response to each of these items, participants are asked to select one of four options: (0) Never; (1) Sometimes; (2) Most of the time; (3) Almost always. An overall measurement of problem gambling is formed from the sum of these scores, with values ranging from 0 to 27. These scores are then commonly transformed into diagnostic categories, with values of 0 indicating a non problem gambler; values of 1–4 indicating an individual at low risk of problem gambling; values of 5–7 representing an individual at moderate risk of problem gambling, and values of 8 or more indicating the presence of problem gambling [41]. The PGSI has demonstrated good internal validity (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.8) in a variety of previous studies (e.g. [8,4244]). In this sample, Cronbach’s alpha was measured at 0.82.

Justification of statistical analyses

This paper seeks to replicate the analyses present within Zendle and Cairns (2018). During review, some questions were raised regarding the justification of these analyses. In order to highlight the decision making regarding these analyses, we incorporate additional information below.

An anonymous reviewer asked why parametric analyses (e.g. ANOVA) were not used here, and nonparametric tests used instead. The intention here was two-fold: First, to replicate prior analyses; second, to use a test which does not require the strict distributional assumptions of an ANOVA. It is not clear that normality assumptions associated with ANOVA would be met in this case.

This reviewer also asked why data were rank-transformed prior to analysis. Again, this was done in an attempt to replicate prior work as closely as possible. One important thing to note here is that the crucial preregistered analyses here are all rank-sum tests: as such, they involve rank transformation during calculation, and thus this transformation cannot change the calculated test statistic. We appreciate that one may (rightly) view this transformation as unnecessary for some analyses, but it is also true that this transformation cannot in any way affect the statistics that these tests calculate, and hence the validity of their results.

The same reviewer also asked whether additional tests were necessary in this case in order to establish median shift via the Kruskal Wallis. This is an interesting point. We would note that the lack of a significant result in the case of all analyses means that equality of variability is not necessary for the interpretation of the Kruskal Wallis statistic (i.e., we are never in the position of trying to work out whether dominance or median shift has occurred, because the test highlights no significant differences). Again, this approach is in line with the replication of prior work.

Finally, it was asked why the PGSI was grouped in the way that was undertaken, and whether this might affect results. The PGSI measure was grouped categorically in line with common practice using this instrument: There is evidence that the diagnostic categories associated with the PGSI have important real-world meaning. We acknowledge that grouping responses by category tends to reduce the information yielded by the data, and that one might suggest that this information encodes a significant relationship between problem gambling and booster pack spending. However, crucially we would also note that we conduct an equivalence test of the correlation between ‘raw’ (i.e. uncategorised) problem gambling scores and both forms of booster pack spending.

Results

A description of spending on booster physical booster packs in both real-world and digital stores, split by problem gambling severity, is given below as Table 1.

Table 1. Medians, inter-quartile ranges, and mean ranks for booster pack spending, split by problem gambling severity and spending in physical vs. online stores.

Problem gambling severity Spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores Spending on physical booster packs in digital stores N
People without gambling problems Median: $12.90, IQR: $39.00 Mean rank: 349.69 Median: $0.00, IQR: $3.52 Mean rank: 365.77 429
Low-risk gamblers Median: $18.75, IQR: $50.00 Mean rank: 379.5 Median: $0.00, IQR: $0.00 Mean rank: 352.21 244
Moderate-risk gamblers Median: $20.00, IQR: $69.00 Mean rank: 388.87 Median: $0.00, IQR: $50.00 Mean rank: 432.00 35
People with gambling problems Median: $33.75, IQR: $118.75 Mean rank: 433.92 Median: $0.00, IQR: $0.00 Mean rank: 329.31 18

Contrary to H1, a Kruskal Wallis H Test indicated that there was no statistically significant effect of problem gambling severity on spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores, χ2(3) = 6.03, p = 0.110, η2 = 0.004. A box-plot showing the amount spent on physical booster packs in real world stores by different problem gambling severity groups is depicted below as Fig 1.

Fig 1. Box-plot showing the relationship between problem gambling severity (people without gambling problems, low-risk gamblers, moderate-risk gamblers, people with gambling problems) and spending on booster packs in real-world stores.

Fig 1

The y-axis is log2 transformed in order to allow the display of extreme spending values. Bars represent medians, boxes represent quartiles, and whiskers represent range of spending.

Consistent with H2, a Kruskal Wallis H Test indicated that there was a statistically significant effect of problem gambling severity on spending on spending on physical booster packs in digital stores, χ2(3) = 8.54, p = 0.035, η2 = 0.008. In order to further explore this effect, pairwise comparisons were then conducted to measure the effects of problem gambling on booster pack spending in digital stores between all groups of gamblers via a series of 6 Mann-Whitney U tests. Bonferroni corrections were applied to the results of these tests, lowering the alpha level of the tests to 0.05/6, or 0.008. Only one significant difference was observed–low-risk gamblers spent significantly less than moderate risk gamblers, U (n1 = 244, n2 = 35) = 3329.5, p = .005. All comparisons are reported below as Table 2, and a box plot showing the amount spent on physical booster packs in digital stores is depicted below as Fig 2.

Table 2. Results of pairwise analysis of effects of problem gambling severity on spending on physical booster packs in digital stores.

Problem gambling severity W n1 n2 p-value r Equivalent η2
Non people with gambling problems Low-risk gamblers 54292 429 244 0.285 0.041 0.001
Moderate-risk gamblers 6139.5 429 35 0.022 0.106 0.011
People with gambling problems 4248 429 18 0.347 0.044 0.001
Low-risk gamblers Moderate-risk gamblers 3329.5 244 35 0.005* 0.166 0.027
People with gambling problems 2335.5 244 18 0.53 0.038 0.001
Moderate-risk gamblers People with gambling problems 404 35 18 0.048 0.272 0.073

Results that are significant at the p<0.008 level are marked *.

Fig 2. Box-plot showing the relationship between problem gambling severity (non people with gambling problems, low-risk gamblers, moderate-risk gamblers, people with gambling problems) and spending on physical booster packs in digital stores.

Fig 2

The y-axis is log2 transformed in order to allow the display of extreme spending values. Bars represent medians, boxes represent quartiles, and whiskers represent range of spending.

To test whether the strength of the relationship between spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores and problem gambling will be equivalent to magnitude η2 = 0.04 or greater (H3 we employed the TOST procedure described by Lakens [45]). An equivalence test was calculated over the correlation between spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores and raw problem gambling severity scores. Equivalence bounds were placed at rs = 0.2 and rs = -0.2, equivalent to η2 = 0.04 and η2 = -0.04. Results indicated that the correlation between booster pack in real-world stores was statistically equivalent to this effect size. The 90% confidence interval for rho was calculated at 0.011–0.133 (η2: 0.000–0.017), p < 0.001.To test whether the strength of the relationship between spending on physical booster packs in digital stores and problem gambling will be equivalent to magnitude η2 = 0.04 or greater” (H4 the same equivalence testing procedure was employed. Results again indicated that the correlation between booster pack in real-world stores was statistically equivalent to η2 = 0.04. The 90% confidence interval for rho was calculated at -0.061–0.061 (η2: -0.003–0.003), p < 0.001.

The procedure we followed is the standard method for carrying out an equivalence test over a correlation, as defined in [45]. This procedure involves transforming a correlation coefficient to a z-score via Fisher’s transformation. However, this calculation typically takes place using Pearson’s r, not Spearman’s rho. Spearman’s rho is strongly related to Pearson’s rho: Indeed, rho is simply a calculation of Pearson’s r over the ranks of a dataset. Pearson’s r and Spearman’s rho therefore share many features: For example, the sampling distribution of both these statistics asymptotically approaches normality as sample size increases. However, they may also differ in important ways: For example, the standard deviation of rho may be differently distributed to the same statistic calculated over r. It is therefore unclear whether this procedure may introduce some measure of error or bias when used in this context.

