Abstract
Background
Public health campaigns aimed at curbing the spread of COVID-19 are important in reducing disease transmission, but traditional information-based campaigns have received unexpectedly extreme backlash.
Objective
This study aimed to investigate whether customizing of public service announcements (PSAs) providing health guidelines to match individuals’ identities increases their compliance.
Methods
We conducted a within- and between-subjects, randomized controlled cross-sectional, web-based study in July 2020. Participants viewed two PSAs: one advocating wearing a mask in public settings and one advocating staying at home. The control PSA only provided information, and the treatment PSAs were designed to appeal to the identities held by individuals; that is, either a Christian identity or an economically motivated identity. Participants were asked about their identity and then provided a control PSA and treatment PSA matching their identity, in random order. The PSAs were of approximately 100 words.
Results
We recruited 300 social media users from Amazon Mechanical Turk in accordance with usual protocols to ensure data quality. In total, 8 failed the data quality checks, and the remaining 292 were included in the analysis. In the identity-based PSA, the source of the PSA was changed, and a phrase of approximately 12 words relevant to the individual’s identity was inserted. A PSA tailored for Christians, when matched with a Christian identity, increased the likelihood of compliance by 12 percentage points. A PSA that focused on economic values, when shown to individuals who identified as economically motivated, increased the likelihood of compliance by 6 points.
Conclusions
Using social media to deliver COVID-19 public health announcements customized to individuals’ identities is a promising measure to increase compliance with public health guidelines.
Trial Registration
ISRCTN Registry 22331899; https://www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN22331899.
Keywords: Amazon Mechanical Turk, compliance, COVID-19, custom, effectiveness, guideline, identity, public health, public health announcement, public service announcement, social media, web-based health information
Introduction
Public compliance with recommended guidelines to limit the spread of SARS-CoV-2 and COVID-19 is an important component in combating the disease [1]. Current guidelines suggest several measures, such as wearing a mask and staying at home [2]; nonetheless, a large number of individuals fail to follow the guidelines provided by public health officials [3]. Public compliance to guidelines remains an issue [3-5].
Public service announcements (PSAs) have long been used to promote public health behaviors, although their success and the success of PSAs in general have been inconsistent [6]. Information-based PSAs are often successful because they present facts about the nature of a threat, explain the benefits of a response, and provide a clear call to action [7]. However, it remains unclear whether presenting information is sufficient in a posttruth era as the world battles the COVID-19 pandemic. There has been strong backlash against wearing masks and staying at home [3,5] and “irrational behavior” in noncompliance with COVID-19 policies [8]. Psychological reactance occurs when individuals feel that previously permitted behaviors are constrained by an external agent, which impugns their freedom [9,10]. In such situations, individuals resist the constraint and attempt to regain their lost freedom [9,10]. Thus, rather than helping, a PSA could backfire by sparking reactance, which triggers individuals to eschew the recommended behavior and even actively impair compliance [11].
The likelihood of reactance may also be increased by the divisions among people in the United States along ideological lines, with stark differences in the extent to which those on the political left and right wings believe that COVID-19 is a legitimate threat [1,12-14]. In the era of tribalism and distrust toward experts, identity has become as central to many arguments as scientific information [14-16]. The interaction of an individual’s identity with the source and content of the message can shape responses as much as the information that the message contains [17,18].
Individuals’ identities determine how they answer the question of “who am I?” [19-21]. According to the social identity theory, these answers are dependent upon both social and personal identities [19-22]. Social identities derive from the social groups to which individuals belong [22], such as race, nationality, and organizational or religious affiliations [23-25]. Personal identities derive from values that individuals consider important [20,25,26], such as volunteering [27], environmentalism [28], or the economy and economic values [29-31]. Both social and personal identities can be potent influences on behavior because people are motivated to act in ways that align with their identity in order to maintain a sense of self-consistency [22,25,32,33].
Persuasive messages such as PSAs can take advantage of this desire for identity-consistent actions by framing a proposed action as being consistent with individuals’ social or personal identity [34]. Speaking the language of an identity by using terms and arguments associated with that identity may render a more persuasive message [35]. Framing the advocated action as identity-consistent can further encourage individuals to adopt the desired behavior [34] because once an individual knows how others with the same identity act, it is easier to convince oneself to act in that same manner [1]. Thus, identity-framed messages are more persuasive than general messages [35].
