Skip to main content
Cerebral Cortex Communications logoLink to Cerebral Cortex Communications
. 2021 Jan 28;2(2):tgab005. doi: 10.1093/texcom/tgab005

Working Memory Capacities Neurally Dissociate: Evidence from Acute Stroke

Randi C Martin 1,, Junhua Ding 2, A Cris Hamilton 3, Tatiana T Schnur 4,5,
PMCID: PMC8030664  NIHMSID: NIHMS1669068  PMID: 33870195

Abstract

Substantial behavioral evidence implies the existence of separable working memory (WM) components for maintaining phonological and semantic information. In contrast, only a few studies have addressed the neural basis of phonological versus semantic WM using functional neuroimaging and none has used a lesion-symptom mapping (LSM) approach. Here, we address this gap, reporting a multivariate LSM study of phonological and semantic WM for 94 individuals at the acute stage of left hemisphere stroke. Testing at the acute stage avoids issues of brain reorganization and the adoption of patient strategies for task performance. The LSM analyses for each WM component controlled for the other WM component and semantic and phonological knowledge at the single word level. For phonological WM, the regions uncovered included the supramarginal gyrus, argued to be the site of phonological storage, and several cortical and subcortical regions plausibly related to inner rehearsal. For semantic WM, inferior frontal regions and the angular gyrus were uncovered. The findings thus provide converging evidence for separable systems for phonological and semantic WM that are distinguished from the systems supporting long-term knowledge representations in those domains.

Keywords: acute stroke, multivariate lesion-symptom mapping, phonological working memory, semantic working memory, support vector regression

Introduction

Models of working memory (WM) in the verbal domain typically focus on the maintenance of phonological information (Page and Norris 1998; Gupta and Tisdale 2009). For instance, the well-known WM model of Baddeley et al. (2020) includes a phonological loop component, which consists of a buffer for maintaining phonological information and a rehearsal process that keeps this information active. However, considerable behavioral evidence from healthy and brain damaged individuals supports a multicomponent view of verbal WM, with separate buffers for maintaining phonological and semantic information (Martin et al. 1999; Shivde and Anderson 2011; see Martin et al. 2020 for review). Figure 1 shows a depiction of a model of WM delineating this approach. On the left-hand side are long-term knowledge representations for words, including their phonological and semantic information, and on the right, separate buffers for maintaining semantic and phonological information. Both for phonological and semantic information, long-term knowledge representations on the left are activated and stored in limited capacity WM buffers on the right.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Model of working memory showing knowledge representations for words on the left and phonological and semantic working memory buffers on the right. (Based on Figure 1a from Martin et al. 2020.)

 Traditional views of memory associated phonological representations with short-term memory and semantic information with long-term memory (Baddeley 1966). However, many findings indicate an influence of long-term representations of phonology on immediate recall of words and nonwords—for example, better recall when the items are phonologically similar to more words in the language and a separate influence of individual phoneme frequency (Thorn and Frankish 2005). Thus, as in Figure 1, both phonological and semantic WM can be thought of involving the maintenance and use of long-term knowledge over the short term. , 

Consistent with the existence of separable WM capacities, the phonological and semantic components have been found to play different roles in sentence processing. The phonological component supports (1) verbatim sentence repetition (Saffran and Marin 1975; Martin et al. 1994) and (2) speech rate in narrative production (Martin and Schnur 2019). In contrast, the semantic component supports (1) the retention of word meanings prior to their integration across some distance during comprehension (e.g., maintaining the meaning of “cups” to integrate with the verb “cracked” in “Cups, vases, and mirrors cracked during the move”) and (2) the elaboration of phrases during sentence and narrative production (Martin and He 2004; Potter 2012; Tan and Martin 2018; Martin and Schnur 2019). In contrast to a wealth of behavioral data suggesting separable capacities, limited evidence from functional or structural neuroimaging is available regarding differential neural bases of semantic and phonological WM capacities. The present study addresses this issue by using a multivariate lesion-symptom mapping (LSM) approach (Zhang et al. 2014; Mirman et al. 2015; Yourganov et al. 2016; Lacey et al. 2017; Schumacher et al. 2019), relating performance on tasks designed to tap semantic and phonological WM to the brain regions damaged in 94 individuals at the acute stage of left hemisphere (LH) stroke. Testing at the acute stage provides important advantages in lesion localization, as it avoids misinterpretation related to reorganization of function and the adoption of task strategies by the participants. By examining the relationship between lesion location and WM performance, we demonstrate the brain regions required for phonological and semantic WM, thus providing evidence as to the neural independence of WM capacities. Below we summarize the current state of the literature regarding the brain basis of phonological and semantic WM.

Left Inferior Parietal Localization of Phonological WM

As with behavioral studies, studies of the neural basis of verbal WM have focused on phonological WM, typically using tasks tapping immediate memory for random digit or letter lists. Early findings using lesion overlap or functional neuroimaging supported the conclusion that phonological WM was supported by an inferior parietal region (with the greatest overlap in the supramarginal gyrus [SMG]; Shallice and Vallar 1990; Paulesu et al. 1993), which is separate from lateral superior temporal regions involved in the phonological processes underlying speech perception and word production (e.g., accessing the sounds associated with word meanings in order to understand or produce speech; Turkeltaub and Branch 2010; Price 2012).

 More recently, however, some authors have argued that phonological WM is inextricably linked to our long-term knowledge of phonology (Martin and Saffran 1997) and presented evidence that the same temporal lobe regions underlying phonological long-term knowledge support the temporary maintenance of phonological information (Leff et al. 2009; Ravizza et al. 2011). Such a view is consistent with embedded processes accounts of WM. Embedded processes accounts assume that, in general, WM consists of activated long-term knowledge representations together with a domain-general attentional system that brings some subset of activated information into the focus of attention (Cowan et al. 2020). In the phonological domain, various criticisms can be raised of these studies, which claim a dependence of WM on long-term phonological knowledge. In neuropsychological studies, some have claimed that the mechanisms that support phonological WM are the same as those that support phonological processes even for tasks with little or no WM demand (such as judging whether a stimulus is a word or nonword or accessing the meaning of a word from spoken input; Martin and Saffran 1997; Belleville et al. 2003). However, the tests of phonological processing often made some demand on phonological WM (e.g., syllable discrimination across a filled delay; Martin and Saffran 1997). Furthermore, Martin and Breedin (1992) demonstrated that individuals who were matched in having mild phonological processing deficits (e.g., scoring slightly below the control range in making word/nonword judgments to stimuli such as “pickle” and “bickle”) varied widely on phonological WM tasks requiring maintenance of digit lists (e.g., repeating back a list of 5 digits), from severely impaired to within the control range, supporting independence between processing and WM maintenance. In recent lesion overlap studies with large sample sizes, some studies have failed to control for patient speech perception abilities (Baldo et al. 2012; Ghaleh et al. 2019) or have factored out performance on tasks that arguably depend on phonological WM (Leff et al. 2009). Since phonological WM tests depend on speech perception, the degree of speech perception deficit should relate to patients’ WM performance. Furthermore, phonological WM should correlate with the degree of damage to superior temporal regions based on the contribution of speech perception to performance on the WM task. The Leff et al. (2009) study controlled for nonword repetition, which has been used as a measure of phonological WM capacity (e.g., Gathercole and Baddeley 1989), and thus its removal may have taken out a large portion of the variance due to phonological WM. Recently, in a large sample study of patients undergoing glioma resection, Pisoni et al. (2019) specifically contrasted phonological processing and WM regions and found only partial overlap between them, with damage to the SMG related only to WM. Thus, the neuropsychological evidence to date supporting a dependence of WM on long-term phonological knowledge is difficult to interpret, given the methodological confounds.