In order to address this concern, bootstrap confidence intervals were calculated over both Spearman rank correlation coefficients (number of repetitions = 1000). Results indicated that the 95% bootstrap confidence interval for the relationship between spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores and problem gambling lay entirely within the equivalence bounds of -0.2 to 0.2. The 95%CI spanned -0.003 to 0.155 (equivalent η2: 0.000–0.024). Results indicated that the 95% bootstrap confidence interval for the relationship between spending on physical booster packs in digital stores and problem gambling also lay entirely within the equivalence bounds of -0.2 and 0.2. The 95%CI spanned 0.000 and 0.075 (equivalent η2: 0.000–0.005).

During peer review, one anonymous peer reviewer noted the small number of moderate-risk gamblers within the study (n = 35). The reviewer suggested that moderate-risk gamblers be merged with high-risk gamblers to form a single group of n = 53, and the analysis re-conducted. Accordingly, data were recoded as suggested and exploratory Kruskal-Wallis tests were run. Under this exploratory coding scheme, no significant difference was observed between groups (non problem, low-risk, moderate-risk or people with gambling problems) for either spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores (χ2(2) = 5.45, p = 0.065, η2 = 0.004) or spending on physical booster packs in digital stores (χ2(2) = 3.64, p = 0.161, η2 = 0.002).

Discussion

The present project investigated whether spending on CCG booster packs differed between groups based on were related to problem gambling symptomology. Results of Kruskal-Wallis tests and follow-up Mann Whitney U tests provided little evidence for the existence of an important link between spending money on booster packs and problem gambling. Specifically, no significant relationship was observed between problem gambling severity and spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores. Similarly, whilst a statistically significant overall link was observed between spending on physical booster packs in digital stores and problem gambling (p = 0.035), the magnitude of this link was trivial in size, and failed to exceed our thresholds for clinical relevance. The starting point for effects of a small size is conventionally set at η2 = 0.01 [46], with effects smaller than this size commonly considered trivial [39]. The effect observed here was of η2 = 0.008.

Indeed, subsequent pairwise comparisons suggested few significant differences between gambling risk groups. The pairwise comparison of groups amongst which a difference would be suggestive of an important link between booster pack spending and problem gambling did not return statistically significant results. For example, the difference in booster pack spending between non people with gambling problems and people with gambling problems was not statistically significant–in fact, the effect size associated with this comparison was only of magnitude η2 = 0.001. Indeed, the only statistically significant effect observed during all pairwise comparisons was a difference in spending between low-risk gamblers and moderate-risk gamblers. However, again, the effect size associated with this difference was of only η2 = 0.016, suggesting it was unlikely to be of practical importance.

An even stronger picture was painted by the results of equivalence tests. In order to determine the practical utility of a test’s result, researchers commonly look at the effect size associated with that test. The idea that small effect sizes may indicate that a statistical result may not have significant real-world importance is discussed by Ferguson [39], a widely-cited primer on the interpretation of effect sizes. Ferguson [39] proposes that a ‘suggested minimum’ for effects to even be considered practically important in a clinical context be placed at r = 0.2 (η2 = 0.04). An equivalence test rejected the hypothesis that the relationship between problem gambling and spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores was equal to or larger than this magnitude. Similarly, a second equivalence test rejected the hypothesis that the relationship between problem gambling and spending on physical booster packs in digital stores was of this magnitude or larger. In order to be as conservative as possible given the nonparametric nature of the analyses employed here, exploratory bootstrap confidence intervals were calculated for the magnitude of the correlation between both problem gambling and spending in real-world stores, and problem gambling and spending in digital stores. These 95% confidence intervals both fell entirely within the equivalence bounds specified above. This is particularly important given that a recent meta-analysis reveals that the association between loot box purchasing and problem gambling symptomology is r > .20 [47]. That loot box purchasing and problem gambling symptomology are associated with r’s exceeding these guidelines for clinical relevance while purchasing of physical card packs for collectable card games were not, suggests potentially important differences between these activities in terms of their risks to people with gambling problems.

Why was there no important association observed between booster pack spending and problem gambling? As we noted in our introduction, there are multiple plausible explanations for why purchases of booster packs might share a fundamentally different relationship with gambling than loot boxes. For example, the physical nature of booster packs may make purchasing and opening them significantly less quick and easy than purchasing and opening loot boxes. A number of interventions designed to limit gambling harm (e.g., Limit Setting, Drummond et al., 2019) [7] are targeted at the slowing of decision making to encourage slow deliberative processing over fast impulsive decision making [48]. One might imagine that the purchasing and opening of loot boxes can occur much faster than booster packs as one would either have to physically drive to a store to purchase the packs, or wait for physical shipping of the products if purchased online. These delays may be enough to disrupt impulsive and potentially problematic behaviour.

Limitations

The study described above represents a first attempt to replicate effects seen amongst loot boxes in the context of physical collectible card games. Whilst it provides important evidence when it comes to this question, it is nonetheless limited in a number of important ways. In this section, we describe the primary limitations of the approach taken above, and put forward suggestions for future work that builds on it.

The most important limitation of the study described above mirrors the most important limitation of contemporary research on loot boxes: As with many studies that explore the potential for harm present in loot boxes, this piece of research is cross-sectional in nature (e.g. [6,8,11,16,20,49,50]). It is fair to note that a causal relationship between booster pack spending and problem gambling cannot be captured via a cross-sectional approach. Evidence for a causal relation would require a longitudinal or experimental approach. Further work in this domain should build on the research conducted here using these designs.

A second key limitation of the research conducted here is the obtained sample: Our participants consisted of 726 volunteers, drawn from the online bulletin board service reddit. Samples drawn from reddit appear to skew young, and skew male (e.g. [2,51]). Indeed, this is a limitation of the work that we seek to replicate, which likewise draws its sample from reddit. A lack of academic research into players of physical collectible card games makes it unclear how representative these samples are of players of collectible card games in general. However, one may credibly propose that selecting samples of volunteers from bulletin boards designed for enthusiastic players of collectible card games may lead researchers to end up with an unrepresentatively engaged sample of players of these games. On the one hand, this is an advantage of the approach taken when it comes to ecological validity: If one is interested in spending on booster packs being linked to problem gambling, it is worthwhile looking at a sample of individuals who are likely to spend money on booster packs. However, on the other hand, one might reasonably argue that such an approach suppresses variability within our sample, and may mask the presence of real correlations within the population. Further research with more representative samples of players of collectible card games is necessary to determine which of these is the case.

Another limitation of this study is the differing timeframe between the PGSI and our measurements of spending behaviour: Whilst the PGSI measures gambling activity over the course of the previous year, we only assessed booster pack spending during the previous month, as in line with other literature (e.g. [52]). One might argue that gamers may not remember how much money they spent 10 or 11 months ago, and that capturing spending during a more proximal period may lead to more accurate measurement. However, one might also imagine a situation in which an individual spends heavily on both booster packs and gambling for 11 months, and then ceases both activities for the month prior to measurement occurring: Such behaviour would be captured adequately by the PGSI, but not by our measure of spending. Further research using more distal spending measures is necessary to determine what the consequence of this approach to measurement is.