This study aimed to investigate whether customizing PSAs in accordance with the source and language of a social identity (specifically Christian) or a personal identity (specifically economically motivated) increases the intention of individuals who identify with those identities to comply with the behaviors advocated by a PSA. We selected these identities because individuals who resist public health guidelines frequently provide religious [36,37] and economic [38] excuses. If individuals with these identities could be persuaded to follow public health guidelines, the benefits could be substantial [39]. We sought to investigate whether framing a PSA in accordance with a Christian social identity or an economically motivated personal identity increases the likelihood of compliance with COVID-19 guidelines among individuals who hold those identities.
Methods
Study Overview
We conducted a within- and between-subjects, randomized controlled cross-sectional, web-based study. All data were collected on the internet and no identifying information was collected in order to protect participants’ privacy and confidentiality. The study was reviewed by the institutional review board of Indiana University (protocol# 2004499544) and was determined to be an exempt study. The study was initiated with the institutional review board approving the study data and if participants consented to participate, they were enrolled in the study.
Participants
In July, 2020, we recruited 300 participants from Amazon Mechanical Turk in accordance with the usual protocols to ensure data quality [40]. We recruited participants only from the United States who held an Amazon Masters classification and included a captcha to preclude nonhuman responses. Participants were paid US $1.25 and spent an average of about 13 minutes participating in the study (minimum: 3.6 minutes, maximum 11 hours). In total, 8 subjects failed 1 or more of the 3 attention checks (that asked participants to select specific answers), thus yielding 292 participants. All participants received both the control and treatment conditions; hence, demographics are described at the study level. Approximately 49% of participants were female, and 82% were White, 8% were Asian, 7% were Black, and 3% were of other racial backgrounds. The median age of the study participants was 30 years (24%: 18-24 years, 33%: 25-34 years, 19%: 35-44 years, 16%: 45-54 years, 6%: 55-64 years, and 1%: ≥65 years).
Study Design and Interventions
At the beginning of the survey, subjects were asked a series of questions to ascertain their identification with Christianity and with the economic health of the country (our selected identities). Participants were provided either the Christian-framed treatment or economy-framed PSAs depending on which identity they identified with more. If subjects identified with both identities equally, they were randomly assigned to 1 of the 2 treatments. The experimental design is presented in Figure 1.
Figure 1.
Experimental design. PSA: public service announcement.
Participants then received two COVID-19 PSAs in random order: 1 advocating wearing a mask and 1 calling on people to stay at home. One was a control PSA with information purportedly from the US Public Health Service and the other was an identity-framed PSA (either Christian-framed or economy-framed). After reading each PSA, participants reported the extent to which they would engage in the advocated behavior. Each subject received 1 identity-framed PSA and 1 control PSA; thus, we could examine the within-person effects of PSA framing and could control for differences in compliance between mask-wearing and staying at home.
The PSAs were of approximately 100 words. The treatments changed the source of the PSA and 1 sentence of their content. The identity-framed PSA contained a single short, substituted phrase designed to appeal either to people who held a Christian social identity (purportedly written by the Chaplain of the US Senate) or to those for whom protecting the country’s economy was a central feature of their personal identity (purportedly written by the US Chamber of Commerce). For example, the control PSA for wearing a mask stated, “You should wear a mask whenever you are in public and see other people,” which was replaced by, “We have a Christian duty to love our neighbors, and wearing a mask whenever you are in public and see other people is a way you can do this” in the Christian-framed PSA and by, “We now know how you can do your part to help us safely reopen our economy: wearing a mask whenever you are in public and see other people” in the economy-framed PSA. All PSAs are provided in Multimedia Appendix 1.
Measurements
In accordance with previous studies, Christian identity, economically motivated identity, and trust in the 3 PSA sources were measured with single-item 7-point Likert scales [41-43]. Identity-framed PSAs were deemed to be aligned with the participant’s identity when the participant’s identity scores were 6 or 7 and the participant did not distrust the source (ie, trust in the source was ≥4). The likelihood of compliance was measured using 7 items adapted from previous studies [44,45]. The outcome compliance items were measured using scales of 0-100, not the same 7-point scales as those used for the independent variables to classify participants, to reduce the risk of a common method bias [46]. The likelihood of compliance proved reliable (Cronbach α=.94), thus indicating convergent validity. An exploratory factor analysis was performed to assess the discriminant validity among the constructs, which was satisfactory. The results of the items and factor analysis are presented in Multimedia Appendix 1.
Statistical Analysis
A power analysis using G*Power [47] determined that a sample of 300 participants with this design would provide a power of .93 to detect a small effect size (Cohen f=0.10). We analyzed the data using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) [48] with robust standard errors using HLM for Windows (version 6.00, Scientific Software International, Inc). HLM accounts for the correlation among the repeated within-subject observations and facilitates the assessment of the alignment or nonalignment of the identity-framed treatment condition with the participant’s identity. All tests were 2-tailed with a significance level of α=.05 α=.05. We used the formula of Snijders and Bosker [49] to calculate R2. The likelihood of complying was nonnormally distributed. For the PSA advocating mask-wearing, the mean likelihood of compliance was 78.75 (SD 29.79), and the median likelihood was 92.93; for the PAS advocating staying at home, the mean likelihood of compliance was 74.51 (SD 29.67), and the median likelihood was 86.75. HLM is robust to departures from the normality assumption for large samples such as this one [48].
Results
Table 1 shows the mean (SD) values of the likelihood of compliance under different experimental conditions. Overall, participants displayed more willingness to wear a mask than to stay at home. Furthermore, we observed higher means for identity-aligned PSAs than for non–identity-aligned PSAs. Table 2 shows the results of statistical analysis. The overall model has a large effect size, with an R2 of 41.6%.
Table 1.
Means for the likelihood of complying with public service announcements (PSAs). Data were collected in July 2020 from 292 participants from Amazon Mechanical Turk, who viewed two PSAs: 1 advocating wearing a mask and 1 advocating staying at home. One PSA was an information-based control PSA and the other was a Christian or economically motivated identity–framed PSA.
PSA type | Likelihood of staying at home | Likelihood of wearing a mask | |||
|
Mean (SD) |
Number of participants | Mean (SD) |
Number of participants | |
Economy-framed PSA when aligned | 78.94 (21.54) |
65 | 84.26 (19.68) |
78 | |
Christian-framed PSA when aligned | 81.99 (26.29) |
31 | 84.62 (27.97) |
24 | |
Nonaligned PSA | 68.09 (34.59) |
51 | 63.35 (37.76) |
43 | |
Control PSA | 71.09 (31.11) |
145 | 77.85 (30.85) |
147 |
Table 2.
Analysis results with beta coefficients (β) for identity-framed public service announcements (PSAs) and information-based control PSAs (584 observations; 292 participants; R2=41.6%).
Parameter | Likelihood of complying with PSAs | ||||
|
β (SE) | P value | |||
Economy-framed PSA | −3.60 (2.19) | .10 | |||
PSA aligned with an economically motivated identity | 6.77 (2.65) | .01 | |||
Christian-framed PSA | −6.86 (4.11) | .10 | |||
PSA aligned with a Christian identity | 12.74 (4.94) | .01 | |||
PSA advocating mask-wearing | 4.06 (1.23) | .001 | |||
Christian identity | −1.60 (0.64) | .01 | |||
Economically-motivated identity | −2.56 (1.41) | .07 | |||
Measure of trust | |||||
|
Trust in the US Public Health Service | 12.40 (1.12) | .00 | ||
|
Trust in the Senate Chaplain | −0.12 (0.64) | .92 | ||
|
Trust in the Chamber of Commerce | −1.42 (1.21) | .24 | ||
Constant | 74.02 (1.61) | .00 |
When participants received an identity-framed PSA that was aligned with their identity, they were more likely to comply with it rather than a purely information-based PSA in the control treatment (Christian-framed PSA: P=.01; economy-framed PSA: P=.01). The effects were significant, increasing compliance by almost 13% to the Christian-framed PSA (95% CI 2.9-22.6) and almost 7% to the economy-framed PSA (95% CI 1.5-12.1) compared to the control non–identity-framed PSA. The average effect sizes (Cohen d) for the Christian-framed and economy-framed PSAs were 0.30 and 0.24, respectively, which are between small and medium. This is congruent with our predictions that providing individuals with customized PSAs that align with their identities will increase their intention to comply with the advocated behaviors including staying at home or wearing a mask in public.
When participants received an identity-framed PSA that was not aligned with their identity, it did not significantly influence their likelihood of complying, although both nonaligned PSAs approached significance with negative coefficients (Christian-framed PSA: P=.10; economy-framed PSA: P=.10), which suggests that a nonaligned PSA may potentially be more damaging to compliance than a control PSA. Compliance was significantly greater for PSAs advocating mask-wearing than for those advocating staying at home (P=.001), which suggests that our participants were more likely to comply with the practice of wearing a mask than staying at home. Likewise, the main effects of some of the other control variables that we used to assess alignment (ie, trust and identity) were significant and some were not.
Discussion
Principal Findings
Our study shows that modifying PSAs to leverage social and personal identities can promote increased compliance with public health guidelines for individuals who hold these identities. The Christian-framed PSA increased compliance by approximately 12.74 points (out of 100) when viewed by those with a Christian identity, and the economy-framed PSA increased compliance by approximately 6.77 points when viewed by those with an economically motivated identity. One might conclude that the Christian framing is more powerful, but this only applies to individuals who hold that identity. Hence, we believe that it is better to compare the 2 different identity-framed PSAs to their own controls, not to each other, as the differences in the coefficients between the 2 identity-aligned PSAs may have resulted from various subject-level factors that influence their different identities. For example, those with a Christian identity were slightly less likely to comply (P=.01) with any PSA, possibly because those with conservative beliefs are less likely to believe that COVID-19 is a legitimate threat [1,14].
Based on the social identity theory, we hypothesized that messages designed to activate an identity by using inclusive language and a consistent message source would be more effective in increasing compliance to PSAs rather than those without consistent identity-framing. Identity-framing was intended to emphasize commonalities between the individual and like-group members and to encourage users to act in accordance with those in their group [1]. Identity-framing harnesses the relevant identity that the individual holds and appeals to those relevant traits. This simple act of creating identity-aligned targeted PSAs significantly increased compliance with the behavior advocated in the PSAs. We decided to examine a Christian identity and an economically motivated identity on the basis of excuses that are commonly invoked to justify noncompliance [36-38]. We found both to be effective in increasing compliance. Various other social or personal identities may also be effective in increasing the compliance to PSAs.
The promise of identity-framed PSAs is noteworthy in view of the myriad of rampant rumors, misinformation, and disinformation regarding COVID-19 [12-14]. Rapidly developing situations, uncertainty, and fear foster the spread of false information (created with or without the deliberate intention to mislead people). It is unfortunate that individuals’ responses to COVID-19 have implied that the provision of simple, information-based PSAs sometimes triggered psychological reactance and led to actions that disrupt public health efforts [1,13]. Our results show that designing PSAs to appeal to specific target demographics can increase their effectiveness beyond that of a message that simply provides correct information.
The vast amount of information about individuals available on social media platforms makes it practical to create multiple versions of a PSA and share the most individually relevant version with people, thereby making the message more persuasive [50]. Social media is an attractive channel to rapidly reach many people as it has more than 2 billion active users [50]. Identity-framed PSAs could facilitate public health goals by enabling the PSA to influence people who would otherwise ignore the message, capturing their attention by speaking their language, activating relevant identities, and reducing psychological reactance by framing the actions as being consistent with their identity [25,35]. By encouraging people to view a situation through the lens of a supportive identity, the effects of countervailing identities that dissuade people from the desired outcome can be reduced. Public Health agencies and nonprofits should take advantage of these tools when designing future public awareness campaigns. By leveraging readily available identity information to make minor adjustments to the framing of PSAs, such groups could facilitate higher compliance with public health guidelines, thus enabling better outcomes. With increasing ease of accessing personal data, the small cost of targeting a PSA toward those individuals that would best respond (similar to targeted advertising) has the potential to yield enormous benefits with increased nationwide health outcomes.
We examined 1 social identity (Christian) and 1 personal identity (economically motivated) linked to noncompliance [36-38]. Many other identities may also be leveraged to enhance the effectiveness of PSAs. By reminding people of a role identity as a parent or grandchild, leveraging social group identities such as sport team loyalty, or by appealing to their self-identity as caring individuals, many opportunities are available to use identities to help persuade people to follow public health guidelines. Our use of 2 identities shows that this method can be successful; however, it is unclear which other identities may also be used. Future studies are required to investigate why some identities may be effective when used to increase compliance while others may not be effective. Furthermore, it remains unclear whether compliance is affected more by the relationship between the identity and the target behavior or that between the identity and the individual, or whether both relationships are equally important.
While we control for trust toward the source of the PSA, a potentially interesting question is one regarding the mediating influence of variables (eg, perceived similarity to the identity used in the PSA). While we did not assess perceived similarity, we controlled for the strength of the identity. Since our study suggests that identity-framed PSAs can be used to influence compliance, more studies are needed to investigate the potential mediating and moderating variables that could strengthen or weaken the effectiveness of identity-framed PSAs. For example, our identity-framed PSA contained only 1 modified sentence; however, it remains unclear whether adding more identity-related framing would increase its effectiveness, or whether it is sufficient to simply invoke the identity as in our PSAs. Moreover, it remains unclear whether the source of the message is a critical factor, or whether the message more important than the source.
Limitations
This study has the usual limitations of randomized controlled cross-sectional study. We assessed self-reported perceptions at one point in time, not actual behavior over several periods of time. Our participants were those who participate in research studies and thus may differ from those who decline to participate in research studies.
Conclusions
Compliance with public health measures designed to mitigate the COVID-19 pandemic has unfortunately become intertwined with identity, and individuals with certain social and personal identities are less likely to comply with the behaviors advocated in the PSAs. Our study shows that identity can also be an effective factor to induce compliance. The development of identity-framed PSAs may be effective in contexts beyond the COVID-19 pandemic. The most important factors to consider when developing effective PSAs are that the identities in question are deeply held and can be associated with recommended actions when coordinated efforts across society are needed.
Abbreviations
- HLM
hierarchical linear modeling
- PSA
public service announcement
Appendix
Experimental Materials.
CONSORT-EHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.1).
Footnotes
Authors' Contributions: ARD had full access to all the data and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Furthermore, ARD supervised the study and performed the statistical analysis. AK acquired the data. All authors conceived and designed the study, drafted the manuscript, and provided administrative, technical, and material support.
Conflicts of Interest: None declared.
Editorial Notice
This randomized study was only retrospectively registered as, according to the authors, their field has not adopted pre-registration as convention. The editor granted an exception from ICMJE rules mandating prospective registration of randomized trials because the risk of bias appears low. However, readers are advised to carefully assess the validity of any potential explicit or implicit claims related to primary outcomes or effectiveness, as retrospective registration does not prevent authors from changing their outcome measures retrospectively.
References
- 1.Maher PJ, MacCarron P, Quayle M. Mapping public health responses with attitude networks: the emergence of opinion-based groups in the UK's early COVID-19 response phase. Br J Soc Psychol. 2020 Jul;59(3):641–652. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12396. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/32621294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. [2020-05-09]. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/prevent-getting-sick/prevention.html.
- 3.Lehmann EY, Lehmann LS. Responding to Patients Who Refuse to Wear Masks During the Covid-19 Pandemic. J Gen Intern Med. 2020 Oct 27;:1–2. doi: 10.1007/s11606-020-06323-x. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/33111236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Kahn R. Masks, Culture Wars, and Public Health Expertise: Confessions of a Mask 'Expert' (forthcoming) University of St. Thomas Law Journal. 2020 https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3644610. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Forsyth DR. Group-level resistance to health mandates during the COVID-19 pandemic: A groupthink approach. Group Dyn Theory Res Pract. 2020 Sep;24(3):139–152. doi: 10.1037/gdn0000132. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.O'Keefe GJ, Reid K. The Uses and Efiects of Public Service Advertising. J Public Relat Res. 1990 Jan;2(1-4):67–91. doi: 10.1207/s1532754xjprr0201-4_3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McGuire W. Theoretical Foundations of Campaigns. In: Rice RE, Atkin CK, editors. Public Communication Campaigns. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Paakkari L, Okan O. COVID-19: health literacy is an underestimated problem. Lancet Public Health. 2020 May;5(5):e249–e250. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30086-4. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S2468-2667(20)30086-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Brehm JW, Stires LK, Sensenig J, Shaban J. The attractiveness of an eliminated choice alternative. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1966 Jul;2(3):301–313. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(66)90086-2. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Miron AM, Brehm JW. Reactance Theory - 40 Years Later. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie. 2006 Jan;37(1):9–18. doi: 10.1024/0044-3514.37.1.9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Siegel JT, Lienemann BA, Rosenberg BD. Resistance, reactance, and misinterpretation: Highlighting the challenge of persuading people with depression to seek help. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2017 Jun 05;11(6):e12322. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12322. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ahmed W, López Seguí F, Vidal-Alaball J, Katz MS. COVID-19 and the "Film Your Hospital" Conspiracy Theory: Social Network Analysis of Twitter Data. J Med Internet Res. 2020 Oct 05;22(10):e22374. doi: 10.2196/22374. https://www.jmir.org/2020/10/e22374/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Douglas KM. COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process Intergroup Relat. 2021 Mar 04;24(2):270–275. doi: 10.1177/1368430220982068. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Calvillo DP, Ross BJ, Garcia RJB, Smelter TJ, Rutchick AM. Political Ideology Predicts Perceptions of the Threat of COVID-19 (and Susceptibility to Fake News About It) Soc Psychol Person Sci. 2020 Jul 22;11(8):1119–1128. doi: 10.1177/1948550620940539. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ball P, Maxmen A. The epic battle against coronavirus misinformation and conspiracy theories. Nature. 2020 May;581(7809):371–374. doi: 10.1038/d41586-020-01452-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Plohl N, Musil B. Modeling compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: the critical role of trust in science. Psychol Health Med. 2021 Jan;26(1):1–12. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2020.1772988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Maoz I, Ward A, Katz M, Ross L. Reactive Devaluation of an “Israeli” vs. “Palestinian” Peace Proposal. J Confl Resolut. 2016 Jul 01;46(4):515–546. doi: 10.1177/0022002702046004003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cruwys T, Stevens M, Greenaway KH. A social identity perspective on COVID-19: Health risk is affected by shared group membership. Br J Soc Psychol. 2020 Jul;59(3):584–593. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12391. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/32474966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ashforth BE, Harrison SH, Corley KG. Identification in Organizations: An Examination of Four Fundamental Questions. J Manag. 2008 Mar 07;34(3):325–374. doi: 10.1177/0149206308316059. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Ellemers N. Social Identity Theory. In: Levine JM, Hogg MA, editors. Encyclopedia of Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2010. pp. 798–801. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ashforth BE, Schinoff BS. Identity Under Construction: How Individuals Come to Define Themselves in Organizations. Annu Rev Organ Psychol Organ Behav. 2016 Mar 21;3(1):111–137. doi: 10.1146/annurev-orgpsych-041015-062322. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hornsey M. Social Identity Theory and Self‐categorization Theory: A Historical Review. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2008;2(1):204–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00066.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dukerich JM, Ashforth BE. Role Transitions in Organizational Life: An Identity-Based Perspective. Acad Manag Rev. 2001 Oct;26(4):670. doi: 10.2307/3560250. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jackson RL, Hogg MA. Encyclopedia of Identity. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2010. Self-Concept; pp. 675–677. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ashforth BE, Mael F. Social Identity Theory and the Organization. Acad Manag Rev. 1989 Jan;14(1):20–39. doi: 10.5465/amr.1989.4278999. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Brewer MB. The Social Self: On Being the Same and Different at the Same Time. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2016 Jul 02;17(5):475–482. doi: 10.1177/0146167291175001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Hitlin S. Values as the Core of Personal Identity: Drawing Links between Two Theories of Self. Soc Psychol Q. 2003 Jun;66(2):118. doi: 10.2307/1519843. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Dermody J, Koenig-Lewis N, Zhao AL, Hanmer-Lloyd S. Appraising the influence of pro-environmental self-identity on sustainable consumption buying and curtailment in emerging markets: Evidence from China and Poland. J Bus Res. 2018 May;86:333–343. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2017.09.041. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Halstead M, Taylor MJ. Values in Education and Education in Values. London: Taylor & Francis Group; 1995. Dec 20, Values and Values Education in Schools; pp. 3–14. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Mikko Vesala K, Peura J, McElwee G. The split entrepreneurial identity of the farmer. Jrnl of Small Bus Ente Dev. 2007 Feb 27;14(1):48–63. doi: 10.1108/14626000710727881. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Miller P, Rose N. Production, identity, and democracy. Theor Soc. 1995 Jun;24(3):427–467. doi: 10.1007/BF00993353. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gecas V. The Self as a Social Force. In: Owens TJ, Stryker S, Goodman N, editors. Extending Self-Esteem Theory and Research. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2001. pp. 85–100. [Google Scholar]
- 33.Shamir B. Meaning, Self and Motivation in Organizations. Organ Stud. 2016 Jul 01;12(3):405–424. doi: 10.1177/017084069101200304. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bator RJ, Cialdini RB. New Ways to Promote Proenvironmental Behavior: The Application of Persuasion Theory to the Development Of Effective Proenvironmental Public Service Announcements. J Social Isssues. 2000 Jan;56(3):527–541. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00182. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Nelson TE, Garst J. Values-based Political Messages and Persuasion: Relationships among Speaker, Recipient, and Evoked Values. Polit Psychol. 2005 Aug;26(4):489–516. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2005.00428.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.DeFranza D, Lindow M, Harrison K, Mishra A, Mishra H. Religion and reactance to COVID-19 mitigation guidelines. Am Psychol. 2020 Aug 10; doi: 10.1037/amp0000717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Singh DE. Role of Religions in the Spread of COVID-19. J Ecum Stud. 2020;55(2):289–310. doi: 10.1353/ecu.2020.0019. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Prosser AMB, Judge M, Bolderdijk JW, Blackwood L, Kurz T. 'Distancers' and 'non-distancers'? The potential social psychological impact of moralizing COVID-19 mitigating practices on sustained behaviour change. Br J Soc Psychol. 2020 Jul;59(3):653–662. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12399. http://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/32584437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Zhang K, Vilches TN, Tariq M, Galvani AP, Moghadas SM. The impact of mask-wearing and shelter-in-place on COVID-19 outbreaks in the United States. Int J Infect Dis. 2020 Dec;101:334–341. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2020.10.002. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S1201-9712(20)32204-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Steelman ZR, Hammer BI, Limayem M. Data Collection in the Digital Age: Innovative Alternatives to Student Samples. MISQ. 2014 Feb 2;38(2):355–378. doi: 10.25300/misq/2014/38.2.02. https://misq.org/data-collectoin-in-the-digital-age-innovative-alterantives-to-student-samples.html. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Reysen S, Katzarska-Miller I, Nesbit SM, Pierce L. Further validation of a single-item measure of social identification. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2013 Aug 05;:463–470. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1973. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Postmes T, Haslam SA, Jans L. A single-item measure of social identification: reliability, validity, and utility. Br J Soc Psychol. 2013 Dec;52(4):597–617. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Süssenbach P, Gollwitzer M, Mieth L, Buchner A, Bell R. Trustworthy Tricksters: Violating a Negative Social Expectation Affects Source Memory and Person Perception When Fear of Exploitation Is High. Front Psychol. 2016;7:2037. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02037. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Venkatesh V, Morris MG, Davis GB, Davis FD. User Acceptance of Information Technology: Toward a Unified View. MISQ. 2003;27(3):425. doi: 10.2307/30036540. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Dennis A, Clay P, Ko D. From Individual Cognition to Social Ecosystem: A Structuration Model of Enterprise Systems Use. AIS Trans Hum-Comput Interact. 2017 Dec 31;9(4):301–338. doi: 10.17705/1thci.00100. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee J, Podsakoff NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. 2003 Oct;88(5):879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007 May;39(2):175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Radenbush SW, Bryk AS. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Analysis Methods (2nd edition) Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001. Dec, [Google Scholar]
- 49.Snijders TAB, Bosker RJ. Multilevel Analysis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling (2nd edition) London: Sage Publications; 1999. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Voorveld HA. Brand Communication in Social Media: A Research Agenda. J Advert. 2019 Apr 09;48(1):14–26. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2019.1588808. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Experimental Materials.
CONSORT-EHEALTH checklist (V 1.6.1).