Abstract
Hydrogen to deuterium isotopic substitution has only a minor effect on physical and chemical properties of water and, as such, is not supposed to influence its neutral taste. Here we conclusively demonstrate that humans are, nevertheless, able to distinguish D2O from H2O by taste. Indeed, highly purified heavy water has a distinctly sweeter taste than same-purity normal water and can add to perceived sweetness of sweeteners. In contrast, mice do not prefer D2O over H2O, indicating that they are not likely to perceive heavy water as sweet. HEK 293T cells transfected with the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 heterodimer and chimeric G-proteins are activated by D2O but not by H2O. Lactisole, which is a known sweetness inhibitor acting via the TAS1R3 monomer of the TAS1R2/TAS1R3, suppresses the sweetness of D2O in human sensory tests, as well as the calcium release elicited by D2O in sweet taste receptor-expressing cells. The present multifaceted experimental study, complemented by homology modelling and molecular dynamics simulations, resolves a long-standing controversy about the taste of heavy water, shows that its sweet taste is mediated by the human TAS1R2/TAS1R3 taste receptor, and opens way to future studies of the detailed mechanism of action.
Subject terms: Biophysical chemistry, Computational biophysics
Ben Abu, Mason and colleagues use molecular dynamics, cell-based experiments, mouse models, and human subjects to determine that, unlike ordinary water, heavy water tastes sweet to humans, but not mice. Mechanistically, this effect is mediated by the human TAS1R/TAS1R3 sweet taste receptor.
Introduction
Heavy water, D2O, has fascinated researchers since the discovery of deuterium by Urey in 19311,2. The most notable difference in physical properties between D2O and H2O is the roughly 10% higher density of the former liquid, which is mostly a trivial consequence of deuterium being about twice as heavy as hydrogen. A more subtle effect of deuteration is the formation of slightly stronger hydrogen (or deuterium) bonds in D2O as compared to H2O3,4. This results in a small increase of the freezing and boiling points by 3.8 °C and 1.4 °C, respectively, and in a slight increase of 0.44 in pH (or pD) of pure water upon deuteration5. In comparison, a mere dissolution of atmospheric CO2 and subsequent formation of dilute carbonic acid in open containers has a significantly stronger influence on the pH of water, changing it by more than one unit6.
Biological effects are observable for high doses of D2O. While bacteria or yeasts can function in practically pure D2O, albeit with somewhat hindered growth rate7–9, for higher organisms damaging effects on cell division and general metabolism occur at around 25% deuteration, with lethal conditions for plants and animals typically occurring at ~40–50% deuteration of the body water2,10,11. Small levels of deuteration are, nevertheless, harmless. This is understandable given the fact that about 1 in every 6400 hydrogens in nature is found in its stable isotope form of deuterium12. Oral doses of several milliliters of D2O are safe for humans13 and are used in the isotopic form D218O for metabolic measurements in clinical praxis (known as “doubly labeled water” technique)14. Probably the best-established effect of D2O is the increase of the circadian oscillation length upon its administration to both animals and plants. This has been attributed to a general slowdown of metabolism upon deuteration, although the exact mechanism of this effect is unknown15,16.
A long-standing unresolved puzzle concerns the taste of heavy water. There is anecdotal evidence from the 1930s that the taste of pure D2O is distinct from the neutral one of pure H2O, being described mostly as “sweet”17. However, Urey and Failla addressed this question in 1935 concluding authoritatively that upon tasting “neither of us could detect the slightest difference between the taste of ordinary distilled water and the taste of pure heavy water”18. This had, with a rare exception19, an inhibitive effect on further human studies, with research concerning effects of D2O focusing primarily on animal or cell models. Experiments in animals indicated that rats developed aversion toward D2O when deuteration of their body water reached harmful levels, but there is conflicting evidence regarding their ability to taste heavy water or use other cues to avoid it20,21.
Within the last two decades, the heterodimer of the taste receptor of the TAS1Rs type of G protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs), denoted as TAS1R2/TAS1R3, was established as the main receptor for sweet taste22. The human TAS1R2/TAS1R3 heterodimer recognizes diverse natural and synthetic sweeteners23. The binding sites of the different types of sweeteners include an orthosteric site (a sugar-binding site in the extracellular Venus flytrap domain of TAS1R2) and several allosteric sites, including sites in the extracellular regions of the TAS1R2 and TAS1R3 subunits and in the transmembrane domain of TAS1R324,25 (Fig. 1). Additional pathways for sweet taste recognition have also been suggested, involving glucose transporters and ATP-gated K+ channel26,27.
Interestingly, not all artificial sweeteners are recognized by rodents28. Differences in human and rodent responses to tastants, as well as sweetness inhibitors such as lactisole, have been useful for delineating the molecular recognition of sweet compounds—using human-mouse chimeric receptors, it was shown that the transmembrane domain (TMD) of human TAS1R3 is required for the activating effects of cyclamate29 and for the inhibitory effect of lactisole30.
A combination of TAS1R3 with another member of the TAS1R family, TAS1R1, results in a dimer that mediates umami taste, elicited by molecules such as glutamate or, in case of the rodent umami receptor, other L-amino acids31. Bitter taste is mediated by the taste 2 receptor (TAS2R) gene family32, a branch of Family A GPCRs24. The human genome has 25 TAS2R subtypes and over a thousand of bitter compounds are currently known33, with numerous additional bitter tastants predicted34.
In this study, we systematically address the question of sweet taste of heavy water by a combination of sensory experiments in humans, behavioral experiments in mice, tests on sweet taste receptor-transfected cell lines, and computational modeling including molecular dynamics (MD) simulations. This combined approach consistently leads to the conclusion that the sweet taste of pure D2O is a real effect for human subjects due to activation of the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 sweet taste receptor. While present simulations show, in accord with previous experiments35, that proteins are systematically slightly more rigid and compact in D2O than in H2O, the specific molecular mechanism of the heavy water effect on the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 receptor remains to be established.
Results and discussion
Water purity
We have paid great attention to the purity of the water samples, further degassing and redistilling under vacuum the purest commercially available D2O and H2O. The lack of non-negligible amounts of organic impurities was subsequently confirmed by gas chromatography with mass spectrometry analysis and by experiments with water samples at different levels of purification, see Supplementary Information (SI), Figures S1 and S2. This is extremely important—note in this context that “the vibrational theory of olfaction”, which suggested distinct perception of deuterium isotopes of odorants due to difference in their vibrational spectra36, has been refuted with some of the observed effects turning out to be due to impurities37,38.
Experiments with a human sensory panel
A human sensory panel was employed to study the D2O taste. Triangle tests based on two samples of H2O and one sample of D2O (or vice versa), with random success rate of one-third, were presented to the panelists in a randomized order. Panelists were asked to pick the odd sample out—to smell only, to taste only (with a nose clips), or to taste with open nose. Our results show that humans perceive D2O as being clearly distinguishable from H2O based on its taste: In open nose taste test 22 out of 28 participants identified the odd sample correctly (p = 0.001), and in taste only test 14 out of 26 identified the odd sample correctly (p = 0.03). However, in smell-only triangle test, only 9 out of 25 panelists chose the odd sample correctly (p > 0.05). Data are summarized in Figure S3 in SI.
Next, the perceived sweetness of D2O in increasing proportion to H2O was reported using a 9-point scale, labeled also with verbal descriptions of perceived intensity (1 = no sensation, 3 = slight, 5 = moderate, 7 = very much, 9 = extreme sensation). Sweetness was shown to increase in a D2O-dose-dependent manner, reaching average 3.3 ± 0.4 sweetness (“slight” sweetness) (Fig. 2a). The perceived sweetness of low concentrations of caloric D-glucose (Fig. 2b), sucrose (Fig. 2c), and an artificial sweetener cyclamate (Fig. 2d) was tested when dissolved in H2O or in D2O, in order to check whether the slight sweetness of D2O adds on top of slight sweetness of known sweeteners. As expected39, D-glucose was perceived as less sweet compared to sucrose at the same concentration (Fig. 2b and c). D2O added to the perceived sweetness of all tested concentrations of D-glucose and cyclamate (see Figure S4 in the SI for cyclamate results excluding two outliers). The sweetness of the two lowest concentrations of sucrose was significantly higher when dissolved in D2O compared to H2O.
We then checked whether the stand-alone and additive effect of D2O is sweetness-specific or general, whereby D2O might elicit other tastes, or add to their intensity. Savory (umami) taste of monosodium glutamate (MSG) and bitter taste of quinine, which are also taste modalities mediated by GPCR receptors expressed in taste cells, were tested in regular and in heavy water. The intensity of savory taste of MSG in D2O did not differ from that in H2O (Fig. 2e), while the perceived bitterness of quinine was in fact slightly reduced in D2O compared to quinine in H2O (Fig. 2f). This is in agreement with the known effect of sweeteners as maskers of bitter taste, that may be due to both local interactions and sensory integration effects40–42. Thus, we have ascertained that D2O is sweet and adds to the sweetness of other sweet molecules, but not to the intensity of other GPCR-mediated taste modalities.
Experiments with mice
Next, we addressed the question whether the sweetness of D2O is perceived also by rodents. Lean mice of the C57BL/6J strain were drinking pure H2O, D2O, or a 43 mmol/l H2O sucrose solution for 16 h during a night period. Namely, each of the three groups of mice had a choice from two bottles containing (i) H2O and D2O, (ii) H2O and sucrose solution, or (iii) H2O and H2O (as a control). The food intake was unaffected in all groups (see SI, Figure S5 and Table S1).
The results of the drinking experiments are presented in Fig. 3a–c, with a snapshot of the experimental setup shown in Fig. 3d. In cages where mice were offered both normal water and heavy water (Fig. 3a) consumption of D2O was within statistical error the same as that of H2O. Previous reports have shown that on longer timescales than those reported here mice learned to avoid D2O, as it is poisonous to them in larger quantities10. It is not clear what is the cue that enables the avoidance learning, but it is evident that the early response to D2O is not attractive, suggesting that it is not eliciting sweet taste in mice.
By contrast, mice exhibit a strong preference for sucrose solution over H2O. Indeed, the consumed volume was significantly increased in line with the predilection of mice for sucrose solutions (Fig. 3b). The amount of H2O consumed by the control group from either of the two bottles, both containing H2O, is depicted in Fig. 3c. Overall, the data shows that in all three experiments mice consumed comparable amounts of H2O and D2O, with significant increase of consumption of the sucrose solution.
Assessing involvement of TAS1R2/TAS1R3 receptor using human sensory panel
The chemical dissimilarity of D2O from sugars and other sweeteners raises the question whether the effect we observed in human subjects is mediated by TAS1R2/TAS1R3, which is the major receptor for sweet taste22. This was first explored by combining water samples with lactisole as an established TAS1R2/TAS1R3 inhibitor30. Using the two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) method, in which the participant must choose between two samples, 18 out of 25 panelists chose pure D2O as sweeter than D2O + 0.9 mM lactisole solution (p < 0.05, Fig. 4a). In an additional experiment, the sweetness of pure D2O was scored significantly higher than that of D2O + 0.9 mM lactisole solution (p = 0.0003), while the same amount of lactisole had no effect on the perception of sweetness of H2O that served as control (Fig. 4b). These results suggest that D2O elicits sweetness via the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 sweet taste receptor.
Cell-based experiments for establishing the role of TAS1R2/TAS1R3
To confirm the involvement of the sweet taste receptor TAS1R2/TAS1R3 in D2O signaling we performed functional calcium mobilization assays using HEK 293 FlpIn T-Rex cells heterologously expressing both required TAS1R subunits as well as the chimeric G protein Gα15Gi343,44. As seen in Fig. 5a, D2O at 1.85 M and 5.84 M concentrations in H2O (3.3% and 10.4%, respectively) elicited robust responses in TAS1R2/TAS1R3 expressing cells. The strong reduction or absence of D2O-elicited fluorescence response in the presence of lactisole confirmed the dependence on TAS1R2/TAS1R3. The inhibitory effect of lactisole on D2O-activation of the human sweet taste receptor was confirmed using an IP1 assay45,46, while lactisole exposure had no effect on cells treated with pure H2O water, as expected (Fig. 5c). As a control, 960 mM D-glucose elicited increase in IP1 levels in TAS1R2/TAS1R3 expressing cell, which was inhibited in the presence of lactisole.
We further used an IP1 assay45,46 on non-transfected HEK 293T cells, where we observed that dose-dependent curves of carbachol—an agonist of the endogenous muscarinic receptor 3 (M3)47—did not show any difference between H2O and D2O-based media (Fig. 6a) and that cell medium that had either 10% or 100% D2O, did not activate basal IP1 accumulation (Fig. 6b). Next, TAS1R2/TAS1R3 receptor along with the chimeric Gα16gust44 subunit44,48 were transiently expressed, and the functionality was illustrated by dose-dependent response to D-glucose (Fig. 6c). Finally, and in agreement with calcium imaging, we found that 10% D2O activated these cells. Activation by 100% D2O was even more pronounced (Fig. 6d).
Molecular modeling
The cellular response results further support the hypothesis that the sweet taste of D2O is mediated via the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 receptor. Various mechanisms governing this effect can be envisioned. As a potential suspect, we focus on a direct effect on the sweet taste receptor, narrowing on the TAS1R3 TMD (see Fig. 1), as it is already known to be a modulation site with functional differences between humans and rodents23,29,30. Furthermore, water-binding sites were discovered at the TMD of many GPCRs49,50, suggesting a potential target for D2O binding. We modeled the human TAS1R3 TMD using the I-TASSER server51. Positions of H2O molecules were compared among mGluR5 structures (PDB: 4OO9, 5CGC, and 5CGD) and two conserved positions were found. The H2O molecules in these two positions were merged with the TAS1R3 model and minimized (Fig. 7a). The water mapping protocol from OpenEye52 enables mapping of water positions based on the energetics of water, and ~40 water molecules were predicted in the binding site using this protocol (Fig. 7a). Water densities of H2O and D2O in the TMD of the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 receptor were calculated from MD simulations as described below. Overall, all three methods suggest the possibility for at least some internal molecules (trapped in the TMD bundle) in addition to water that surrounds the extracellular and intracellular loops (Fig. 7a).
Next, we carried out microsecond MD simulations of the TMD embedded in a phospatidylcholine (POPC) bilayer in either H2O or D2O (for details including our model of D2O effectively including nuclear quantum effects, see SI, Tables S2–S4). Note that water molecules enter the TMD domain and cluster at positions that partially overlap with the modeled water positions, see Fig. 7a. More precisely, H2O and D2O have mutually slightly shifted densities inside the protein cavity, with H2O overlapping better than D2O with the modeled water positions. Furthermore, MD simulations show clustered water molecules close to the lactisole binding site. These internal positions may have a differential effect between H2O and D2O, though differences between the averaged water densities are not very pronounced. Figure 7b shows the time evolution of the radius of gyration of the TMD domain, while Fig. 7c and d presents the root mean square fluctuations (RMSF) of individual residues of the proteins superimposed on its structure and plotted in a graph together with the mean value of RMSF. A small but significant difference is apparent in the behavior of the protein in H2O vs D2O. Namely, structural fluctuations of most residues (particularly those directly exposed to the aqueous environment) and of the protein as a whole are slightly attenuated in D2O, in which environment the protein is also somewhat more compact than in H2O (Fig. 7b). Additional simulations on other representative systems show that the rigidifying effect of heavy water is apparent also in small soluble proteins (see SI, Figs. S6–S8).
Summary and outlook
Sweet taste never ceases to surprise. Over a decade ago, water was shown to elicit sweet taste by rinsing away inhibitors of sweet taste receptors, both in human sensory experiments and in cell-based studies. This effect was explained in terms of a two-state model, where the receptor shifts to its activated state when released from inhibition by rinsing with water43. Here, we have studied the taste of D2O and H2O per se, not related to washing away of sweet taste inhibitors. Using psychophysics protocols, we show that humans differentiate between D2O and H2O based on taste alone. Importantly, by employing gas chromatography/mass spectrometry analysis we demonstrate that the sweet taste of deuterated water is not due to impurities. Being only isotopically different from H2O, in principle, D2O should be indistinguishable from H2O with regard to taste, namely it should have no taste of its own. Yet, we illustrate that human subjects consistently perceive D2O as being slightly sweet and significantly sweeter than H2O. Furthermore, D2O added to perceived sweetness of low concentrations of other sweeteners. In contrast, it did not elicit umami or bitter taste on its own, nor did it add to the umami taste perception of MSG. D2O did not add to the bitterness of quinine, and reduced the perceived bitterness of 0.1 mM quinine, in agreement with the known effect of bitterness suppression by sweet molecules.
A further important finding is that lactisole, which is an established blocker of the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 sweet taste receptor that acts at the TAS1R3 transmembrane domain30, suppresses both the sweet perception of D2O in sensory tests, as well as the activation of TAS1R/TAS1R3 in calcium imaging and in IP1 cell-based assays. In support of these observations we have demonstrated that HEK 293T cells transfected with TAS1R2/TAS1R3 and Gα16gust44 chimera, but not the non-transfected cells, are activated by D2O, as measured by IP1 accumulation compared to control values. Finally, taste experiments on mice show that these animals do not prefer D2O over H2O.
Our findings point to the human sweet taste receptor TAS1R2/TAS1R3 as being essential for sweetness of D2O. Molecular dynamics simulations show, in agreement with experiment35, that proteins in general are slightly more rigid and compact in D2O than in H2O. At a molecular level, this general behavior may be traced back to the slightly stronger hydrogen bonding in D2O vs H2O, which is due to a nuclear quantum effect, namely difference in zero-point energy3,4. Biologically relevant situations where one may expect strong nuclear quantum effects as implications of H/D substitution directly involve proton or deuteron transfer9. Unless a yet unknown indirect mechanism is involved, this is not the case for the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 sweet taste receptor, thus the nuclear quantum effect is probably weak in the present case. Future studies should be able to elucidate the precise sites and mechanisms of action, as well as the reason why D2O activates TAS1R2/TAS1R3 in particular, resulting in sweet (but not other) taste. To this end, site-directed mutagenesis as well as determination of the precise structure of the TAS1R2/TAS1R3 receptor will be of key importance.
The finding that deuterated water elicits sweet taste via activation of TAS1R/TAS1R2 receptor is of fundamental interest. The difference between hydrogen isotopes is the largest possible isotope effect (doubling of mass in case of deuterium, while tripling in case of tritium), yet deuteration effects on water are generally mild. Nevertheless, water deuteration leads to activation of a GPCR heterodimer to a level that is perceived by humans as sweet taste. While clearly not a practical sweetener, heavy water provides a glimpse into the wide-open chemical space of sweet molecules. Since heavy water may be used in medical procedures, our finding that it can elicit responses of the sweet taste receptor, which is not only located on the tongue but also in other tissues of the human body, represents an important information for clinicians and their patients. Moreover, due to wide application of D2O in chemical structure determination by NMR, chemists will benefit from being aware of the present observations.
Methods
Sensory evaluation experiments
A human sensory panel was used to resolve the gustatory effect in perception of D2O taste. Subjects between the ages 20 and 43 years were recruited. The study included 10 experiments with different groups of participants (15–30 subjects; between 4 and 12 males). The perception was tested by sensory tests as detailed below. Either sterile syringes with solutions (0.3 ml) or identical cups with solutions (7 ml), were presented in randomized order, unless otherwise noted. Participants were required to taste each solution using either ‘tip of the tongue’ or ‘sip and spit’ procedures, rinse their mouth with water after each solution and to wait for 30 s before moving to the next taste sample. All research procedures were ethically approved by the Committee for the Use of Human Subjects in Research in The Robert H. Smith Faculty of Agriculture Food and Environment, the Hebrew University of Jerusalem.
99.9% purity D2O was purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Corp, while 18 MΩ ultrapure grade was used for pure H2O. All water samples were placed under vacuum and treated by ultrasound to remove any dissolved gases. See below for more details on water purification.
D-glucose (CAS Number: 50-99-7), sucrose (CAS Number: 57-50-1), cyclamate (CAS Number: 139-05-9), quinine (CAS Number: 207671-44-1), and MSG (CAS Number: 142-47-2) were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich Corp. All compounds were dissolved in both types of water to a final concentration of 50, 75, and 100 mM for D-glucose and sucrose; 3.5, 4.5, and 5.5 mM for cyclamate; 0.1 and 0.32 mM for quinine; 10, 25, and 50 mM for MSG. Lactisole (CAS Number: 150436-68-3) was purchased from Domino Specialty Ingredients and dissolved to a final concentration of 0.9 mM in D2O water as well as in H2O. The concentration of lactisole was selected based on previous data43,53 and preliminary experiments in our lab, which showed that 0.9 mM decreases the sweetness of 100 mM sucrose. Sweeteners concentrations were selected to be in low intensity of sweetness. All solutions were prepared in the morning of the day of the experiment and were stored in individual plastic syringes (1 ml) for each participant.
The sweetness of heavy water and its effect on other taste compounds was evaluated in several independent experiments: (1) D2O sweetness relative to H2O (Fig. 2a); (2) D2O effect on sweetness of D-glucose (Fig. 2b), sucrose (Fig. 2c), and cyclamate (Fig. 2d); (3) D2O effect on quinine (Fig. 2e) and MSG (Fig. 2f); (4) Lactisole effect on D2O sweetness (Fig. 4b); Intensity of each taste modality—sweetness/bitterness/umami was evaluated on a 9-point scale on Compusense Cloud, ranging from 1 (no sensation) to 9 (extremely strong sensation). In addition, participants had to report any additional tastes they recognized. Statistical tests were conducted using JMP Pro 13 (JMP, Version 13. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, 1989–2019).
Data were first analyzed employing ANOVA with participants as a random effect54. Thereafter, the Tukey Kramer test was used to compare mean sweetness between all samples54. Significance was set at p < 0.05, and preplanned comparison t-tests were used where relevant.
Details concerning the Two-Alternative Forced Choice test55 were as follows: Participants were presented with two blind coded water samples of D2O and D2O + 0.9 mM lactisole. The participants were asked to choose the sweeter solution. For the data analysis, the highest number of responses for one sample was compared to a statistical table55 which states the minimum number of responses required for a significant difference.
Panelists were presented with two identical and one different water samples. All three samples were presented to the subjects at once, and the panelists were instructed to taste or smell the samples from left to right and identify the odd sample. Triangle tests were used to examine the difference in taste, as well as in smell, between H2O and D2O.
Heterologous expression
TAS1R2/TAS1R3 stimulated activation of the G-protein-mediated pathway was measured applying the IP-One HTRF assay (Cisbio) based on the manufacturer’s protocol. In brief, HEK 293T cells (ATCC) were grown to a confluency of ~85–90% and transiently transfected with 6 μg/plate DNA (TAS1R2, TAS1R3, Gα16gust44) by applying LipofectamineTM 2000 (Invitrogen, USA, 30 μl/plate) transfection reagent, according to the manufacturer’s protocol. The next day, cells were suspended with fresh Dulbecco’s modified Eagle’s medium (DMEM), containing 10% fetal bovine serum (FBS), 1% L-glutamine amino acid and 1% penicillin streptomycin (10% DMEM), seeded (0.5 ml cells per well) into 24-well culture plate, and maintained for 8–12 h at 37 °C. Then cells were “starved” overnight by changing the medium to 0.1% DMEM (containing 0.1% FBS), in order to reduce the basal activity of the cells. Cells exposure was performed by addition of 0.5 ml tested compound (pH = 7.4) dissolved in 0.1% DMEM with 50 mM lithium chloride (LiCl) for 5 min directly into the wells. The presence of LiCl in this step is crucial because LiCl leads to IP1 accumulation45. At the end of exposure time, tastant solution was replaced with fresh medium (0.1% DMEM) containing 50 mM LiCl for another 55 min. Later, wells were washed with 100 μl cold phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) + Triton X-100, and kept at −80 °C for a few hours, in order to dissolve the cell membrane. For the IP-One HTRF assay, cell lysate was mixed with the detection reagents (IP1-d2 conjugate and Anti-IP1 cryptate TB conjugate, each dissolved in lysis buffer), and added to each well in a 384-well plate for 60 min incubation at room temperature. Finally, the plate was read using Clariostar plate reader (BMG, Germany) equipped with 620 ± 10 nm and 670 ± 10 nm filters. IP1 levels were measured by calculating the 665 nm/620 nm emission ratio.
All responses are presented as the means ± SEM of IP1 accumulation (%). Dose–response curves were fitted by non-linear regression using the algorithms of PRISM 7 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA). Column figures were analyzed using one-way ANOVA with a Dunnett’s54. Each compound was tested in triplicate in three individual experiments in comparison to the reference (carbachol dissolved in H2O or basal levels)45.
In the case of D2O, in order to test its specific effect, we used a powder DMEM medium (CAS Number: D5030, Sigma-Aldrich), dissolved in the needed amount of D2O instead of the liquid one. Other ligands than D2O were purchased from Sigma-Aldrich or Domino Specialty Ingredients as noted above. Unless noted otherwise, ligands were used at final concentrations of 5, 50, 480, 960, 1.8 × 103, and 2.1 × 103 mM for D-glucose; 0.1, 1, and 15 mM for carbachol and D2O at 0–100% proportionately to H2O. Since there was no prior literature on concentrations of lactisole for this assay, the value of 2 mM was chosen based on preliminary experiment with several lactisole concentrations and their effect on IP1 values due to exposure to D-glucose.
For the functional assays with the human sweet taste receptor, we used a cell line (HEK 293 FlpIn T-Rex), which constitutively expresses the sweet taste receptor subunit TAS1R2 as well as the chimeric G protein Gα15gi3, whereas the sweet taste receptor subunit TAS1R3 can be induced by tetracycline43,44. Gαi3 is one of the major species of Gα-subunits capable of coupling to taste receptors, along with Gα-gustducin, GαS, and Gαi256,57 and TAS1R2-TAS1R3-Gαi3 cell system was successfully used in a previous study43. The functional experiments were done as described before44. Briefly, the cells were grown in DMEM supplemented with 10% fetal bovine serum, 100 U Penicillin/ml, 0.1 mg/ml Streptomycin, 2 mM L-glutamine, at 37 °C and 5%-CO2, 100% air humidity. The day before the experiment, cells were seeded to a density of 50–60% onto 96-well plates coated with 10 µg/ml poly-D-lysine and 0.5 µg/ml tetracycline was added. Next, cells were loaded with Fluo-4 AM in the presence of 2.5 mM probenecid for 1 h. After this, cells were washed twice with C1-buffer (130 mM NaCl, 5 mM KCl, 10 mM HEPES, 1 mM sodium pyruvate, and 2 mM CaCl2, pH 7.4) before placing them in a fluorometric imaging plate reader (FLIPRtetra, Molecular Devices) for measurements.
C1-buffer prepared with D2O was mixed with C1-buffer made with H2O to result in the following final D2O-concentrations (a further threefold dilution, which occurs upon application of 50 µL stimulus to 100 µL of C1-buffer in the 96-well plates is already included): 18.47, 5.84, 1.85, 0.584, 0.185, 0.058, 0.018, and 0.000 M. Fluorescence changes were monitored after automated application of stimuli. As specificity control C1-D2O including 0.9 mM lactisole, a selective inhibitor of the human sweet taste receptor58, was applied to identically treated cells. This concentration is the same as used in sensory experiments and close to the 1 mM used in calcium assays in previous work43. Experimental results from five biological replicates performed in quadruplicates were used to establish the dose–response relationship using the software SigmaPlot as before44. As the highest D2O-concentration resulted in fluorescence changes largely resistant to lactisole blocking, the 18.47 M concentration was excluded. Student’s t-test was used to confirm that D2O-induced fluorescence changes above baseline were significantly (p ≤ 0.01) different from lactisole-treated controls.
Animal experiments
All animal experiments followed the ethical guidelines for animal experiments and the Act of the Czech Republic Nr. 246/1992 and were approved by the Committee for Experiments with Laboratory Animals of the Czech Academy of Sciences. Three-month-old male C57BL/6J mice (n = 34) from Charles Rivers Laboratories (Sulzfeld, Germany) were housed at a temperature of 23 °C with a daily cycle of 12 h light and dark (lights on at 6 am). The mice were placed in groups of two in cages with automatic drinking monitoring system (Developmental Workshops of Institute of Organic Chemistry and Biochemistry of the Czech Academy of Sciences, Prague, Czech Republic). They were given ad libitum water and a standard rodent chow diet (Ssniff Spezialdiäten GmbH, Soest, Germany). On the day of the experiment, during the dark phase of the cycle, freely fed mice were given weighed food pellets and two 30 ml glass bottles. The two bottles contained pure H2O and pure D2O (n = 12), or H2O and sucrose solution (43 mmol/l sucrose solution in H2O) (n = 10), see Table S1. Mice drinking H2O in both bottles served as a control group (n = 12). Drinking was monitored every 10 min for 16 h (starting from 6 pm) and food intake was determined at the end of the experiment (Figure S5).
All responses are presented as the means ± SEM. Statistical analysis was performed using ANOVA with a Dunnett’s test54 for food intake. Two-way ANOVA with a Bonferroni’s multiple comparisons test was used for analysis of average volume of liquid consumption. Analysis was performed using GraphPad Software, Inc., Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA). The differences between the control and treated groups were considered significant at p < 0.05.
Modeling and docking
All models were prepared with I-Tasser web server51. The templates that were used by I-Tasser for each of the TAS1R2 and TAS1R3 monomers were: 6N51, 5X2M, and 5K5S. To model the full heterodimer, the monomer models were aligned to a Class-C GPCR Cryo-EM structure (PDB: 6N51) and minimized with Schrödinger Maestro 2019-1. To illustrate the orthosteric binding site of sugars D-glucose was prepared (Schrödinger Maestro 2019-1, LigPrep) and docked with Glide XP, to the TAS1R2 VFT domain model that was based on 5X2M in a protocol that was validated in previous work59. The TAS1R3-binding site is based on a lactisole molecule docked to TAS1R3 TMD model (Schrödinger Maestro 2018-2, Glide SP, template PDB ID: 4OR2 and 4OO9). The figure was made using ChimeraX (version 0.93)60. Water molecules were predicted with a water mapping software (SZMAP 1.5.0.2: OpenEye52) after a successful benchmark over conserved water templates (PDB IDs 5CGC and 5CGD), in which the software was able to identify overall 8 out of 10 crystal water molecules around the ligands of the templates.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank R.F. Margolskee for the pcDNA of chimeric Gα16gust44 and P. Jiang and Y. Wang for hosting N.B.A. in Monell Chemical Senses Center during preliminary stages of this project. V.P. and L.M. acknowledge Ondřej Pačes and his team for the development of automatic drinking monitoring system, and Hedvika Vysušilová for outstanding technical assistance and animal handling. M.B. thanks Catherine Delaporte for excellent technical assistance. P.J. thanks the Czech Science Foundation for support via an EXPRO grant no. 19-26854X. V.C.C. and C.T. acknowledge support from the Charles University in Prague and from the International Max Planck Research School in Dresden. Funding by ISF grant #1129/19 and UHJ-France and the Foundation Scopus to M.Y.N. is gratefully acknowledged. M.Y.N. is a member of COST actions Mu.Ta.Lig (CA15135) and ERNEST (CA18133). P.E.M. acknowledges support from the viewers of his YouTube popular science channel.
Author contributions
N.B.A., P.E.M., M.Y.N., and P.J. designed the study, N.B.A. and H.K. performed sensory experiments, N.B.A. and E.M. carried out IP1 experiments, M.B. conducted calcium imaging experiments, N.D. and Y.B.S.G. performed homology modeling and structural analyses, P.E.M. performed water purification, J.C. performed water purity tests, V.P. and L.M. performed experiments on mice, V.C.C. developed the heavy water interaction potential, and C.T. performed the molecular dynamics simulations. P.J., M.Y.N., and N.B.A. wrote the paper with input from all co-authors.
Data availability
The data sets generated during the current study are available as Supplementary Data.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors contributed equally: Natalie Ben Abu, Philip E. Mason.
Contributor Information
Masha Y. Niv, Email: masha.niv@mail.huji.ac.il
Pavel Jungwirth, Email: pavel.jungwirth@uochb.cas.cz.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s42003-021-01964-y.
References
- 1.Urey HC, Brickwedde FG, Murphy GM. A hydrogen isotope of mass 2. Phys. Rev. 1932;39:164–165. doi: 10.1103/PhysRev.39.164. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Francl M. The weight of water. Nat. Chem. 2019;11:284–285. doi: 10.1038/s41557-019-0242-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Clark T, Heske J, Kühne TD. Opposing electronic and nuclear quantum effects on hydrogen bonds in H2O and D2O. ChemPhysChem. 2019;20:2461–2465. doi: 10.1002/cphc.201900839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Paesani F, Voth GA. The properties of water: insights from quantum simulations. J. Phys. Chem. B. 2009;113:5702–5719. doi: 10.1021/jp810590c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Macdonald F, Lide DR. CRC handbook of chemistry and physics: from paper to web. Abstr. Pap. Am. Chem. Soc. 2003;225:U552–U552. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bogan RAJ, Ohde S, Arakaki T, Mori I, McLeod CW. Changes in rainwater pH associated with increasing atmospheric carbon dioxide after the industrial revolution. Water, Air, Soil Pollut. 2009;196:263–271. doi: 10.1007/s11270-008-9774-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Van Horn E, Ware GC. Growth of bacterium coli and staphylococcus albus in heavy water. Nature. 1959;184:833–833. doi: 10.1038/184833a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Mosin, O., Ignatov, I., Skladnev, D. & Shvets, V. Studying of phenomenon of biological adaptation to heavy water. Eur. J. Mol. Biotechnol.6, 180–209 (2014).
- 9.Kampmeyer C, et al. Mutations in a single signaling pathway allow cell growth in heavy water. Acs Synth. Biol. 2020;9:733–748. doi: 10.1021/acssynbio.9b00376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Barbour HG. The basis of the pharmacological action of heavy water in mammals. Yale J. Biol. Med. 1937;9:551–565. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Czajka DM, Finkel AJ, Fischer CS, Katz JJ. Physiological effects of deuterium on dogs. Am. J. Physiol.-Leg. Content. 1961;201:357–362. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1961.201.2.357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dansgaard W. Stable isotopes in precipitation. Tellus. 1964;16:436–468. doi: 10.3402/tellusa.v16i4.8993. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jones PJH, Leatherdale ST. Stable isotopes in clinical research: safety reaffirmed. Clin. Sci. 1991;80:277–280. doi: 10.1042/cs0800277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Speakman J, Speakman JR. The history and theory of the doubly labeled water technique. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1998;68:932S–938S. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/68.4.932S. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pittendrigh CS, Caldarola PC, Cosbey ES. A differential effect of heavy water on temperature-dependent and temperature-compensated aspects of the circadian system of Drosophila pseudoobscura. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1973;70:2037. doi: 10.1073/pnas.70.7.2037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Richter CP. Heavy water as a tool for study of the forces that control length of period of the 24-hour clock of the hamster. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1977;74:1295–1299. doi: 10.1073/pnas.74.3.1295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hansen K, Rustung E. Untersuchen uber die Biologische Wirkungen von “Schwerem Wasser” bei Warmblutigen Tieren. Klinische Wochenschr. 1935;14:104–108. doi: 10.1007/BF01780466. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Urey HC, Failla G. Concerning the taste of heavy water. Science. 1935;81:273. doi: 10.1126/science.81.2098.273-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kirby RH, Pick DF, Riddick MS. Discrimination between heavy water and deionized water using gustation vs. olfaction in humans. Physiological Psychol. 1976;4:102–104. doi: 10.3758/BF03326557. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Richter CP. Study of taste and smell of heavy-water (99.8percent) in rats. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 1976;152:677–684. doi: 10.3181/00379727-152-39466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Miller IJ, Mooser G. Taste responses to deuterium-oxide. Physiol. Behav. 1979;23:69–74. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(79)90124-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Nelson G, et al. Mammalian sweet taste receptors. Cell. 2001;106:381–390. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(01)00451-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Li X, et al. Human receptors for sweet and umami taste. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2002;99:4692. doi: 10.1073/pnas.072090199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Di Pizio A, Niv MY. Computational studies of smell and taste receptors. Isr. J. Chem. 2014;54:1205–1218. doi: 10.1002/ijch.201400027. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Gutierrez, R., Fonseca, E. & Simon, S. A. The neuroscience of sugars in taste, gut-reward, feeding circuits, and obesity. Cell. Mol. Life Sci.77, 3469-3502 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 26.Damak S, et al. Detection of sweet and umami taste in the absence of taste receptor T1r3. Science. 2003;301:850–853. doi: 10.1126/science.1087155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yee KK, Sukumaran SK, Kotha R, Gilbertson TA, Margolskee RF. Glucose transporters and ATP-gated K+ (KATP) metabolic sensors are present in type 1 taste receptor 3 (T1r3)-expressing taste cells. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:5431–5436. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100495108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Bachmanov AA, Tordoff MG, Beauchamp GK. Sweetener preference of C57BL/6ByJ and 129P3/J mice. Chem. Senses. 2001;26:905–913. doi: 10.1093/chemse/26.7.905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jiang P, et al. Identification of the cyclamate interaction site within the transmembrane domain of the human sweet taste receptor subunit T1R3. J. Biol. Chem. 2005;280:34296–34305. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M505255200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Jiang P, et al. Lactisole interacts with the transmembrane domains of human T1R3 to inhibit sweet taste. J. Biol. Chem. 2005;280:15238–15246. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M414287200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Zhao GQ, et al. The receptors for mammalian sweet and umami taste. Cell. 2003;115:255–266. doi: 10.1016/S0092-8674(03)00844-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chaudhari N, Roper SD. The cell biology of taste. J. cell Biol. 2010;190:285–296. doi: 10.1083/jcb.201003144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Dagan-Wiener A, et al. BitterDB: taste ligands and receptors database in 2019. Nucleic Acids Res. 2019;47:D1179–D1185. doi: 10.1093/nar/gky974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Dagan-Wiener A, et al. Bitter or not? BitterPredict, a tool for predicting taste from chemical structure. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:12074. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12359-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cioni P, Strambini GB. Effect of heavy water on protein flexibility. Biophysical J. 2002;82:3246–3253. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(02)75666-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gane S, et al. Molecular vibration-sensing component in human olfaction. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e55780. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Block E, Jang S, Matsunami H, Batista VS, Zhuang H. Reply to Turin et al.: Vibrational theory of olfaction is implausible. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 2015;112:E3155. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1508443112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Vosshall LB. Laying a controversial smell theory to rest. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2015;112:6525. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1507103112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Low JYQ, McBride RL, Lacy KE, Keast RSJ. Psychophysical evaluation of sweetness functions across multiple sweeteners. Chem. Senses. 2017;42:111–120. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjw109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mennella JA, Reed DR, Mathew PS, Roberts KM, Mansfield CJ. “A spoonful of sugar helps the medicine go down”: bitter masking by sucrose among children and adults. Chem. Senses. 2014;40:17–25. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bju053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ben Abu N, Harries D, Voet H, Niv MY. The taste of KCl—What a difference a sugar makes. Food Chem. 2018;255:165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2018.01.175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Dubovski, N., Ert, E. & Niv, M. Bitter mouth-rinse affects emotions. Food Quality. Prefer. 60, 154–164 (2017).
- 43.Galindo-Cuspinera V, Winnig M, Bufe B, Meyerhof W, Breslin PAS. A TAS1R receptor-based explanation of sweet ‘water-taste’. Nature. 2006;441:354–357. doi: 10.1038/nature04765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Behrens M, Blank K, Meyerhof W. Blends of non-caloric sweeteners saccharin and cyclamate show reduced off-taste due to TAS2R bitter receptor inhibition. Cell Chem. Biol. 2017;24:1199–119. doi: 10.1016/j.chembiol.2017.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Trinquet E, et al. d-myo-Inositol 1-phosphate as a surrogate of d-myo-inositol 1,4,5-tris phosphate to monitor G protein-coupled receptor activation. Anal. Biochem. 2006;358:126–135. doi: 10.1016/j.ab.2006.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Zhang R, Xie X. Tools for GPCR drug discovery. Acta Pharmacol. Sin. 2012;33:372–384. doi: 10.1038/aps.2011.173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hussmann GP, Yasuda RP, Xiao Y, Wolfe BB, Kellar KJ. Endogenously expressed muscarinic receptors in HEK293 cells augment up-regulation of stably expressed α4β2 nicotinic receptors. J. Biol. Chem. 2011;286:39726–39737. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M111.289546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ueda T, Ugawa S, Yamamura H, Imaizumi Y, Shimada S. Functional interaction between T2R taste receptors and G-protein alpha subunits expressed in taste receptor cells. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:7376–7380. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-19-07376.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mason JS, Bortolato A, Congreve M, Marshall FH. New insights from structural biology into the druggability of G protein-coupled receptors. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 2012;33:249–260. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2012.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Venkatakrishnan AJ, et al. Diverse GPCRs exhibit conserved water networks for stabilization and activation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2019;116:3288–3293. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1809251116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Yang J, Zhang Y. I-TASSER server: new development for protein structure and function predictions. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43:W174–W181. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkv342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bayden AS, Moustakas DT, Joseph-McCarthy D, Lamb ML. Evaluating free energies of binding and conservation of crystallographic waters using SZMAP. J. Chem. Inf. Modeling. 2015;55:1552–1565. doi: 10.1021/ci500746d. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Galindo-Cuspinera V, Breslin PAS. The liaison of sweet and savory. Chem. Senses. 2005;31:221–225. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjj022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Sutton S. Encyclopedia of research design. Libr. J. 2010;135:105–106. [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kemp, S. E., Hollowood, T. & Hort, J. Sensory Evaluation: A Practical Handbook xi, 196p (Ames Wiley-Blackwell, 2009).
- 56.McLaughlin SK, McKinnon PJ, Spickofsky N, Danho W, Margolskee RF. Molecular-cloning of G-proteins and phosphodiesterases from rat taste cells. Physiol. Behav. 1994;56:1157–1164. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90360-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Kusakabe Y, et al. Comprehensive study on G protein alpha-subunits in taste bud cells, with special reference to the occurrence of G alpha i2 as a major G alpha species. Chem. Senses. 2000;25:525–531. doi: 10.1093/chemse/25.5.525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Sclafani A, Perez C. Cypha(TM) propionic acid, 2-(4-methoxyphenol) salt inhibits sweet taste in humans, but not in rats. Physiol. Behav. 1997;61:25–29. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00316-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ben Shoshan-Galeczki Y, Niv MY. Structure-based screening for discovery of sweet compounds. Food Chem. 2020;315:126286. doi: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2020.126286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Goddard TD, et al. UCSF ChimeraX: meeting modern challenges in visualization and analysis. Protein Sci. 2018;27:14–25. doi: 10.1002/pro.3235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data sets generated during the current study are available as Supplementary Data.