Our sample contained a small number of individuals who gambled in a problematic manner, and a small number of moderate-risk gamblers. Our analyses may therefore be underpowered to determine differences between these groups and other groups when it comes to booster pack spending. Indeed, as noted above, further research using larger and more representative samples is needed when it comes to understanding spending on collectible card games. It is also important to note that all equivalence tests rejected the idea, even when power was increased by combining moderate and high risk groups (ps <0.001), presenting strong evidence that the observed relationship was equivalent to one of a clinically significant magnitude.

An additional limitation to our approach relates to our strict replication of the techniques used in an early paper on loot box spending [2]. It was our intention to replicate this study as closely as possible, in order to minimise any additional factors that might be responsible for an observed difference in results between our paper and the study that we were attempting to replicate. However, the study that we replicated, and by extension, the present study did not involve extensive data cleaning: For example, very fast responses were not purged from either study. One may reasonably suggest that replicating this approach may lead to issues with data quality. This is a legitimate criticism: In fact, the authors of the original paper that is replicated here later replicated both their analyses (and results) with such procedures in place [20]. As such, further work should focus on replicating the results obtained here with attention checks and greater data cleaning.

A final limitation of our employed approach relates to loot box spending. The purpose of this study was to attempt to address questions regarding the relationship between booster pack spending and gambling problems. As such, we neither measured nor analysed data regarding loot box purchasing. However, during review it was noted that augmenting our analyses with data on loot box purchasing might give a more holistic and rounded picture of how individuals spent money on randomised rewards, and the inter-relationship between this factor and gambling problems. Indeed, one could imagine a situation in which this factor were measured, and results indicated that (within a single sample), loot box spending was linked to gambling problems, whilst booster pack spending were not. Further work that adopts such a holistic approach is recommended in order to both replicate and increase confidence in the findings observed here.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that spending money on booster packs does not appear to be linked to problem gambling in the same way that spending money on loot boxes is. Loot boxes have repeatedly been linked to problem gambling symptoms (e.g., [79,16,52]. In contrast, these results show that CCGs are not linked to problem gambling in the same way. Two clear corollaries are evident from these findings 1) individual examination of the potentially harmful effects of activities that resemble gambling are required to determine which activities may require legislation, and 2) that loot boxes share some features with other unregulated activities should not be taken to suggest that loot boxes do not require regulation. Loot boxes have consistently been linked to problem gambling, whereas CCGs do not appear to similarly relate to increased spending for players with gambling problems. Investigation of other activities with similar structural features to loot boxes and traditional forms of gambling may be valuable to understand whether there are other activities which users with gambling problems spend more on than peers without gambling problems.

Acknowledgments

No further parties contributed to this study but did not meet criteria for authorship.

Data Availability

The data and R script underlying this study are available on OSF (https://osf.io/abx2t/).

Funding Statement

PC receives buyout from the EPSRC Doctoral Centre for Intelligent Games and Games Intelligence. AD is supported by the Marsden Fund Council from NZ Government funding, managed by Royal Society Te Apārangi; MAU1804. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Drummond A. and Sauer J. D., “Video game loot boxes are psychologically akin to gambling,” Nat. Hum. Behav., pp. 530–532, June. 2018, 10.1038/s41562-018-0360-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Zendle D. and Cairns P., “Video game loot boxes are linked to problem gambling: Results of a large-scale survey,” PLoS One, vol. 13, no. 11, p. e0206767, 2018. 10.1371/journal.pone.0206767 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Drummond A., Sauer J. D., Hall L. C., Zendle D., and Loudon M. R., “Why loot boxes could be regulated as gambling,” Nat. Hum. Behav., pp. 1–3, June. 2020, 10.1038/s41562-020-0818-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.IGEA, “Submission to Senate Environment and Communications References Committee,” text, Jul. 2018. Accessed: Dec. 01, 2018. [Online]. Available: https://www.aph.gov.au/Parliamentary_Business/Committees/Senate/Environment_and_Communications/Gamingmicro-transactions/Submissions.
  • 5.Craddock K. D., “The Cardstock Chase, Trading Cards: A Legal Lottery?,” Gaming Law Rev., vol. 8, no. 5, pp. 310–317, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Brooks G. A. and Clark L., “Associations between loot box use, problematic gaming and gambling, and gambling-related cognitions,” Addict. Behav., 2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Drummond A., Sauer J. D., and Hall L. C., “Loot box limit-setting: A potential policy to protect video game users with gambling problems?,” Addiction, 2019. 10.1111/add.14583 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Drummond A., Sauer J. D., Ferguson C. J., and Hall L. C., “The relationship between problem gambling, excessive gaming, psychological distress and spending on loot boxes in Aotearoa New Zealand, Australia, and the United States—A cross-national survey,” PLOS ONE, vol. 15, no. 3, p. e0230378, March. 2020, 10.1371/journal.pone.0230378 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Li W., Mills D., and Nower L., “The relationship of loot box purchases to problem video gaming and problem gambling,” Addict. Behav., vol. 97, pp. 27–34, October. 2019, 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.05.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zendle D., “Only problem gamblers spend less money when loot boxes are removed from a game: A before and after study of Heroes of the Storm,” June. 2019, 10.31234/osf.io/a3kp2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zendle D., “Beyond loot boxes: a variety of gambling-like practices in video games are linked to both problem gambling and disordered gaming,” PeerJ, vol. 8, July. 2020, 10.7717/peerj.9466 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Zendle D., Cairns P., Barnett H., and McCall C., “Paying for loot boxes is linked to problem gambling, regardless of specific features like cash-out and pay-to-win,” Comput. Hum. Behav., July. 2019, 10.1016/j.chb.2019.07.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zendle D. and Cairns P., “Loot box spending in video games is again linked to problem gambling,” PloS One, In press, https://psyarxiv.com/u5dmr. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Zendle D., Meyer R., Waters S., Cairns P., and Ballou N., “The prevalence of loot boxes in mobile and desktop games,” Addiction, January. 2020, Accessed: Jan. 20, 2020. [Online]. Available: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/add.14973?casa_token=JnwIbkbkPxQAAAAA:lZfA6Xzl98biEsPPIGmWlJA0r3lYtoG4GKXoSb9qghAnX-S0UPa4Js6npuwBP5T1mCmxaRafalt71Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Zendle D., Meyer R., and Ballou N., “The changing face of desktop video game monetisation: An exploration of exposure to loot boxes, pay to win, and cosmetic microtransactions in the most-played Steam games of 2010–2019,” PLOS ONE, vol. 15, no. 5, p. e0232780, May 2020, 10.1371/journal.pone.0232780 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Zendle D., Meyer R., and Over H., “Adolescents and loot boxes: links with problem gambling and motivations for purchase,” R. Soc. Open Sci., vol. 6, no. 6, p. 190049, June. 2019, 10.1098/rsos.190049 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Blaszczynski A. and Nower L., “A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling,” Addiction, vol. 97, no. 5, pp. 487–499, 2002. 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00015.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Erickson L., Molina C. A., Ladd G. T., Pietrzak R. H., and Petry N. M., “Problem and pathological gambling are associated with poorer mental and physical health in older adults,” Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry, vol. 20, no. 8, pp. 754–759, 2005. 10.1002/gps.1357 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Zendle D. and Cairns P., “Loot box spending in video games is linked to problem gambling severity,” September. 2018, None. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zendle D. and Cairns P., “Loot boxes are again linked to problem gambling: Results of a replication study,” PLoS One, vol. 14, no. 3, p. e0213194, March. 2019, 10.1371/journal.pone.0213194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Zendle D. and Bowden-Jones H., “Loot boxes and the convergence of video games and gambling,” Lancet Psychiatry, vol. 6, no. 9, pp. 724–725, 2019. 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30285-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Griffiths M., Adolescent Gambling. Psychology Press, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Taylor H., “Loot boxes: An industry at war with itself over a technicality,” GamesIndustry.biz, Dec. 12, 2018. https://www.gamesindustry.biz/articles/2018-12-12-loot-boxes-a-year-in-review (accessed Jan. 26, 2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 24.DCMS, “Loot Boxes in Video Games—Call for Evidence,” GOV.UK, September. 23, 2020. https://www.gov.uk/government/consultations/loot-boxes-in-video-games-call-for-evidence/loot-boxes-in-video-games-call-for-evidence (accessed Jan. 26, 2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 25.AnEsonGib, THE WORLDS LUCKIEST TOTS PACK OPENING!!! FIFA 19. 2019.
  • 26.Rockloff M. J., Signal T., and Dyer V., “Full of sound and fury, signifying something: the impact of autonomic arousal on EGM gambling,” J. Gambl. Stud., vol. 23, no. 4, pp. 457–465, December. 2007, 10.1007/s10899-007-9061-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Baudinet J. and Blaszczynski A., “Arousal and gambling mode preference: A review of the literature,” J. Gambl. Stud., vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 343–358, 2013. 10.1007/s10899-012-9304-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Noori H. R., Cosa Linan A., and Spanagel R., “Largely overlapping neuronal substrates of reactivity to drug, gambling, food and sexual cues: A comprehensive meta-analysis,” Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol., vol. 26, no. 9, pp. 1419–1430, September. 2016, 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.06.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Crockford D. N., Goodyear B., Edwards J., Quickfall J., and el-Guebaly N., “Cue-Induced Brain Activity in Pathological Gamblers,” Biol. Psychiatry, vol. 58, no. 10, pp. 787–795, November. 2005, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Griffiths and J. Parke, “The psychology of music in gambling environments: an observational research note,” J. Gambl. Issues JGI, 2005, Accessed: Nov. 03, 2019. [Online]. Available: http://www.camh.net/egambling/issue13/jgi_13_griffiths.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Seatin Man of Legends, 300x Cavalier Crystal Opening—Pep’s $2,000 5 & 6 Star Opening—Marvel Contest of Champions. 2019.
  • 32.ChrisMD, What Does Spending £5000 on FIFA 18 Packs Get You? 2017.
  • 33.Davies R., “Government makes U-turn over delay to £2 FOBT maximum stake,” The Guardian, November. 14, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Hamilton K. R. and Potenza M. N., “Relations among delay discounting, addictions, and money mismanagement: Implications and future directions,” Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse, vol. 38, no. 1, pp. 30–42, 2012. 10.3109/00952990.2011.643978 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dixon M. R., Marley J., and Jacobs E. A., “Delay Discounting by Pathological Gamblers,” J. Appl. Behav. Anal., vol. 36, no. 4, pp. 449–458, 2003, 10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Petry N. M. and Casarella T., “Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems,” Drug Alcohol Depend., vol. 56, no. 1, pp. 25–32, August. 1999, 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00010-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ballou N., Gbadamosi C. T. T., and Zendle D., “The hidden intricacy of loot box design: A granular description of random monetized reward features,” PsyArXiv, November. 2020. 10.31234/osf.io/xeckb. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Petrovskaya E. and Zendle D., “Predatory monetisation? A categorisation of unfair, misleading, and aggressive monetisation techniques in digital games from the perspective of players,” PsyArXiv, January. 2021. 10.31234/osf.io/cdwhq [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ferguson C. J., “An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers.,” Prof. Psychol. Res. Pract., vol. 40, no. 5, p. 532, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Ferris J. A. and Wynne H. J., The Canadian problem gambling index. Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse Ottawa, ON, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Currie S. R., Hodgins D. C., and Casey D. M., “Validity of the problem gambling severity index interpretive categories,” J. Gambl. Stud., vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 311–327, 2013. 10.1007/s10899-012-9300-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Barbaranelli C., Vecchione M., Fida R., and Podio-Guidugli S., “Estimating the prevalence of adult problem gambling in Italy with SOGS and PGSI,” J. Gambl. Issues, no. 28, pp. 1–24, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bertossa S., Harvey P., Smith D., and Chong A., “A preliminary adaptation of the Problem Gambling Severity Index for Indigenous Australians: Internal reliability and construct validity,” Aust. N. Z. J. Public Health, vol. 38, no. 4, pp. 349–354, 2014. 10.1111/1753-6405.12254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Orford J., Wardle H., Griffiths M., Sproston K., and Erens B., “PGSI and DSM-IV in the 2007 British Gambling Prevalence Survey: reliability, item response, factor structure and inter-scale agreement,” Int. Gambl. Stud., vol. 10, no. 1, pp. 31–44, April. 2010, 10.1080/14459790903567132 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lakens D., “Equivalence Tests,” Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci., vol. 8, no. 4, pp. 355–362, May 2017, 10.1177/1948550617697177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Cohen J., “A power primer.,” Psychol. Bull., vol. 112, no. 1, p. 155, 1992. 10.1037//0033-2909.112.1.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Garea S., Drummond A., Sauer J., Hall L., and Williams M., Meta-Analysis of the Relationship between Problem Gambling, Excessive Gaming and Loot Box Purchasing. 2020. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Harris A. and Griffiths M. D., “A critical review of the harm-minimisation tools available for electronic gambling,” J. Gambl. Stud., vol. 33, no. 1, pp. 187–221, 2017. 10.1007/s10899-016-9624-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Macey J. and Hamari J., “eSports, skins and loot boxes: Participants, practices and problematic behaviour associated with emergent forms of gambling,” New Media Soc., vol. 21, no. 1, pp. 20–41, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Wardle H. and Zendle D., “Loot Boxes, Gambling, and Problem Gambling Among Young People: Results from a Cross-Sectional Online Survey,” Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw., 2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Petrovskaya E. and Zendle D., “The Battle Pass: a Mixed-Methods Investigation into a Growing Type of Video Game Monetisation,” OSF Preprints, September. 2020. 10.31219/osf.io/vnmeq [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zendle D., “Problem gamblers spend less money when loot boxes are removed from a game: a before and after study of Heroes of the Storm,” PeerJ, vol. 7, p. e7700, 2019. 10.7717/peerj.7700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Stefano Triberti

22 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-32484

Links between problem gambling and spending on booster packs in collectible card games: A conceptual replication of research on loot boxes

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Zendle,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Two Reviewers evaluated the manuscript and suggested maior revisions, focusing in particular on methodological aspects and clarification of background and hypotheses. 

To their detailed revisions I would add just a comment on CCGs: it could be important to notice that, when one is deeply engaged in some card game, they end up possessing a significant portion of their decks or cards by means different from buying. You can win cards, packs or event entire decks (e.g., Keyforge) by participating to regular store tournaments, or have exchanges with other players that could be more or less advantageous (e.g., entire composite decks in exchange of one particularly valuable card). Moreover, packs or decks could be easily received as a gift when participating to official events. This could be useful to understand the difference between CCGs and gambling. 

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Feb 05 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Stefano Triberti, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent.

In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed).

If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians.

If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for the opportunity to review the present manuscript examining the relationship between collectible card games and problem gambling. The manuscript was well-written and investigates a timely research question given the comparison of loot boxes to trading cards. I believe the study adds valuable insight into the rapidly growing literature on the convergence of gaming-gambling. My main concern is the small sample size of people in the moderate-risk to problem gambling categories. I note my further comment below.

1. I would suggest using person centered language such as people with gambling problems rather to reduce the stigma associated with problem gambling.

2. On page 13, the authors state that “Whilst booster packs may be bought either in a physical store or via an online storefront, they must always be opened in the real world.” I believe this is not entirely accurate. Taking the MAGIC the Gathering example, Magic Online and Magic Arena allow booster packs to be open virtually. Thus, the paragraph on key distinction between loot boxes and CCGs may need to be amended.