Criticisms can also be raised with respect to functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies claiming a reliance of phonological WM on phonological processing regions. One issue is that the speech perception regions have not typically been established in the same participants carrying out the phonological WM test, and sometimes the regions assumed to support phonological processing are remote from those indicated in meta-analyses of speech perception (e.g., Ravizza et al. 2011). Also, most studies have used visually rather than auditorily presented word lists (e.g., Ravizza et al. 2011; Lewis-Peacock et al. 2012). Although there is substantial evidence that visual verbal stimuli are recoded phonologically during WM tasks, one would expect more direct and consistent activation of phonological codes from auditory input. With visual input, participants may, at least on some trials, rely on memory for visual or orthographic features of the stimuli, reducing the sensitivity in detecting regions involved in phonological maintenance. Recently, Yue et al. (2018) carried out an fMRI study using a probe recognition task with auditorily presented lists of nonword stimuli (e.g., list: treb, plim, suke; probe: trem). They found that the SMG showed activation and WM load effects during a delay period between the list and the probe, whereas the superior temporal region identified for the same individuals as supporting speech perception did not. Moreover, using multivariate decoding methods (MVPA), phonological information could be decoded in the SMG irrespective of the classifier used, whereas in the superior temporal gyrus (STG), decoding was successful for only 1 classifier, and, moreover, decoding accuracy across individuals in the SMG correlated with their WM performance, whereas decoding accuracy in the STG did not. In sum, the differential evidence for similar or different regions involved in phonological long-term knowledge versus its maintenance may be the result of the use of tasks, which did not strongly require phonological WM, did not control for phonological input processing, or the assessment of phonological long-term knowledge using tasks that also required WM.

Left Inferior Frontal Involvement in Semantic WM

In comparison to phonological WM, relatively little is known about the neural basis of semantic WM. Substantial evidence indicates that long-term semantic knowledge is represented in bilateral middle and inferior temporal lobes (Mummery et al. 1999; Visser et al. 2012). With regard to semantic WM, Martin (2005) noted that individuals identified as having semantic WM deficits had lesions encompassing left inferior frontal regions, which distinguished them from those with disruptions of semantic knowledge per se (Mummery et al. 1999) and from those with phonological WM deficits and inferior parietal lesions (Shallice and Vallar 1990). These findings would again argue against an embedded processes approach to WM, given the different localization of the WM and long-term memory (LTM) regions for semantic WM. However, large sample studies of brain damaged individuals have not been carried out examining the regions involved in semantic WM maintenance, while controlling for semantic knowledge deficits.

In early fMRI studies with healthy subjects, Martin et al. (2003) and Shivde and Thompson-Schill (2004) contrasted performance on short-term memory probe tasks tapping WM for phonological (rhyme or vowel probe) versus semantic (synonym probe) information (e.g., subjects heard a list of 1 or 4 words and, after a short delay, answered whether a probe stimulus was similar in rhyme/vowel/or meaning with one of the list words). Both reported that left parietal regions were more activated for the phonological than the semantic task, whereas inferior/middle frontal regions were more activated for the semantic tasks. Hamilton et al. (2009) examined regions involved in maintaining word meanings prior to their integration in a task contrasting high and low demands on semantic WM. In this study, subjects judged whether adjectives could be sensibly integrated with a noun (e.g., green emerald vs. green sun). In the high WM demand condition, adjectives came before a noun (e.g., green, shining, bright emerald/sun), whereas in the low demand condition, the adjectives came after the noun (e.g., emerald/sun bright shining, green). The logic was that in the “before” condition, the meanings of the adjectives had to be maintained until the noun was processed, whereas in the “after” condition, each adjective could be integrated with the noun as it was perceived. Again, left inferior/middle frontal regions were more activated in the high than the low semantic WM demand condition. In an fMRI study using MVPA, Lewis-Peacock et al. (2012; Exp 2) found that semantic maintenance could be discriminated from phonological and visual maintenance in left anterior frontal and superior temporal regions. (It should be noted though that their stimuli were presented visually and the regions showing the greatest differentiation of phonological from semantic and visual maintenance were in bilateral occipital lobes, suggesting that participants may have relied on orthographic coding to complete the supposedly phonological task.) In sum, there are only limited findings regarding the neural basis of semantic WM and those that exist tend to suggest the involvement of a left inferior frontal region.

Complicating Factors: Additional Regions Involved in Phonological and Semantic WM

The above findings are consistent with the claim of a contrast between left frontal regions supporting semantic WM and left parietal regions supporting phonological WM. However, this claim is complicated by other findings indicating a role for left frontal regions in articulatory rehearsal and a role of a left parietal region (i.e., the angular gyrus [AG]) in semantic processes (Binder et al. 2009). With regard to rehearsal, a long-standing assumption has been that subvocal articulatory rehearsal is used to support the maintenance of phonological forms (Baddeley et al. 1975), and rehearsal is a major component of the phonological loop in Baddeley et al.’s (2020) model. In the Yue et al. (2018) neuroimaging study, left frontal regions (including the precentral gyrus, posterior inferior frontal gyrus [IFG], and supplementary motor area) showed a load effect during the delay period of the phonological WM task. Previous imaging studies have provided evidence that these regions are involved in either subvocal rehearsal or executive processes related to motor planning (Smith et al. 1998; Chein and Fiez 2001). In Yue et al. (2018), the putamen and cerebellum also showed load effects, and these regions are also likely involved in rehearsal, given their role in controlling motor aspects of speech production. With regard to left parietal regions’ involvement in semantic maintenance, many neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies have reported evidence that the AG plays an important role in semantic processing (Binder et al. 2009; Bemis and Pylkkänen 2012; Jefferies 2013; Price et al. 2015), with some of that evidence pointing to a role in semantic WM, given its contribution in integrating word meanings during phrase or sentence comprehension (Humphries et al. 2006; Price et al. 2015). Yue et al. (2018) found evidence supporting a role for the AG in semantic WM, as they showed that semantic representations could be decoded in the AG during the delay period of a semantic WM task, which involved judging the relatedness of word meanings across a delay.

Thus, there is strong evidence suggesting both a frontal–parietal dissociation for phonological versus semantic WM as well as the reverse. Although the frontal areas proposed to be involved in semantic WM (left inferior/middle frontal gyrus [MFG]) differ from those proposed to be involved in subvocal articulatory rehearsal (more posterior left inferior frontal, precentral gyrus, and supplementary motor areas; Smith et al. 1998), some of these areas overlap and are not clearly distinguished based on early fMRI results (e.g., left IFG involvement in rehearsal from Chein and Fiez 2001, and left IFG involvement in semantic WM from Shivde and Thompson-Schill 2004). Similarly, the left inferior SMG proposed to be involved in phonological WM is bordered by the left AG, which potentially plays a role in semantic WM, and prior neuroimaging studies have not directly contrasted the roles of these 2 neighboring regions in semantic versus phonological WM. Furthermore, because activation in a region revealed through functional neuroimaging approaches does not establish its necessity in processing (Rorden and Karnath 2004), it is important to have converging lesion-based data, where behavioral impairments following damage to a region strongly implicate its necessary role. Thus, the current study provides critical data through examining the neural basis of semantic and phonological WM in the same subjects using a multivariate LSM approach where we have lesion coverage for these regions to determine if they can truly be differentiated regarding the type of WM that they support.

Current Study

To address the distinctiveness of the brain regions involved in phonological and semantic WM, we used multivariate LSM to relate disruption of phonological and semantic WM to brain damage in acute LH stroke, while controlling for individuals’ single word phonological and semantic processing abilities. To test phonological WM, we used a digit matching span task, in which participants heard 2 lists of digits and decided whether they matched (Martin et al. 1994; Tan and Martin 2018). Digit lists were used as there is relatively little semantic information conveyed by random sets of digits. To test semantic WM, we used a category probe task in which participants judged whether a probe word was in the same semantic category as any list word (Martin et al. 1994; Tan and Martin 2018). Neither task required speech output, thus avoiding contributions of overt speech production deficits to WM performance. Both of these measures have been used in prior behavioral studies and have been found to relate to different aspects of language comprehension and production, as discussed earlier (Martin and He 2004). Although these measures tap into different capacities, prior results have shown a significant correlation between them (Tan et al. 2017; Tan and Martin 2018), which is unsurprising in that an ability to retain phonological information would help to support performance on the category probe task. That is, even if semantic representations of the to-be-remembered items had been lost by the time the probe was presented, a surviving phonological record could be used to re-access semantics. In the other direction, there is evidence for a boost from semantics, or at least familiarity, in digit span tasks, in that lists containing subsequences of digits that are more familiar (e.g., 1492) are better recalled (Jones and Macken 2015). Thus, to determine regions specific to one of the WM capacities, we factored out performance on the other WM task in our analyses. In order to control for participants’ speech perception and semantic knowledge, we also factored out performance on a picture–word matching task with semantically and phonologically related distractors (Martin and Schnur 2019). To our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the neural dissociation between phonological and semantic WM in a large group of persons with acute LH stroke, while accounting for previous confounds of phonological input processing, semantic knowledge, the covariation between measures of the 2 capacities, and reorganization of function.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 94 acute LH stroke patients (51 males; 81 right handed; 88 ischemic stroke; 6 hemorrhagic stroke; age: M = 61 years; standard deviation [SD] = 12 years; range = 25–85 years; education: M = 14 years; SD = 4 years; range = 0–33 years) were recruited from the Memorial Hermann, Houston Methodist, and St. Luke’s hospitals in Houston, TX, USA, as part of an ongoing project (Martin and Schnur 2019; Ding et al. 2020). Subjects met the following inclusion criteria: native English speaker, no other neurological diseases (e.g., tumor, dementia), no neuroradiological evidence of previous nonlacunar LH stroke (cf. Corbetta et al. 2015). Behavioral testing was completed within 1 week after stroke (7 subjects within 2 weeks; median = 3 days, range = 1–12 days). We recruited 13 nonbrain damaged participants as controls (3 males; 11 right handed; age: M = 55 years; SD = 14 years; range = 37–78 years; education: M = 16 years; SD = 3 years; range = 12–22 years) matched to patients on demographic variables (age: t(105) = −1.53, P = 0.13; education: t(86) = 1.66, P = 0.10; handedness: x2 = 0.02, P = 0.88). Informed consent was approved by the Baylor College of Medicine Institutional Review Board.

Behavioral Tests

Phonological WM

We measured phonological WM with the digit matching span task (Allen et al. 2012; Martin and Schnur 2019; 4 participants completed the digit span task using the standard Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised [WAIS-R] procedure; Wechsler 1981; Allen et al. 2012; Martin and Schnur 2019). With respect to digit matching span, participants first heard 2-digit lists, in which 1 digit was presented per second. Participants judged (yes or no) whether the 2 lists were the same or not. In the “non-match” trials, the second list reversed 1 pair of 2 adjacent digits (e.g., 5 3 1 8–5 1 3 8). This reversed position was randomized. List length increased from 2 to 6 digits, with 6, 8, 6, 8, and 10 trials per list length, respectively. Half of the trials matched, half did not. Different numbers of lists were used per list length such that the position of the reversal was approximately equal across serial positions within each list length, while keeping the number of lists low overall. We stopped testing when participants’ accuracy fell below 75% for a particular list length. We calculated the phonological WM span-dependent measure by linear interpolation between the accuracy of the 2 list lengths spanning 75% accuracy. For example, if the subject scored 7/8 (87.5%) correct at list length 3 and 4/6 (66.7%) correct at list length 4, then span would be 3 + ([87.5–75]/[87.5–66.7]) = 3.60. If accuracy for the 2-digit lists was <75%, we assumed 100% accuracy for a 1-item list length. If accuracy for the 6-digit lists was >75%, we assumed 50% accuracy for a 7-item list length.

Semantic WM

We measured semantic WM using the category probe task (Martin et al. 1994; Martin and Schnur 2019). Participants judged whether a spoken probe word was in the same semantic category as any word in a preceding spoken list. The categories were animals, body parts, clothing, fruit, and kitchen equipment. For example, for the list: bear dress apple leg and probe: pear, the answer would be “yes,” since pear and apple are in the same category (fruit). The matched position in lists was randomized. List length increased from 1 to 4 items, with 8, 8, 12, and 16 trials per list length, respectively. On half of the trials, the probe matched the category of one of the list items, whereas on the other half, it did not. The number of trials per list length varied such that the position of the matching word was approximately equal across serial positions within each list length, while keeping the number of trials low overall. We presented words 1 per second. We stopped task administration when participants’ accuracy was below 75% for a given list length. Scoring proceeded as for digit matching span.

Phonological and Semantic Input Processing

We measured the integrity of phonological and semantic input processing via a word picture matching task (Martin et al. 1999; Breese and Hillis 2004; Martin and Schnur 2019). We presented 17 pictures four times. Each picture was presented with an auditorily presented matching word, phonologically related foil, semantically related foil, and an unrelated foil, for a total of 68 trials distributed across 4 different presentation sets. Participants judged (yes/no, verbally or nonverbally) whether the picture and word represented the same object. We calculated phonological and semantic d’ scores to estimate participants’ ability to discriminate between matching trials and phonological and semantic foil trials. We used a composite measure of input processing because the phonological and semantic input processing scores were highly correlated (r = 0.69; P < 0.001). The composite measure was generated by a principal component analysis including phonological and semantic input processing d’ scores (explained variance = 85%; phonological input processing loading = 0.92; semantic input processing loading = 0.92).

Image Acquisition and Preprocessing

We identified participants’ lesions from diffusion weighted, T1, T2 FLAIR images (scanned in the axial direction) and computed tomography (CT) scans for those contraindicated for magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimaging was collected within 1.5 days after stroke (range = 0–8 days). The resolution of diffusion-weighted and T1/T2 images was 1 × 1 × 4.5 mm and 0.5 × 0.5 × 4.5 mm, respectively, and 0.5 × 0.5 × 5.0 mm for CT scans (n = 6 subjects).

To quantify patients’ lesions, we first registered diffusion-weighted images to T1/T2 images using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI; https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/). Then lesions were demarcated on the diffusion-weighted images using the Insight Toolkit-SNAP (ITK-SNAP; http://www.itksnap.org/pmwiki/pmwiki.php). Finally, we normalized T1/T2 and lesion images into the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space using Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs; https://stnava.github.io/ANTs/;  Avants et al. 2008). With regard to CT images, lesions were demarcated directly on the Colin-27 template while referring to CT images.

Multivariate LSM

To identify lesion location patterns associated with phonological and semantic WM, we conducted support vector regression (SVR; libsvm 3; https://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/∼cjlin/libsvm/) LSM using MATLAB 2018b (https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html). To control for potential confounding factors, we measured the relationship between brain damage location and either phonological or semantic WM performance independent of the contribution to performance from lesion size (cf. DeMarco and Turkeltaub 2018; Sperber and Karnath 2018), input processing deficits, and the opposing WM ability. Specifically, we calculated the residuals of phonological and semantic WM scores by regressing out lesion size (number of lesion mask voxels), the composite measure of input processing, and the other WM score. We controlled for the other WM score because the 2 WM measures were significantly correlated (r = 0.51; P < 0.001), and we wished to determine the relation to brain areas for the component of these measures specific to either semantics or phonology. We did not control for demographic variables (age, education, sex, handedness, and days tested poststroke) because correlations with the 2 WM scores were not significant after multiple comparison correction (Bonferroni corrected P > 0.27). In order to normalize dependent variables (Zhang et al. 2014), the residuals were further scaled ([value−min]/[max−min]) to fall within the continuous range of 0–1 (Hsu et al. 2003). We only included voxels lesioned in at least 5 (5%) of 94 patients. Voxels with the same lesion pattern across patients were combined as a patch (Pustina et al. 2018). We used all the patches to predict WM residuals. A grid search was conducted to select the optimal parameters (cost: 0.01–109 and gamma: 10−9–1000; the same range as scikit-learn: https://scikit-learn.org/stable/index.html) for radial basis function-kernel SVR (Mirman et al. 2015). For each parameter combination, we used 5-fold cross-validation to acquire its mean square error of real and predicted dependent scores. We used 1000 permutations (shuffling dependent scores) to acquire the parameter combination significance level (the rank of the real model’s mean square error in 1000 random models). The parameter pair with the lowest P value was considered the optimal combination, which we then used in subsequent analysis. Finally, patterns of brain region damage significantly related to WM performance were determined via permutation test (1000 times) using the optimal parameter pair model (Zhang et al. 2014). The significance level of each patch was the rank of its beta value in beta values of 1000 random models. Only negative beta values were of interest (i.e., our expected direction). We set the significance threshold to P < 0.05 for models and patches. We defined brain regions based on the brain connectome atlas (Fan et al. 2016). The brain map was generated by Resting State fMRI Data Analysis Toolkit (REST; http://www.restfmri.net/forum/;  Song et al. 2011). To note, consistent with previous literature and the brain connectome atlas’s gyral subdivisions, we subdivided the insula using an anatomical solution (i.e., anterior dorsal, anterior ventral, and posterior insula; Deen et al. 2010) as the brain connectome atlas’s division of the insula uses cytoarchitectonic nomenclature.

Results

Behavior

Compared with the nonbrain damaged participants, patients showed significant impairments on phonological WM (span length scores; controls: M = 6.12, SD = 0.50, range = 5.00–6.38; patients: M = 4.94, SD = 1.59, range = 1.37–6.50; t(55) = 5.48, P < 0.0001), semantic WM (span length scores; controls: M = 4.59, SD = 0.80, range = 3.35–5.45; patients: M = 2.16, SD = 1.38, range = 0.33–4.50; t(23) = 9.22, P < 0.0001), phonological input processing (d’ scores; controls: M = 3.70, SD = 0.19, range = 3.44–3.78; patients: M = 3.14, SD = 0.71, range = −0.15–3.78; t(92) = 6.69, P < 0.0001), and semantic input processing (d’ scores; controls: M = 3.18, SD = 0.39, range = 2.60–3.78; patients: M = 2.70, SD = 0.74, range = 0–3.78; t(105) =2.32, P = 0.02; see Fig. 2).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Patient and nonbrain damaged control behavioral results. From top to bottom: phonological working memory span; semantic working memory span; phonological input processing; semantic input processing.

Lesion Distribution

Figure 3A displays lesion coverage across patients (lesion size: M = 14 098 mm3, SD = 18 641 mm3, range = 135–104 243 mm3). The primary damaged regions (regions damaged in at least 5 subjects and with >100 lesioned voxels) included the middle and inferior frontal gyri, pre- and post-central gyri, superior temporal gyrus (STG) and sulcus, superior and inferior parietal lobules, insula, basal ganglia, and thalamus. Figure 4 shows correlations between regional proportion damage and the distances between the damaged regions. Figure 4 reveals lower correlations in proportion damage between remote regions, indicating feasibility to test for functional dissociations between remote regions (e.g., SMG/Brodmann area [BA] 39 vs. IFG/BA 44, 45). Moreover, not all adjacent damaged regions presented with higher correlations between proportion damage, indicating feasibility to test for functional dissociations between adjacent regions’ functions (e.g., SMG/BA 39 vs. AG/BA 40).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Lesion overlap distribution (A) and lesion-symptom mapping results (B and C). (A) Lesion overlap in 94 acute left hemisphere stroke subjects where only regions damaged in at least 5 subjects (>5%) were included in the lesion-symptom mapping analyses. B and C show the beta values of the regions significantly associated with decreased performance in the phonological WM (B) and semantic WM (C) measures after accounting for lesion volume, input processing (input processing composite score of semantically and phonologically related word–picture matching d’ scores), and the respective opposing working memory task (P’s < 0.05). p/sWM, phonological/semantic working memory.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Matrix of between-region damage correlations and distances. Increasing color intensity reflects either increasing correlations of proportion damage between regions (in red) or decreasing distance between regions (in green). Blue and yellow labels indicate the significant regions from the phonological and semantic WM LSM analyses, respectively. PrG, precentral gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; IFG, inferior frontal gyrus; INS, insula; PoG, postcentral gyrus; SPL, superior parietal lobe; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; pTL, posterior temporal lobe; STG, superior temporal gyrus; BG, basal ganglia; Tha, thalamus; BA, Brodmann area; IFJ, inferior frontal junction; IFS, inferior frontal sulcus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; Ca, caudate; GP, globus pallidus; NAC, nucleus accumbens; Pu, putamen; PM, premotor; S, sensory; PP, posterior parietal; O, occipital; PF, prefrontal; tl, tongue and larynx region; c, caudal; v, ventral; d, dorsal; l, lateral; m, medial; a, anterior; p, posterior; op, opercular.

Notably, the primary regions damaged (with n > 4) did not include lateral aspects of the STG thought to be involved in speech perception and the representation of phonetic features (Turkeltaub and Branch 2010; Mesgarani et al. 2014), nor did they include middle and inferior temporal regions thought to represent semantic knowledge (Mummery et al. 1999; Visser et al. 2012). According to an embedded processes view of WM (Cowan et al. 2020), persisting activation in such regions supports phonological and semantic WM. Regions beyond these would be those that underlie domain-general attentional processes. Thus, to the extent that our findings uncover regions specific to supporting either phonological or semantic WM, they would argue against the embedded processes view.

LSM

Regarding the LSM, both the phonological and semantic WM models were statistically significant (phonological WM: cost = 108, gamma = 10−8, P = 0.03; semantic WM: cost = 0.1, gamma = 0.001, P = 0.02). Figure 3 and Table 1 display the brain regions whose damage (region size >100 mm3) significantly related to either phonological (Fig. 3B) or semantic WM (Fig. 3C). Because the models controlled for the other WM measure, brain regions significantly related to phonological and semantic WM performance were nonoverlapping.

Table 1.

Lesion-symptom mapping results

Lobe Gyrus Anatomical and cytoarchitectonic descriptions Phonological WM (mm3) Semantic WM (mm3)
Frontal lobe Middle frontal gyrus Inferior frontal junction: junction of BAs 44 and 6/junction of inferior frontal sulcus and inferior precentral sulcus 436 1
Inferior frontal gyrus Opercular BA 44 (opercular inferior frontal gyrus) 0 233
Insular lobe Insular gyrus Anterior dorsal insula 32 237
Parietal lobe Inferior parietal lobule Rostrodorsal BA 40 (supramarginal gyrus) 1129 49
Rostroventral BA 39 (angular gyrus) 110 325
Rostroventral BA 40 181 88
Postcentral gyrus BA 1/2/3 (tongue and larynx region) 538 6
BA 2 230 0
Temporal lobe Superior temporal gyrus BA 41/42: primary auditory cortex 209 29
Posterior superior temporal sulcus Caudoposterior superior temporal sulcus (BA 21, 37) 0 140
Subcortical nuclei Basal ganglia Ventral caudate 168 0
Globus pallidus 102 0
Ventromedial putamen 377 0
Dorsal caudate 333 0
Dorsolateral putamen 137 0
Thalamus Occipital thalamus 104 0
Lateral prefrontal thalamus 187 9

Notes: Phonological and semantic WM lesion-symptom mapping results of regions with size >100 mm3 significantly associated with these tasks in their respective lesion-symptom mapping analyses (P’s < 0.05). Lesion-symptom mapping analyses included regions damaged in at least 5 subjects (>5%).

With respect to phonological WM, the region of the largest size and with the greatest difference between phonological and semantic WM was in the rostrodorsal BA 40 (with suggestion of extension rostroventrally, i.e., SMG; see Table 1), in line with prior findings (Shallice and Vallar 1990; Paulesu et al. 1993). Interestingly, while a region in the STG related to phonological WM, the region was not in lateral aspects of the STG (BA 21, 22) related to speech perception, but instead in primary auditory cortex (BA 41, 42). The other cortical regions specific to phonological WM are all plausibly related to rehearsal, including the inferior frontal junction (junction of BAs 44 and 6, a region joining the inferior frontal sulcus [between the IFG and MFG] and the inferior precentral sulcus), and the postcentral gyrus (BAs 1, 2, 3), which includes tongue and larynx regions. A number of areas (basal ganglia and thalamus) were also found, all of which have been implicated in articulatory aspects of speech production (Bohland and Guenther 2006).

With respect to semantic WM, a region in a somewhat more anterior part of the left IFG (opercular region of BA44) was obtained relative to that for phonological WM. A temporal lobe region bordering the posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) (BA 21, 37) was found, which is often implicated in the mapping from phonology to semantics (Okada and Hickok 2006; Wilson et al. 2018). As predicted, an inferior parietal region was obtained in BA39 (AG), which was thus differentiated from the inferior parietal SMG region obtained for phonological WM. The only other region related to semantic WM was the anterior dorsal aspect of the insula. Although there is controversy regarding the functional roles of different subregions of the insula, some evidence suggests that anterior dorsal regions are involved in more cognitive processes, including semantic processing (Deen et al. 2010; Ardila et al. 2014).

Discussion

We examined whether damage to different brain regions caused phonological and semantic WM deficits in a large group of participants during the acute stage of stroke. We avoided the confound of reorganization of function while also controlling for the degree of phonological input processing and access to semantic knowledge. Multivariate lesion LSM revealed distinct regions underlying phonological and semantic WM. Although damage to both frontal and parietal lobules reduced WM capacity, the specific regions necessary for each WM capacity differed and did not include regions supporting phonological or semantic long-term knowledge. These results are consistent with a multicomponent view of WM, where functionally and anatomically distinct buffers maintain phonological and semantic information independent from the long-term memory of domain-specific representations.

Phonological WM

The region with the largest damage associated with reduced phonological WM in comparison with semantic WM was the SMG in the inferior parietal lobe. Based on neuroimaging and neuropsychological findings, this SMG region has often been postulated to support the storage of phonological information (Paulesu et al. 1993; Yue et al. 2018; Shallice and Papagno 2019). Other frontal and subcortical regions related to phonological WM were, for the most part, regions carrying out articulatory and motor planning processes, most likely due to their involvement in subvocal rehearsal. Two frontal regions often ascribed to a role in motor planning in functional neuroimaging studies (Chein and Fiez 2001) were also observed here (supplementary motor area, posterior IFG). In addition, several subcortical regions were found, all of which have been implicated in motor planning and motor control (Crosson et al. 2004; Bohland and Guenther 2006). A few prior LSM studies have reported the involvement of subcortical regions (caudate in Leff et al. 2009 and caudate and putamen in Ivanova et al. 2018), but attributed their role to supporting executive functions involved in short-term memory performance. Although we cannot rule out this possibility, both regions have also been found to be involved in simple tasks with little executive function demand involving the production of syllable sequences (Bohland and Guenther 2006). The current study uncovered a number of other subcortical regions not reported in other studies. It is possible that our research was able to detect the involvement of these regions due to testing at the acute stage, whereas other studies have been carried out at the chronic stage (Leff et al. 2009; Baldo et al. 2012; Ivanova et al. 2018; Ghaleh et al. 2019). There is evidence that individuals with subcortical lesions are likely to recover language functions (Démonet 1997; Heiss et al. 1999), thus limiting the ability to detect the involvement of these regions in supporting phonological WM in a chronic sample.

Interestingly, reduced phonological WM capacity was also related to damage to sensory regions: (1) a somatosensory region representing the tongue and larynx and (2) primary auditory areas (Heschl’s gyrus; BAs 41, 42). If an fMRI study of WM revealed these activations, one might suppose that these were the consequence of implicit (or perhaps explicit) motor execution. Thus, one might hypothesize that these regions might be activated during rehearsal, but were not necessary for it. However, in the LSM framework, the association of damage to these regions to reduced WM capacity suggests a necessary role. Such a necessary role might be accommodated on the grounds that a somatosensory target is needed to guide motor movements (or, in this case, covert motor movements involved in rehearsal), a proposal that is consistent with the model of Walker and Hickok (2016), which bridges speech production and motor control models. The assumption of a role of somatosensory targets is common in motor theories in the visual–motor domain, supported by a range of findings from humans and nonhuman animals, and has more recently been extended to speech production (see Hickok 2012 for a review). In contrast, that damage to primary auditory regions reduced phonological WM capacity was somewhat more unexpected. A possible but unlikely explanation is that damage to such regions impaired speech perception, which played a greater role in phonological than semantic WM. However, it is unclear why that should be the case, since one might have expected the small set of digits to be more easily recognized than the words in the category probe task, which varied from trial to trial. More critically, the multivariate LSM analyses controlled for speech perception abilities, by factoring out performance on phonologically related trials in picture–word matching. Instead, we speculate that the involvement of primary auditory regions might be consistent with theories postulating that a match between articulatory and anticipated auditory targets is also used in modulating motor control (Hickok 2012). Overall, for phonological WM, the set of regions hang together as a network plausibly involved in phonological storage and covert rehearsal processes requiring motor planning and execution, though some questions remain regarding the role of the observed somatosensory and primary auditory regions.

Semantic WM

For semantic WM, damage to a smaller number of regions was associated with deficits. Previous neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings had suggested a critical role for the left inferior/middle frontal region. Although an inferior frontal and insular region was obtained, it was somewhat more posterior than anticipated based on prior fMRI results of semantic WM (Martin et al. 2003; Hamilton et al. 2009). The region that was uncovered (opercular left IFG, BA44) has been implicated in some studies as being involved in semantic selection (Martin and Chao 2001; Heim et al. 2009) and in other studies as playing a role in a semantic maintenance process termed “refreshing” (Johnson et al. 2005; see subsequent section for further discussion). Lesion coverage was not extensive in more anterior aspects of the left inferior frontal and middle frontal gyri, thus limiting our ability to detect the involvement of more dorsal or anterior regions. In posterior areas, a parietal region in the AG (BA 39) and a pSTS region (BA21, 37) were obtained. Necessity of the AG for semantic WM is consistent with considerable neuroimaging evidence of its role in semantic processing and, specifically, in semantic integration—that is, the integration of word meanings (Humphries et al. 2006; Binder et al. 2009; Bemis and Pylkkänen 2012). Semantic integration (e.g., integrating noun–noun, “apple core,” and verb–noun, “throw dart,” combinations) would seem to necessarily draw on semantic WM to maintain the 2 concepts such that an appropriate integration could be carried out. Thus, we are suggesting that the same capacity that supports the maintenance of semantic representations for unrelated words in word lists is drawn upon to hold the meanings of words in sentences prior to their integration. With respect to the pSTS, damage to a similar pSTS locus after acute stroke in a subset of patients studied here was associated with reduced ability to produce nouns and increasingly complex word combinations during a narrative production task (Ding et al. 2020). In a behavioral analysis of another subset of these patients, semantic WM was necessary for producing increasingly complex word combinations (Martin and Schnur 2019). Elsewhere, a recent fMRI study investigating spoken and written narrative comprehension in nonbrain damaged individuals (Wilson et al. 2018) revealed the involvement of specific subregions of the pSTS in phonological, lexical, and semantic processes. Based on their findings, they argued that the dorsal posterior region of the superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) represented phonological and orthographic lexical representations, whereas the ventral posterior region supported higher level language processes involved in semantic and syntactic processing. Thus, one might postulate that the pSTS region uncovered in the semantic WM task is required either for linking lexical phonological representations with semantic representations or with maintaining word meanings during phrase integration in comprehension and in holding several word meanings during phrase construction in production.

Lambon Ralph et al. (2017) have argued for a semantic control system encompassing frontal and parietal regions that is engaged in accessing and manipulating semantic representations. Thus, one might question the extent to which the regions we uncovered map onto the regions they suggest support semantic control. According to a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on semantic control (Noonan et al. 2013), the region closest to our pSTS region was a posterior middle temporal gyrus region; however, the peak activation point was more ventral and posterior than our pSTS region, with no apparent overlap of their cluster and our region. Similarly, with regard to our regions in the AG, their AG region was much more medial and superior to our AG regions. In frontal regions, they uncovered a very large region encompassing most middle and inferior frontal gyrus regions, and thus it would be difficult for our region not to overlap theirs. We would also note, however, that behavioral work has called into question the basis for claims about a distinct semantic control system (Allen et al. 2012; Chapman et al. 2020).

Thus, in general, our results converged with prior findings implicating the left IFG (Martin et al. 2003; Shivde and Thompson-Schill 2004; Lewis-Peacock et al. 2012) and the AG (Humphries et al. 2006; Yue et al. 2018) in semantic WM. Although the current findings cannot distinguish the role of these 2 regions, other findings in the literature suggest that the left IFG region is involved in retrieving or refreshing semantic representations (Martin and Chao 2001; Johnson et al. 2005; Heim et al. 2009), whereas the AG is involved more directly in maintaining semantic representations to support meaning integration (Humphries et al. 2006). The results showing the involvement of the pSTS are more novel, but may underlie the maintenance of several word meanings during complex phrase construction (Ding et al. 2020). Further investigation is needed to determine if the engagement of this region replicates and what its distinctive role might be.

Rehearsal Versus Refreshing

As discussed previously, several regions involved in motor control and articulatory planning were found to support phonological WM performance, likely due to their involvement in inner rehearsal. Given that the LSM analyses controlled for the other WM component, these results imply that articulatory rehearsal processes were more important for performance on the phonological than the semantic WM task. In the behavioral literature on healthy adults, a separate process, termed refreshing, has been argued to keep semantic representations in mind (Loaiza and Camos 2018; Nishiyama 2018). Refreshing is held to be a process by which recently activated representations are “rethought” and thereby given a boost of activation, with evidence suggesting independence of refreshing from both articulatory rehearsal and retrieval from long-term memory (Loaiza and Camos 2018). Neuroimaging studies have suggested that left middle frontal regions are involved in refreshing semantic representations for words, though, in some experiments, BA44 and the insula were also involved (Johnson et al. 2005), coinciding with the left frontal regions found here. Thus, our findings are consistent with claims of different maintenance mechanisms for phonological and semantic WM.

Relation to Buffer Versus Embedded Process Theories of WM

As discussed in the Introduction, some have argued that there are no dedicated regions for maintaining different types of representations in WM. According to the embedded processes view, persisting activations in regions devoted to long-term memory representations in that domain are thought to underlie WM (Cowan et al. 2020). Our findings for both phonological and semantic WM argue against this view. In the phonological domain, the embedded processes view would lead one to predict that lateral aspects of the STG (BA22), which have been found to underlie speech perception and the representation of sublexical phonological codes, would be critical to phonological WM. However, in our sample, there were very few individuals with damage to such regions. Thus, while the present results cannot refute the possibility that lateral STG is a necessary component of the network involved in phonological WM, it is clear that it is not sufficient, as many of our patients had substantially impaired phonological WM capacity resulting from damage to regions elsewhere. In particular, a large region in the inferior parietal lobe in the SMG was found to be related to phonological WM, consistent with prior studies, implicating this region as a phonological store (Deschamps et al. 2014; Yue et al. 2018). Most other areas obtained were plausibly related to covert rehearsal (Chein and Fiez 2001; Bohland and Guenther 2006), a process specific to maintaining phonological information in WM (Loaiza and Camos 2018; Baddeley et al. 2020). Although 2 sensory areas were obtained, these regions might also be engaged as part of the motor rehearsal process in providing sensory targets used to assess motor accuracy (Guenther et al. 2006; Hickok 2012; Walker and Hickok 2016).

In the semantic domain, we also observed varying degrees of impairment in semantic WM, even though our patients did not have damage to middle or inferior temporal regions thought to house long-term semantic representations for objects or to provide a semantic hub for linking together different aspects of concepts (Visser et al. 2012; Mesulam et al. 2015). Instead, the regions uncovered included frontal (BA44) and parietal regions (BA39), which are plausibly involved in retrieving, maintaining, and integrating semantic information (Humphries et al. 2006; Heim et al. 2009).

Conclusions

In summary, this study uncovered distinct regions involved in phonological and semantic WM, while controlling for phonological and semantic knowledge. Most regions that were identified in both domains were separate from regions postulated to be involved in regions representing long-term knowledge of phonology or semantics. Moreover, these results were obtained in a large sample of individuals at the acute stage of stroke, thus ruling out confounds due to reorganization of function. The organization of the regions involved in phonological WM seems fairly clear, consisting of regions involved in phonological storage and motor processes involved in rehearsal (including regions representing sensorimotor targets for articulation). This is the first LSM study of semantic WM, and while distinct frontal and parietal regions were uncovered, future work will be needed to elucidate how these regions interact in supporting semantic WM.

Footnotes

1

Traditional views of memory associated phonological representations with short-term memory and semantic information with long-term memory (Baddeley 1966). However, many findings indicate an influence of long-term representations of phonology on immediate recall of words and nonwords—for example, better recall when the items are phonologically similar to more words in the language and a separate influence of individual phoneme frequency (Thorn and Frankish 2005). Thus, as in Figure 1, both phonological and semantic WM can be thought of involving the use of long-term knowledge over the short term.

2

These studies showed a preservation of gist rather than exact wording in sentence repetition for patients with a phonological WM deficit (Martin et al. 1994) or an auditory–verbal short-term memory deficit (Saffran and Marin 1975). Although the terminology for their deficits was different, it seems highly likely that both patients had similar WM deficits, as they overlapped on several features, including fluent speech and better WM performance with visual than auditory presentation for word list recall. The preservation of gist implies that despite their WM deficits, they could rapidly derive the meaning of sentences and use that meaning to reconstruct the sentence when the phonological code was no longer available to support recall.

3

The embedded processes account shares some similarities to the earlier levels of processing approach of Craik and Lockhart (1972), in that both emphasize processes rather than storage buffers for WM for maintaining information in mind. However, the embedded processes account does not ascribe to notions of depth of processing determining long-term recall, instead attributing findings of better long-term recall with semantic processing to the relative degree of interference among semantic versus among phonological representations (Cowan et al. 2020). Of more relevance to the issues described here, both views would lead to the prediction that the same brain regions involved in long-term memory representations and processing in a given domain should support WM in that domain.

4

Lambon Ralph et al. (2017) have argued for a semantic control system encompassing frontal and parietal regions that is engaged in accessing and manipulating semantic representations. Thus, one might question the extent to which the regions we uncovered map onto the regions they suggest support semantic control. According to a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on semantic control (Noonan et al. 2013), the region closest to our pSTS region was a posterior middle temporal gyrus region; however, the peak activation point was more ventral and posterior than our pSTS region, with no apparent overlap of their cluster and our region. Similarly, with regard to our regions in the AG, their AG region was much more medial and superior to our AG regions. In frontal regions, they uncovered a very large region encompassing most middle and inferior frontal gyrus regions, and thus it would be difficult for our region not to overlap theirs. We would also note, however, that behavioral work has called into question the basis for claims about a distinct semantic control system (Allen et al. 2012; Chapman et al. 2020).

Contributor Information

Randi C Martin, Department of Psychological Sciences, Rice University, Houston, TX 77251, USA.

Junhua Ding, Department of Neurosurgery, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA.

A Cris Hamilton, Department of Institution Reporting, Research and Information Systems, University of Texas at Austin, Austin 78701, TX, USA.

Tatiana T Schnur, Department of Neurosurgery, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA; Department of Neuroscience, Baylor College of Medicine, Houston, TX 77030, USA.

Funding

This work was supported by an award to the Baylor College of Medicine from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders (R01DC014976).

Notes

The authors wish to thank Chia-Ming Lei, Danielle Rossi, and Miranda Brenneman for data collection. The authors gratefully acknowledge and thank their research subjects and their caregivers for their willingness to participate in this research. This work was presented at the Academy of Aphasia (2020).

Conflict of Interest: None declared.

References

  1. Allen  CM, Martin  RC, Martin  N. 2012. Relations between short-term memory deficits, semantic processing, and executive function. Aphasiology. 26:428–461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ardila  A, Bernal  B, Rosselli  M. 2014. Participation of the insula in language revisited: a meta-analytic connectivity study. J Neurol. 29:31–41. [Google Scholar]
  3. Avants  BB, Epstein  CL, Grossman  M, Gee  JC. 2008. Symmetric diffeomorphic image registration with cross-correlation: evaluating automated labeling of elderly and neurodegenerative brain. Med Image Anal. 12:26–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baddeley  AD. 1966. The influence of acoustic and semantic similarity on long-term memory for word sequences. Q J Exp Psychol. 18:302–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley  AD, Hitch  GJ, Allen  A. 2020. A multicomponent model of working memory. In: Logie  RH, Camos  V, Cowan  N, editors. Working memory: state of the science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Baddeley  AD, Thomson  N, Buchanan  M. 1975. Word length and the structure of short-term memory. J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 14:575–589. [Google Scholar]
  7. Baldo  JV, Katseff  S, Dronkers  NF. 2012. Brain regions underlying repetition and auditory-verbal short-term memory deficits in aphasia: evidence from voxel-based lesion symptom mapping. Aphasiology. 26:338–354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Belleville  S, Caza  N, Peretz  I. 2003. A neuropsychological argument for a processing view of memory. J Mem Lang. 48:686–703. [Google Scholar]
  9. Bemis  DK, Pylkkänen  L. 2012. Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cereb Cortex. 23:1859–1873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Binder  JR, Desai  RH, Graves  WW, Conant  LL. 2009. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. 19:2767–2796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bohland  JW, Guenther  FH. 2006. An fMRI investigation of syllable sequence production. Neuroimage. 32:821–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Breese  EL, Hillis  AE. 2004. Auditory comprehension: is multiple choice really good enough?  Brain Lang. 89:3–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Chapman  CA, Hasan  O, Schulz  PE, Martin  RC. 2020. Evaluating the distinction between semantic knowledge and semantic access: evidence from semantic dementia and comprehension-impaired stroke aphasia. Psychon Bull Rev. 27:607–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chein  JM, Fiez  JA. 2001. Dissociation of verbal working memory system components using a delayed serial recall task. Cereb Cortex. 11:1003–1014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Corbetta  M, Ramsey  L, Callejas  A, Baldassarre  A, Hacker Carl  D, Siegel Joshua  S, Astafiev Serguei  V, Rengachary  J, Zinn  K, Lang Catherine  E  et al.  2015. Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron. 85:927–941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Cowan  N, Morey  CC, Naveh-Benjamin  N. 2020. An embedded-processes approach to working memory: how is it distinct from other approaches, and to what ends? In: Logie  RH, Camos  V, Cowan  N, editors. Working memory: state of the science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  17. Crosson  B, Benefield  H, Cato  MA, Sadek  JR, Moore  AB, Wierenga  CE, Gopinath  K, Soltysik  D, Bauer  RM, Auerbach  EJ  et al.  2004. Left and right basal ganglia and frontal activity during language generation: contributions to lexical, semantic, and phonological processes. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 9:1061–1077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Deen  B, Pitskel  NB, Pelphrey  KA. 2010. Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis. Cereb Cortex. 21:1498–1506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. DeMarco  AT, Turkeltaub  PE. 2018. A multivariate lesion symptom mapping toolbox and examination of lesion-volume biases and correction methods in lesion-symptom mapping. Hum Brain Mapp. 39:4169–4182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Démonet  JF. 1997. Subcortical aphasia(s): a controversial and promising topic. Brain Lang. 58:410–417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Deschamps  I, Baum  SR, Gracco  VL. 2014. On the role of the supramarginal gyrus in phonological processing and verbal working memory: evidence from rTMS studies. Neuropsychologia. 53:39–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ding  J, Martin  RC, Hamilton  AC, Schnur  TT. 2020. Dissociation between frontal and temporal-parietal contributions to connected speech in acute stroke. Brain. 143:862–876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fan  L, Li  H, Zhuo  J, Zhang  Y, Wang  J, Chen  L, Yang  Z, Chu  C, Xie  S, Laird  AR  et al.  2016. The human brainnetome atlas: a new brain atlas based on connectional architecture. Cereb Cortex. 26:3508–3526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gathercole  SE, Baddeley  AD. 1989. Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: a longitudinal study. J Mem Lang. 28:200–213. [Google Scholar]
  25. Ghaleh  M, Lacey  EH, Fama  ME, Anbari  Z, DeMarco  AT, Turkeltaub  PE. 2019. Dissociable mechanisms of verbal working memory revealed through multivariate lesion mapping. Cereb Cortex. 30:2542–2554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Guenther  FH, Ghosh  SS, Tourville  JA. 2006. Neural modeling and imaging of the cortical interactions underlying syllable production. Brain Lang. 96:280–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gupta  P, Tisdale  J. 2009. Does phonological short-term memory causally determine vocabulary learning? Toward a computational resolution of the debate. J Mem Lang. 61:481–502. [Google Scholar]
  28. Hamilton  AC, Martin  RC, Burton  PC. 2009. Converging functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a role of the left inferior frontal lobe in semantic retention during language comprehension. Cogn Neuropsychol. 26:685–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Heim  S, Eickhoff  SB, Friederici  AD, Amunts  K. 2009. Left cytoarchitectonic area 44 supports selection in the mental lexicon during language production. Brain Struct Funct. 213:441–456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Heiss  W-D, Kessler  J, Thiel  A, Ghaemi  M, Karbe  H. 1999. Differential capacity of left and right hemispheric areas for compensation of poststroke aphasia. Ann Neurol. 45:430–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hickok  G. 2012. Computational neuroanatomy of speech production. Nat Rev Neurosci. 13:135–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hsu  C-W, Chang  C-C, Lin  C-J. 2003. A Practical Guide to Support Vector Classification. Available from: https://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/∼cjlin/papers/guide/guide.pdf.
  33. Humphries  C, Binder  JR, Medler  DA, Liebenthal  E. 2006. Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. J Cogn Neurosci. 18:665–679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ivanova  MV, Dragoy  O, Kuptsova  SV, Yu Akinina  S, Petrushevskii  AG, Fedina  ON, Turken  A, Shklovsky  VM, Dronkers  NF. 2018. Neural mechanisms of two different verbal working memory tasks: a VLSM study. Neuropsychologia. 115:25–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jefferies  E. 2013. The neural basis of semantic cognition: converging evidence from neuropsychology, neuroimaging and TMS. Cortex. 49:611–625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Johnson  MK, Raye  CL, Mitchell  KJ, Greene  EJ, Cunningham  WA, Sanislow  CA. 2005. Using fMRI to investigate a component process of reflection: prefrontal correlates of refreshing a just-activated representation. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 5:339–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Jones  G, Macken  B. 2015. Questioning short-term memory and its measurement: why digit span measures long-term associative learning. Cognition. 144:1–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lacey  EH, Skipper-Kallal  LM, Xing  S, Fama  ME, Turkeltaub  PE. 2017. Mapping common aphasia assessments to underlying cognitive processes and their neural substrates. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 31:442–450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lambon Ralph  MA, Jefferies  E, Patterson  K, Rogers  TT. 2017. The neural and computational bases of semantic cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 18:42–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Leff  AP, Schofield  TM, Crinion  JT, Seghier  ML, Grogan  A, Green  DW, Price  CJ. 2009. The left superior temporal gyrus is a shared substrate for auditory short-term memory and speech comprehension: evidence from 210 patients with stroke. Brain. 132:3401–3410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lewis-Peacock  JA, Drysdale  AT, Oberauer  K, Postle  BR. 2012. Neural evidence for a distinction between short-term memory and the focus of attention. J Cogn Neurosci. 24:61–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Loaiza  VM, Camos  V. 2018. The role of semantic representations in verbal working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 44:863–881. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Martin  A, Chao  LL. 2001. Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 11:194–201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Martin  N, Saffran  EM. 1997. Language and auditory-verbal short-term memory impairments: evidence for common underlying processes. Cogn Neuropsychol. 14:641–682. [Google Scholar]
  45. Martin  RC. 2005. Components of short-term memory and their relation to language processing: evidence from neuropsychology and neuroimaging. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 14:204–208. [Google Scholar]
  46. Martin  RC, Breedin  SD. 1992. Dissociations between speech perception and phonological short-term memory deficits. Cogn Neuropsychol. 9:509–534. [Google Scholar]
  47. Martin  RC, He  T. 2004. Semantic short-term memory and its role in sentence processing: a replication. Brain Lang. 89:76–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Martin  RC, Lesch  MF, Bartha  MC. 1999. Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. J Mem Lang. 41:3–29. [Google Scholar]
  49. Martin  RC, Purcell  J, Rapp  B. 2020. Domain-specific working memory: perspectives from cognitive neuropsychology. In: Logie  RH, Camos  V, Cowan  N, editors. Working memory: state of the science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  50. Martin  RC, Schnur  TT. 2019. Independent contributions of semantic and phonological working memory to spontaneous speech in acute stroke. Cortex. 112:58–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Martin  RC, Shelton  JR, Yaffee  LS. 1994. Language processing and working memory: neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. J Mem Lang. 33:83–111. [Google Scholar]
  52. Martin  RC, Wu  D, Freedman  M, Jackson  EF, Lesch  M. 2003. An event-related fMRI investigation of phonological versus semantic short-term memory. J Neurol. 16:341–360. [Google Scholar]
  53. Mesgarani  N, Cheung  C, Johnson  K, Chang  EF. 2014. Phonetic feature encoding in human superior temporal gyrus. Science. 343:1006–1010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Mesulam  M-M, Thompson  CK, Weintraub  S, Rogalski  EJ. 2015. The Wernicke conundrum and the anatomy of language comprehension in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 138:2423–2437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Mirman  D, Zhang  Y, Wang  Z, Coslett  HB, Schwartz  MF. 2015. The ins and outs of meaning: behavioral and neuroanatomical dissociation of semantically-driven word retrieval and multimodal semantic recognition in aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 76:208–219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mummery  CJ, Patterson  K, Wise  RJS, Vandenbergh  R, Price  CJ, Hodges  JR. 1999. Disrupted temporal lobe connections in semantic dementia. Brain. 122:61–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nishiyama  R. 2018. Separability of active semantic and phonological maintenance in verbal working memory. PLoS One. 13:e0193808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Noonan  KA, Jefferies  E, Visser  M, Lambon Ralph  MA. 2013. Going beyond inferior prefrontal involvement in semantic control: evidence for the additional contribution of dorsal angular gyrus and posterior middle temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci. 25:1824–1850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Okada  K, Hickok  G. 2006. Identification of lexical–phonological networks in the superior temporal sulcus using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport. 17:1293–1296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Page  MP, Norris  D. 1998. The primacy model: a new model of immediate serial recall. Psychol Rev. 105:761–781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Paulesu  E, Frith  CD, Frackowiak  RS. 1993. The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature. 362:342–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Pisoni  A, Mattavelli  G, Casarotti  A, Comi  A, Riva  M, Bello  L, Papagno  C. 2019. The neural correlates of auditory-verbal short-term memory: a voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study on 103 patients after glioma removal. Brain Struct Funct. 224:2199–2211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Potter  M. 2012. Conceptual short term memory in perception and thought. Front Psychol. 3:113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Price  AR, Bonner  MF, Peelle  JE, Grossman  M. 2015. Converging evidence for the neuroanatomic basis of combinatorial semantics in the angular gyrus. J Neurosci. 35:3276–3284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Price  CJ. 2012. A review and synthesis of the first 20 years of PET and fMRI studies of heard speech, spoken language and reading. Neuroimage. 62:816–847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Pustina  D, Avants  B, Faseyitan  OK, Medaglia  JD, Coslett  HB. 2018. Improved accuracy of lesion to symptom mapping with multivariate sparse canonical correlations. Neuropsychologia. 115:154–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Ravizza  SM, Hazeltine  E, Ruiz  S, Zhu  DC. 2011. Left TPJ activity in verbal working memory: implications for storage- and sensory-specific models of short term memory. Neuroimage. 55:1836–1846. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Rorden  C, Karnath  H-O. 2004. Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age?  Nat Rev Neurosci. 5:812–819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Saffran  EM, Marin  OSM. 1975. Immediate memory for word lists and sentences in a patient with deficient auditory short-term memory. Brain Lang. 2:420–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Schumacher  R, Halai  AD, Lambon Ralph  MA. 2019. Assessing and mapping language, attention and executive multidimensional deficits in stroke aphasia. Brain. 142:3202–3216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Shallice  T, Papagno  C. 2019. Impairments of auditory-verbal short-term memory: do selective deficits of the input phonological buffer exist?  Cortex. 112:107–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Shallice  T, Vallar  G. 1990. The impairment of auditory-verbal short-term storage. In: Vallar  G, Shallice  T, editors. Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  73. Shivde  G, Anderson  MC. 2011. On the existence of semantic working memory: evidence for direct semantic maintenance. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 37:1342–1370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Shivde  G, Thompson-Schill  SL. 2004. Dissociating semantic and phonological maintenance using fMRI. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 4:10–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Smith  EE, Jonides  J, Marshuetz  C, Koeppe  RA. 1998. Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 95:876–882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Song  X-W, Dong  Z-Y, Long  X-Y, Li  S-F, Zuo  X-N, Zhu  C-Z, He  Y, Yan  C-G, Zang  Y-F. 2011. REST: a toolkit for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data processing. PLoS One. 6:e25031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Sperber  C, Karnath  H-O. 2018. On the validity of lesion-behaviour mapping methods. Neuropsychologia. 115:17–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Tan  Y, Martin  RC. 2018. Verbal short-term memory capacities and executive function in semantic and syntactic interference resolution during sentence comprehension: evidence from aphasia. Neuropsychologia. 113:111–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Tan  Y, Martin  RC, Van Dyke  JA. 2017. Semantic and syntactic interference in sentence comprehension: a comparison of working memory models. Front Psychol. 8:198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Thorn  ASC, Frankish  CR. 2005. Long-term knowledge effects on serial recall of nonwords are not exclusively lexical. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 31:729–735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Turkeltaub  PE, Branch  CH. 2010. Localization of sublexical speech perception components. Brain Lang. 114:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Visser  M, Jefferies  E, Embleton  KV, Lambon Ralph  MA. 2012. Both the middle temporal gyrus and the ventral anterior temporal area are crucial for multimodal semantic processing: distortion-corrected fMRI evidence for a double gradient of information convergence in the temporal lobes. J Cogn Neurosci. 24:1766–1778. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Walker  GM, Hickok  G. 2016. Bridging computational approaches to speech production: the semantic–lexical–auditory–motor model (SLAM). Psychon Bull Rev. 23:339–352. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Wechsler  D. 1981. Manual for the Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. San Antonio (TX): Psychological Corp. [Google Scholar]
  85. Wilson  SM, Bautista  A, McCarron  A. 2018. Convergence of spoken and written language processing in the superior temporal sulcus. Neuroimage. 171:62–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Yourganov  G, Fridriksson  J, Rorden  C, Gleichgerrcht  E, Bonilha  L. 2016. Multivariate connectome-based symptom mapping in post-stroke patients: networks supporting language and speech. J Neurosci. 36:6668–6679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yue  Q, Martin  RC, Hamilton  AC, Rose  NS. 2018. Non-perceptual regions in the left inferior parietal lobe support phonological short-term memory: evidence for a buffer account?  Cereb Cortex. 29:1398–1413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zhang  Y, Kimberg  DY, Coslett  HB, Schwartz  MF, Wang  Z. 2014. Multivariate lesion-symptom mapping using support vector regression. Hum Brain Mapp. 35:5861–5876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Cerebral Cortex Communications are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES