Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Mar 31;10:e65012. doi: 10.7554/eLife.65012

Neural network of social interaction observation in marmosets

Justine C Cléry 1,, Yuki Hori 1, David J Schaeffer 1,2, Ravi S Menon 1,3, Stefan Everling 1,3,
Editors: Thorsten Kahnt4, Tamar R Makin5
PMCID: PMC8024015  PMID: 33787492

Abstract

A crucial component of social cognition is to observe and understand the social interactions of other individuals. A promising nonhuman primate model for investigating the neural basis of social interaction observation is the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), a small New World primate that shares a rich social repertoire with humans. Here, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging acquired at 9.4 T to map the brain areas activated by social interaction observation in awake marmosets. We discovered a network of subcortical and cortical areas, predominately in the anterior lateral frontal and medial frontal cortex, that was specifically activated by social interaction observation. This network resembled that recently identified in Old World macaque monkeys. Our findings suggest that this network is largely conserved between New and Old World primates and support the use of marmosets for studying the neural basis of social cognition.

Research organism: Other

Introduction

Observing and interpreting social interactions of other individuals is crucial for our everyday life. We share this remarkable ability with other primates that can also readily recognize social interactions such as play, fight, and grooming of other group members (Ambady et al., 2000; Ebenau et al., 2019; Spelke and Kinzler, 2007). Monkeys use this information to infer social hierarchies, adjust their behavior, and find allies (Bergman et al., 2003; Cheney et al., 1986). In Old World macaque monkeys, Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017 recently identified areas in parietal and frontal cortex that were exclusively activated by the observation of social interactions of other macaques using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Interestingly, this network in macaques resembles the human default mode network (Mars et al., 2012) and the theory of mind network (Gallagher and Frith, 2003). As these networks share anatomical and functional characteristics, it suggests that the social interaction network may play a key role in these two human networks and be a conserved feature across primates (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017).

Although macaque monkeys are widely used to study the neural basis of higher cognitive function, they may not be an ideal nonhuman primate model for studying the neural basis of social cognition as laboratory studies of social behavior are challenging due to their size and aggression toward conspecifics. Different species of macaques display varying type of prosociality but no cooperative breeding (Balasubramaniam et al., 2018; Ballesta and Duhamel, 2015; Beisner et al., 2016; de Waal, 1986; Joly et al., 2017; McCowan et al., 2011; Thierry et al., 2000) and are thus still an important model for studying some features and behaviors shared with humans (evolutionarily closer to humans than marmosets, larger size of societies, hierarchical societies, social behaviors including alliances, retaliation, redirected aggression). However, pairing and matching more than two macaque monkeys is often difficult under laboratory conditions. For these reasons, the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus), a small New World nonhuman primate species that lives in family groups and shares certain similarities with humans such as prosocial behavior, social cognition (Burkart and Finkenwirth, 2015; Miller et al., 2016), and cooperative breeding (Burkart et al., 2009; Caselli et al., 2018), has garnered interest as a powerful primate model for human social behavior (Huang et al., 2020; Miller et al., 2016; Saito, 2015; Yokoyama et al., 2013; Yokoyama and Onoe, 2015).

Here, we aimed to identify the neural circuits involved in social interaction observation in marmosets using ultra-high-field (9.4 T) fMRI in fully awake animals. Similar to Sliwa and Freiwald’s study (2017), we acquired fMRI data from marmosets while they viewed videos of two marmosets interacting with each other (social condition) or of two marmosets engaged in independent goal-directed behavior (nonsocial condition).

Results

In this study, we employed a block-design task that consisted of videos showing social interactions of two marmosets (social condition), two side-by-side videos showing a marmoset in each video interacting with its own environment (nonsocial condition), and phase-scrambled versions of these videos. In the social conditions, the actions performed in the videos included eating, playing, fighting, and grooming between two marmosets. In the nonsocial conditions, the actions performed in the videos included eating, exploring objects, looking around, and scratching. A more detailed description of these actions is provided in Tables 1 and 2. Three common marmosets were scanned, and eight runs per marmosets were used for group analysis (24 runs in total).

Table 1. Detailed description of the actions in each of the 'social interaction’ condition for each order displayed, each clip within a block (two clips of 6 s) and each repetition (two repetitions) within the run.

Social interaction
Order 1 Playing (roll over the other and touching the head) Grooming (one is lying down, the other one is sitting and grooming the other one's body)
Grooming (one is lying down, the other is sitting and grooming the other's head) Eating a biscuit (sharing food between a mother and her baby)
Order 2 Playing in the forest (roll over the other and touching all body parts) Playing in the forest (roll over the other and touching all body parts including playful biting)
Grooming on a branch tree (one is lying down, the other one is sitting and grooming the other one's head) Fighting and catching each other
Order 3 Grooming on a branch tree (one is lying down, the other one is sitting and grooming the other one's head) Fighting and catching each other
Playing (roll over the other and touching the head) Grooming (one is lying down, the other one is sitting and grooming the other one's body)

Table 2. Detailed description of the actions in each of the 'nonsocial interaction’ condition for each order displayed, each clip within a block (two clips of 6 s) and each repetition (two repetitions) within the run.

Nonsocial interaction
Order 1 Exploring a camera tripod Eating (mostly chewing) Eating banana peel Scratching and exploring its environment
Eating lettuce while lying on a nest bed Eating on a tree branch Eating on a tree branch Eating while sitting on a table and looking at the camera
Order 2 Eating banana and other fruits, frontal face view Eating while sitting on a table and looking at the camera Exploring and looking at its hand Eating fruits profile view
Frontal face view before eating sweet potatoes Eating bananas and visually exploring Looking backward, chasing insects Looking backward before eating sweet potatoes
Order 3 Eating lettuce while lying on in a nest bed Eating on a tree branch Eating on a tree branch Eating while sitting on a table and looking at the camera
Frontal face view before eating sweet potatoes Eating bananas and visually exploring Looking backward, chasing insects Looking backward before eating sweet potatoes

Figure 1 shows group maps for four conditions (social, social scrambled, nonsocial, and nonsocial scrambled conditions) on fiducial brain surface (Figure 1, right hemisphere; see Figure 1—figure supplement 1 for the left hemisphere and Figure 1—figure supplement 2 for individual analyses, cluster-defining threshold. p<0.05, α = 0.05 for all contrasts). Both social (Figure 1a) and nonsocial (Figure 1c) videos activated a large network including visual (V1, V2, V3, V4, V4T, V6, V6A, 19M, MT), temporal (FST, TE1, TE2, TE3, TEO, IPa), parietal (MIP, VIP, LIP, AIP, PGM), and frontal areas (45, 47L, 46v, 10, 8av, 8ad, 8C, 6DR). The scrambled versions (Figure 1b, d) showed strong activation in visual (V1, V2, V3, V4, V4T, MT) and temporal areas (FST, TEO, IPa). Activations were also found in frontal cortex (47L, 45, 8av). The scrambled version of the nonsocial condition (Figure 1d) also showed activation in S2E, 1/2, 3a, 3b. Overall, the social and nonsocial conditions elicited a similar network, substantially stronger than those elicited by the scrambled versions.

Figure 1. Group functional topology maps for social (a), social scrambled (b), nonsocial (c), and nonsocial scrambled conditions (d).

Maps are displayed on the right fiducial brain surface (lateral and medial views). The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 5.4 (p<0.001, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –5.4 (p<0.001, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). Group maps for the left hemisphere and individual maps for both hemispheres are shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1 and Figure 1—figure supplement 2, respectively.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Group functional topology maps for social (a), social scrambled (b), nonsocial (c), and nonsocial scrambled conditions (d).

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Maps are displayed on the left fiducial brain surface (lateral and medial views). The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 5.4 (p<0.001, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –5.4 (p<0.001, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05).

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Individual functional topology maps for social (a), social scrambled (b), nonsocial (c), and nonsocial scrambled conditions (d) in monkeys 1–3.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Maps are displayed on both fiducial brain surface (lateral and medial views). The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05) for monkeys 1 and 2; and t-scores ≥ 5.4 (p<0.001, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05) for monkey 3. The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05) for monkeys 1 and 2; and t-scores ≥ −5.4 (p<0.001, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05) for monkey 3.

To identify areas that were more active for the observation of social interactions, we compared the social with the nonsocial condition (Figure 2; see Figure 2—figure supplement 1 for individual analyses, cluster-defining threshold p<0.05, α = 0.05 for all contrasts). This analysis revealed stronger activations in frontal, somatosensory, and cingulate cortex for the social condition. Significant differences were present in frontal areas 10, 9, 14c, 14r, 45, 46d, 47L, 8ad, 8b, 8av, 8C, 6av, 6bv, and 6DC. More posteriorly, somatosensory areas S2I, S2PV, S2PR, 1/2, 3a, and 3b, and motor areas 4ab and ProM were more active for the social than nonsocial condition. We also observed activations in parietal areas (MIP, VIP), rostral temporal areas (STR, TPO, AuCPB, AuRPB), and in cingulate areas (25, 23b, and 31). The largest differences were present in motor/premotor cortex at the border of areas 4ab and 6DC, at the border of somatosensory area 1/2 and premotor areas 6av and 6bc, and in area 25 and orbitofrontal area 14. Differences between social and single interaction conditions are also found at the subcortical level in the SC, Tha, Amy, and CeB. Stronger activations for the nonsocial condition were mainly located in visual (V1, V2, V3, V4, V6), cingulate area 24b, and temporal areas (TF, TE2, TEO).

Figure 2. Group functional topology comparisons between social and nonsocial conditions.

Groups maps are displayed on the right and left fiducial marmoset brain surface (lateral, medial, ventral, and dorsal views) and coronal slices. The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. Y-coordinates are calculated with respect to the anterior commissure (in millimeters). The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). Nomenclature for coronal slices is as follows: Amy/LT: amygdala/lateral temporal; CeB: cerebellum; MIP: medial intraparietal area; Nacb: nucleus accumbens; SC: superior colliculus; Tha: thalamus; VIP: ventral intraparietal area. Individual maps for both hemispheres are shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Individual functional topology comparisons between social and nonsocial conditions.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Individual maps are displayed on the right and left fiducial marmoset brain surface (lateral, medial, ventral, and dorsal views) for monkeys 1–3. The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. Y-coordinates are calculated with respect to the anterior commissure (in millimeters). The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05).

Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR) map for the group data.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

The group map is represented on the axial slices of the marmoset brain surface. The left hemisphere shows slightly lower tSNR than the right hemisphere. A, P, L, and R indicate anterior, posterior, left, and right orientation, respectively.

Overall numerous activations looked very similar in both hemispheres but are less strong in the left hemisphere (Figure 2; see Figure 2—figure supplement 2 for temporal signal-to-noise ratio maps).

To better control for low-level visual features, we also reported the interaction effect [social vs. social scrambled] minus [nonsocial vs. nonsocial scrambled] (Figure 3; see Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for individual analyses, cluster-defining threshold p<0.05, α = 0.05 for all contrasts). Overall, this analysis revealed most of the activations found by the previous contrast (Figure 2) but with less spread activations (Figure 3). Significant interaction effect was present in frontal areas 10, 9, 14c, 45, 46d, 47L, 8ad, 8b, 8C, 6bv, and 6DC, and more posteriorly, in somatosensory areas S2I, S2PR, 1/2, 3a, and 3b, motor area 4ab, and rostral temporal areas (STR, TPO, AuCPB). We still observed activations in parietal areas (VIP), in cingulate area 25, and at the subcortical level in the SC, Tha, Amy, and CeB. We did not observe any activation in area 8aV, indicating that the differences are not primarily related to saccades.

Figure 3. Group functional topology of the interaction effect [social vs. social scrambled] minus [nonsocial vs. nonsocial scrambled].

Groups maps are displayed on the right and left fiducial marmoset brain surface (lateral, medial, ventral, and dorsal views) and coronal slices. The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. Y-coordinates are calculated with respect to the anterior commissure (in millimeters). The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). Nomenclature for coronal slices is as follows: Amy/LT: amygdala/lateral temporal; CeB: cerebellum; Nacb: nucleus accumbens; SC: superior colliculus; Tha: thalamus; VIP: ventral intraparietal area. Individual maps for both hemispheres are shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Individual functional topology of the interaction effect [social vs. social scrambled] minus [nonsocial vs. nonsocial scrambled].

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

Individual maps are displayed on the right and left fiducial marmoset brain surface (lateral, medial, ventral, and dorsal views) for monkeys 1–3. The white line delineates the regions based on the atlas from Paxinos et al., 2011. Y-coordinates are calculated with respect to the anterior commissure (in millimeters). The regions associated with yellow/orange scale correspond to t-scores ≥ 2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05). The regions associated with blue scale correspond to t-scores ≤ –2.3 (p<0.05, AFNI’s 3dttest++, cluster-size correction from Monte Carlo simulation, α = 0.05).

By extracting the time series from 12 regions of interest defined by the Paxinos et al., 2011 atlas and based on the group activation maps, we assessed the differences of response magnitude between the four conditions (Figure 4). Some areas showed stronger activation for the social condition than for the nonsocial condition (paired t-tests, p<0.5; right area 25 p=0.0435; right area 10 p=0.0075; left area 10L p=0.0012; right area 14c p=0.0016; right area VIP p=0.0052; right area 6va p=0.027; right area 6vb p=0.021; right area 46d p=0.0186; left area 46d p=0.005; and right area 45 p=0.0291). These areas also showed stronger activations for the social condition than for its scrambled version (paired t-tests, p<0.05; right area 25 p=0.0355; right area 10 p=0.049; left area 10L p=0.011; right area 14c p=0.002; right area VIP p=0.0257; right area 6va p=0.038; right area 46d p=0.0074; and right area 45 p=2.8 10−6) except for right area 6vb and left 46d.

Figure 4. The magnitude of the difference between conditions within 12 regions of interests.

Figure 4.

These differences were calculated after extraction of the time series from these regions defined in the left and right hemisphere using the Paxinos et al., 2011 atlas and based on the group activation maps. The differences from the baseline (below each bar graph) and conditions (horizontal bars) were assessed using paired t-test, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. The error bars correspond to the standard error.

Other areas did not show significant difference between social and nonsocial conditions but stronger activations for the social conditions than for its scrambled version (paired t-tests, p<0.05; left area 25 p=0.027; left area 6va p=0.012; right area 8ad p=0.007; left area 8ad p=0.014; right area 8av p=6.9 10−6; left area 8av p=0.004; and left area 45 p=0.004).

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to identify the neural circuitry underlying the observation of social interactions in New World marmosets. To that end, we employed a block-design task that consisted of videos showing social interactions of two marmosets (social condition), two side-by-side videos showing a marmoset in each video interacting with its own environment (eating, foraging) (nonsocial condition), and phase-scrambled versions of these videos. Both the social and nonsocial conditions showed strong occipitotemporal activation when compared to their scrambled versions, consistent with the location of face and body patches. In addition, these comparisons revealed activations in premotor and prefrontal areas. Like in macaques (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017), the comparison of the social with the nonsocial condition revealed a network of mainly frontal cortical areas that is specifically activated by the observation of social interactions.

The occipitotemporal activations in the intact vs. scrambled movies resemble face-selective patches previously found in marmoset fMRI studies using photos of marmoset faces and body parts (Hung et al., 2015) or videos of faces (Schaeffer et al., 2020). They include the occipital (V2/V3), anterior dorsal (rostral TE), middle dorsal (caudal TE), posterior dorsal (FST), and posterior ventral (V4/TEO) face patches. This was to be expected as marmoset faces were visible for most of the time during these conditions. We also confirmed a recent report of a frontal face patch located in the lateral frontal cortex (45/47L) in both the social and nonsocial condition (Schaeffer et al., 2020). These areas are thus recruited for face processing independent of social interactions.

Recently, Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017 identified an exclusively social interaction brain network in macaques using fMRI. This network included predominately frontal and cingulate areas (14, 24c, 25, 32, 9, 10, 8B, 8D, 46D, 46V, 47L, 47O, 44, 6VR [F5], 6DC [F2], ProM). These overlap well with the frontal areas that we found here for a similar comparison in marmosets. In addition, we observed strong right hemisphere activation at the border of somatosensory area 1/2 and premotor areas 6av and 6bc and in the primary motor cortex 4ab in the marmoset.

We also found activation of area PG (homologous with area 7a in macaque) in both social, social scrambled, and nonsocial condition but not during nonsocial scrambled version. In macaques (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017), area PG/7a is strongly activated during social interaction observation but also showed, albeit weaker, activation during nonsocial interaction observation. In macaques, this area contains motor neurons coding goal-directed motor acts in macaques (Rozzi et al., 2008), which were present both in our social and nonsocial condition. The role of area PG is still poorly understood in marmosets, except that it seems to be a major functional hub of the marmoset brain (Ghahremani et al., 2017). Further studies need to be performed to fully unravel its function, in particular for social scrambled stimulation.

Most of the areas activated by the social vs. nonsocial comparison (and present in the interaction effect [social vs. social scrambled] minus [nonsocial vs. nonsocial scrambled]) are already known to form a strong structural network in marmosets and many have been implicated in various aspects of social cognition in humans and macaques. It should be noted that none of the oculomotor areas were activated by this contrast, precluding any confounds with eye movements. Eye movements, and particularly visual saccades, mostly activate the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and the frontal eye field in primates. Our group has mapped the saccade circuity in marmosets with multimodal approaches (electrophysiology and fMRI) (Ghahremani et al., 2017; Johnston et al., 2019; Schaeffer et al., 2019b; Selvanayagam et al., 2019). Together, this work demonstrates that saccades can be evoked by electrical stimulation of areas 45, 8aV, 8C, and 6DR. These regions are also activated in task-based fMRI with a visuomotor task and are functionally connected to the superior colliculus and other saccade-related regions as shown with RS-fMRI. In our contrast between the social and nonsocial conditions, only a small portion of these areas was activated and only in the right hemisphere. In addition, in the interaction effect contrast, no activation was present in area 8aV and only weak activations were observed in the dorsal part of 8aD and 6DR, suggesting that the activations we observed in our contrast are not directly related to oculomotor areas and so preclude most of the confound with eye movements. Furthermore, the PSC analysis of areas 8aV and 8aD showed no significant difference between social and nonsocial conditions. However, stimulation of area 45 evokes small saccades (Selvanayagam et al., 2019), and we observed here activation of the right area 45 in both social and nonsocial conditions as well as more activation for the social than nonsocial condition. The social condition may have elicited more small saccades related to the exploration of the facial expression and the interaction itself compared to the nonsocial interaction, which may involve more large explorations. This point raises some important limitations of our current setup. The animals were not rewarded for fixation or staying engaged during the task, and the quality of the video eye tracking did not allow us to confidently evaluate the animal’s engagement/fixation across conditions and runs.

Area 10 is densely connected with areas 14 and 9, to the temporal pole (STR) and parabelt auditory cortex (Buckner and Margulies, 2019; Burman et al., 2011). Area 10 also receives projections from parahippocampal, ventral somatosensory, and premotor areas. It is a key node of a social cognition circuitry in humans (Simmons et al., 2010) and linked to areas integrating information about social attention, intentionality, and other’s body actions in space in macaques (Jellema et al., 2000; Nummenmaa and Calder, 2009; Oram and Perrett, 1996).

Area 10 is also connected with area 25 in marmosets (Burman et al., 2011) and macaques (Joyce and Barbas, 2018). Connections with the amygdala, ventral striatum, hypothalamus, and periaqueductal gray, as well as with the anterior insula in macaques suggest a role of area 25 in emotion, visceromotor functions, and memory (Alexander et al., 2020; Alexander et al., 2019; Joyce and Barbas, 2018; Öngür et al., 2003). Our videos for the social condition showed marmosets playing or grooming each other, which may have triggered emotions and activated memories in the animals.

Tracer studies of area 14 in marmosets (Burman and Rosa, 2009), and some tracing data of the orbitofrontal cortex, encompassing area 14, have shown connections with amygdala, thalamus, and parahippocampal areas (Roberts et al., 2007). Area 14 seems to play a crucial role for appetitive value, reward processing, and decision making in humans (Elliott et al., 2010; FitzGerald et al., 2009) and in marmosets (Roberts and Wallis, 2000; Roberts and Clarke, 2019; Stawicka et al., 2020). Lesions in this area have been associated with social and emotional disturbance in humans (Bechara et al., 1996). Rostral area 14 also provides a major input to area 9 and send outputs to area 10 (Burman et al., 2011; Burman et al., 2006).

Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017 also identified a parieto-premotor network involved in social interaction analysis. This network included the posterior part of area AIP and premotor area F5 that have been shown to contain mirror neurons in macaques (Cléry et al., 2015; Cléry and Hamed, 2018; Rizzolatti et al., 1996; Rizzolatti and Fogassi, 2014; Rizzolatti and Matelli, 2003; Rizzolatti and Rozzi, 2018). This literature, in addition to the study of Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017, suggests a role for this mirror system in recognizing and understanding other agent’s intention and action. However, in this macaque study (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017), this AIP/F5 network also responded to physical interactions between objects (not tested here), suggesting a broader role for this network. Here, we found that these areas were activated by the observation of nonsocial (i.e., ‘agent-object interaction’) and social interactions (i.e., ‘agent-agent interaction’) in marmosets. The mirror neuron system is still poorly understood in marmosets; however, neurons with characteristic ‘mirror’ properties have been identified in the superior temporal sulcus and in area 6V in marmosets (Suzuki et al., 2015).

Interestingly, the contrast between the social and nonsocial condition also revealed strong activations of parietal areas MIP and VIP in addition to premotor area 6V (corresponding to areas F4 and F5 in macaques, see Matelli et al., 1985). A parieto-frontal network related to VIP and F4 has been suggested to contribute to the construction of both the representation of one own’s body and of the body of others in macaques (Cléry et al., 2015). Both areas VIP and F4 are strongly linked to peripersonal space representation, defense, avoidance behavior, and social cognition (Brozzoli et al., 2013; Cléry et al., 2018; Cléry et al., 2017; Ishida et al., 2010; for review, see Cléry et al., 2015; Cléry and Hamed, 2018). Our current study suggests that areas VIP and 6V also contribute to social interaction analysis in marmosets.

Even though the monkeys were simply watching other marmosets interacting with other marmosets or objects, we also found strong activations of somatosensory (1/2, 3a, 3b, SII) and motor areas (4ab, PGM) that were absent in macaque monkeys (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017). Activations were localized in areas that are also activated by tactile stimulation of the face and arm (Cléry et al., 2020), body parts where tactile stimulation would often occur during social interactions (grooming, playing). Particularly SII (Cléry et al., 2020; Huffman and Krubitzer, 2001; Krubitzer and Kaas, 1990; H-x et al., 2002) was activated in both hemispheres. A role of SII in social interaction processing is consistent with its involvement in the observation of others’ actions, as demonstrated both by human (Keysers et al., 2004; Blakemore et al., 2005; Lee Masson et al., 2018; for reviews, see Keysers et al., 2010; Bretas et al., 2020) and macaque studies (Hihara et al., 2015; Raos et al., 2014).

Our data showed stronger activations in the right hemisphere compared to the left hemisphere, even if the overall pattern looked similar. The temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR) map showed a slightly lower tSNR in the left hemisphere (Figure 2—figure supplement 2), which may have led to the difference of activation we observed. However, this difference may also be due to a real lateralization effect during social behaviors. Indeed, it has been shown that the right hemisphere has a relative advantage over the left hemisphere, particularly in identifying social stimuli, understanding the intentions of others, social relationships, and social interactions in humans (see review, Hecht, 2014). Consequently, our data may reflect a similar lateralization for social processing in marmosets.

An interesting finding with our current study is the deactivation observed in both the social and nonsocial condition that were not present during the scrambled versions. This deactivation may have been triggered by an expectation of hearing vocalizations that were not met, but the actions during nonsocial interaction were mainly associated with eating so there is not necessarily vocalization associated with this kind of action. However, the fact that we observed this deactivation in both interaction conditions and not in the scrambled conditions may be the result of the marmosets' interest and engagement during the videos. Looking at conspecifics interacting with each other or with their environment is more entertaining than blurred videos. Further studies need to be performed to test a more complex paradigm such as videos showing social interactions with or without vocalizations.

In summary, we have identified the neural circuitry for social interaction observation in marmosets that resembles the one identified in Old World macaque monkeys (Sliwa and Freiwald, 2017). As Old and New World primates separated ~40 million years ago, this suggests that neural circuits involved in social interaction observation are conserved between macaques and marmosets or may be the result of convergent evolution (they may have underwent similarities in selection pressure but also show differences in their social behavior; Preuss, 2019). Social interaction observation recruited multiple brain networks, at both the cortical and subcortical levels, likely involved in emotion and affective processing, memory, peripersonal space representation, and the mirror neuron system. Overall, the identification of a preserved cortical and subcortical circuitry involved in social interaction observation in marmosets provides the map for studying the neural processes underlying primate social cognition.

Materials and methods

All experimental methods described were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care policy on the care and use of experimental animals and an ethics protocol #2017-114 approved by the Animal Care Committee of the University of Western Ontario.

Subjects and experimental setup

Three common marmosets (C. jacchus) completed this study (one female, two males). Their ages and weights at the time of the experiment were 30 months and 363 g (M1), 38 months and 410 g (M2), and 35 months and 310 g (M3), respectively. The animals were prepared for awake fMRI experiments by implanting an MRI-compatible head restraint/recording chamber (for details, see Johnston et al., 2018). Each animal was initially habituated to the MRI environment over the course of 3 weeks. This included the acclimatization to the animal holder (tube closed by neck and tail plates), the head-fixation system, and periodic sounds produced by the MRI sequences (for details, see Schaeffer et al., 2019a). The behavior of the animals was monitored throughout the training to assess the tolerance and well-being of the marmosets (Silva et al., 2011). Their progression was based on their performance on the behavioral scale defined by Silva et al., 2011.

For MRI acquisition, the animal was placed in the sphinx position in an animal holder consisting of an MRI-compatible restraint system (Schaeffer et al., 2019a). The animal was first restrained using neck and tail plates, then the head was restrained by the fixation of the head chamber to the five-channel receive coil. An MRI-compatible camera (model 12M-i, 60 Hz sampling rate, MRC Systems GmbH, Heidelberg, Germany) was positioned in front of the animal holder to allow monitoring of the animals during the acquisition sessions by a veterinary technician. However, this camera and the setup did not allow a proper recording of eye movements as the pupil signal was frequently lost. In the MR scanner, the animal faced a translucent plastic screen placed at the front of the bore (119 cm from the animal’s head) where visual stimuli were displayed via back-projection after being reflected off of a first-surface mirror. Images were projected with a SONY VPL-FE40 projector. The maximum visual angle from the center to the side of the screen was 6.5°. Stimuli were presented via Keynote (version 10.1, Apple Incorporated, CA) and launched in synchronization with MRI TTL pulse triggers via a custom-written program running on a Raspberry Pi (model 3 B+, Raspberry Pi Foundation, Cambridge, UK).

Task and stimuli

Four task conditions were presented: social condition videos where two marmosets directly interacted with each other (playing, grooming …) and nonsocial videos where two marmosets interacted with their own environment (eating, foraging) in two separate videos but displayed at the same time (Figure 1a, c, right panel). We chose real-world videos to maximize cognitive engagement (Finn and Bandettini, 2020; Meer et al., 2020). Videos were edited into 12 s clips using custom video-editing software (iMovie, Apple Incorporated). Two video clips were displayed within a block (6 s each), and 20 different videos clips were used in total. The videos within the same run were different between the two repetitions of the block condition but played following the same order. Tables 1 and 2 describe which actions were performed for each social (Table 1) and nonsocial (Table 2) blocks. Scrambled versions of social interaction and nonsocial interaction videos (Figure 1b, d, right panel) were created by random rotation of the phase information using a custom program (MATLAB, The MathWorks, Matick, MA). To preserve motion and luminance components, the same random rotation matrix was used for each frame in the scrambled conditions. Videos were presented in the center of the screen (7° height × 13° width) during acquisition sequences.

These four pseudo-randomized conditions were each displayed twice (social condition, nonsocial condition, and scrambled versions of each) following a block design (Figure 5). These eight task condition blocks (12 s each) were separated by baseline blocks (18 s each) where a 0.36° circular black cue was displayed in the center of a gray background. Three different stimulus sets were used and counterbalanced within the same animal and between animals. A sequence lasted 253 s (4.3 min). As motion and luminance varied across videos, mainly due to where the action took place, we counterbalanced these differences across the selected videos, conditions, and order of presentation to avoid any link with the condition block itself. No reward has been provided to the monkeys during the scan session.

Figure 5. Stimuli and task-based functional magnetic resonance imaging block design.

Figure 5.

Social condition videos, nonsocial condition videos, and their scrambled versions were displayed in a screen during task conditions blocks (12 s each) and separated by baseline blocks where a central dot was displayed in the center of the screen (18 s each). A gray background was displayed behind the dot and videos through the sequence.

Scanning

We performed data acquisition using a 9.4 T, 31 cm horizontal-bore magnet (Varian/Agilent, Yarnton, UK) and Bruker BioSpec Avance III HD console with the software package Paravision-6 (Bruker BioSpin Corp, Billerica, MA) at the Centre for Functional and Metabolic Mapping at the University of Western Ontario. We used an in-house, custom-built integrated receive coil with five channels (Schaeffer et al., 2019a) paired with a custom-built high-performance 15-cm-diameter gradient coil with 400 mT/m maximum gradient strength (xMR, London, Canada; Peterson et al., 2018). We used an in-house quadrature birdcage coil (12 cm inner diameter) for the transmit coil.

For each animal, a T2-weighted structural image was acquired, to allow an anatomical registration, with the following parameters: repetition time (TR) = 5500 ms; echo time (TE) = 53 ms; field of view (FOV) = 51.2 × 51.2 mm; voxel size = 0.133 × 0.133 × 0.5 mm; number of slices = 42 (axial); bandwidth = 50 kHz; GRAPPA acceleration factor = 2. For functional imaging, gradient-echo-based, single-shot echo-planar images covering the whole brain were acquired over multiple sessions (TR = 1500 ms; TE = 15 ms; flip angle = 40°; FOV = 64 × 64 mm; matrix size = 128 × 128; voxel size = 0.5 mm isotropic; number of slices = 42 [axial]; bandwidth = 500 kHz; GRAPPA acceleration factor = 2 [anterior-posterior]).

Analysis

Monkey 1 performed 8 runs in four sessions, monkey 2 performed 11 runs in five sessions, and Monkey 3 performed 9 runs in four sessions. We selected eight runs per monkeys to have the same number of runs across subjects for the group analysis (the selection was based on the best engagement of the animal as assessed by visual monitoring during the sessions). Time series were preprocessed using AFNI (Cox, 1996), FSL (Smith et al., 2004), and ANTS software (Advanced Normalization Tools, Avants et al., 2011). Raw functional images were converted to NifTI format using dcm2niix (Li et al., 2016) and reoriented from the sphinx position using FSL. The images were then despiked (AFNI’s 3dDespike) and volume registered to the middle volume of each time series (AFNI’s 3dvolreg). The motion parameters from volume registration were stored for later use with nuisance regression. Images were smoothed by a 1.5 mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel to reduce noise (AFNI’s 3dmerge). An average functional image was then calculated for each session and linearly registered (FSL’s FLIRT) to each animal’s T2-weighted image – the 4D time-series data was carried over using this transformation matrix. Anatomical images were manually skull-stripped, and this mask was applied to the functional images in anatomical space. The T2-weighted images were then nonlinearly registered to the NIH marmoset brain atlas (Liu et al., 2018) using Advanced Normalization Tools and the resultant transformation matrices stored for later transformation (see below). The olfactory bulb was manually removed from the T2-weighted images of each animal prior to registration as it was not included in the template image.

The task timing was convolved to the hemodynamic response (using AFNI’s ‘BLOCK’ convolution), and a regressor was generated for each condition to be used in a regression analysis (AFNI’s 3dDeconvolve) for each run. All four conditions were entered into the same model, along with polynomial (N = 5) detrending regressors, bandpass regressors, and the motion parameters derived from the volume registration described above (these nuisance regressors correspond to the baseline variance for the statistical analysis model). The resultant regression coefficient maps were then registered to template space using the transformation matrices described above. The T-value maps for each monkey were then compared at the individual level via paired-sample t-tests and fixed effect analyses across the eight runs for each marmoset (AFNI’s 3dttest++) and at the group level via a three-way analysis of variance and mixed effect analysis (AFNI’s 3danova3) using eight runs per marmosets (for a total of 24 runs). To protect against false positives, a cluster-size correction derived from Monte Carlo simulations was applied to the resultant t-test maps for α = 0.05 (using AFNI’s 3dClustsim).

The contrast between the social and nonsocial condition as well as the interaction effect [social vs. social scrambled] minus [nonsocial vs. nonsocial scrambled] was compared to previous fMRI and electrophysiological studies performed in marmosets (Ghahremani et al., 2017; Johnston et al., 2019; Schaeffer et al., 2019b; Selvanayagam et al., 2019) to check if the observed activations may be related to eye movements.

The resultant T-value maps are displayed on coronal sections or fiducial maps obtained with Caret (Van Essen et al., 2001; http://www.nitrc.org/projects/caret/) using the NIH marmoset brain template (Liu et al., 2018). Coordinates are presented with respect to the anterior commissure. The labeling refers to the histology-based atlas of Paxinos et al., 2011 for the cortical regions and to the atlas of Liu et al., 2018 for the subcortical regions (Figure 1, Figure 1—figure supplement 1, Figure 2, Figure 3).

Twelve regions of interest, areas 25, 10, 14c, VIP, PG, AIP, 6va, 6vb, 46d, 45, 8av and 8ad, were defined based on the Paxinos et al., 2011 atlas to evaluate the difference in response magnitude between conditions. First, masks of these regions were generated using AFNI’s 3dcalc in the right or left hemispheres of the ‘NIH_Marmoset_atlas_Paxinos_cortex’. Then, time series were extracted for each condition from the resultant regression coefficient maps using AFNI’s 3dmaskave. The mean and standard errors were computed and paired t-tests were calculated using MATLAB (version 2018a) after the data were tested for normality (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test).

Acknowledgements

Support was provided by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (FRN 148365) and the Canada First Research Excellence Fund to BrainsCAN. We thank Miranda Bellyou, Cheryl Vander Tuin, and Hannah Pettypiece for animal preparation and care and Dr. Alex Li for scanning assistance.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Justine C Cléry, Email: jclery@uwo.ca.

Stefan Everling, Email: severlin@uwo.ca.

Thorsten Kahnt, Northwestern University, United States.

Tamar R Makin, University College London, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Canadian Institutes of Health Research FRN 148365 to Stefan Everling.

  • Canada First Research Excellence Fund BrainsCAN to Stefan Everling.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Resources, Validation, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All experimental methods described were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care policy on the care and use of experimental animals and an ethics protocol #2017-114 approved by the Animal Care Committee of the University of Western Ontario. Animals were monitoring during the acquisition sessions by a veterinary technician.

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The datasets generated during this study are available at https://github.com/JClery/Social_interaction_paper and a copy archived at https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:rev:c9381727d3448d97d4f4263c1bb34c68ca296968/.

References

  1. Alexander L, Clarke H, Roberts A. A focus on the functions of area 25. Brain Sciences. 2019;9:129. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9060129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander L, Wood CM, Gaskin PLR, Sawiak SJ, Fryer TD, Hong YT, McIver L, Clarke HF, Roberts AC. Over-activation of primate subgenual cingulate cortex enhances the cardiovascular, behavioral and neural responses to threat. Nature Communications. 2020;11:5386. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-19167-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ambady N, Bernieri FJ, Richeson JA. Toward a histology of social behavior: judgmental accuracy from thin slices of the behavioral stream. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. 2000;32:201–271. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80006-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. NeuroImage. 2011;54:2033–2044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Balasubramaniam KN, Beisner BA, Berman CM, De Marco A, Duboscq J, Koirala S, Majolo B, MacIntosh AJ, McFarland R, Molesti S, Ogawa H, Petit O, Schino G, Sosa S, Sueur C, Thierry B, de Waal FBM, McCowan B. The influence of phylogeny, social style, and sociodemographic factors on macaque social network structure. American Journal of Primatology. 2018;80:e22727. doi: 10.1002/ajp.22727. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Ballesta S, Duhamel JR. Rudimentary empathy in macaques' social decision-making. PNAS. 2015;112:15516–15521. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504454112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bechara A, Tranel D, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 1996;6:215–225. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Beisner BA, Hannibal DL, Finn KR, Fushing H, McCowan B. Social power, conflict policing, and the role of subordination signals in rhesus macaque society. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2016;160:102–112. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.22945. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bergman TJ, Beehner JC, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM. Hierarchical classification by rank and kinship in baboons. Science. 2003;302:1234–1236. doi: 10.1126/science.1087513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Blakemore SJ, Bristow D, Bird G, Frith C, Ward J. Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia. Brain. 2005;128:1571–1583. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bretas RV, Taoka M, Suzuki H, Iriki A. Secondary somatosensory cortex of primates: beyond body maps, toward conscious self-in-the-world maps. Experimental Brain Research. 2020;238:259–272. doi: 10.1007/s00221-020-05727-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Brozzoli C, Gentile G, Bergouignan L, Ehrsson HH. A shared representation of the space near oneself and others in the human premotor cortex. Current Biology. 2013;23:1764–1768. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Buckner RL, Margulies DS. Macroscale cortical organization and a default-like apex transmodal network in the marmoset monkey. Nature Communications. 2019;10:1976. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09812-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Burkart JM, Hrdy SB, Van Schaik CP. Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 2009;18:175–186. doi: 10.1002/evan.20222. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Burkart JM, Finkenwirth C. Marmosets as model species in neuroscience and evolutionary anthropology. Neuroscience Research. 2015;93:8–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Burman KJ, Palmer SM, Gamberini M, Rosa MGP. Cytoarchitectonic subdivisions of the dorsolateral frontal cortex of the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus), and their projections to dorsal visual Areas. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2006;495:149–172. doi: 10.1002/cne.20837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Burman KJ, Reser DH, Yu HH, Rosa MG. Cortical input to the frontal pole of the marmoset monkey. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21:1712–1737. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Burman KJ, Rosa MG. Architectural subdivisions of medial and orbital frontal cortices in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus) The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2009;514:11–29. doi: 10.1002/cne.21976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Caselli CB, Ayres PHB, Castro SCN, Souto A, Schiel N, Miller CT. The role of extragroup encounters in a neotropical, cooperative breeding primate, the common marmoset: a field playback experiment. Animal Behaviour. 2018;136:137–146. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cheney D, Seyfarth R, Smuts B. Social relationships and social cognition in nonhuman primates. Science. 1986;234:1361–1366. doi: 10.1126/science.3538419. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cléry J, Guipponi O, Wardak C, Ben Hamed S. Neuronal bases of peripersonal and extrapersonal spaces, their plasticity and their dynamics: knowns and unknowns. Neuropsychologia. 2015;70:313–326. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cléry J, Guipponi O, Odouard S, Pinède S, Wardak C, Ben Hamed S. The prediction of impact of a looming stimulus onto the body is subserved by multisensory integration mechanisms. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:10656–10670. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0610-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Cléry J, Guipponi O, Odouard S, Wardak C, Ben Hamed S. Cortical networks for encoding near and far space in the non-human primate. NeuroImage. 2018;176:164–178. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cléry JC, Hori Y, Schaeffer DJ, Gati JS, Pruszynski JA, Everling S. Whole brain mapping of somatosensory responses in awake marmosets investigated with ultra-high-field fMRI. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2020;124:1900–1913. doi: 10.1152/jn.00480.2020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cléry J, Hamed SB. Frontier of self and impact prediction. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9:73. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Computers and Biomedical Research. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. de Waal FB. The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. The Quarterly Review of Biology. 1986;61:459–479. doi: 10.1086/415144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ebenau A, von Borell C, Penke L, Ostner J, Schülke O. Personality homophily affects male social bonding in wild assamese macaques, Macaca assamensis. Animal Behaviour. 2019;155:21–35. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.05.020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Elliott R, Agnew Z, Deakin JF. Hedonic and informational functions of the human orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:198–204. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Finn ES, Bandettini PA. Movie-watching outperforms rest for functional connectivity-based prediction of behavior. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.08.23.263723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  31. FitzGerald TH, Seymour B, Dolan RJ. The role of human orbitofrontal cortex in value comparison for incommensurable objects. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8388–8395. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0717-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2003;7:77–83. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ghahremani M, Hutchison RM, Menon RS, Everling S. Frontoparietal functional connectivity in the common marmoset. Cerebral Cortex. 2017;27:3890–3905. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. H-x Q, Lyon DC, Kaas JH. Cortical and thalamic connections of the parietal ventral somatosensory area in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus) Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2002;443:168–182. doi: 10.1002/cne.10113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hecht D. Cerebral lateralization of pro- and anti-social tendencies. Experimental Neurobiology. 2014;23:1–27. doi: 10.5607/en.2014.23.1.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Hihara S, Taoka M, Tanaka M, Iriki A. Visual responsiveness of neurons in the secondary somatosensory area and its surrounding parietal operculum regions in awake macaque monkeys. Cerebral Cortex. 2015;25:4535–4550. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Huang J, Cheng X, Zhang S, Chang L, Li X, Liang Z, Gong N. Having infants in the family group promotes altruistic behavior of marmoset monkeys. Current Biology. 2020;30:4047–4055. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Huffman KJ, Krubitzer L. Area 3a: topographic organization and cortical connections in marmoset monkeys. Cerebral Cortex. 2001;11:849–867. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Hung CC, Yen CC, Ciuchta JL, Papoti D, Bock NA, Leopold DA, Silva AC. Functional mapping of face-selective regions in the extrastriate visual cortex of the marmoset. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35:1160–1172. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2659-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ishida H, Nakajima K, Inase M, Murata A. Shared mapping of own and others' Bodies in visuotactile bimodal area of monkey parietal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:83–96. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jellema T, Baker CI, Wicker B, Perrett DI. Neural representation for the perception of the intentionality of actions. Brain and Cognition. 2000;44:280–302. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2000.1231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Johnston KD, Barker K, Schaeffer L, Schaeffer D, Everling S. Methods for chair restraint and training of the common marmoset on oculomotor tasks. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2018;119:1636–1646. doi: 10.1152/jn.00866.2017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Johnston K, Ma L, Schaeffer L, Everling S. Alpha oscillations modulate preparatory activity in marmoset area 8ad. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:1855–1866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2703-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Joly M, Micheletta J, De Marco A, Langermans JA, Sterck EHM, Waller BM. Comparing physical and social cognitive skills in macaque species with different degrees of social tolerance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;284:20162738. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.2738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Joyce MKP, Barbas H. Cortical connections position primate area 25 as a keystone for interoception, emotion, and memory. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:1677–1698. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2363-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Keysers C, Wicker B, Gazzola V, Anton JL, Fogassi L, Gallese V. A touching sight: sii/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch. Neuron. 2004;42:335–346. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00156-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Keysers C, Kaas JH, Gazzola V. Somatosensation in social perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2010;11:417–428. doi: 10.1038/nrn2833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Krubitzer LA, Kaas JH. The organization and connections of somatosensory cortex in marmosets. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1990;10:952–974. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-03-00952.1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lee Masson H, Van De Plas S, Daniels N, Op de Beeck H. The multidimensional representational space of observed socio-affective touch experiences. NeuroImage. 2018;175:297–314. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Li X, Morgan PS, Ashburner J, Smith J, Rorden C. The first step for neuroimaging data analysis: dicom to NIfTI conversion. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2016;264:47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2016.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Liu C, Ye FQ, Yen CC, Newman JD, Glen D, Leopold DA, Silva AC. A digital 3D atlas of the marmoset brain based on multi-modal MRI. NeuroImage. 2018;169:106–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mars RB, Neubert FX, Noonan MP, Sallet J, Toni I, Rushworth MF. On the relationship between the "default mode network" and the "social brain". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:189. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Matelli M, Luppino G, Rizzolatti G. Patterns of cytochrome oxidase activity in the frontal agranular cortex of the macaque monkey. Behavioural Brain Research. 1985;18:125–136. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90068-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. McCowan B, Beisner BA, Capitanio JP, Jackson ME, Cameron AN, Seil S, Atwill ER, Fushing H. Network stability is a balancing act of personality, power, and conflict dynamics in rhesus macaque societies. PLOS ONE. 2011;6:e22350. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Meer JNV, Breakspear M, Chang LJ, Sonkusare S, Cocchi L. Movie viewing elicits rich and reliable brain state dynamics. Nature Communications. 2020;11:5004. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18717-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Miller CT, Freiwald WA, Leopold DA, Mitchell JF, Silva AC, Wang X. Marmosets: a neuroscientific model of human social behavior. Neuron. 2016;90:219–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.03.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nummenmaa L, Calder AJ. Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13:135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Öngür D, Ferry AT, Price JL. Architectonic subdivision of the human orbital and medial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2003;460:425–449. doi: 10.1002/cne.10609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Oram MW, Perrett DI. Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1996;76:109–129. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Paxinos G, Watson C, Petrides M, Rosa M, Tokuno H. The Marmoset Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. Academic Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  61. Peterson J, Chaddock R, Dalrymple B, Van Sas F, Gilbert KM, Klassen LM, Gati JS, Handler WB, Chronik BA. Development of a gradient and shim insert system for marmoset imaging at 9.4. T26th Annual Meeting of International Society for Magnetic Resonance in Medicine.2018. [Google Scholar]
  62. Preuss TM. Critique of pure marmoset. Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 2019;93:92–107. doi: 10.1159/000500500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Raos V, Kilintari M, Savaki HE. Viewing a forelimb induces widespread cortical activations. NeuroImage. 2014;89:122–142. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rizzolatti G, Fadiga L, Gallese V, Fogassi L. Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research. 1996;3:131–141. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00038-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L. The mirror mechanism: recent findings and perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014;369:20130420. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Rizzolatti G, Matelli M. Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions. Experimental Brain Research. 2003;153:146–157. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1588-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Rizzolatti G, Rozzi S. Chapter 28 - The mirror mechanism in the parietal lobe. In: Vallar G, Coslett H. B, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2018. pp. 555–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Roberts AC, Tomic DL, Parkinson CH, Roeling TA, Cutter DJ, Robbins TW, Everitt BJ. Forebrain connectivity of the prefrontal cortex in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus): An anterograde and retrograde tract-tracing study. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2007;502:86–112. doi: 10.1002/cne.21300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Roberts AC, Clarke HF. Why we need nonhuman primates to study the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the regulation of threat- and reward-elicited responses. PNAS. 2019;116:26297–26304. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1902288116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Roberts AC, Wallis JD. Inhibitory control and affective processing in the prefrontal cortex: neuropsychological studies in the common marmoset. Cerebral Cortex. 2000;10:252–262. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Rozzi S, Ferrari PF, Bonini L, Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L. Functional organization of inferior parietal lobule convexity in the macaque monkey: electrophysiological characterization of motor, sensory and mirror responses and their correlation with cytoarchitectonic Areas. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:1569–1588. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06395.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Saito A. The marmoset as a model for the study of primate parental behavior. Neuroscience Research. 2015;93:99–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Schaeffer DJ, Gilbert KM, Hori Y, Gati JS, Menon RS, Everling S. Integrated radiofrequency array and animal holder design for minimizing head motion during awake marmoset functional magnetic resonance imaging. NeuroImage. 2019a;193:126–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Schaeffer DJ, Gilbert KM, Hori Y, Hayrynen LK, Johnston KD, Gati JS, Menon RS, Everling S. Task-based fMRI of a free-viewing visuo-saccadic network in the marmoset monkey. NeuroImage. 2019b;202:116147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Schaeffer DJ, Selvanayagam J, Johnston KD, Menon RS, Freiwald WA, Everling S. Face selective patches in marmoset frontal cortex. Nature Communications. 2020;11:4856. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18692-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Selvanayagam J, Johnston KD, Schaeffer DJ, Hayrynen LK, Everling S. Functional localization of the frontal eye fields in the common marmoset using microstimulation. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:9197–9206. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1786-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Silva AC, Liu JV, Hirano Y, Leoni RF, Merkle H, Mackel JB, Zhang XF, Nascimento GC, Stefanovic B. Longitudinal Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Animal Models. In: Modo M, Bulte J. W. M, editors. Magnetic Resonance Neuroimaging: Methods and Protocols, Methods in Molecular Biology. Humana Press; 2011. pp. 281–302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Simmons WK, Reddish M, Bellgowan PSF, Martin A. The selectivity and functional connectivity of the anterior temporal lobes. Cerebral Cortex. 2010;20:813–825. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Sliwa J, Freiwald WA. A dedicated network for social interaction processing in the primate brain. Science. 2017;356:745–749. doi: 10.1126/science.aam6383. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage. 2004;23:S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Spelke ES, Kinzler KD. Core knowledge. Developmental Science. 2007;10:89–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00569.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Stawicka ZM, Massoudi R, Horst NK, Koda K, Gaskin PLR, Alexander L, Santangelo AM, McIver L, Cockcroft GJ, Wood CM, Roberts AC. Ventromedial prefrontal area 14 provides opposing regulation of threat and reward-elicited responses in the common marmoset. PNAS. 2020;117:25116–25127. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2009657117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Suzuki W, Banno T, Miyakawa N, Abe H, Goda N, Ichinohe N. Mirror neurons in a new world monkey, common marmoset. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2015;9:459. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2015.00459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Thierry B, Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM. The influence of phylogeny on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: cercopithecidae, genus Macaca) Ethology. 2000;106:713–728. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00583.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Van Essen DC, Drury HA, Dickson J, Harwell J, Hanlon D, Anderson CH. An integrated software suite for surface-based analyses of cerebral cortex. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association. 2001;8:443–459. doi: 10.1136/jamia.2001.0080443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yokoyama C, Kawasaki A, Hayashi T, Onoe H. Linkage between the midline cortical serotonergic system and social behavior traits: positron emission tomography studies of common marmosets. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:2136–2145. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yokoyama C, Onoe H. Positron emission tomography imaging of the social brain of common marmosets. Neuroscience Research. 2015;93:82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Thorsten Kahnt1

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

This marmoset fMRI study will be important for researchers interested in social cognition and non-human primate models. The results describe a brain network activated when marmoset monkeys observe social and non-social interactions. This network is similar to regions previously shown to be involved in processing social information in humans and macaques.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Social interaction observation network in marmosets" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, one of whom is a member of our Board of Reviewing Editors, and the evaluation has been overseen by Tamar Makin as the Senior Editor. The reviewers have opted to remain anonymous.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

We would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). Specifically, we are asking editors to accept without delay manuscripts, like yours, that they judge can stand as eLife papers without additional data, even if they feel that they would make the manuscript stronger. Thus the revisions requested below only address clarity and presentation.

Summary:

This fMRI study maps brain areas activated when marmoset monkeys observe social interactions. During fMRI data acquisition, animals viewed short videos of social interactions, non-social interactions, scrambled social interactions, or scrambled non-social interactions. The authors report that a distributed set of brain regions is involved in processing social information. Importantly the identified regions are similar to regions previously shown to be involved in processing social information in humans and macaques.

All reviewers agreed that your manuscript addresses and important question and that your findings will fill an important gap in the literature. However, reviewers also identified several issues that need to be addressed in a revision.

Essential revisions:

1. It would be important to more clearly describe the statistical group-level model (fixed-effect, mixed effect, random effect?). Most importantly, it was not clear whether and how the group model accounted for the nested structure of technical (8 runs) and biological replicates (3 animals). Specifically, it seems the 24 runs were treated as independent observations (given they are not independent, they should not be treated as such). Related to this, it would be important to also show results for each of the individual 3 subjects.

2. There was a general concern that the description of stimuli and the quantification of animal behavior was insufficient. This concern could be overcome by providing additional information and analyses, as listed below.

2.1. L75/L158/L297 Please provide a more detailed description of the actions in each of the 'social' and 'nonsocial' videos in the Results and Methods sections, and provide the test videos as supplementary material.

2.2. L 93 "AFNI's 3dttest++" Please describe in the Methods section the kind of test that is coded by this AFNI function (fixed effect? Mixed effect analysis? corrected for multiple comparison?).

2.3. Figure 1: What is the baseline used for comparison in the GLM and tests? Were all the baseline times concatenated? A baseline in the middle or at the end or at the beginning of each run?

2.4. L192 "It should be noted that none of the oculomotor areas were activated by this contrast, precluding any confound with eye movements". This is an important information appearing only in the Discussion. Please describe in the Methods section how it was assessed, and in the Results section provide more details on this result.

2.5. L279-280 "this camera and setup did not allow a proper recording of eye movements": Did it allow to monitor other interesting information: e.g. how long eyes were open during each condition, and/or to monitor blinks?

2.6. L296-306: Was there one video per block? Is there a difference in motion or luminance between conditions?

2.7. L307-312: Are conditions presented in reverse order the second time during the run? Which baseline is used for contrasts presented in figure 1? Where the animals rewarded with juice during the scan? If so, for what type of behavior (fixation during baseline? looking at the videos, looking at the screen? Staying engaged?).

2.8. Additional information should be given regarding time courses of regions highlighted on maps and in the discussion, ie PG, PMv, AIP, 8A/45.

2.9. How many video clips were used in total? How many runs/sessions were collected?

3. Contrasts in Figure 2 and 3 are shown at p<0.05, uncorrected. Given the large number of voxels, whole brain tests should be corrected for multiple comparisons. Alternatively, the should be displayed at a more conservative uncorrected threshold (e.g., p<0.001 as for) as in Figure 1.

4. The authors isolate areas involved in observing social interactions by comparing responses to social and non-social interactions. To better control for low level visual features, please report the interaction effect ([social minus social scrambled] minus [nonsocial minus nonsocial scrambled]).

eLife. 2021 Mar 31;10:e65012. doi: 10.7554/eLife.65012.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1. It would be important to more clearly describe the statistical group-level model (fixed-effect, mixed effect, random effect?). Most importantly, it was not clear whether and how the group model accounted for the nested structure of technical (8 runs) and biological replicates (3 animals). Specifically, it seems the 24 runs were treated as independent observations (given they are not independent, they should not be treated as such). Related to this, it would be important to also show results for each of the individual 3 subjects.

The statistical analyses are now described in more detail in the Methods section, and also pasted below. We now show individual maps in the supplementary data and refined our analysis by using paired-t-test for individual analyses (fixed-effect) and analysis of variance (three-factor ANOVA, mixed effect) to take into account that the data are not independent.

This has been modified in the Methods section as follows:

“The T-value maps for each monkey were then compared at the individual level via paired-sample t-tests and fixed effect analyses across the 8 runs for each marmoset (AFNI’s 3dttest++) and at the group level via a three-way analysis of variance and mixed effect analysis (AFNI’s 3danova3) using 8 runs per marmosets (for a total of 24 runs). To protect against false positives, a cluster-size correction derived from Monte Carlo simulations was applied to the resultant t-test maps for α=0.05 (using AFNI’s 3dClustsim).”

2. There was a general concern that the description of stimuli and the quantification of animal behavior was insufficient. This concern could be overcome by providing additional information and analyses, as listed below.

2.1. L75/L158/L297 Please provide a more detailed description of the actions in each of the 'social' and 'nonsocial' videos in the Results and Methods sections, and provide the test videos as supplementary material.

Thank you, this is a good point. Additional details about the social and nonsocial videos have been added to the manuscript in two tables (Tables 1 and 2) and in the Results section as follows:

“In the social conditions, the actions performed in the videos included eating, playing, fighting and grooming between two marmosets. […] A more detailed description of these actions is provided in tables 1 and 2 in the Methods section.”

The videos are available upon request due to commercial copyright. Please see new statement in the manuscript.

2.2. L 93 "AFNI's 3dttest++" Please describe in the Methods section the kind of test that is coded by this AFNI function (fixed effect? Mixed effect analysis? corrected for multiple comparison?).

Details about this function have been added to the Methods section as follow:

“The T-value maps for each monkey were then compared at the individual level via paired-sample t-tests and fixed effect analyses across the 8 runs for each marmoset (AFNI’s 3dttest++) and at the group level via a three-way analysis of variance and mixed effect analysis (AFNI’s 3danova3) using 8 runs per marmosets (for a total of 24 runs). To protect against false positives, a cluster-size correction derived from Monte Carlo simulations was applied to the resultant t-test maps for α=0.05 (using AFNI’s 3dClustsim).”

2.3. Figure 1: What is the baseline used for comparison in the GLM and tests? Were all the baseline times concatenated? A baseline in the middle or at the end or at the beginning of each run?

The baseline used for comparison in the GLM and tests using AFNI corresponds to the regressors of no interest during the run (constant, drift, etc). Here it also included the no stimulation blocks and estimated motion parameters.

This has been added in the Methods section as follow:

“All four conditions were entered into the same model, along with polynomial (N = 5) detrending regressors, bandpass regressors, and the motion parameters derived from the volume registration described above (these nuisance regressors correspond to the baseline variance for the statistical analysis model).”

2.4. L192 "It should be noted that none of the oculomotor areas were activated by this contrast, precluding any confound with eye movements". This is an important information appearing only in the Discussion. Please describe in the Methods section how it was assessed, and in the Results section provide more details on this result.

This has been added to the manuscript as follows:

Methods section:

“The contrast between the social and nonsocial condition as well as the interaction effect [social vs. social scrambled] minus [nonsocial vs. nonsocial scrambled] were compared to previous fMRI and electrophysiological studies performed in marmosets (Ghahremani et al., 2017; Johnston et al., 2019; Schaeffer et al., 2019; Selvanayagam et al., 2019) to check if the observed activations may be related to eye movements.”

Results section:

“We did not observe any activation in area 8aV, indicating that he differences are not primarily related to saccades.”

Discussion section:

“Eye movements, and particularly visual saccades mostly activate the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) and the frontal eye field (FEF) in primates. […] The social condition may have elicited more small saccades related to the exploration of the facial expression and the interaction itself compared to the nonsocial interaction which may involve more large explorations.”

2.5. L279-280 "this camera and setup did not allow a proper recording of eye movements": Did it allow to monitor other interesting information: e.g. how long eyes were open during each condition, and/or to monitor blinks?

Unfortunately, the quality of the recordings was poor. We cannot assess how long the eyes were open during each condition as we sometimes lost the tracking signal. It means that even if the eyes were still open, the signal would be recorded as eyes close. Video-based eye tracking relies on tracking the center of the pupil which is extremely large in marmosets. Therefore, even small drops of the eye lids change the center of the pupil or lead to complete signal loss.

2.6. L296-306: Was there one video per block? Is there a difference in motion or luminance between conditions?

Two video clips were displayed within a block (6 seconds each) and 20 different videos clips were used in total. The videos within the same run were different between the two repetitions of the block condition.

Motion and luminance varied across videos, depending of the action and where the action took place, but we tried as much as possible to counterbalance these differences across the selected videos, conditions and order of presentation. In addition, to preserve motion and luminance components, the same random rotation matrix was used for each frame for the scrambled versions of the videos.

This has been added in the Methods section as follows:

“Two video clips were displayed within a block (6 seconds each) and 20 different videos clips were used in total. The videos within the same run were different between the two repetitions of the block condition but played following the same order. Tables 1 and two describe which action were performed for each social (Table 1) and nonsocial (Table 2) blocks.”

“To preserve motion and luminance components, the same random rotation matrix was used for each frame in the scrambled conditions.”

“As motion and luminance varied across videos, mainly due to where the action took place, we counterbalanced these differences across the selected videos, conditions and order of presentation to avoid any link with the condition block itself.”

2.7. L307-312: Are conditions presented in reverse order the second time during the run? Which baseline is used for contrasts presented in figure 1? Where the animals rewarded with juice during the scan? If so, for what type of behavior (fixation during baseline? looking at the videos, looking at the screen? Staying engaged?).

Conditions were presented in the same order the second time within the same run but this order was counterbalanced across runs. The baseline for the contrasts in Figure 1 corresponded to the regressors of no interest (no stimulation period, movement regressors, see point 2.3).

For this task, no reward was provided during the scan. Marmosets only received pudding before and after the scan session.

This has been added to the Methods section as follow:

“The videos within the same run were different between the two repetitions of the block condition but played following the same order.”

“No reward has been provided to the monkeys during the scan session.”

2.8. Additional information should be given regarding time courses of regions highlighted on maps and in the discussion, ie PG, PMv, AIP, 8A/45.

The PSC analysis includes now twelve regions of interest corresponding to the regions defined by the Paxinos atlas and based on the activation maps and regions highlighted in the discussion (areas 25, 10, 14c, VIP, PG, AIP. 6va, 6vb, 46d, 45, 8av and 8ad).

Figures, Results and Methods sections have been modified accordingly to this new data.

2.9. How many video clips were used in total? How many runs/sessions were collected?

Twenty different videos clips were used in total. Monkey 1 performed 8 runs in 4 sessions; Monkey 2, 11 runs in 5 sessions and Monkey 3, 9 runs in 4 sessions keep only 8. We selected 8 runs per monkey (based on their best engagement as assessed by the camera monitoring across each session) to have the same number across subjects and run the group analysis.

This has been added to the Methods section as follow:

“Monkey 1 performed eight runs in four sessions, Monkey 2 eleven runs in five sessions and Monkey 3 nine runs in four sessions. We selected eight runs per monkeys to have the same number of runs across subjects for the group analysis (the selection was based on the best engagement of the animal as assessed by visual monitoring during the sessions).”

3. Contrasts in Figure 2 and 3 are shown at p<0.05, uncorrected. Given the large number of voxels, whole brain tests should be corrected for multiple comparisons. Alternatively, the should be displayed at a more conservative uncorrected threshold (e.g., p<0.001 as for) as in Figure 1.

As mentioned previously, we have performed statistical analysis at both individual and group levels using paired t-tests and analysis of variance (three-factor ANOVA) but also the clustering method derived from Monte Carlo simulations to protect against false positives (AFNI’s 3dClustsim).

This has been modified in the Methods section as follow:

“The T-value maps for each monkey were then compared at the individual level via paired-sample t-tests and fixed effect analyses across the 8 runs for each marmoset (AFNI’s 3dttest++) and at the group level via a three-way analysis of variance and mixed effect analysis (AFNI’s 3danova3) using 8 runs per marmosets (for a total of 24 runs). To protect against false positives, a cluster-size correction derived from Monte Carlo simulations was applied to the resultant t-test maps for α=0.05 (using AFNI’s 3dClustsim).”

4. The authors isolate areas involved in observing social interactions by comparing responses to social and non-social interactions. To better control for low level visual features, please report the interaction effect ([social minus social scrambled] minus [nonsocial minus nonsocial scrambled]).

This contrast has been added and corresponds now to the new Figure 3 (and Figure 3—figure supplement 1 for individual maps).


Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

RESOURCES