3. Related to above, some of the concerns raised of loot boxes that are different compared to CCGs are: (i) questionable advertising practices targeting vulnerable individuals, and (ii) not knowing the odds of loot boxes, and or the ease in which the odds can be manipulated. These concerns may add context to the difference between loot boxes and CCGs and provide further argument for there potential differences. These may also help explain why there were no observed relationship.

4. Very minor comment, but the text for data availability and ethics are single spaced.

5. Were participants compensated for the study?

6. I am curious if the authors took any measures to screen for data quality such as attention checks, checking unusually fast attention checks? In other words, how are the authors sure that the responses gathered were of high quality?

7. Did the authors also assess past year spending on CCGs? If not, this should be mentioned as a limitation given that the PGSI measures past 12 months, while spending was measured in the past year. For example, it may be possible that some participants may have begun to limit their spending on CCGs in the past month, which would not be captured and may attenuated the relationship between PGSI and CCGs

8. I would also be curious if the authors assessed loot box purchases as well. I believe looking at the relationship between loot boxes, CCGs, and problem gambling would provide a more holistic picture.

9. The low sample sizes in the moderate-risk and problem gambling category may account for the null effects. Was a power analyses conducted to examine sample size to detect the clinically meaningful effect? I would like to see results of combining the moderate-risk and problem categories to boost the Ns of the category.

10. The authors discuss clinically meaningful effect. I believe the discussion section would be strengthened if the authors speak to the effect size in the relationship between loot boxes and problem gambling severity, which I believe are generally above .04. I believe this would help strengthen the argument that CCGs do not pose similar risks of problem gambling as loot boxes.

11. The limitation section is lacking (non-existent). Overall, I really enjoyed the paper but it is limited in some ways:

- Differing time frames between PGSI and CCG spending behavior

- Highly selected and convenience sample from Reddit, and as such results may not

generalize to CCG players

- Did not (?) assess loot boxes, which would have provided a more complete picture.

- Cross-sectional. Perhaps a clinically meaningful effect would be observed longitudinally?

- Small sample size of people in the moderate risk and problem gambling categories.

Reviewer #2: The study examines the relationship between collectable card game (CCG) booster pack purchasing, in the real world and online, and problem gambling symptom severity. A rationale for examining this relationship is that there is a positive relationship for loot box purchasing and problem gambling which is suggested to guide public policy decisions for such activities. It was hypothesized that because CCGs are similar to loot boxes, then there would be a significant relationship between categories of problem gambling severity and CCG pack purchasing. The study sought to answer this question using online surveys that were completed by a cross-sectional sample of 731 participants recruited from “Reddit.” It was ultimately concluded that there is no relationship between problem gambling severity and spending on booster packs. Specifically, there was no relationship between purchasing of physical booster packs and gambling severity and only a weak relationship between virtual booster pack purchasing and gambling severity (which failed to meet the threshold selected by the researchers). The study was interesting and seeks to answer an important question that could impact policy decisions surrounding the regulation of activities that resemble gambling. However, there are several limitations. Please take the following comments into consideration:

1. Introduction: An issue with the manuscript in the current form is whether the information presented in the introduction leads, conceptually, to the hypotheses that are initially tested. Based on my reading of the introduction, the reasoning, and how the two activities are contrasted, it seemed clear that CCG booster packs are not the same as loot boxes and, therefore, there should be no relationship between problem gambling and CCG booster purchasing (such an assumption would require equivalence testing, which the authors do, but I wonder why this approach wasn’t selected from the outset). Though some similarities between loot boxes and CCGs are discussed, many more differences are presented and the arguments seems stronger that the two activities are not the same. Thus, it is as if there is a disconnect between the conceptual framework offered in the introduction and the initial hypotheses the study seeks to test. To give an example, in the first paragraph, the statement challenges the case that CCGs are a form of gambling (i.e., "just because something appears to resemble gambling…") and statements such as these reoccurs throughout the introduction, but in the last paragraph the similarities between CCG boosters and loot boxes are evoked to justify that there should be a connection between this activity and problem gambling. Please consider changing the tone or rewording sections of the introduction to provide more congruence.

2. Pg. 3, Ln. 4: This sentence does not read well. Perhaps this is a syntax error or typographical error?

3. Methods: For the PGSI, please report previous reliabilities from other studies that used the PGSI as well as how the instrument performed in the current study (this is especially important in this context because the sample was international and questionnaires were collected fully online).

4. Pg. 10, Ln.17: The fact that one of the groups contains a small number of female participants would need to be evaluated given that there are differences between women and men in how they process and regulate the effects of problem gambling. Also, are there gender differences that should be addressed in terms of Reddit users or gambling in general? If so, please mention in the methods section or, if it cannot be addressed, as a study limitation in the discussion section.

5. Methods: Please consider including more information about recruitment of participants via Reddit.

6. Methods: The data analytic plan should be described in greater detail including the decisions for the analyses that are conducted. Why was the Kruskal Wallis H Test used rather than ANOVA? The problem of outliers is mentioned and a transformation was used to correct for this, but then why the use of a nonparametric test which is robust to outliers? Why not use the untransformed values then? Also, were all assumptions for the Kruskal Wallis H Test met (e.g., equal variability)? What was the rationale for using the PGSI measure categorically rather than continuously in this study? In short, there appear to be many statistical decisions and interpretations made by the authors that aren’t explained or justified to the reader.

7. Discussion: Some of the statements made are far too strong given the exploratory nature and limitations of this study and should not be generalized to all unregulated gambling-like activities (e.g., “Arguments that loot boxes are equivalent to other unregulated activities that resemble gambling appear to be empirically unjustified.”)

8. Discussion: There are a number of limitations to this study, but none are mentioned. Please include a through discussion of the limitations. Also, please consider including recommendations for future research to build upon and expand the results of the current study (e.g., an experimental or longitudinal design?).

9. Overall: The manuscript should undergo additional proofreading. There are misplaced commas, extra spaces, and some peculiar formatting choices (e.g., double parentheses for citations, authors' names referred to directly in sentence but still placed in parentheses, references without page numbers or only the starting page). As I believe this journal uses Vancouver style, which I am less familiar with than APA, perhaps I am simply unfamiliar with such conventions, but I would recommend reviewing the potential issues I mention here to be sure. Finally, the formatting for some of the graphs and figures made it difficult to follow the data.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Apr 6;16(4):e0247855. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247855.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


4 Feb 2021

Dear Professor Triberti and the editorial team,

Thank you for the opportunity to revise this manuscript “Links between problem gambling and spending on booster packs in collectible card games: A conceptual replication of research on loot boxes.” (PONE-D-20-32484). We wish to thank the editor and reviewers for their detailed comments. We provide a point-by-point response to all issues that were raised below (in bold).

We believe that the reviewer comments have substantially improved the manuscript.

We look forward to hearing from you in due course,

Kind regards,

Editor Comments

You said: “Two Reviewers evaluated the manuscript and suggested maior revisions, focusing in particular on methodological aspects and clarification of background and hypotheses.

To their detailed revisions I would add just a comment on CCGs: it could be important to notice that, when one is deeply engaged in some card game, they end up possessing a significant portion of their decks or cards by means different from buying. You can win cards, packs or event entire decks (e.g., Keyforge) by participating to regular store tournaments, or have exchanges with other players that could be more or less advantageous (e.g., entire composite decks in exchange of one particularly valuable card). Moreover, packs or decks could be easily received as a gift when participating to official events. This could be useful to understand the difference between CCGs and gambling.”

Our response: This is an excellent point, and one that you are right in stating we do not mention. We have extended our introduction to briefly touch on this topic, though we were unable to find any academic sources that discuss it. Specifically on page 10, we now state: “Furthermore, it is important to note that the way that cards are played and collected amongst players of physical collectible card games may differ from how loot box contents are accrued by players of video games. For instance, anecdotal reports suggest that players of collectible card games may end up owning large number of cards through means other than buying. For example, the tangible nature of physical card games mean that players may gift or trade cards with each other. In a video game, rules for the transfer of property between players are controlled by the architects of the game, and may not allow such transfers to take place.: For example, loot box contents in the video game Last Shelter: Survival are locked to a player’s account, and may not be traded between players (Ballou et al., 2020). Furthermore, winning packs, cards, or decks by participating in formal and informal tournaments is thought to be a common occurrence amongst players of physical card games.: Such bonuses may not translate to all loot box implementations.”

Reviewer 1 Comments

You said: “Thank you for the opportunity to review the present manuscript examining the relationship between collectible card games and problem gambling. The manuscript was well-written and investigates a timely research question given the comparison of loot boxes to trading cards. I believe the study adds valuable insight into the rapidly growing literature on the convergence of gaming-gambling. My main concern is the small sample size of people in the moderate-risk to problem gambling categories. I note my further comment below.

1. I would suggest using person centered language such as people with gambling problems rather to reduce the stigma associated with problem gambling.”

Our response: We thank the reviewer and agree with their suggestion. The manuscript has been redrafted in such a way that person-centred language is used throughout.

You said: “2. On page 13, the authors state that “Whilst booster packs may be bought either in a physical store or via an online storefront, they must always be opened in the real world.” I believe this is not entirely accurate. Taking the MAGIC the Gathering example, Magic Online and Magic Arena allow booster packs to be open virtually. Thus, the paragraph on key distinction between loot boxes and CCGs may need to be amended.”

Our response: This is a really good point, and shows a key area in which we must clarify our manuscript: Magic Arena is a video game in which individuals spend real-world money in order to obtain a randomised digital reward. Functionally, there is no difference between Magic Arena’s booster packs and -say- player packs in FIFA. Under the definitions used in this manuscript, Magic Online and Magic Arena would therefore be selling ‘loot boxes’ rather than ‘booster packs’. The key distinction we make between these forms is not what things look like, but whether they are physical or digital: Hearthstone (a video game) does not sell booster packs. We did not gather data from players of video games for this project, but only from people who bought physical booster packs. The questions within our survey were framed in such a way as to make this clear. We have augmented our manuscript both to make this key point clear; and to highlight the distinction between booster packs and loot boxes outlined above. Specifically, we now note that: “Indeed, some similarities between physical card collections and digital loot boxes are so great that some video game loot boxes take the appearance of ‘virtual’ card packs. For example, FIFA Ultimate Team’s ‘player packs’ are presented in-game in a way that mirrors the opening of physical packs of playing cards. Even more strikingly, some video games adopt overarching in-game metaphors that directly mimic traditional physical collectible card games, and incorporate loot boxes into these games in the form of digital card packs. Hearthstone, for example, is a computer game in which players collect and battle using virtual cards in a similar manner to physical card games such as Magic: The Gathering and the Pokemon collectible card games. Players of Hearthstone may pay real world money to purchase virtual ‘packs’ of digital cards. However, crucially, this monetisation technique involves disbursing virtual and not physical goods, and takes place within the context of a video game: whilst it may have graphics which seek to mimic real-world booster packs, it nonetheless constitutes a video game loot box under our definitions, due to its virtual content and presence within a video game rather than a booster pack which distributes physical cards for a physical card game.

The similarities outlined above have led to comparisons between collectible card games and loot boxes by members of the video game industry (Taylor, 2018). These similarities have led to significant policy interest in whether the effects of loot boxes and booster packs are fundamentally equivalent: The terms of reference of a recent evidence call by the UK government, for example, requested information regarding “Whether any harms identified [to be associated with loot boxes] also apply to offline equivalents of chance mechanisms, such as buying packs of trading cards.”(DCMS, 2020).” (p.7).

You said: “3. Related to above, some of the concerns raised of loot boxes that are different compared to CCGs are: (i) questionable advertising practices targeting vulnerable individuals, and (ii) not knowing the odds of loot boxes, and or the ease in which the odds can be manipulated. These concerns may add context to the difference between loot boxes and CCGs and provide further argument for there potential differences. These may also help explain why there were no observed relationship”.

Our response: This is a salient point, and we agree that the manuscript could be enhanced by taking it into account. We have augmented both our discussion and introduction in order to take it into consideration. Specifically on page 10 we now state: “Finally, it is important to note that specific technological differences between loot boxes and booster packs may be responsible for different relationships between these things and problem gambling. For example, many video games do not disclose the odds of loot boxes, and sometimes even algorithmically adjust the odds of receiving different rewards as people spend money (Ballou et al., 2020): This kind of manipulation is not possible when buying booster packs. In a similar vein, aggressive or otherwise misleading advertising regarding microtransactions are reported by players of video games (Petrovskaya & Zendle, 2021): Such advertising may differ in its form and prevalence within the context of physical collectible card games.”

You said: “4. Very minor comment, but the text for data availability and ethics are single spaced.”

Our response: We have adjusted the formatting to make sure that these are double-spaced.

You said: “5. Were participants compensated for the study?”

Our response: Participants receive no compensation for participation. We have added this detail to the manuscript. “No reimbursement was given to participants in return for their participation in this study” (p.12).

You said: “6. I am curious if the authors took any measures to screen for data quality such as attention checks, checking unusually fast attention checks? In other words, how are the authors sure that the responses gathered were of high quality?”

Our response: This is an interesting point. Our focus here was to replicate a previous paper. That paper did not apply any such cut-offs, so we did not put any in place. We have described this as a limitation of the approach taken. “An additional limitation to our approach relates to our strict replication of the techniques used in an early paper on loot box spending (Zendle & Cairns, 2018a). It was our intention to replicate this study as closely as possible, in order to minimise any additional factors that might be responsible for an observed difference in results between our paper and the study that we were attempting to replicate. However, the study that we replicated, and by extension, the present study did not involve extensive data cleansing: For example, very fast responses were not purged from either study. One may reasonably suggest that replicating this approach may lead to issues with data quality. This is a legitimate criticism: In fact, the authors of the original paper that is replicated here later replicated both their analyses (and results) with such procedures in place (Zendle & Cairns, 2019). As such, further work should focus on replicating the results obtained here with attention checks and greater data cleaning.” (Page 24).

You said: “7. Did the authors also assess past year spending on CCGs? If not, this should be mentioned as a limitation given that the PGSI measures past 12 months, while spending was measured in the past year. For example, it may be possible that some participants may have begun to limit their spending on CCGs in the past month, which would not be captured and may attenuated the relationship between PGSI and CCGs”

“8. I would also be curious if the authors assessed loot box purchases as well. I believe looking at the relationship between loot boxes, CCGs, and problem gambling would provide a more holistic picture.”

Our response: These are both salient points. We assessed neither of these things during this paper. We agree with the reviewer that these are interesting analyses, and have expanded our discussion to highlight them as topics for future research. Specifically on page 23-24 of the revised manuscript we note “Another limitation of this study is the differing timeframe between the PGSI and our measurements of spending behaviour: Whilst the PGSI measures gambling activity over the course of the previous year, we only assessed booster pack spending during the previous month, as in line with other literature (e.g. (Zendle, 2019a)). One might argue that gamers may not remember how much money they spent 10 or 11 months ago, and that capturing spending during a more proximal period may lead to more accurate measurement. However, one might also imagine a situation in which an individual spends heavily on both booster packs and gambling for 11 months, and then ceases both activities for the month prior to measurement occurring: Such behaviour would be captured adequately by the PGSI, but not by our measure of spending. Further research using more distal spending measures is necessary to determine what the consequence of this approach to measurement is.” And on page

9. The low sample sizes in the moderate-risk and problem gambling category may account for the null effects. Was a power analyses conducted to examine sample size to detect the clinically meaningful effect? I would like to see results of combining the moderate-risk and problem categories to boost the Ns of the category.

Our response: As suggested by the reviewer, we have combined these categories and repeated our analyses, taking care to flag these new analyses as exploratory. In addition, we would note that we calculated an equivalence test over the correlation coefficient between raw problem gambling severity scores and both forms of booster pack spending. No effect was again observed here. Specifically on page 20 of the revised manuscript we note: “During peer review, one anonymous peer reviewer noted the small number of moderate-risk gamblers within the study (n=35). The reviewer suggested that moderate-risk gamblers be merged with high-risk gamblers to form a single group of n=53, and the analysis re-conducted. Accordingly, data were recoded as suggested and exploratory Kruskal-Wallis tests were run. Under this exploratory coding scheme, no significant difference was observed between groups (non problem, low-risk, moderate or high risk) for either spending on physical booster packs in real-world stores (χ2(2) = 5.45, p=0.065, η2 = 0.004) or spending on physical booster packs in digital stores (χ2(2) = 3.64, p=0.161, η2 = 0.002).”

10. The authors discuss clinically meaningful effect. I believe the discussion section would be strengthened if the authors speak to the effect size in the relationship between loot boxes and problem gambling severity, which I believe are generally above .04. I believe this would help strengthen the argument that CCGs do not pose similar risks of problem gambling as loot boxes.

Our response: This is an excellent point and clearly something that we have missed from our discussion. We have extended it to take this point into account. On page 21 we now elaborate that “In order to determine the practical utility of a test’s result, researchers commonly look at the effect size associated with that test. The idea that small effect sizes may indicate that a statistical result may not have significant real-world importance is discussed in (Ferguson, 2009), a widely-cited primer on the interpretation of effect sizes. (Ferguson, 2009) proposes that a ‘suggested minimum’ for effects to even be considered practically important in a clinical context be placed at r = 0.2” and on page 22 we note “This is particularly important given that a recent meta-analysis reveals that the association between loot box purchasing and problem gambling symptomology is r > .20 (Garea et al., 2020). That loot box purchasing and problem gambling symptomology are associated with r’s exceeding these guidelines for clinical relevance while purchasing of physical card packs for collectable card games were not, reveals important differences between these activities in terms of their risks to people with gambling problems.”

You said: “11. The limitation section is lacking (non-existent). Overall, I really enjoyed the paper but it is limited in some ways:

- Differing time frames between PGSI and CCG spending behavior

- Highly selected and convenience sample from Reddit, and as such results may not

generalize to CCG players

- Did not (?) assess loot boxes, which would have provided a more complete picture.

- Cross-sectional. Perhaps a clinically meaningful effect would be observed longitudinally?

- Small sample size of people in the moderate risk and problem gambling categories.”

Our response: We agree with the limitations defined by the reviewer, and regret not making them clearer in the manuscript. In response to this holistic point, we have created a separate ‘Limitations’ subsection within our discussion (pp. 22-25). We have used this section to discuss each of the points raised by the reviewer, and have also used this section to raise some additional points with reference to the limitations of this study.

Reviewer 2 Comments

You said: “The study examines the relationship between collectable card game (CCG) booster pack purchasing, in the real world and online, and problem gambling symptom severity. A rationale for examining this relationship is that there is a positive relationship for loot box purchasing and problem gambling which is suggested to guide public policy decisions for such activities. It was hypothesized that because CCGs are similar to loot boxes, then there would be a significant relationship between categories of problem gambling severity and CCG pack purchasing. The study sought to answer this question using online surveys that were completed by a cross-sectional sample of 731 participants recruited from “Reddit.” It was ultimately concluded that there is no relationship between problem gambling severity and spending on booster packs. Specifically, there was no relationship between purchasing of physical booster packs and gambling severity and only a weak relationship between virtual booster pack purchasing and gambling severity (which failed to meet the threshold selected by the researchers). The study was interesting and seeks to answer an important question that could impact policy decisions surrounding the regulation of activities that resemble gambling. However, there are several limitations. Please take the following comments into consideration:

1. Introduction: An issue with the manuscript in the current form is whether the information presented in the introduction leads, conceptually, to the hypotheses that are initially tested. Based on my reading of the introduction, the reasoning, and how the two activities are contrasted, it seemed clear that CCG booster packs are not the same as loot boxes and, therefore, there should be no relationship between problem gambling and CCG booster purchasing (such an assumption would require equivalence testing, which the authors do, but I wonder why this approach wasn’t selected from the outset). Though some similarities between loot boxes and CCGs are discussed, many more differences are presented and the arguments seems stronger that the two activities are not the same. Thus, it is as if there is a disconnect between the conceptual framework offered in the introduction and the initial hypotheses the study seeks to test. To give an example, in the first paragraph, the statement challenges the case that CCGs are a form of gambling (i.e., "just because something appears to resemble gambling…") and statements such as these reoccurs throughout the introduction, but in the last paragraph the similarities between CCG boosters and loot boxes are evoked to justify that there should be a connection between this activity and problem gambling. Please consider changing the tone or rewording sections of the introduction to provide more congruence.”

Our response: This point is really well-taken. You are right: We are trying to walk a thin line here, and possibly not treading it perfectly. I think that the place where we fell down was highlighting the existence of two competing narratives: There is an argument to be made that loot boxes are the same as collectible card games; there is an argument to be made that they are fundamentally different to collectible card games. This has led to governmental stakeholders being unsure whether these things are different or the same, which may have important consequences. We have adjusted our introduction to describe this more clearly, and hope that our adjusted introduction is better at motivating the research presented here. For instance we now note on page 7 of the revised manuscript: “The similarities outlined above have led to comparisons between collectible card games and loot boxes by members of the video game industry (Taylor, 2018). These similarities have led to significant policy interest in whether the effects of loot boxes and booster packs are fundamentally equivalent: The terms of reference of a recent evidence call by the UK government, for example, requested information regarding “Whether any harms identified [to be associated with loot boxes] also apply to offline equivalents of chance mechanisms, such as buying packs of trading cards.”(DCMS, 2020).”

You said: “2. Pg. 3, Ln. 4: This sentence does not read well. Perhaps this is a syntax error or typographical error?”

Our response: Thanks for catching this. We have rephrased the statement and hopefully it now reads more clearly.

You said: “3. Methods: For the PGSI, please report previous reliabilities from other studies that used the PGSI as well as how the instrument performed in the current study (this is especially important in this context because the sample was international and questionnaires were collected fully online).”

Our response: We have augmented the manuscript with details about PGSI reliability in previous studies, and in this study. Specifically on page 15-16 of the revised manuscript we state: The PGSI has demonstrated good internal validity (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.8) in a variety of previous studies (e.g. (Barbaranelli et al., 2013; Bertossa et al., 2014; Drummond, Sauer, Ferguson, et al., 2020; Orford et al., 2010)). In this sample, Cronbach’s alpha was measured at 0.82.”

You said: “4. Pg. 10, Ln.17: The fact that one of the groups contains a small number of female participants would need to be evaluated given that there are differences between women and men in how they process and regulate the effects of problem gambling. Also, are there gender differences that should be addressed in terms of Reddit users or gambling in general? If so, please mention in the methods section or, if it cannot be addressed, as a study limitation in the discussion section.”

Our response: Reddit samples appear to be overwhelmingly young and male. This is a limitation of the approach (and a limitation of the work we seek to replicate). We have augmented our newly-created limitations subsection to discuss the generalisability of our sample in detail. On page 23-24 we now note that “A second key limitation of the research conducted here is the obtained sample: Our participants consisted of 726 volunteers, drawn from the online bulletin board service reddit. Samples drawn from reddit appear to skew young, and skew male (e.g. (Petrovskaya & Zendle, 2020; Zendle & Cairns, 2018a)). Indeed, this is a limitation of the work that we seek to replicate, which likewise draws its sample from reddit. A lack of academic research into players of physical collectible card games makes it unclear how representative these samples are of players of collectible card games in general. However, one may credibly propose that selecting samples of volunteers from bulletin boards designed for enthusiastic players of collectible card games may lead researchers to end up with an unrepresentatively engaged sample of players of these games. On the one hand, this is an advantage of the approach taken when it comes to ecological validity: If one is interested in spending on booster packs being linked to problem gambling, it is worthwhile looking at a sample of individuals who are likely to spend money on booster packs. However, on the other hand, one might reasonably argue that such an approach suppresses variability within our sample, and may mask the presence of real correlations within the population. Further research with more representative samples of players of collectible card games is necessary to determine which of these is the case.”

You said: “5. Methods: Please consider including more information about recruitment of participants via Reddit.”

Our response: We have augmented our manuscript to include significantly more detail about recruitment via Reddit. On page 12-23 we now state: “The specific subreddits that the survey was posted to are as follows: r/wixoss; r/cardfightvanguard; r/FoWtcg; r/CardWarsTCG; r/PokemonTCG; r/TCG; r/pkmntcg; r/yugioh. Posts mentioned that the survey is about “physical card games, and more specifically, spending preferences in physical card games” but does not mention loot boxes. It invites individuals to take part in “a *very short* questionnaire (5 minutes max) that asks a couple of questions about spending on card games”. No reimbursement was given to participants in return for their participation in this study.”

You said: “6. Methods: The data analytic plan should be described in greater detail including the decisions for the analyses that are conducted. Why was the Kruskal Wallis H Test used rather than ANOVA? The problem of outliers is mentioned and a transformation was used to correct for this, but then why the use of a nonparametric test which is robust to outliers? Why not use the untransformed values then? Also, were all assumptions for the Kruskal Wallis H Test met (e.g., equal variability)? What was the rationale for using the PGSI measure categorically rather than continuously in this study? In short, there appear to be many statistical decisions and interpretations made by the authors that aren’t explained or justified to the reader.”

Our response: These are all sound points. They all stem from one key source: This paper attempts to replicate a prior ‘loot box’ paper, but in the domain of booster packs; our analyses were conducted to mirror the analyses within that paper. With that said, we would suggest that our analyses still make statistical and conceptual sense.

However, we appreciate that we have perhaps failed to communicate this within our manuscript – your objections are well-taken! We have tried to augment the manuscript to make it a bit more clear why we did what we did, with specific detail added to a ‘Justification of statistical analyses’ subsection within our method. This subsection contains the following specific content:

“This paper seeks to replicate the analyses present within Zendle and Cairns (2018). During review, some questions were raised regarding the justification of these analyses. In order to highlight the decision making regarding these analyses, we incorporate additional information below.

An anonymous reviewer asked why parametric analyses (e.g. ANOVA) were not used here, and Kruskal Wallis tests used instead. The intention here was two-fold: First, to replicate prior analyses; second, to use a test which does not require the strict distributional assumptions of an ANOVA. It is not clear that normality assumptions associated with ANOVA would be met in this case.

This reviewer also asked why data were rank-transformed prior to analysis. Again, this was done in an attempt to replicate prior work as closely as possible. One important thing to note here is that the crucial preregistered analyses here are all rank-sum tests: as such, they involve rank transformation during calculation, and thus this transformation does not change the calculated test statistic. We appreciate that one may (rightly) view it as unnecessary for some analyses, but it is also true that this transformation cannot in any way affect the statistics that these tests calculate, and hence the validity of their results.

The same reviewer also asked whether additional tests were necessary in this case in order to establish median shift via the Kruskal Wallis. This is an interesting point. We would note that the lack of a significant result in the case of all analyses means that equality of variability is not necessary for the interpretation of the Kruskal Wallis statistic (i.e., we are never in the position of trying to work out whether dominance or median shift has occurred, because the test highlights no significant differences).

Finally, it was asked why the PGSI was grouped in the way that was undertaken, and whether this might affect results. The PGSI measure was grouped categorically in line with common practice using this instrument: There is evidence that the diagnostic categories associated with the PGSI have important real-world meaning. We acknowledge that grouping responses by category tends to reduce the information yielded by the data, and that one might suggest that this information encodes a significant relationship between problem gambling and booster pack spending. However, crucially we would also note that we conduct an equivalence test of the correlation between ‘raw’ (i.e. uncategorised) problem gambling scores and both forms of booster pack spending: These tests both suggest a relationship between these variables that is equivalent to zero.”In addition to this, we would note that when we have grouped participants in the alternative fashion proposed by Reviewer 1, the analysis remained nonsignificant. We believe that these points highlight the robustness of our results to alternative statistical approaches.

You said: “7. Discussion: Some of the statements made are far too strong given the exploratory nature and limitations of this study and should not be generalized to all unregulated gambling-like activities (e.g., “Arguments that loot boxes are equivalent to other unregulated activities that resemble gambling appear to be empirically unjustified.”)”

Our response: We thank the reviewer for this comment and have removed this statement.

You said: “8. Discussion: There are a number of limitations to this study, but none are mentioned. Please include a through discussion of the limitations. Also, please consider including recommendations for future research to build upon and expand the results of the current study (e.g., an experimental or longitudinal design?).”

Our response: In response to both this feedback, and the suggestions of Reviewer 1, we have augmented the manuscript with an additional ‘Limitations’ subsection (pages 22-25 of the revised manuscript).

You said: “9. Overall: The manuscript should undergo additional proofreading. There are misplaced commas, extra spaces, and some peculiar formatting choices (e.g., double parentheses for citations, authors' names referred to directly in sentence but still placed in parentheses, references without page numbers or only the starting page). As I believe this journal uses Vancouver style, which I am less familiar with than APA, perhaps I am simply unfamiliar with such conventions, but I would recommend reviewing the potential issues I mention here to be sure. Finally, the formatting for some of the graphs and figures made it difficult to follow the data.”

Our response: We have undertaken a thorough proofread of the manuscript and corrected a number of small errors. We hope that we have addressed the concerns that you describe here.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers AD DZ Final.docx

Decision Letter 1

Stefano Triberti

15 Feb 2021

Links between problem gambling and spending on booster packs in collectible card games: A conceptual replication of research on loot boxes

PONE-D-20-32484R1

Dear Dr. Zendle,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Stefano Triberti, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I would like to thank the authors for the careful consideration given to my suggestions in the previous reviews. I believe the authors have addressed all my comments and feel they are submitting a stronger paper for consideration. I wish the authors the best of luck with their work and look forward to seeing the paper in print!

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Acceptance letter

Stefano Triberti

11 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-32484R1

Links between problem gambling and spending on booster packs in collectible card games: A conceptual replication of research on loot boxes

Dear Dr. Zendle:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Stefano Triberti

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers AD DZ Final.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The data and R script underlying this study are available on OSF (https://osf.io/abx2t/).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES