Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Apr 12.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Biol. 2021 Feb 4;31(7):1428–1437.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.01.027

Enhanced reinstatement of naturalistic event memories due to hippocampal-network-targeted stimulation

Melissa Hebscher 1,*, James E Kragel 1, Thorsten Kahnt 2,3,4, Joel L Voss 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC8044012  NIHMSID: NIHMS1664460  PMID: 33545044

Summary

Episodic memory involves the reinstatement of distributed patterns of brain activity present when events were initially experienced. The hippocampus is thought to coordinate reinstatement via its interactions with a network of brain regions, but this hypothesis has not been causally tested in humans. The current study directly tested the involvement of the hippocampal network in reinstatement using network-targeted noninvasive stimulation. We measured reinstatement of multi-voxel patterns of fMRI activity during encoding and retrieval of naturalistic video clips depicting everyday activities. Reinstatement of video-specific activity patterns was robust in posterior-parietal and occipital areas previously implicated in event reinstatement. Theta-burst stimulation targeting the hippocampal network increased video-specific reinstatement of fMRI activity patterns in occipital cortex and improved memory accuracy relative to stimulation of a control out-of-network location. Furthermore, stimulation targeting the hippocampal network influenced the trial-by-trial relationship between hippocampal activity during encoding and later reinstatement in occipital cortex. These findings implicate the hippocampal network in the reinstatement of spatially distributed patterns of event-specific activity, and identify a role for the hippocampus in encoding complex naturalistic events that later undergo cortical reinstatement.

eTOC blurb

Hebscher et al. test the effects of noninvasive brain stimulation targeting the hippocampal network on memory for naturalistic events. Stimulation improves memory accuracy, enhances cortical reinstatement of event-specific patterns of neural activity, and alters the association between hippocampal activity and cortical reinstatement.

Introduction

Episodic memory involves re-experiencing past events in detail. A prevailing view is that this involves reinstating patterns of neural activity that were present during the encoding of an event in a process supported by the hippocampus [1-3]. The hippocampus is thought to bind together episodic information distributed throughout the brain during encoding and to coordinate reinstatement of this distributed activity at retrieval [4-6]. The current study evaluated this hypothesis by testing whether modulating the hippocampal network via noninvasive brain stimulation affects cortical reinstatement, measured using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).

Episodic memory is related to reinstatement of cortical activity as measured by fMRI in humans. These experiments have used multivariate measures [7] to quantify the similarity of multi-voxel activity patterns between initial encoding and subsequent retrieval [8]. This metric of memory-related activity reinstatement is typically robust in occipitotemporal and parietal cortex [4-6,9-11], within areas thought to be involved in representing the sensory features comprising the reactivated memory [3]. Event-specific fMRI reinstatement occurs for complex video clips that approximate the content of naturally occurring events [9-11], thus demonstrating relevance to real-world memory function. Some experiments have further found that trial-by-trial fluctuations in hippocampal activity are related to corresponding fluctuations in cortical reactivation strength [4-6], suggesting that the hippocampus may be important for coordinating distributed reinstatement.

The hippocampus accomplishes memory-related functions via interactions with a distributed network of interconnected brain regions, including medial and lateral temporal and parietal cortex, medial frontal cortex, and a variety of subcortical nuclei [12-16]. The hippocampus and its network can be functionally influenced using a transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) approach referred to as “hippocampal-network-targeted” (HNT) stimulation [17]. In this approach, TMS is applied to subject-specific cortical locations based on their high functional connectivity to the hippocampus. HNT stimulation using a theta-burst pattern has enhanced performance on hippocampal-dependent word-list [18] and spatial-precision [19] tests of episodic memory, with associated changes in resting-state fMRI connectivity between the hippocampus and regions of its network [17,18], and in memory-related neural activity in the hippocampal network [20,21]. Notably, although previous studies have demonstrated that HNT stimulation can alter hippocampal-network function and episodic memory [17,20,22], the neural mechanisms by which this alteration occurs are unclear. In particular, the impact of HNT on cortical reinstatement of event-specific activity patterns has not been tested.

The current study used HNT stimulation to test theorized hippocampal contributions to cortical reinstatement of naturalistic event memories. Participants received theta-burst stimulation to a lateral parietal region defined by high functional connectivity with the hippocampus (HNT stimulation) and then immediately completed a naturalistic video memory task during fMRI scanning. On a different day, the same procedure was performed but with stimulation of a control out-of-network location (vertex). On each day, memory encoding occurred immediately after stimulation during fMRI scanning. Participants viewed a series of short video clips depicting everyday activities, which afford high experimental control while more closely approximating memory for life events compared to traditional laboratory memory tasks. At retrieval, subjects used recall cues to mentally replay studied video clips. To enable comparison with previous episodic and autobiographical memory studies, we measured both objective and subjective memory for video clips (See Figure 1 for experimental design).

Figure 1. Experimental design overview.

Figure 1.

(A) HNT stimulation was delivered to subject-specific locations in lateral parietal cortex (blue spheres) defined based on resting-state fMRI connectivity (arrow) with anatomically defined subject-specific hippocampal locations (red spheres). Subject-specific vertex control sites (purple spheres) were anatomically defined in the interhemispheric fissure. (B) In each of two distinct experimental sessions, participants first received stimulation (HNT or vertex control in counterbalanced order across participants). Immediately following stimulation, participants performed a memory task during fMRI. (C) Participants watched a series of videos (51 per session) at encoding and made odd/even judgments to numbers following each video to limit continued processing of videos during the intertrial interval. At retrieval, participants were cued with titles describing each of the studied videos. Subjects mentally replayed each video while viewing a blank screen. They then rated the vividness of the memory and answered two true/false (yes/no) questions to test memory accuracy, including a 4-point confidence judgment.

We measured reinstatement of individual video clips by comparing patterns of fMRI activity between encoding and retrieval [7]. Importantly, the video clips used here were unique from those used in previous studies to identify cortical reinstatement [9,23] in that the clips had relatively high feature overlap, including the same actor, similar contexts, and similar content. This is similar to natural memory demands in which specific events must be remembered given the consistent actors, locations, and objects typical of one’s everyday life. Further, relatively high trial counts (51 trials per session) supported event-specific analyses that allowed us to examine how similarities between neural activity associated with different videos at encoding influences later reinstatement of distinct, event-specific patterns of neural activity.

Based on prior findings of memory enhancement by HNT theta-burst stimulation [18,19] we predicted that, compared to control stimulation, HNT stimulation would improve memory accuracy and therefore also increase reinstatement of stimulus-specific activity patterns. Based on evidence for the impact of HNT stimulation on hippocampus [17,20] and the putative role of hippocampus in coordinating cortical reinstatement [4-6], we hypothesized that HNT stimulation would modulate hippocampal activity and its association with cortical reinstatement.

Results

Effects of stimulation on memory

We first examined the effects of HNT stimulation on memory for videos. Previous experiments have demonstrated improved memory accuracy due to HNT stimulation [17], and we tested for similar effects here by asking two objective yes/no questions about each video clip (Figure 1C). To assess the influence of stimulation on these two questions, we conducted a 2x2 repeated-measures ANOVA with accuracy as the dependent variable, and question number (Q1 vs Q2) and stimulation (HNT vs control) as factors (Figure 2A). There was a significant main effect of question number (F(1,57) = 4.81 , p = 0.033, η2p = 0.078), no main effect of stimulation on accuracy (F(1,57) = 1.15, p = 0.288, η2p = 0.020), and a significant stimulation by question number interaction (F(1,57)=6.09, p=0.017, η2p = 0.097). Paired-samples t-tests were used to determine the effects that led to this interaction. Compared to Q1, Q2 accuracy was significantly worse for control stimulation (t(19) = −3.40, p = 0.003, d = 0.838) but not for HNT stimulation (t(19) = 0.21, p = 0.833, d = 0.045). This indicates that the typical pattern of performance on the task (under control stimulation) was worse accuracy for Q2 than Q1, and that this relative Q2 disadvantage was protected by HNT stimulation. Indeed, HNT stimulation significantly improved Q2 accuracy relative to control stimulation (t(19) = 3.20, p = 0.005, d = 0.751), but had no effect on Q1 accuracy relative to control stimulation (t(19) = −0.74, p = 0.470, d = −0.206). A repeated-measures ANOVA revealed that the order of stimulation sessions did not affect accuracy, as demonstrated by no significant counterbalance order (HNT vs. control stimulation first) by session (HNT vs. control) interaction on Q2 (F(1,18) = 3.33, p=0.085) or Q1 accuracy (F(1,18) = 0.149, p = 0.704). Overall, these findings suggest that HNT stimulation improved memory relative to a typical pattern of worse performance for Q2 than Q1 evident under control stimulation. This may be because Q2 included more questions about the background context of the episode than Q1, and context is likely dependent on the posterior portions of the hippocampal network that were targeted by HNT stimulation [24] (See Episodic memory task in STAR Methods for more information on the breakdown of question content).

Figure 2. Effects of HNT stimulation on video memory accuracy.

Figure 2.

(A) Accuracy by question number and stimulation session (HNT and control stimulation). Accuracy on Q2 was significantly better for HNT (red) than control (blue) stimulation. For control stimulation, accuracy was significantly worse on Q2 than on Q1, but there was no difference between questions for HNT stimulation sessions. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate tests that were significant after correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05). (B) Difference scores for Q2 accuracy (HNT minus control) by participant, showing a relatively consistent improvement in Q2 accuracy for HNT versus control stimulation. 15 out of 20 subjects had higher Q2 accuracy for HNT versus control stimulation, and only 3 out of 20 subjects showed moderately lower Q2 accuracy for HNT versus control stimulation.

To assess subjective qualities of memory, we measured vividness and confidence (See Episodic memory task in Methods). There was no effect of HNT relative to control stimulation on vividness or on confidence for either question (all p’s > 0.49; See Table 1). Consistent with previous findings and interpretations [17], this suggests that HNT stimulation affects hippocampal-related function, as indicated by improved memory accuracy, more than parietal-related functions [25], as indicated by no change in memory vividness and confidence judgments.

Table 1.

Objective and subjective memory assessment values.

Measure Mean (SD) t-value
Control HNT
Accuracy Q1 0.73 (.09) 0.71 (.10) −0.76
Accuracy Q2 0.67 (.07) 0.72 (.06) 3.24a
Vividness 2.71 (.36) 2.68 (.37) −0.49
Confidence Q1 1.60 (.19) 1.63 (.17) 0.70
Confidence Q2 1.16 (.23) 1.16 (.24) 0.01

t-value reflects HNT vs. control stimulation

a

p< 0.01

Effects of stimulation on reinstatement

We next tested the effect of HNT stimulation on reinstatement, which we defined as the video-specific similarity between encoding and retrieval fMRI activity patterns [4,7,9] within cortical regions of interest (ROIs) in each hemisphere. ROIs were chosen based on their identification in a previous video reinstatement study [9] (Figure 3D). For each ROI, encoding-retrieval similarity was computed as the Pearson correlation of a video’s fMRI activity pattern during encoding with the corresponding activity pattern when subjects attempted to mentally replay the same video during retrieval. Encoding-retrieval similarity was computed for all pairs of videos and the average Fisher-transformed correlation of all mismatched pairs (different videos at encoding versus retrieval) was subtracted from the mean correlation of all matched pairs (same video at encoding and retrieval), and z-scored against a null distribution of correlation difference scores obtained by permuting video labels (See Encoding-retrieval reinstatement in STAR Methods). The resulting z-scores reflect pattern similarity, or reinstatement, of video-specific activity for a particular ROI.

Figure 3. HNT stimulation increases video-specific reinstatement.

Figure 3.

(A) Reinstatement was significantly greater than zero in the indicated posterior parietal and occipital regions following control stimulation. Left (LH) and right (RH) hemispheres are plotted separately. (B) HNT stimulation led to significant reinstatement in a larger set of ROIs, including all of those showing reinstatement in control stimulation sessions. (C) Direct comparison of the two sessions (HNT – control) showing that HNT stimulation increased reinstatement in left middle occipital gyrus. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate regions that were significant after correction for multiple comparisons. (D) ROIs shown on a glass brain, with the average lateral parietal HNT stimulation location plotted as a red sphere.

For control-stimulation sessions, one-sample t-tests corrected for multiple comparisons (see Encoding-retrieval reinstatement in Methods) indicated that reinstatement was significantly greater than the null distribution for left fusiform, right middle occipital, right angular, and bilateral middle temporal ROIs (See Figure 3A; Table 2). For HNT-stimulation sessions, t-tests corrected for multiple comparisons revealed more ROIs with significant reinstatement, including all of the same ROIs with significant reinstatement in the control-stimulation condition, plus additional ROIs (left middle occipital gyrus and bilateral precuneus) (See Figure 3B; Table 2). The additional ROIs showing reinstatement following HNT stimulation relative to control stimulation were predominately in the left hemisphere, which is notable as HNT stimulation was delivered to left hemisphere based on connectivity with left hippocampus, and connectivity of posterior cortex with hippocampus is primarily unilateral [14,26].

Table 2.

Reinstatement by ROI and stimulation condition

Region   Mean z score (SE) p value
Control HNT HNT - control Control HNT HNT vs.
control
L fusiform gyrus 0.74 (.21) 0.57 (.24) −0.17 (.29) 0.003a 0.032a 0.628
R middle occipital gyrus 0.95 (.29) 1.04 (.25) 0.09 (.35) 0.004a <0.001a 0.810
R angular gyrus 0.91 (.33) 0.88 (.27) −0.03 (.39) 0.012a 0.004a 0.925
L middle temporal gyrus 0.70 (.27) 0.64 (.27) −0.07 (.32) 0.017a 0.031a 0.754
R middle temporal gyrus 0.85 (.32) 0.84 (.31) −0.01 (.40) 0.015a 0.013a 0.884
L middle occipital gyrus 0.28 (.31) 1.45 (.24) 1.18 (.31) 0.388 <0.001a 0.002a
R precuneus 0.38 (.27) 0.70 (16) 0.32 (.30) 0.183 <0.001a 0.300
L precuneus 0.23 (.25) 0.60 (.21) 0.36 (.35) 0.362 0.010a 0.318
R fusiform gyrus 0.55 (.27) 0.63 (.29) 0.08 (.35) 0.054 0.042 0.828
L angular gyrus 0.41 (.23) 0.64 (.31) 0.23 (.37) 0.096 0.054 0.546
a

reported p-value passed Benjamini-Hochberg correction for multiple comparisons

Bolded mean values are significant as measured by one-sample t-tests

Difference z-score reflects the average of within-subject differences between HNT - control

For all ROIs that showed significant reinstatement for either session, paired-samples t-tests corrected for multiple comparisons were used to test whether HNT stimulation led to greater reinstatement than control stimulation. HNT stimulation significantly increased reinstatement in the left middle occipital gyrus (t(19) = 3.66, p = 0.002, d = 0.936), but not in other ROIs (all p’s > 0.1). See Figure 3C; Table 2. Notably, this ROI showing greater reinstatement due to HNT stimulation is spatially distinct from the parietal location where HNT stimulation was applied (Figure 3D), with no subject’s HNT stimulation location falling within this ROI, and does not belong to the hippocampal network as defined in previous experiments [24,27].

Specificity of the effects of stimulation on reinstatement

Although the hippocampus is thought to be important for cortical reinstatement [1-3], we did not predict that such reinstatement would occur within the hippocampus itself, in line with several previous studies [4,5]. To test this, we used left and right hippocampus as ROIs for the same reinstatement analysis described above, and found no evidence of significant reinstatement for either control-stimulation (p’s > 0.063) or HNT-stimulation sessions (p’s > 0.183), and no difference in reinstatement between stimulation sessions (p’s > 0.110).

To test the anatomical specificity of the effects of stimulation on reinstatement within the cortical ROIs that were used for the main analysis (Figure 3D), we performed the same analyses for ROIs that were not expected to show reinstatement effects: bilateral supplementary motor cortex and middle frontal gyrus. This ROI-based approach has similar power to the primary analyses reported above. No control regions had significant reinstatement for either the control or HNT stimulation sessions (all p’s > 0.087), and reinstatement did not differ between stimulation sessions for any region (all p’s > 0.643). These findings demonstrate that cortical reinstatement and the effects of stimulation on reinstatement were relatively specific to the ROIs selected a priori.

To test that the effects of stimulation on reinstatement were not merely due to different levels of univariate activity across the stimulation sessions [28], we assessed the effect of HNT stimulation on univariate activity at encoding and retrieval. We focused on the left middle occipital gyrus ROI because this allowed us to test whether the observed effect of HNT on reinstatement was driven by changes in univariate activity. Voxel-wise analysis of univariate activity within this region did not differ between stimulation sessions for either encoding or retrieval (no clusters > 10 voxels at p < 0.01 threshold). We next computed the trial-wise correlation between univariate activity and reinstatement within the left middle occipital gyrus. At encoding, average univariate activity was negatively correlated with reinstatement within this region for HNT stimulation (−t(19) = 2.98, p = 0.008), and trending towards a negative relationship for control stimulation (t(19) = −2.00, p = 0.060), but the correlation did not differ between sessions (p = 0.270). At retrieval, univariate activity and reinstatement were positively correlated for both HNT (t(19) = 7.48, p < 0.001), and control stimulation (t(19) = 9.43, p < 0.001), however the correlation did not differ between sessions (p = 0.386). Together these control analyses demonstrate that the effect of HNT stimulation on reinstatement is related to event-specific, distributed patterns of fMRI activity rather than to an overall effect on the magnitude of the fMRI response.

We next tested whether HNT stimulation affected relevant reinstatement (matched videos), or if it also affected irrelevant reinstatement (mismatched videos). There was a significant effect of stimulation on relevant reinstatement (matched videos) such that similarity was greater for HNT (mean = 1.43) than control stimulation (mean = 0.267) (t(19) = 3.67, p = 0.002). There was also an effect of stimulation on irrelevant reinstatement such that similarity was more negative for HNT (mean = −1.465) than control stimulation (mean = −0.27) (t(19) = −3.78, p = 0.001). This indicates that HNT stimulation both enhanced the reinstatement of video-specific relevant videos and reduced irrelevant reinstatement of related videos.

To determine whether the effects of HNT stimulation on neural reinstatement are related to its effects on memory accuracy, we performed an additional control analysis comparing reinstatement between accurate and inaccurate trials. Comparison of left middle occipital reinstatement between trials on which Q2 was answered correctly versus incorrectly revealed a significant difference between for HNT (t(19) = 2.80, p = 0.011) but not control stimulation (t(19) = 1.61, p = 0.124), such that reinstatement was higher for correct trials. Comparing reinstatement between trials on which Q1 was correct vs. incorrect revealed no significant difference for HNT (t(19) = 1.77, p = 0.093) or control stimulation (t(19) = 1.58, p = 0.130). These findings suggest that the effects of HNT stimulation on reinstatement and on behavior are related.

Effects of stimulation on hippocampal contributions to reinstatement

To test the hypothesis that HNT stimulation would affect hippocampal activity, we compared univariate hippocampal activity between stimulation sessions. We focused on left hippocampus because it was the indirect target of HNT stimulation, chosen a priori on anatomical grounds and by indirect targeting via a lateral parietal site with high resting-state fMRI connectivity. A voxel-wise paired t-test on hippocampal activity at encoding revealed a cluster in the left posterior hippocampus showing reduced activity for HNT compared to control stimulation (7 voxels at p < 0.005, cluster corrected; peak t = −4.44) (Figure 4A). Control and HNT stimulation both showed positive activity for this cluster, although this activity was sub-threshold for HNT stimulation (control: peak t = 5.81 ; HNT: peak t = 1.29). Moreover, activity in the left hippocampus as a whole was predominately positive for both control and HNT stimulation. This suggests that HNT stimulation reduced the degree of positive hippocampal activity rather than causing negative deflections in activity. No clusters survived thresholding for the comparison between stimulation sessions on hippocampal activity at retrieval. There were no differences between stimulation sessions in the right hippocampus for either encoding or retrieval. Thus, HNT stimulation led to reduced activity at encoding, specifically in the left hippocampus which was indirectly targeted. This is consistent with previous findings of HNT stimulation’s effects on left hippocampal activity (12), and of reduced hippocampal activity due to the specific single-session theta-burst TMS parameters that were used here [20].

Figure 4. Effects of HNT stimulation on hippocampal contributions to reinstatement.

Figure 4.

(A) Univariate activity during encoding masked by the left hippocampus showed a significant cluster of negative activation for HNT vs control stimulation sessions (p < 0.005 cluster corrected, image thresholded at p = 0.01 for visualization purposes). Voxel-wise t-values are overlaid on a template MNI brain. (B) Left hippocampal mask used for (A), and for calculating average hippocampal activity for (C) and (D). (C) Mean within-subject (trial-wise) correlation between left hippocampal activity at encoding and reinstatement in left middle occipital gyrus (L-MOG) for control (blue) and HNT (red) stimulation sessions. Error bars represent standard error of the mean. Asterisks indicate significant comparisons (p< 0.05). (D) Within-subjects correlation between hippocampal activity and L-MOG reinstatement plotted for control and HNT stimulation sessions separately. Each line represents an individual participant’s linear fit for a given session. See Figure S1 for additional within-subjects correlations exploring the association between hippocampal activity and reinstatement.

To assess the relevance of HNT stimulation’s effects on hippocampal encoding activity for later reinstatement, we analyzed the trial-by-trial association between hippocampal activity and cortical reinstatement, and the effect of stimulation on this relationship. Based on the effects of HNT stimulation described above, we focused on the trial-by-trial relationship between left hippocampal univariate activity during encoding (Figure 4B) and left middle occipital gyrus encoding-retrieval reinstatement. For control stimulation, there was no significant relationship between univariate hippocampal activity and occipital reinstatement (t(19) = 0.02, p = 0.986, d = 0.004), whereas for HNT stimulation there was a significant negative association (t(19) = −3.08, p = 0.006, d = 0.689). 17 out of 20 participants demonstrated this negative relationship following HNT stimulation, compared to only 9 out of 20 for control stimulation. Paired-samples t-tests indicated that HNT stimulation led to a significantly more negative relationship relative to control stimulation (t(19) = 2.32, p = 0.032, d = 0.634). See Figure 4C and 4D. This suggests that the degree to which HNT stimulation decreased hippocampal activity during encoding was associated with increased encoding-retrieval similarity. As we will describe below, this relationship may be related to the uniqueness of the activity patterns evoked by individual videos during encoding (i.e. the degree of fMRI activity pattern overlap between similar videos at encoding).

We next tested the alternative possibility that the local effects of HNT stimulation on the stimulated cortex rather than on hippocampal activity may have been responsible for its effects on cortical reinstatement. We measured the trial-by-trial correlation between left middle occipital gyrus reinstatement and univariate activity at the subject-specific sites of stimulation. Within-subject correlations were not significantly different from zero for either control or HNT stimulation, and there was no difference between the sessions (all p’s > 0.516). These findings suggest that effects of HNT stimulation on activity at the stimulation site did not affect cortical reinstatement. Taken together with the reported effect of HNT stimulation on the trial-by-trial correlation between univariate hippocampal activity and reinstatement, these findings suggest that the hippocampus rather than the stimulated cortex is related to the effects of stimulation on cortical reinstatement.

We also examined the relationship between hippocampal activity at retrieval and cortical reinstatement [4,5]. Left hippocampal activity was positively associated with left middle occipital gyrus reinstatement for both control (t(19) = 3.95, p < 0.001, d = 0.883) and HNT stimulation (t(19) = 4.07, p < 0.001, d = 0.910). This association did not significantly differ between sessions (p = 0.784). Thus, hippocampal activity during retrieval was positively associated with reinstatement, without variation by stimulation condition, indicating that positive deflections in hippocampal activity reflect retrieval of video-specific content and that the effects of HNT stimulation were primarily on encoding.

Testing the role of hippocampus in encoding activity that supports reinstatement

We performed an exploratory analysis to test a hypothesis about why HNT stimulation induced a negative association between left hippocampal activity during encoding and subsequent reinstatement in left middle occipital gyrus. We reasoned that greater hippocampal activity at encoding to a specific video might partially reflect retrieval of similar content from other videos, and that this could increase the similarity among video representations and thereby lead to reduced potential for reinstatement of specific videos. Thus, we hypothesized that greater hippocampal activity at encoding would be related to higher similarity of fMRI activity patterns between videos at encoding, and that this “encoding similarity” would in turn be related to reduced video-specific cortical reinstatement at retrieval. Encoding similarity was calculated as the average Fisher-transformed correlation between the pattern of fMRI activity for each video and all other videos at encoding (See Encoding similarity in STAR Methods).

Within the left middle occipital gyrus, encoding similarity was significantly greater than zero for control (t(19) = 14.16, p < 0.001) and HNT sessions (t(19) = 14.49, p < 0.001) and did not differ by stimulation session (t(19) = 1.68, p = 0.109). Although they were not the focus of this exploratory analysis, we also assessed encoding similarity within each of the 5 bilateral ROIs (Figure 3D). Values for all ROIs were significantly greater than zero for both stimulation sessions (all p’s < 0.001) with no significant differences between stimulation sessions (all p’s > 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons), however all ROIs showed a trend towards lower encoding similarity for HNT versus control stimulation. This high similarity of activity patterns across videos is unsurprising and likely reflects similarity in video content and task demands across trials. The critical question of interest is thus whether encoding similarity is related to univariate hippocampal encoding activity and subsequent pattern reinstatement. Indeed, there was a significant within-subject correlation between encoding similarity and hippocampal activity at encoding, with greater hippocampal activity associated with higher encoding similarity in left middle occipital gyrus for both control (t(19) = 9.42, p < 0.001) and HNT sessions (t(19 = 5.37, p < 0.001), with no difference between sessions (p = 0.692) (Supplemental Information: Figure S1A). Thus, greater hippocampal activity was associated with more overlap of fMRI activity patterns among different videos in occipital cortex at encoding.

As hypothesized, higher encoding similarity was in turn related to less cortical reinstatement. There was a significant negative within-subjects correlation between encoding similarity and reinstatement for both control (t(19) = −3.98, p < 0.001) and HNT stimulation sessions (t(19)= −3.69, p = 0.002), with no difference by stimulation session (p = 0.532) (Supplemental Information: Figure S1B). Together these exploratory findings suggest that lower hippocampal activity was generally related to more dissimilarity among video activity patterns during encoding, and that more dissimilar encoding patterns in turn predicted greater video-specific activity reinstatement during retrieval. The beneficial effect of HNT stimulation on reinstatement thus could have resulted from its suppressive effect on hippocampal activity during encoding.

Discussion

This study tested the effect of HNT stimulation on memory for naturalistic episodes. We found that stimulation of parietal regions showing high functional connectivity with the hippocampus improved memory accuracy and induced reinstatement in a greater number of posterior cortical regions relative to control stimulation, significantly enhancing reinstatement in the middle occipital gyrus. We further found that HNT stimulation led to reduced univariate activity in the left hippocampus at encoding and identified a stimulation-induced negative relationship between trial-wise hippocampal activity and cortical reinstatement that appeared to be mediated by the neural distinctness of videos at encoding. These findings demonstrate that perturbing one node of the episodic memory network can alter large-scale, multimodal patterns of neural activity which are considered to be fundamental to memory.

Episodic memory is thought to critically depend on cortical reinstatement mediated by the hippocampus [1-3]. Consistent with prior research [9,23,29], we identified reinstatement of video-specific patterns of neural activity within a set of posterior parietal and occipital regions following control stimulation. A greater number of regions showed significant reinstatement following HNT stimulation, and many of these regions were located in the left hemisphere, suggesting that left lateralized stimulation preferentially affects cortical areas in the left hemisphere. Direct comparison of the two sessions revealed that HNT stimulation led to greater reinstatement in the left middle occipital gyrus. This region might be have been affected by stimulation due to its reported role in episodic memory encoding success [30], recall of visual content [31,32] including videos [33], and reinstatement of video clips [9,23]. In addition, this region shows high functional connectivity with the hippocampus during memory formation [34] and retrieval. Importantly, these findings demonstrate that HNT stimulation alters reinstatement in posterior parietal and occipital regions.

We found that a single session of theta-burst HNT stimulation improves memory accuracy and enhances cortical reinstatement of naturalistic episodic memories. These findings add to prior research which identified improvements in hippocampal-dependent word-list [18] and spatial precision tests [19] of episodic memory using the same stimulation paradigm, with associated increases in resting-state fMRI connectivity between the hippocampus and its network [18]. Our findings of enhanced episodic memory accuracy and reinstatement are consistent with the improvements identified in these [18,19] as well as other HNT stimulation studies using a multi-day paradigm [21,35-37]. Conversely, theta-burst HNT was recently shown to reduce evoked anterior hippocampal activity and performance on a task thought to rely on episodic memory [20]. Differences between that study’s findings and ours may be related to the seed used to determine stimulation location, which was located in anterior hippocampus in that study and in hippocampal body in ours, and task differences, with that study measuring divergent thinking and episodic simulation, and ours measuring episodic memory. Others have similarly reported disrupted episodic memory following stimulation of lateral parietal regions [20,22,38-42], although these studies did not use an HNT stimulation approach. Notably, many of the studies showing disruptions tested the effects of stimulation on memory processes thought to be partially dependent on the parietal lobe, such as multimodal integration [38], generation [20] and recall of detailed multisensory information [22,39], and memory vividness [40], which differ from the predominately hippocampal-dependent measure of memory in ours and similar studies [19,21,35-37,43]. This pattern of results is consistent with a recent proposal that stimulation may disrupt local processing at the site of stimulation while enhancing processing in downstream, functionally connected regions such as the hippocampus [17]. This proposal may also explain why HNT stimulation altered memory accuracy but not vividness in the present study. That is, although the parietal lobe plays an established role in subjective memory [40,41], stimulation targeted the hippocampus and was therefore more likely to alter hippocampal- than parietal-based functions. Thus, discrepancies in the effects of parietal stimulation on memory may be due to differences in the neural basis of the targeted memory function.

Prior work has shown that delivering TMS to accessible portions of the episodic memory network can alter performance on standard laboratory tests of episodic memory [18,19,21,37,44]. The present findings demonstrate that this approach can be used to improve objective memory for complex stimuli that more closely approximate memory demands experienced outside the laboratory. We found that HNT stimulation altered objective memory accuracy on the second of two true/false questions. Interestingly, participants performed worse on the second question than they did for the first question, but only for control stimulation. One possible explanation for this finding is that participants were better able to answer the second question following HNT stimulation because it more often asked about the contextual details of an event (see Methods), which may depend more on the posterior hippocampal network [24] that was targeted by HNT stimulation than answering other types of questions. This could have led to a greater effect of stimulation on the more contextual-biased second question. Another possible explanation is that HNT stimulation improved memory encoding, leading to higher fidelity reinstatement of memories which were more temporally stable throughout the question phase, allowing participants access to the details of a memory for longer and therefore to answer the second question more accurately. A final possibility is that HNT stimulation altered retrieval mechanisms that lessened interference between the elements retrieved to answer the second question from those retrieved to answer the first question. While the present behavioral data is not sufficient to formally test these idea, future studies could experimentally test the mechanism by which HNT stimulation improves memory accuracy.

We stimulated areas of the lateral parietal lobe with high functional connectivity to the hippocampus, aiming to indirectly target the hippocampus, and found that HNT stimulation led to reduced univariate activity at encoding in a small left posterior hippocampal cluster. At the trial-level, HNT stimulation led to a significant negative association between left hippocampal activity at encoding and reinstatement in the left middle occipital gyrus. Notably, there was no such relationship between activity at the stimulated parietal location and reinstatement, indicating that the effects of HNT stimulation on reinstatement were based on hippocampal rather than cortical activity modulation. Follow-up analyses revealed that this negative association was related to the similarity or interference between videos as they were being encoded. Greater neural similarity in the left middle occipital gyrus between different videos at encoding was associated with concurrent increases in hippocampal activity and lower cortical reinstatement during retrieval. This suggests that hippocampal activity during encoding might partially reflect retrieval of content from other videos, which in turn makes later reinstatement of the unique elements of an individual video more difficult. The stimulation-induced reduction in hippocampal activity at encoding might therefore reflect less neural overlap (and interference) between similar videos at encoding, which in turn leads to greater reinstatement of unique videos and lower reinstatement of irrelevant videos. Indeed, we found that HNT stimulation both enhanced reinstatement of relevant videos and reduced reinstatement of irrelevant videos. Future studies could directly test the relationship between encoding similarity, hippocampal activity, and neural reinstatement by formally manipulating content similarity or by obtaining corresponding metrics of behavioral confusion or interference between videos.

Although the hippocampus is thought to be important for supporting cortical reinstatement [1-3], we did not observe video-specific reinstatement in the hippocampus itself. Previous findings are mixed [45], with some fMRI studies failing to find evidence for item- [4,5] or category-level [46] reinstatement within the hippocampus and other studies finding evidence for item-level [47] or category-level [48] reinstatement. It is unclear how the dominant trisynaptic circuit within the hippocampus could exhibit patterns of activity that could support item-specific reinstatement as measured by fMRI. However, the reported trial-wise correlation between hippocampal activity and cortical reinstatement implicates the hippocampus in the orchestration of cortical reinstatement, perhaps through a mechanism such as memory strength [49] or indexing of the location of cortical memory traces [50], rather than by reinstating video content itself.

To conclude, the present findings demonstrate that noninvasive stimulation of functionally connected regions can alter large-scale patterns of event-specific activity beyond the site of stimulation, leading to enhanced memory for complex, naturalistic events and their spatiotemporal contexts. This provides important direct evidence to support the causal role of the hippocampal network in supporting memory reinstatement. Together with prior findings of altered episodic memory and neural activity following network-targeted stimulation [18,19,21,37,44], the current findings support the ability of HNT stimulation to alter hippocampal-cortical networks important for naturalistic expressions of episodic memory, which could be of use in treating disorders of memory for life events.

STAR Methods

RESOURCE AVAILABILITY

Lead Contact

Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Joel Voss (joel-voss@northwestern.edu).

Materials Availability

This study did not generate new unique reagents

Data and Code Availability

Data and code are available from the lead contact upon request.

EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

Subjects

Twenty adults participated in the current study (11 females, mean age = 24.26, SD = 3.57, range = 19-32). A restricted age range was used to avoid age-related effects on memory and approximate male/female balance was used such that sex could not be responsible for reported effects. All participants were native or fluent English speakers, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and were free from a history of neurological illness or injury, psychiatric condition, substance abuse, or serious medical conditions. All participants passed standard MRI and TMS safety screenings [51]. Participants provided informed consent prior to participating in the experiment and were paid for participation. Study procedures were approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board.

METHOD DETAILS

Experimental design

Participants completed a baseline session and two experimental sessions on separate days in a within-subjects design. During the baseline session, resting-state fMRI and structural MRI were acquired, and TMS resting motor threshold was determined. Resting-state fMRI from this session was used to define the stimulation locations for subsequent sessions. Each experimental session lasted approximately 2 hours and occurred on separate days, at least 48 hours apart (mean = 3.1 days, range = 2-7). In experimental sessions, participants received continuous theta burst TMS to either the control site (vertex) or the lateral parietal target (HNT), with the order of stimulation site counterbalanced across participants. TMS was administered in a room adjacent to the MRI scanner room. Immediately following the final TMS pulse, participants were moved to the scanner where they performed an episodic memory task while fMRI was collected. See Figure 1B for an overview of experimental sessions. During the encoding phase of the episodic memory task, participants watched short video clips, and at retrieval they recalled the videos, answered true/false questions to test their accuracy for the videos, and rated their subjective vividness of the memory. The encoding phase of the memory task began on average 6.9 mins (SD = 1.1, range = 5-10 mins) after the end of TMS delivery and was completed within approximately 21 minutes (length of encoding phase = 14 mins) post-TMS. Retrieval began immediately after encoding and was completed within approximately 55 mins of the final TMS pulse (length of retrieval phase = 32 mins).

Subject-specific stimulation target identification

HNT lateral parietal stimulation locations were identified separately for each participant based on resting-state fMRI connectivity with a left hippocampal seed, measured during the baseline session. This procedure was adapted from previous studies from our laboratory [37,44].

Baseline resting-state and structural MRI data were preprocessed using AFNI Software (https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/) [52]. Preprocessing steps included motion correction, slice-timing correction to the first slice, functional/structural co-registration, resampling to a resolution of 1.5x1.5x1.5 mm, stereotactic transformation using Montreal Neurologic Institute 305 (MNI-305) template, band-pass filtering (0.01-0.10Hz), spatial smoothing (with a 4-mm FWHM Guassian kernel), despiking, linear de-trending, and regressing out the motion timeseries. Participant-specific hippocampal coordinates were located by identifying a voxel in the left hippocampal body nearest to MNI [−29,−25,−13] that demonstrated high connectivity with bilateral hippocampus. This hippocampal voxel was then used as a seed in a whole-brain resting-state functional connectivity analysis. We then identified clusters of voxels in left parietal cortex with high functional connectivity to the hippocampal seed that fell within left angular and supramarginal gyri, nearest to MNI [−47, −68, 36]. Left parietal TMS target coordinates were chosen as the peak of the parietal cluster that fell within ~2cm of the skull and was located on a gyrus (mean correlation between seed-target = .38, range = .23-.48). As a control site, the vertex was located on each participant at the MNI coordinate [0,−15,74], and adjusted slightly so it fell within the interhemispheric fissure. HNT and control stimulation locations were transformed from MNI space into each participant’s native MRI space for anatomically guided TMS. Individual hippocampal seeds, lateral parietal targets, and vertex targets are plotted in Figure 1A.

TMS procedure

Participants received TMS to the left parietal target or to the control vertex site on separate experimental days. Resting motor threshold (RMT), determined during the baseline session, was defined as the minimum stimulator output necessary to produce a visible contraction of the right thumb (abductor pollicis brevis) for 5 out of 10 consecutive single biphasic pulses delivered to the left M1 thumb area. Continuous theta burst stimulation was applied at 80% RMT for both stimulation locations. The Localite (GmBH, Germany; https://www.localite.de/en/home/) frameless stereotactic system was used to target the selected stimulation locations. Five anatomical landmarks located on the face were used to co-register the anatomical MRI to the participant’s head, and landmarks on the scalp were used to improve registration based on the curvature of the participant’s head. An infrared camera recorded sensors attached to the participant and the TMS coil, allowing for real-time tracking of the TMS coil over the participant’s MRI. A MagPro X100 stimulator connected to a Cool-B65 butterfly coil (MagVenture A/S, Farum, Denmark; https://www.magventure.com/us) was used to deliver stimulation. The continuous theta burst stimulation protocol consisted of a total of 600 biphasic pulses arranged in bursts delivered every 200 ms (5-Hz), with each burst containing three pulses delivered at 50-Hz (40 s duration). The coil was positioned perpendicular to the brain surface at the stimulation site with the magnetic field oriented across the gyrus.

Episodic memory task

Practice sessions were performed prior to TMS administration to familiarize participants with the task and to ensure correct performance. Once moved to the MRI scanner, participants completed the episodic memory task in which they watched and recalled a series of short video clips. Videos consisted of 102 short (7 s) depictions of common events such as draining pasta, kicking a ball, and putting a sheet on the bed. Each video depicted a unique event centered around the same one character and occurred in a fixed number of locations, resulting in high overlap between elements of the videos. All videos were presented without sound. Videos were divided into 2 lists of 51 for presentation on separate experimental sessions, the order of which was counterbalanced across days and stimulation conditions. Within each list, videos were presented in a randomized order at both encoding and retrieval phases.

At the start of the encoding phase, participants were reminded of the task instructions and were instructed to pay close attention to all elements of the videos as their memory for the videos would later be tested. Each video was then presented alone for 7 s, followed by an interstimulus interval (ISI) of 1 s. Participants then judged whether a number was odd or even for 2 s to discourage continued processing of the video. Previous evidence suggests that such judgments drive activity in key regions such as hippocampus back to zero during the intertrial interval (ITI) [53]. The trial ended with an ITI (fixation cross), jittered at 4, 6, and 8 s. Each encoding trial was 16 s on average. All encoding trials occurred within 1 fMRI run lasting 13.9 mins.

The retrieval phase began immediately following completion of the encoding phase. Following a reminder of the instructions for this phase, participants were presented with the description of a video for 3 s and told to mentally replay the video within the allotted time, within a blank box that remained on the screen alone for 7 s. After mentally recalling each video, participants rated their vividness of the memory on a scale from 1-4, with 1 meaning they did not recall the video at all, and 4 meaning they recalled it vividly. 4 s were allotted for vividness ratings. Following the vividness rating, participants answered two true/false (yes/no) accuracy questions about the videos, incorporating their confidence (1- definitely yes, 2- maybe yes, 3-maybe no, 4-definitely no). 5 s were allotted for each accuracy question. Accuracy questions asked about (1) the contextual details of an event (i.e. “Is the actor’s foot on a wood floor”, for a video about tying shoelaces, or “Is there a toaster on the counter” for a video about opening a can of soup; 18/51 questions for Q1, 23/51 questions for Q2), (2) perceptual details central to the event (i.e. “Are the shoes black and white”, for a video about tying shoelaces, or “Is there a snowman in the globe” for a video about shaking a snow globe; 19/51 questions for Q1, 18/51 questions for Q2), or (3) the story or the series of actions performed (i.e. “Does the actor blow on the spoon before eating” for a video about eating soup, or “Does the lid come off of the can” for a video about opening a can of soup; (14/51 questions for Q1, 10/51 questions for Q2). There was a total of 1.2 s ISI within each trial, and an ITI (fixation cross) jittered at 4, 6, and 8 s, with an average trial length of 34 s. Responses were made using 2 response boxes, each with 2 buttons. Retrieval trials occurred over 3 fMRI runs (17 trials each), each lasting 10 mins. Presentation software was used to display the task and collect response data (https://www.neurobs.com/).

MRI acquisition

Structural and functional images were acquired using a Siemens 3 T Prisma whole-body scanner with a 64-channel head coil located in the Northwestern University Center for Translational Imaging Facility. Baseline resting-state and functional images during experimental sessions were acquired using a whole-brain BOLD EPI sequence (TR = 2000 ms, TE = 20 ms, FOV = 1116x1080 mm, flip angle = 80°, and 1.7x1.7x1.7 mm voxel resolution, over 275 volumes). Structural images were acquired using a T1-weighted MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2170 ms, TE = 1.69 ms, FOV = 256x256 mm, flip angle = 7°, voxel resolution: 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm, 1-mm thick sagittal slices). During the baseline resting-state scan, participants were instructed to lie still with their eyes open. The baseline resting-state scan was 275 volumes (9.2 mins). The encoding phase of the memory task consisted of one 13.9 min run (418 volumes), while retrieval was split among three 10 min runs (299 volumes each).

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Behavioral analyses

To determine the effect of HNT stimulation on objective episodic memory, we conducted a 2x2 repeated-measures ANOVA with accuracy as the dependent variable, and question number (Q1 vs Q2) and stimulation session (HNT vs control) as factors. Follow-up paired-samples t-tests were performed to assess significant interactions and main effects. Effect sizes were reported for primary behavioral outcomes. Confidence ratings were extracted from accuracy ratings, such that responses 1 and 4 reflected high confidence and responses 2 and 3 reflected low confidence. Paired-samples t-tests were conducted comparing HNT to control stimulation on vividness and confidence ratings. Trials were excluded from behavioral analyses if there was no response to either of the three retrieval questions. On average, 97% of trials were included for HNT stimulation sessions, and 98% of trials were included for control stimulation sessions. Means, standard deviations, and statistics on behavioral data are reported in Table 1. All paired-sample t-tests reported as significant passed Benjamini-Hochberg [54] correction for multiple comparisons.

MRI preprocessing

Functional MRI data for experimental sessions were preprocessed using AFNI software. Preprocessing included functional-structural co-registration, motion correction, spatial smoothing using a 1.7-mm full-width-half-maximum (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel, and signal intensity normalization by the mean of each voxel. Motion parameters were calculated for each volume, and volumes with excessive frame to frame displacement (>0.3 mm) were flagged for later censoring.

Single-trial estimates were generated for multivariate analyses using a general linear model (GLM) in AFNI (3dDeconvolve). A separate model was constructed for each individual trial to estimate its’ activity separately from all other trials and nuisance variables, an approach known to work effectively for single-trial estimation for multivoxel pattern analyses [55]. For each functional run, individual trials were modelled separately against all other trials using a response model of a 7-s block convolved with a canonical hemodynamic response function. Nuisance variables included the six affine motion estimates generated by motion correction as well as linear drift. Motion estimates were regressed out using framewise displacement and the derivative of framewise displacement as a covariate. For encoding trials, the 7-s block began at the start of video presentation, while at retrieval the 7-s block began when participants saw the cued video title. The resulting single-trial t-maps for each trial were used for subsequent analyses, based on recommendations that using t-maps rather than beta maps for representational similarity analyses reduces the influence of noisy voxels [28]. All multivariate analyses were carried out in native space.

For univariate analyses, encoding- and retrieval-related neural activity were separately modelled for all trials using 7 s block functions convolved with a hemodynamic response function in addition to six motion regressors. Neural activity associated with accurate trials (measured by accuracy on the second question) were modelled separately from inaccurate trials, and then added together to represent all trials. For both sets of GLMs, trials were excluded from analyses if there was no response to the odd/even question at encoding, or no response to either of the three retrieval questions. This resulted in inclusion of an average of 97% trials for control sessions, and 96% trials for HNT sessions.

Regions of interest

Multivariate analyses were focused on a set of ROIs. ROIs included bilateral precuneus, angular gyrus, middle occipital gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and fusiform gyrus. These regions were chosen because they are known to show reinstatement effects of video-specific patterns of activity [9-11]. Moreover, the memory task and analyses in [9] were very similar to ours, prompting us to adopt ROIs from that study. In addition to these main ROIs, we examined multivariate activity in three regions not expected to show reinstatement: (1) bilateral hippocampus, which may be involved in coordinating reinstatement but we did not expect to show reinstatement itself, (2) bilateral middle frontal gyrus, which is thought to be involved in episodic memory but has not been implicated in reinstatement of specific episodic memories, and (3) bilateral motor cortex, which should neither be involved in episodic memory nor in reinstating the content of videos. ROIs were defined using the Eickhoff-Zilles macro labels from N27 in MNI space (CA_ML_18_MNI atlas in AFNI) on a template brain in MNI space and warped to native space for each participant by calculating the transformation matrix needed to warp into MNI space and then reverse-transforming all ROIs into native space using affine transformation (3dAllineate). ROIs are shown in Figure 3D.

Encoding-retrieval reinstatement

We examined reinstatement of video-specific patterns of neural activity by conducting a series of representational similarity analyses (RSAs) on patterns of neural activity within ROIs. RSAs measured encoding-retrieval similarity by computing the Pearson correlation between neural activity patterns for all pairs of videos, for each ROI. Only trials that were self-reported as recollected (vividness score of > 1) were included in these analyses (86% trials for control; 85% trials for HNT). Pairwise correlations between all videos resulted in a matrix with the diagonal reflecting correlations between the same video at encoding and retrieval (matched pairs), and the off-diagonal reflecting correlations between different videos (mismatched pairs). Pairwise correlation values were Fisher transformed. We then subtracted the mean of the off-diagonal correlation values from the mean of the diagonal correlations, resulting in a metric that reflects the degree to which neural pattern similarity was greater for matched versus mismatched video pairs. This metric was then z-scored against a null distribution of correlation difference scores, calculated by randomly shuffling trials over 1000 permutations. Group-level statistics were performed on these z-scored pattern similarity values. RSAs were computed in MATLAB 2018a (The MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA; https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html), using the CoSMoMVPA toolbox (http://www.cosmomvpa.org/) [56] functions to load and organize fMRI datasets (cosmo_fmri_dataset), and calculate encoding-retrieval similarity matrices (cosmo_correlation_measure). All subsequent analyses on correlation matrices were performed using in-house MATLAB scripts.

To assess the presence of neural reinstatement of video-specific patterns of neural activity, we first ran one-sample t-tests on z-scored pattern similarity values for each ROI, within each session. We next ran a series of paired-samples t-tests to determine the effect of stimulation on reinstatement. Only ROIs that showed reinstatement significantly greater than zero for individual sessions were entered into paired-samples t-tests. All p-values were submitted to Benjamini-Hochberg correction for multiple comparisons [54] using a q of 0.05, and only p values that passed this correction are listed as significant. Effect sizes were reported for primary outcomes. Means, standard errors, and statistics for reinstatement analyses are reported in Table 2.

To test whether HNT stimulation affected relevant reinstatement (matched videos), or if it also affected irrelevant reinstatement (mismatched videos), we extracted the average Fisher-transformed correlation of all matched and mismatched pairs of videos separately, and z-scored them against a null distribution of correlation. This differs from the reinstatement measure reported above which consists of a correlation difference (matched – mismatched). Group-level statistics were performed on these z-scored values.

Univariate analyses

To determine whether HNT stimulation alters hippocampal activity, maps for both stimulation sessions were submitted to paired t-tests. Maps were first warped from native space to MNI space using affine transformation (3dAllineate), and masked by the left hippocampus, defined using the CA_ML_18_MNI atlas. The voxel-wise threshold was p < .005, with cluster size determined based on the spatial smoothness of the data using Monte Carlo simulations from the 3dClustSim tool in AFNI using the spatial autocorrelation function (ACF). Based on this procedure, the minimum cluster size at p = 0.005 for a combined threshold of p < 0.05 was determined to be 5 voxels. All univariate analyses were performed in AFNI.

Association between univariate activity and cortical reinstatement

We next examined the association between left univariate hippocampal activity and reinstatement in the left middle occipital gyrus. Average hippocampal activity was calculated for individual subjects by taking the average across all voxels encompassing the left hippocampus (CA_ML_18_MNI ROI reverse-transformed into native space using affine transformation). For each participant, we computed the trial-level correlation between mean hippocampal activity and reinstatement of a given trial. Trial-level reinstatement was calculated for each participant by taking the difference of the Fisher transformed correlation between the diagonal value for a trial (matched videos at encoding and retrieval) and the mean of the off-diagonal correlation values for that trial (mismatched videos). Correlation values were submitted to one-sample t-tests within stimulation sessions to determine if the association between univariate activity and reinstatement is greater than zero, and paired-sample t-tests were used to assess differences between stimulation sessions.

We also tested whether the effects of HNT stimulation on the stimulated cortex rather than on hippocampal activity were responsible for its effects on cortical reinstatement. We measured the trial-by-trial correlation between left middle occipital gyrus reinstatement and univariate activity at the subject-specific sites of stimulation, defined as average univariate activity within a 5-mm-radius sphere centred around each subject’s lateral parietal stimulation site, and submitted these correlation values to group-level statistics.

Encoding similarity

We performed a series of exploratory analyses examining similarity between patterns of neural activity for different videos at encoding. Encoding similarity was measured by computing the dissimilarity matrix between each video and all other videos at encoding using the cosmo_dissimilarity_matrix function in CoSMoMVPA [56]. This function computes the Pearson correlation distance between patterns of neural activity, resulting in a correlation matrix reflecting the dissimilarity of each video to every other video. Dissimilarity values were subtracted from 1 to reflect similarity, or the correlation between videos, and Fisher transformed. Each video/trial’s average correlation with all other trials was calculated across a row, including values in the upper triangle of the correlation matrix only, and excluding the diagonal value. We computed the within-subjects correlation between encoding similarity values and neural reinstatement in the left middle occipital gyrus, measured as the correlation difference between matched videos and the mean of mismatched videos for that trial. Finally, we computed the within-subjects correlation between encoding similarity and univariate left hippocampal activity. All correlations were performed using encoding similarity within the left middle occipital gyrus. Only trials that were self-reported as recollected (vividness score of > 1) were included in these analyses.

Supplementary Material

3

KEY RESOURCES TABLE

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER
Deposited Data
Subject IDs This paper Subject_List.csv
Software and Algorithms
AFNI NIH https://afni.nimh.nih.gov/
Localite GmBH https://www.localite.de/en/home/)
Presentation Neurobehavioral Systems https://www.neurobs.com/
MATLAB 2018a MathWorks https://www.mathworks.com/products/matlab.html
CoSMoMVPA toolbox Nikolaas N. Oosterhof & Andrew C. Connolly http://www.cosmomvpa.org/
Other
MagPro X100 stimulator connected to a Cool-B65 butterfly coil MagVenture https://www.magventure.com/us
Siemens 3 T Prisma Siemens https://www.siemens-healthineers.com/

Highlights.

  • TMS tested whether the hippocampus coordinates cortical memory reinstatement

  • Hippocampal network-targeted (HNT) TMS improved memory for video clips

  • HNT-TMS enhanced reinstatement of video-specific fMRI activity patterns in cortex

  • HNT-TMS altered coupling between hippocampal activity and cortical reinstatement

Acknowledgments

We thank Stephanie Wert, Brennan Durr, and Erica Karp for their assistance with data collection. This research was supported in part through the computational resources and staff contributions for Quest, the high-performance computing facility at Northwestern University, which is jointly supported by the Office of the Provost, the Office for Research, and Northwestern University Information Technology. This research was supported by R01-MH106512 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official view of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

Declaration of Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Marr D, Willshaw D, and McNaughton B (1971). Simple memory: a theory for archicortex. In From the Retina to the Neocortex, Vaina LM, ed. (Birkhäuser; ), pp. 59–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Norman KA, and O’Reilly RC (2003). Modeling Hippocampal and Neocortical Contributions to Recognition Memory: A Complementary Learning Systems Approach. Psychol. Rev 110, 611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Danker JF, and Anderson JR (2010). The ghosts of brain states past: remembering reactivates the brain regions engaged during encoding. Psychol. Bull 136, 87–102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Wing EA, Ritchey M, and Cabeza R (2015). Reinstatement of Individual Past Events Revealed by the Similarity of Distributed Activation Patterns during Encoding and Retrieval. J. Cogn. Neurosci 27, 679–691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Ritchey M, Wing EA, LaBar KS, and Cabeza R (2013). Neural similarity between encoding and retrieval is related to memory via hippocampal interactions. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2818–2828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Gordon AM, Rissman J, Kiani R, and Wagner AD (2014). Cortical reinstatement mediates the relationship between content-specific encoding activity and subsequent recollection decisions. Cereb. Cortex 24, 3350–3364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kriegeskorte N, Mur M, and Bandettini PA (2008). Representational similarity analysis-connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Front. Syst. Neurosci 2, 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rissman J, and Wagner AD (2012). Distributed Representations in Memory: Insights from Functional Brain Imaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol 63, 101–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Oedekoven CSH, Keidel JL, Berens SC, and Bird CM (2017). Reinstatement of memory representations for lifelike events over the course of a week. Sci. Rep, 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.St-Laurent M, Abdi H, and Buchsbaum BR (2015). Distributed Patterns of Reactivation Predict Vividness of Recollection. J. Cogn. Neurosci 27, 2000–2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bird CM, Keidel JL, Ing XLP, Horner XAJ, and Burgess N (2015). Consolidation of Complex Events via Reinstatement in Posterior Cingulate Cortex. 35, 14426–14434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mesulam M (1990). Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann. Neurol. Off. J. Am. Neurol. Assoc. Child Neurol. Soc 28, 597–613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Spreng RN, Mar RA, and Kim ASN (2008). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J. Cogn. Neurosci 21, 489–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kahn I, Andrews-Hanna JR, Vincent JL, Snyder AZ, and Buckner RL (2008). Distinct cortical anatomy linked to subregions of the medial temporal lobe revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol 100, 129–139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ranganath C, and Ritchey M (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 13, 713–726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ferguson MA, Lim C, Cooke D, Darby RR, Wu O, Rost NS, Corbetta M, Grafman J, and Fox MD (2019). A human memory circuit derived from brain lesions causing amnesia. Nat. Commun. 10, 3497. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hebscher M, and Voss JL (2020). Testing network properties of episodic memory using non-invasive brain stimulation. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci 32, 35–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Hermiller MS, VanHaerents S, Raij T, and Voss JL (2019). Frequency-specific noninvasive modulation of memory retrieval and its relationship with hippocampal network connectivity. Hippocampus 29, 595–609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Tambini A, Nee DE, and D’Esposito M (2018). Hippocampal-targeted Theta-burst Stimulation Enhances Associative Memory Formation. J. Cogn. Neurosci 30, 1452–1472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Thakral PP, Madore KP, Kalinowski SE, and Schacter DL (2020). Modulation of hippocampal brain networks produces changes in episodic simulation and divergent thinking. PNAS 117, 12729–12740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kim S, Nilakantan AS, Hermiller MS, Palumbo RT, Vanhaerents S, and Voss JL (2018). Selective and coherent activity increases due to stimulation indicate functional distinctions between episodic memory networks. Sci. Adv 4, eaar2768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Thakral PP, Madore KP, and Schacter DL (2017). A role for the left angular gyrus in episodic simulation and memory. J. Neurosci 37, 1319–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.St-Laurent M, Abdi H, and Buchsbaum BR (2016). Distributed Patterns of Reactivation Predict Vividness of Recollection. J. Cogn. Neurosci 10, 431–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ritchey M, Libby LA, and Ranganath C (2015). Cortico-hippocampal systems involved in memory and cognition: The PMAT framework. In Progress in Brain Research (Elsevier B.V.), pp. 45–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bonnici HM, Richter FR, Yazar Y, and Simons JS (2016). Multimodal Feature Integration in the Angular Gyrus during Episodic and Semantic Retrieval. J. Neurosci 36, 5462–5471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Mesulam M-M, Van Hoesen GW, Pandya DN, and Geschwind N (1977). Limbic and sensory connections of the inferior parietal lobule (area PG) in the rhesus monkey: a study with a new method for horseradish peroxidase histochemistry. Brain Res. 136, 393–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, and Schacter DL (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci 1124, 1–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dimsdale-Zucker HR, and Ranganath C (2019). Representational Similarity Analyses: A Practical Guide for Functional MRI Applications. Handb. Behav. Neurosci 28, 509–525. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kauttonen J, Hlushchuk Y, Jääskeläinen IP, and Tikka P (2018). Brain mechanisms underlying cue-based memorizing during free viewing of movie Memento. Neuroimage 172, 313–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Spaniol J, Davidson PSR, Kim ASN, Han H, Moscovitch M, and Grady CL (2009). Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia 47, 1765–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Wheeler ME, Petersen SE, and Buckner RL (2000). Memory’s echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A 97, 11125–11129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yonelinas AP, Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Kroll NEA, and Baynes K (2001). Hippocampal, parahippocampal and occipital-temporal contributions to associative and item recognition memory: an fMRI study. Neuroreport 12, 359–363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.St-Laurent M, Moscovitch M, and McAndrews MP (2016). The retrieval of perceptual memory details depends on right hippocampal integrity and activation. Cortex 84, 15–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ranganath C, Heller A, Cohen MX, Brozinsky CJ, and Rissman J (2005). Functional connectivity with the hippocampus during successful memory formation. Hippocampus 15, 997–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Warren KN, Hermiller MS, Nilakantan AS, and Voss JL (2019). Stimulating the Hippocampal posteriormedial network enhances task-dependent connectivity and memory. Elife 8, 1–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nilakantan AS, Mesulam M-M, Weintraub S, Karp EL, VanHaerents S, and Voss JL (2019). Network-targeted stimulation engages neurobehavioral hallmarks of age-related memory decline. Neurology 92, e2349–e2354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wang JX, Rogers LM, Gross EZ, Ryals AJ, Dokucu ME, Brandstatt KL, Hermiller MS, and Voss JL (2014). Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks and associative memory. Science (80-. ) 345, 1054–1057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Yazar Y, Bergström ZM, and Simons JS (2017). Reduced multimodal integration of memory features following continuous theta burst stimulation of angular gyrus. Brain Stimul. 10, 624–629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bonnici HM, Cheke LG, Green DAE, FitzGerald THMB, and Simons JS (2018). Specifying a causal role for angular gyrus in autobiographical memory. J. Neurosci 38, 10438–10443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Hebscher M, Meltzer J, and Gilboa A (2019). A causal role for the precuneus in network-wide theta and gamma oscillatory activity during complex memory retrieval. Elife 8, e43114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Yazar Y, Bergström ZM, and Simons JS (2014). Continuous Theta Burst Stimulation of Angular Gyrus Reduces Subjective Recollection. PLoS One 9, e110414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hebscher M, Ibrahim C, and Gilboa A (2020). Precuneus stimulation alters the neural dynamics of autobiographical memory retrieval. Neuroimage 210, 116575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Hermiller MS, Chen YF, Parrish TB, and Voss JL (2020). Evidence for immediate enhancement of hippocampal memory encoding by network-targeted theta-burst stimulation during concurrent fMRI. J. Neurosci 40, 7155–7168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Nilakantan AS, Bridge DJ, Gagnon EP, Haerents S.A. Van, and Voss JL (2017). Stimulation of the posterior cortical-hippocampal network enhances precision of memory recollection. Curr Biol 27, 465–470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Brunec IK, Robin J, Olsen RK, Moscovitch M, and Barense MD (2020). Integration and differentiation of hippocampal memory traces. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev 118, 196–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Shanahan LK, Gjorgieva E, Paller KA, Kahnt T, and Gottfried JA (2018). Odor-evoked category reactivation in human ventromedial prefrontal cortex during sleep promotes memory consolidation. Elife 7, 1–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Tompary A, Duncan K, and Davachi L (2016). High-resolution investigation of memory-specific reinstatement in the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex. Hippocampus 26, 995–1007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Liang JC, and Preston AR (2017). Medial temporal lobe reinstatement of content-specific details predicts source memory. Cortex 91, 67–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rutishauser U, Ye S, Koroma M, Tudusciuc O, Ross IB, Chung JM, and Mamelak AN (2015). Representation of retrieval confidence by single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe. Nat. Neurosci 18, 1041–1050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Teyler TJ, and DiScenna P (1986). The hippocampal memory indexing theory. Behav. Neurosci 100, 147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A, Avanzini G, Bestmann S, Berardelli A, Brewer C, Canli T, Cantello R, et al. (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol 120, 2008–2039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Cox RW (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res 29, 162–173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Stark CEL, and Squire LR (2001). When zero is not zero: the problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 98, 12760–12766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Benjamini Y, and Hochberg Y (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B 57, 289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Mumford JA, Turner BO, Ashby FG, and Poldrack RA (2012). Deconvolving BOLD activation in event-related designs for multivoxel pattern classification analyses. Neuroimage 59, 2636–2643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Oosterhof NN, Connolly AC, and Haxby JV (2016). CoSMoMVPA: multi-modal multivariate pattern analysis of neuroimaging data in Matlab/GNU Octave. Front. Neuroinform 10, 27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

3

Data Availability Statement

Data and code are available from the lead contact upon request.

RESOURCES