Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2020 Dec 13;62(2):170–184. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12700

Longitudinal pathways of engagement, social interaction skills, hyperactivity and conduct problems in preschool children

Berit M Gustafsson 1,2,3,, Per A Gustafsson 1,4, Mats Granlund 3,5, Marie Proczkowska 1, Lena Almqvist 3,6
PMCID: PMC8048902  PMID: 33314170

Abstract

Preschool children’s engagement/social interaction skills can be seen as aspects of positive functioning, and also act as protective aspects of functioning. On the other hand, hyperactivity/conduct problems are risk aspects that negatively affect children’s everyday functioning. Few studies have investigated such orchestrated effects on mental health in young children over time. The aims of the study are first, to identify homogeneous groups of children having similar pathways in mental health between three time points. Second, to examine how children move between time points in relation to risk and protective factors. Alongitudinal study over 3 years, including 197 Swedish preschool children was used. Questionnaire data collected from preschool teachers. Statistical analysis using person‐oriented methods with repeated cluster analyses. Children high in engagement/social skills and low in conduct problems continue to function well. Children with low engagement/social skills exhibiting both hyperactivity and conduct problems continue to have problems. Children with mixed patterns of protective factors and risk factors showed mixed outcomes. The stability of children’s pathways was quite high if they exhibited many positive protective factors but also if they exhibited many risk factors. Children exhibiting a mixed pattern of protective and risk factors moved between clusters in a less predictable way. That stability in mental health was related to the simultaneous occurrence of either many protective factors or many risk factors supports the notion of orchestrated effects. The results indicate that early interventions need to have a dual focus, including both interventions aimed at enhancing child engagement and interventions focused on decreasing behavior problems.

Keywords: Preschool children, engagement, hyperactivity, conduct problems, risk indicators

Introduction

Children’s mental health has primarily been studied as a lack of mental health problems or, for young children, a lack of behavior problems (Goodman, 1997). However, good mental health can also be seen as engagement in everyday activities. According to earlier studies, engagement is negatively correlated with behavior problems (Sjöman, Granlund & Almqvist, 2015). In mental health studies of adults, Westerhof and Keyes et al. (2010) propose that mental health and mental ill‐health exist on two different continua, with mental health able to co‐exist with mental health problems but also to protect from mental ill‐health. According to the World Health Organisation (2019), mental health is a state of wellbeing in which every individual realizes his or her own potential, can cope with the normal stresses of life, can work productively and fruitfully and is able to make a contribution to his or her community. In children, positive expressions of mental health relate to active participation in everyday life (Augustine, Lygnegård, Granlund & Adolfsson, 2018). Participation has two dimensions: being there and being engaged while being there. Engagement can be described as the extent to which the child is actively involved in daily activities such as play and social interaction with adults or other children in a manner relevant to the context and the child’s functional level (McWilliam, Bailey, Bailey & Wolery, 1992). Engagement may protect from mental health problems by enhancing mental health.

The orchestrated effects of protective factors and risk factors

Although most young children who are highly engaged in everyday activities display low levels of behavior problems, some will depart from this pattern (Sjöman et al., 2016). It is also possible to display low levels of engagement in the absence of behavior problems or emotional symptoms (Almqvist, Sjöman, Golsäter & Granlund, 2018). Engaged children usually have positive social interactions with both adults and peers. This is not necessarily true, however, when studying this relationship for the individual child (Almqvist, 2006). Therefore, a profile approach considering several factors simultaneously is necessary when studying trajectories of mental health in young children. We hypothesize that several mental health profiles characterized by different combinations of engagement, social interaction and behavior problems are detectable within a limited sample of preschool children. In this study, we explore mental health in groups of young children having homogeneous profiles with varying levels of engagement, social interaction skills, hyperactivity and conduct problems. These profiles represent interrelated, but distinctly different, aspects of mental health.

Mental health profiles probably evolve differently over time and also differ in stability, thus leading to different mental health outcomes. These differences are related to several factors, both within the child and in the environment (Beernink, Swinkels & Buitelaar, 2007; Henricsson & Rydell, 2006; Jones & Forehand, 2003). Some of the influential intrinsic factors increase the risk of mental health problems, such as behavior problems or emotional symptoms (Jones & Forehand, 2003). Others are protective of mental health, such as children’s engagement and social skills (Henricsson & Rydell, 2006). In addition, some factors are static over time (e.g., sex), while others are dynamic over time (e.g., peer interaction in preschool) and therefore possible to change. This assumption is based on the revised bioecological model presented by Bronfenbrenner and Evans (2000). Personal intrinsic factors mentioned above affect the child–environment fit (Bergman, Cairns, Nilsson & Nystedt, 2000; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006). The exploration of a combined effect over time of both positive and negative factors on children’s mental health can generate a more holistic picture than if single variables are studied in isolation (Magnusson & Stattin, 2006). Thus, in order to identify dysfunctional and/or functional mental health pathways, several intrinsic and environmental factors need to be investigated simultaneously (Hatakenaka & Hirano, 2015; Rutter, 1994; Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008).

Factors known to affect mental health when orchestrated

Research focusing on older children indicates that, when several risk factors co‐occur, the prevalence of mental health problems increases (Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008). Conversely, a smaller number of risk indicators coincides with a reduced occurrence of mental health problems (Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008). This may also be true for younger children. Together with one or several protective factors, resilience may further increase and the child may function and develop well despite the presence of risks.

Protective and risk factors related to children’s mental health profiles are nested within the child’s microsystem, including family and preschool environment (Bergman et al., 2000; Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006; Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008). Characteristics such as age, gender and mother tongue are static child factors and may occur with dynamic factors within different preschool structures, such as child–teacher ratio, age‐homogeneous or heterogeneous group compositions and the number of children entitled to mother tongue in the preschool group (Almqvist et al., 2018; Bradley & Corwyn, 2002). Dynamic factors in the preschool environment, considered in this study, include teacher responsiveness, peer‐to‐child interaction and collaboration with parents (Coolahan, Fantuzzo, Mendez & McDermott, 2000; Luttropp & Granlund, 2010; Petrenko, 2013).

The child’s interaction with the environment and the environment’s response to the child are important in the socioemotional development of the child (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; Denham, Bassett, Mincic et al., 2012). Hyperactive behavior and/or conduct problems often manifest as low frustration tolerance and difficulty controlling emotions and impulses. Such behavior affects children’s engagement and interaction with others, leading to a higher risk of negative social experiences (Campbell, Halperin & Sonuga‐Barke, 2014; Shaw, Stringaris, Nigg & Leibenluft, 2015). Children with behavior problems may attract more negative attention from teachers, peers and parents, and have a higher risk of experiencing conflicts (Barkley, 2014; Dodge, Lansford, Burks et al., 2003). In addition, children with conduct problems may experience less closeness and lower social response from teachers (Denham, McKinley, Couchoud & Holt, 1990; Nurmi, 2012). Difficulties with activity competence (developmental delay in body functions, cognition and language) may also affect children’s functioning in an array of everyday activities (Andersson, Martin, Brodd & Almqvist, 2016; Lewis, 2011; Prior, Bavin, Cini, Eadie & Reilly, 2011).

This study

A child’s mental health profile is affected over time by factors intrinsic to the child, other characteristics of the child, such as developmental delay or other mother tongue, and risk and protective factors in the preschool, such as child–teacher ratio and collaboration with parents, and the child–environment fit may vary (Denham et al., 1990; Prior et al., 2011; Wlodarczyk, Pawils, Metzner, Kriston, Klasen & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2017). Thus, it is not meaningful or representative to assume that the total group average values (Bergman, Magnusson & El‐Khouri, 2003) for a single variable, as commonly used in studies of young children, are representative of the mental health of all children. Rather, we can expect to find a number of mental health profiles that over time display mental health pathways differently related to protective and risk factors. We assume that the accumulation of risk or protective factors is a better predictor of mental health pathways than single risk or protective factors. Identifying patterns of risk and protective factors may thus contribute to the development of better interventions.

Aims

The aims of this study were, first, to identify homogeneous groups of children with similar profiles in mental health for three time points and, second, to examine developmental pathways of mental health profiles over time, and to study how these are related to risk and protective factors at different ecological levels.

Hypothesis

High‐risk functional profiles and stable pathways between such profiles are expected to be associated with a larger number of risk factors and fewer protective factors, while low‐risk functional profiles and stable pathways between such profiles are expected to be associated with fewer risk factors and more protective factors.

Method

Procedures

Cluster analyses were performed for each year of a 3‐year longitudinal study (2012–2014) of preschool children. Preschools from a stratified sample of various‐sized Swedish municipalities were invited to participate (Granlund et al., 2016; Gustafsson, 2019; Gustafsson, Gustafsson & Proczkowska‐Bjorklund, 2016; Gustafsson, Proczkowska‐Björklund & Gustafsson, 2017; Sjöman et al., 2015). The management of 31 preschools in various municipalities were contacted and consent was requested for the participation of their preschool units (92 preschool classes). The preschool teachers then informed the parents and asked for written informed consent. The preschool teachers answered the questionnaires at three time points, basing their answers on their knowledge of the child covering a period of at least two months. Their evaluation was based on how the child had acted during the previous 2 weeks. The questionnaires could be filled in by different teachers for different children in the same class. The instruction was that the same preschool teacher should fill in the questionnaire on any given child at the different times points. The researcher visited the preschool at each data collection for information, questionnaire management and to answer the preschool teachers’ and parents’ questions about the study (Gustafsson, 2019, 2016; Gustafsson et al., 2016).

This study is one of several sub‐studies in a longitudinal project (Granlund, Almqvist, Gustafsson et al., 2016; Gustafsson, 2019; Gustafsson et al., 2016; Gustafsson, Proczkowska‐Björklund & Gustafsson, 2017). All the sub‐studies were planned in advance and were approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Linköping (Dnr 2012/199‐31). Preschool management, preschool teachers and both parents of all children provided written informed consent. In Sweden it is mandated that both parents give consent when it comes to asking about “sensitive issues” for material involving healthy children. All questionnaires were given a code number, and the coding key was kept separate from the questionnaires after the data was collected. If preschool teachers identified children with previously unknown mental health problems during the course of the study, they were instructed to refer them to Child Healthcare for support.

Participants

In the first year, 651 children were included in the project. One hundred and ninety‐seven children participated in all three data collections (110 (56%) boys, 87 (44%) girls), with a mean age of 2 years and 8 months, or 32 months (SD = 9, range 15–57). They came from 43 different preschool classes, with between one and five participants in each class. There were 174 children who could have been included (regarding their age in data collection year 1) who did not participate in all data collections due to the child being moved to another preschool or because the preschool teachers could not answer the questionnaires due to lack of time in their work situation in year 2 and/or year 3. The only significant difference (p = 0.019) between these two groups was the proportion of mother tongue other than Swedish (participant group 23%, nonparticipant group 34%).

Instruments

Instruments used to define clusters

Cluster variables were chosen based on previous knowledge about children’s mental health and functioning in preschool (Bergman & Wångby, 2014; Frønes, 1995; Fuchs, Klein, Otto & von Klitzing, 2013; Goodman, 1997; Sterba & Bauer, 2010; Van Lier, der Ende, Koot & Verhulst, 2007). The cluster variables were specific child variables, including both positive variables, such as engagement and social interaction skills in child‐to‐other‐children encounters, and negative variables, such as hyperactivity and conduct problems. Hyperactivity was the most stable variable over time (Gustafsson, Danielsson, Granlund, Gustafsson & Proczkowska, 2018). The focus of the analysis was on typical pathways between cluster profiles based on these variables at three time points.

Children’s Engagement Questionnaire (CEQ)

Engagement was measured, by the preschool teacher, using the Children’s Engagement Questionnaire (McWilliam, 1991). This is a 32‐item instrument designed to rate preschool children’s global engagement by free recall impressions as: (1) “not at all typical,” (2) “somewhat typical,” (3) “typical,” or (4) “very typical.” Translation of the CEQ into Swedish resulted in minor adaptations and the use of 29 of the original 32 items (Almqvist, 2006), and this 29‐item version was used in this study. The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient for internal consistency was 0.92 for teachers’ ratings (Almqvist, 2006). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.94 for the total scale.

Social interaction skills in preschool questionnaire

Social interaction with other children and teachers were measured using two subscales in the questionnaire, in which preschool teachers rated their experiences of different types of social interactions between child, teacher and peers in preschool (Granlund & Olsson, 1998). The instrument consists of a total of 36 items, and responses are based on a five‐point Likert scale: (1) “seldom”; (2) “quite often”; (3) “50% of the time”; (4) “fairly often”; and (5) “often.” In the cluster analysis in the present study, scores for how the child interacted with the teacher (10 items), and how the child interacted with other children (11 items) were used for measuring the child’s social interaction skills. Cronbach’s alphas were 0.86 and 0.092, respectively.

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

The SDQ is a well‐known, 25‐item questionnaire measuring child behaviors. In this study, the SDQ teacher version for children aged 2–4 was used (Goodman, 2016). The instrument consists of 25 items, divided into five subscales of five items each (conduct problems, hyperactivity, emotional problems, peer problems and prosocial behaviors), and responses are scored on a three‐point Likert scale; (0) “not true,” (1) “somewhat true,” (2) “certainly true” (apart from questions 7, 11, 14, 21, and 25, which are scored in reverse) (Goodman, 1997, 2016). In an earlier report using data from the first year of this longitudinal study, the SDQ was validated, and it was found that preschool teachers can use it in a preschool setting as a valid instrument for identifying early signs of distress/behavioral problems in young children. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for the subscale hyperactivity was 0.85 and split half 0.73; conduct problems 0.77, and split half 0.75 (Gustafsson et al., 2016).

Instruments used to compare pathways

To compare the characteristics of children and their preschool environments in the different pathways with total sample means, the following variables were used. For preschool environment: collaboration with parents, proportion of children with a mother tongue other than Swedish in the preschool group, number of children per teacher, teacher’s response to the child and other children’s interactions with the child. For child characteristics: age, sex, developmental delay, emotional problems, peer problems, and prosocial behavior.

Children and Youth version (ICF‐CY code sets)

The Developmental code set was based on the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF‐CY code sets (Ellingsen, 2011). In this study, seven items were used to assess developmental delay regarding bodily function and cognition (3 questions about each) and language (1 question), on a scale of (1) “not true at all,” (2) “partly true,” (3) “completely true.” In this study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.76. In the following text, results from the ICF‐CY developmental code sets will be referred to as “developmental delay.”

SDQ

From the SDQ, two subscales describing child characteristics were used for comparison: emotional symptoms and prosocial behavior (Goodman, 2016). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for the subscale emotional symptoms was 0.66 and split half 0.63; prosocial behavior 0.82, and split half 0.75; and peer problems 0.66, and split half 0.56. The peer problems subscale was used as an interactional indicator, with items involving both child and environmental factors that ask about both the child’s behavior and other children’s behavior towards the child.

Collaboration with parents

A questionnaire about collaboration with parents was used as an environmental measure. This instrument was developed for use in the project Educational Efforts in Preschool – Generally and Specifically (PEGS) (Lillvist & Granlund, 2010) and concerns how preschool teachers judge collaboration with the parents of the child. It consists of five items, on a scale of: (1) “not true at all; (2) “disagree somewhat”; (3) “partly true”; and (4) “completely true.” In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.70.

Social interaction skills in preschool questionnaire

The questionnaire in which preschool teachers rated their experiences of different types of social interactions among the child, teacher and peers in preschool (Granlund & Olsson, 1998), used two subscales, with a five‐point Likert scale: (1) “seldom”; (2) “quite often”; (3) “50% of the time”; (4) “fairly often”; and (5) “often,” to assess the interactional environment. The subscales were: teacher’s responsiveness towards the child (10 items), and other children’s interactions with the child (5 items). The Cronbach’s alpha coefficients for internal consistency were 0.73 and 0.90, respectively.

Information given by the preschool managers

The preschool managers answered questions (i)–(iii) that are used in the environmental measures. First: (i) “How many children in the preschool class are entitled to support in their mother tongue?” Then, the number of children per teacher was calculated from the questions; (ii) “How many preschool teachers work in the preschool class?” and (iii) “How many children are placed in the preschool class?”

Statistical analyses

Both person‐oriented and variable‐oriented analyses were used. For the person‐oriented analyses, we used the statistical package for person‐oriented analyses SLEIPNER 2.1. (Bergman et al., 2003). The variable‐based analyses were performed using SPSS package, version 23 (IBM SPSS). Initial analyses were performed, including reparatory analyses of skewness, analyses of internal consistency, normal probability and multicollinearity were implemented and showed approximately normally distributed values. Pearson’s correlation analysis was used to identify associations and possible multicollinearity in the cluster variables.

Cluster analyses

The person‐oriented analyses were performed in three steps following the procedure of Linking of Clusters after Removal of a Residue (LICUR) (Bergman et al., 2003). First, we identified and removed possible residues separately at each age. The decision about whether individuals should be included in the clusters or not is a matter to be decided in each specific case, although a strict statistical criterion is often used. In this study, cases with less than one twin and with an Average Squared Euclidean Distance (ASED) of > 0.5 were identified as outliers and excluded prior to clustering (Bergman, 1998). As Bergman (1988) argues, it is not reasonable to believe that all subjects will fit into a small number of homogeneous profiles. Second, we performed a hierarchical cluster analysis using Ward’s method, separately for each of the three time points. The variables hyperactivity, conduct problems, engagement, child‐to‐teacher interaction skills and child‐to‐other‐children interaction skills were summed as follows: hyperactivity and conduct problems by means of the sum of the scores; engagement, child‐to‐teacher interaction and child‐to‐other‐children interaction by means of the score. The possibility of using cases in the analyses was investigated. In cases with one missing value, a new value was imputed by the use of a twin procedure (i.e., a missing value in one case was replaced with the value from a “twin” case with complete data) (Bergman & Magnusson, 2003). Cases with more than one missing value were excluded before the cluster analyses. In estimating the cluster solution with the best fit, solutions were chosen using the following criteria: a maximum number of 15 clusters, a minimum percentage of explained variance (ESS) over 67% before a sharp increase in explained error sums of squares (EESS), an approved homogeneity in the clusters, and a theoretically meaningful and interpretable solution (Bergman, 1998). The homogeneity of clusters should preferably not exceed 1.00. The centroids within each cluster were also taken into consideration, as well as the associated variances. In the third step, we related the cluster solutions from the three time points to one another by cross‐tabulation to test for individual stability, that is, a tendency for individuals in a specific cluster to demonstrate a similar pattern over time. We tested for significant typical pathways and atypical pathways by using the EXACON module in SLEIPNER, (Bergman & El‐Khouri, 2002) for single‐cell contingency analysis. A typical pathway is defined as an observed frequency that is significantly higher than expected by chance, while an atypical pathway denotes an observed frequency that is significantly lower than expected. To correct for the mass significance problem, we first analyzed the cluster solutions from each year for structural stability, which is obtained when clusters from the different time points are at least partially identical. Structural stability was assessed by comparing the centroids (= cluster means) between the three time points (T1–T2, T2–T3). The homogeneity between cluster centroids should preferably be < 1.00. The probability of individual stability is higher between highly matched clusters (i.e., when the structural stability is high), so cells representing such matching were tested at the nominal significance level. For the rest of the cells, typical and atypical pathways were tested by adjusting the nominal significance level according to the Bonferroni procedure (Bergman et al., 2003). In this study, the typical pathways form the basis for comparative analyses.

The typical pathways were compared in terms of the following categorical and continuous protective and risk factors: children’s personal characteristics: age, developmental delay, emotional problems, peer problems and prosocial behavior, and preschool environmental variables: collaboration with parents, percentage of children with another mother tongue in the group, number of children per teacher, teachers’ responsiveness to the child and other children’s interactions with the child. For the categorical variable children’s sex, we used cross‐tabulation with Fisher’s Exact Test to compare types. For the other continuous variables, we used one‐way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Tukey’s B Post Hoc Test in order to compare children in each of the typical pathways.

Results

Preparatory analyses

A correlation analysis was conducted to determine whether it was possible to use the variables selected for clusters (i.e., engagement, child‐to‐teacher interaction, child‐to‐other‐children interaction, hyperactivity and conduct problems) as independent in the cluster analysis (i.e., the overlap between the variables).The correlation between the variables ranged between r = −0.444 and 0.821, p < 0.001 year 1; r = −0.470 and 0.794, p < 0.001 year 2; and r = −0.637 and 0.848, p < 0.001 year 3. The most significant positive relationships in all 3 years were found between engagement and child‐to‐teacher social interaction skills, and between engagement and child‐to‐other‐children social interaction skills. The most significant negative relationships were found between hyperactivity and child‐to‐teacher social interaction skills, and between hyperactivity and child‐to‐other‐children social interaction skills. Due to the high correlations between the variables engagement and social interaction skills, a principal component analysis (PCA) with Varimax rotation was conducted. The PCA extracted 13 components of the 54 included items, with eigenvalues exceeding 1 and explaining 74% of the variance. A scree plot inspection revealed a clear break after the second component, indicating that the first two factors primarily loaded in the instrument‐specific factors. Although this two‐component solution explained only 22% of the variance, the result revealed a minor degree of overlap between items from different instruments. Based on these results, it was decided to use engagement and social interaction skills as separate variables in the cluster analysis, despite this minor overlap.

Cluster analysis

A six‐cluster solution (i.e., homogeneous patterns of engagement, social interaction, hyperactivity and conduct problems), was chosen for each year, based on the pre‐specified statistical criteria. Cluster titles, means and standard deviations for each year are presented in Tables 1, 2, 3. The cluster solution in year 1 explained 68% of the variance, and cluster homogeneity coefficients (HCs) ranged from 0.24 to 1.22. The cluster solution in year 2 explained 67% of the variance and cluster HCs ranged from 0.24 to 1.62. The cluster solution in year 3 explained 70% of the variance and cluster HCs ranged from 0.25 to 1.58. Four residue cases were found in year 2 and six residue cases in year 3. The cluster solutions showed acceptable structural stability between the time points, with an ASED ranging from 0.01 to 0.15 (years 1–2) and 0.01 to 0.39 (years 2–3) (see Table 4).

Table 1.

The six‐cluster solution from year 1 (Y1)

Cluster

Engaged

(range 1–4)

Child/teacher

interaction

(range 1–5)

Child/child

interaction

(range 1–5)

Hyper‐

activity

(range 0–10)

Conduct

problem

(range 0–10)

HC
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
ENG/SOC (n.=44) 3.64 0.26 4.61 0.28 4.66 0.26 0.70 0.70 0.34 00.68 0.24
AVERAGE (n.=36) 2.94 0.26 4.03 0.27 4.12 0.35 1.78 0.92 0.67 0.89 0.34
PASSIVE (n.=53) 2.54 0.40 3.52 0.34 3.10 0.52 3.29 1.77 1.80 1.19 0.77
ENG/COND (n.=45) 3.40 0.39 4.32 0.34 4.05 0.49 3.91 2.18 4.09 2.00 1.04
HYPER/COND (n.=6) 2.36 0.34 3.18 0.19 2.36 0.52 9.50 0.93 7.75 0.89 0.64
HYPER (n.=13) 1.92 0.43 2.47 0.51 2.05 0.71 4.90 3.23 0.90 1.07 1.22
Sample (n.=197) 2.99 0.63 3.95 0.68 3.75 0.90 2.77 2.35 1.87 2.10

Engaged and interacting are analyzed by means of the score, hyperactivity and conduct problems are analyzed by means of the sum of the scores. Bold text = 1/2 SD or less (engagement, interaction)/more (hyperactivity, conduct problems) difference from Sample M. Note; explanation name of the clusters: ENG/SOC Engagement and interaction high, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean. AVERAGE Engagement, interaction and hyperactivity average, conduct problems below mean. PASSIVE Engagement and interaction below mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems average. ENG/COND Engagement above mean, interaction and hyperactivity average, conduct problems high. HYPER/COND Engagement and interaction low, hyperactivity and conduct problems very high. HYPER Engagement and interaction very low, hyperactivity above mean, conduct problems average

Table 2.

The six‐cluster solution from year 2 (Y2)

Cluster

Engaged

(range 1–4)

Child/teacher

interaction

(range 1–5)

Child/child

interaction

(range 1–5)

Hyper‐

activity

(range 0–10)

Conduct

problem

(range 0–10)

HC
M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
ENG/SOC (n.=44) 3.64 0.26 4.61 0.28 4.66 0.26 0.70 0.70 0.34 00.68 0.24
AVERAGE (n.=36) 2.94 0.26 4.03 0.27 4.12 0.35 1.78 0.92 0.67 0.89 0.34
PASSIVE (n.=53) 2.54 0.40 3.52 0.34 3.10 0.52 3.29 1.77 1.80 1.19 0.77
ENG/COND (n.=45) 3.40 0.39 4.32 0.34 4.05 0.49 3.91 2.18 4.09 2.00 1.04
HYPER/COND (n.=6) 2.36 0.34 3.18 0.19 2.36 0.52 9.50 0.93 7.75 0.89 0.64
HYPER (n.=13) 1.92 0.43 2.47 0.51 2.05 0.71 4.90 3.23 0.90 1.07 1.22
Sample (n.=197) 2.99 0.63 3.95 0.68 3.75 0.90 2.77 2.35 1.87 2.10

Engaged and interacting are analyzed by means of the score, hyperactivity and conduct problems are analyzed by means of the sum of the scores. Bold text = 1/2 SD or less (engagement, interaction)/more (hyperactivity, conduct problems) difference from Sample M. Note; explanation name of the clusters: ENG/SOC Engagement and interaction above mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean. AVERAGE Engagement, interaction, hyperactivity and conduct problems average. PASSIVE Engagement and interaction low, hyperactivity and conduct problems average. ENG/HYPER Engagement above mean, interaction average, hyperactivity above mean, conduct problems average. HYPER/COND Engagement and interaction average, hyperactivity high, conduct problems very high. HYPER Engagement and interaction very low, hyperactivity very high, conduct problems above mean.

Table 3.

The six‐cluster solution from year 3 (Y3)

Cluster

Engaged

(range 1–4)

Child/teacher

interaction

(range 1–5)

Child/child

interaction

(range 1–5)

Hyper‐

activity

(range 0–10)

Conduct problem

(range 0–10)

M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD HC
ENG/SOC (n.=78) 3.88 0.14 4.60 0.22 4.76 0.18 0.42 0.73 0.26 0.60 0.25
AVERAGE (n.=51) 3.39 0.25 4.30 0.20 4.34 0.32 1.44 1.24 0.94 1.35 0.63
PASSIVE (n.=18) 2.77 0.31 3.57 0.41 3.55 0.49 3.06 2.04 0.94 0.98 1.17
ENG/SOC/HYPER (n.=19) 3.86 0.19 4.56 0.27 4.75 0.14 3.69 1.23 1.06 1.41 0.52
HYPER/COND/HIGH (n.=12) 3.65 0.17 4.29 0.17 4.19 0.33 5.92 1.21 5.33 1.83 0.72
HYPER/COND (n.=19) 3.00 0.47 3.41 0.22 3.42 0.40 6.50 2.14 4.22 2.23 1.58
Sample (n.=197) 3.52 0.51 4.25 0.56 4.32 0.66 2.25 2.52 1.29 1.96

Engaged and interacting are analyzed by means of the score, hyperactivity and conduct problems are analyzed by means of the sum of the scores. Bold text = 1/2 SD or less (engagement, interaction)/more (hyperactivity, conduct problems) difference from Sample M. Note; explanation name of the clusters: ENG/SOC Engagement and interaction above mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean. AVERAGE Engagement, interaction, hyperactivity and conduct problems average. PASSIVE Engagement and interaction low, hyperactivity and conduct problems average. ENG/SOC/HYPER Engagement, interaction and hyperactivity above mean, conduct problems average. HYPER/COND/HIGH Engagement and interaction average, hyperactivity high, conduct problems very high. HYPER/COND Engagement and interaction low, hyperactivity very high, conduct problems high.

Table 4.

Names and sizes of the clusters and the ASEDs between matched clusters in years 1, 2, and 3

Six‐cluster solution

year 1

y.1

ASED

between

years 1–2

Six‐cluster

solution

year 2

n.

y.2

ASED between

years 2–3

Six‐cluster solution

year 3

n.

y.3

ENG/SOC 44 0.01 ENG/SOC 50 0.01 ENG/SOC 78
AVERAGE 36 0.04 AVERAGE 59 0.01 AVERAGE 51
PASSIVE 53 0.04 PASSIVE 18 0.01 PASSIVE 18
ENG/COND 45 0.08 HYPER/COND/HIGH 22 0.05 HYPER/COND/HIGH 12
HYPER/COND 6 1.99 ENG/HYPER 35 0.03 ENG/SOC/HYPER 19
HYPER 13 0.15 HYPER 12 0.39 HYPER/COND 19

Longitudinal typical pathways

Longitudinal pathways between cluster profiles of engagement, social interaction skills, hyperactivity and conduct problems were analyzed for typical pathways between years 1 and 2, and between years 2 and 3. Additionally, non‐significant pathways were found in the analysis; these are not reported in this paper, but do explain why many children were not included in the significant pathways. Five significant typical pathways were found in years 1–2, including 37% of the children in the total sample for year 1, and four significant typical pathways in years 2–3, including 30% of the children in the total sample for year 2. Significant typical pathways including at least four children are reported in Fig. 1. The typical pathways including the largest numbers of children were found between cluster groups “Engagement and interaction above mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean” (ENG/SOC). The lowest proportion of children (n = 4), was found in the pathway from cluster “Engagement and interaction very low, hyperactivity above mean, conduct problems average” (HYPER) in year 1 to cluster “Engagement and interaction low, hyperactivity and conduct problems average” (PASSIVE) in year 2. The children in this pathway were younger than the children in the other pathways, with a mean age of 25 months in the first year of data collection. There was no significant typical pathway for the cluster AVERAGE in years 1 and 3, or from PASSIVE in year 2 (i.e., the children followed no stable pathways).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Significant longitudinal typical pathways between years 1–2 and between years 2–3.

Protective and risk indicators associated with typical pathways

The typical pathways were compared in relation to environmental and personal protective and risk indicators (Table 5). The total number of protective and risk indicators was then calculated for each pathway (Figure 2); personal protective or risk factors (gender, developmental delay, emotional problems, prosocial behavior, another mother tongue than Swedish), factors at micro (teacher responsiveness, other‐children‐to‐child interaction), meso (collaboration with parents), and exo (proportion of another mother tongue than Swedish in the preschool group) levels. The total number of both protective and risk indicators associated with the typical pathways were higher between years 1 and 2 than between years 2 and 3.

Table 5.

Typical pathways between clusters, column, years 1–2 (means year 1) and typical pathways years 2–3 (means year 2)

Total sample

ENG/

SOC

PASSIVE/

AVERAGE

ENG/

COND/

HYPER

COND/

HYPER

Difference Total Sample

ENG/

SOC

AVERAGE/

PASSIVE

ENG/

SOC/

HYPER

COND/

HYPER/HIGH

Difference

Years 1–2 n = 22 n = 23 n = 14 n = 9 p (df, N) = F Years 2–3 n = 35 n = 10 n = 7 n = 6 p (df, N) = F

Personal charact‐

eristics

Age months

32 39b 29a,c 37b 35 <0.0011 F(4,195) = 5.809 44 48f 38e,g,h 49f 48f 0.001 F(4,157) = 5.144
Male sex 55% 41% 52% 57% 78% 0.3412 X2(4,196) = 4.516 55% 31%f,h 60% e 43% 100% e,g

0.0062

X2(4,159) = 13.611
Developmental delay 2.60 2.90 b,d 2.41 a 2.69 2.60 a <0.0011 F(4,192) = 6.578 2.79 2.98 f.h 2.63 e 2.88 2.65 e <0.0011 F(4,158) = 8.030

SDQ

emotional

0.87 0.41 1.57 0.43 0.67 0.0371 F(4,190) = 2.608 0.75 0.54 1.10 0.86 1.17 0.5891 F(4,158) = 0.705

SDQ

prosocial

5.89 8.14 b,c,d 5.0 a 5.5 a 4.33 a 0.0011 F(4,190) = 7.056 7.24 8.97 h 7.40 7.14 5.50 e <0.0011 F(4,158) = 9.437
Both Pers./Environm SDQ peer problems 1.86 0.55 b,d 2.70 a 1.57 2.33 a 0.0021 F(4,190) = 4.383 0.94 0.46 h 1.00 0.86 2.17 e 0.0241 F(4,158) = 2.904

Environ‐mental factors

Collaboration with parents 3.53 3.77 3.43 3.47 3.47 0.1261 F(4,192) = 1.824 3.51 3.81f 3.10 e,g 3.71f 3.53 <0.0011 F(4,158) = 6.156
Entitled to other mother tongue 22% 19%d 11%d 10%d 74% a,b,c 0.0041 F(4,169) = 4.081 26% 9% 21% 9% 30% 0.0221 F(4,150) = 2.969
Child:teacher ratio 5.54 6.21 5.44 5.92 5.61 0.0341 F(4,195) = 2.662 6.03 6.54 5.61 6.59 6.03 0.0331 F(4,152) = 2.706
Teacher responsiveness 4.56 4.78 b 4.47 a 4.69 4.59 0.0021 F(4,190) = 4.283 4.62 4.76 4.67 4.76 4.62 0.0721 F(4,158) = 2.199
Other child interaction 3.83 4.7 b,c,d 3.11 a,c,d 3.99 a,b 3.91 a,b <0.0011 F(4,191) = 10.452 4.33 4.85 f,h 4.16 e 4.71 h 4.10 e,g <0.0011 F(4,158) = 10.105

1= ANOVA, 2 = Chi2, Bold text = Risk indicators, Italics = Protective indicators

a = Tukey’s B compared to cluster ENG/SOC (Y1‐2) p < 0.05, b = Tukey’s B compared to cluster PASSIVE/AVERAGE (Y1‐2) p < 0.05, c = compared to cluster ENG/COND/HYPER (Y1‐2) p < 0.05, d = compared to cluster COND/HYPER (Y1‐2) p < 0.05, e = compared to cluster ENG/SOC (Y2‐3) p < 0.05, f = compared to cluster AVERAGE/PASSIVE (Y2‐3) p < .05, g = compared to cluster ENG/SOC/HYPER (Y2‐3) p < 0.05, h = compared to cluster COND/HYPER (Y2‐3) p < 0.05.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Number of protective and risk indicators for each typical pathway year 1–2 (Y1–2) and year 2‐3 (Y 2–3).

The following variables were identified as personal protective indicators: developmental indicators scored as no delay, low incidence of emotional problems and high degree of prosocial behavior. A low degree of peer problems was perceived as both a personal and an environmental protective indicator. The environmental protective indicators were: low proportion of mother tongue other than Swedish in the preschool group, high teacher responsiveness and high interaction from other children. The pathways with high/above‐mean engagement and/or social interaction without conduct problems were characterized by predominantly having relations to protective indicators and no risk indicators.

The personal risk indicators were: male sex, developmental delay, high level of emotional problems and low level of prosocial behavior. A high degree of peer problems was perceived as a risk indicator related to both the child and the environment. The environmental risk indicators were: low teacher ratings of collaboration with parents, high proportion of another mother tongue than Swedish in the preschool group, low teacher responsiveness and low other‐children‐to‐child interaction.

Typical pathways ENG/SOC years 1–2 (Y1–2) (n = 22) and ENG/SOC years 2–3 (Y2–3) (n = 35). Engagement and interaction high, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean

The pathways between “Engagement and interaction high, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean” ENG/SOC Y1, “Engagement and interaction above mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean” ENG/SOC Y2, and “Engagement and interaction above mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems below mean” ENG/SOC Y3 were characterized by several protective indicators and no risk indicator. The children in these pathways were older compared to the total sample (p = 0.001) and had the lowest proportion of boys (ENG/SOC Y2–3, p = 0.031). There was a lower proportion of children with developmental delay in ENG/SOC Y1–2 than in the total sample (p < 0.001), and a lower degree of emotional problems (p = 0.037) and peer problems (p = 0.002 and Y2–3, p = 0.024), together with a higher degree of prosocial behavior (p = 0.001). The child‐to‐teacher ratio (p = 0.034), and the teachers’ collaboration with parents (ENG/SOC Y2–3, p < 0.001) were higher in these preschool groups in relation to the total sample. Preschool teachers rated their own responsiveness towards the children as higher compared to the total sample (ENG/SOC Y1–2, p = 0.002). The peers of these children were perceived to interact better with the children in these pathways compared to the average in the total sample (p < 0.001).

Typical pathways PASSIVE/AVERAGE Y1–2 (n = 23) and AVERAGE/PASSIVE Y2–3 (n = 10). Engagement, interaction, hyperactivity and conduct problems average

The pathways between “Engagement and interaction below mean, hyperactivity and conduct problems average” PASSIVE Y1, “Engagement, interaction, hyperactivity and conduct problems average” AVERAGE Y2, and “Engagement and interaction low, hyperactivity and conduct problems average” PASSIVE Y3 were characterized by a maximum of one protective indicator and several risk indicators. The children in these pathways were younger compared to the total sample (p = 0.001). The pathway AVERAGE/PASSIVE Y2–3 included a higher proportion of boys than in the total sample (p = 0.031). More children than expected were rated with developmental delay (p < 0.001). The children in PASSIVE/AVERAGE Y1–2 had a higher degree of emotional (p = 0.037) and peer problems (p = 0.002), and lower prosocial behavior (p = 0.001) than in the total sample. The teachers’ collaboration with parents was rated lower in AVERAGE/PASSIVE Y2–3 than in the total sample (p < 0.001). The preschool teachers in PASSIVE/AVERAGE Y1–2 judged themselves as showing lower responsiveness to these children than to children in the total sample (p = 0.002). The peers of the children in these pathways were perceived to have more negative interactions with the children than in the total sample (p < 0.001).

Typical pathways ENG/COND/HYPER Y1–2 (n = 14) and ENG/SOC/HYPER Y2–3 (n = 7). Engagement, interaction and hyperactivity high, conduct problems average

The pathways between “Engagement above mean, interaction and hyperactivity average, conduct problems high” ENG/COND Y1, “Engagement above mean, interaction average, hyperactivity above mean, conduct problems average” ENG/HYPER Y2, and “Engagement, interaction and hyperactivity above mean, conduct problems average” ENG/SOC/HYPER Y3, were characterized by children with two protective indicators and a maximum of two risk indicators. The children in these pathways were older than in the total sample (p = 0.001). The children in the pathway ENG/COND/HYPER Y1–2 had a lower degree of emotional problems than the total sample (p = 0.037). Concerning environmental measures, a smaller proportion of children in these pathways were entitled to support in a mother tongue other than Swedish in the preschool groups than in the total sample (ENG/COND/HYPER Y1–2, = 0.004, ENG/SOC/HYPER Y2–3, p = 0.022). The peers of the children in ENG/COND/HYPER Y1–2 had more negative social interaction with these children than in ENG/SOC Y1–2 (p <= 0.050), but more positive social interaction with them than in PASSIVE/AVERAGE Y1–2 (p <= 0.050). The peers in ENG/SOC/HYPER Y2–3 had more positive interaction with the children than in COND/HYPER/HIGH Y2–3 (p ≤ 0.050).

Typical pathways COND/HYPER Y1–2 (n = 9) and COND/HYPER/HIGH Y2–3 (n = 6). Engagement and interaction average, hyperactivity high, conduct problems very high

The pathways between “Engagement above mean, interaction and hyperactivity average, conduct problems high” ENG/COND Y1, “Engagement and interaction average, hyperactivity high, conduct problems very high” HYPER/COND/HIGH Y2, and “Engagement and interaction average, hyperactivity high, conduct problems very high” HYPER/COND/HIGH Y3, were characterized by no protective indicator, but several risk indicators. A higher proportion of boys was present in these pathways (COND/HYPER/HIGH Y2–3 p = 0.031). The children showed less prosocial behavior (p = 0.001), and more peer problems than the total sample (COND/HYPER Y1–2 p = 0.002, COND/HYPER/HIGH Y2–3 p = 0.023). There was a higher proportion of children entitled to support in a mother tongue other than Swedish in the preschool groups than in the total sample (ENG/COND/HYPER Y1–2 p ≤ 0.004, COND/HYPER/HIGH Y2–3 p = 0.022).

Discussion

We aimed to investigate the orchestrated effects of protective factors and risk factors on the development of mental health in young children by identifying homogeneous groups of children with similar mental health profiles at three time points. In addition, we aimed to examine the developmental pathways of mental health profiles over time. Developmental pathways with high structural stability over time were found (i.e., similar cluster patterns emerged at all three time points). This supports the notion of Bergman and El‐Khouri (2002) that in a population of individuals, such as children in preschool, it is probable that a limited number of profiles will occur more often than expected by chance. Several such profiles of pathways of young children in our sample did indeed occur more often than by chance. The identified clusters with structural stability were characterized by a high number of risk or protective factors, respectively. Thus, our findings support the importance of the orchestrated effects of several factors.

The children in this study who displayed high engagement and positive interaction (that is, good mental health) continued to function well, even in the presence of hyperactivity. Children with low levels of engagement and interaction alone (poor mental health) or in combination with both hyperactivity and conduct problems (many mental health problems) continued to have problems. Stability in pathways of mental health profiles was related to a number of protective or/and risk indicators. Thus, as previously stated, our findings support our hypothesis that stable pathways between high‐risk functional patterns were associated with a larger number of risk factors, while stable pathways between low‐risk functional patterns were associated with fewer risk factors and more protective factors. These results support earlier studies on older children that report the importance of cumulative protection and risk factors (Aguiar & McWilliam, 2013; Galéra, Côté, Bouvard et al., 2011; Leblanc, Boivin, Dionne et al., 2008; Romano, Tremblay, Farhat & Côté, 2006; Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008; Willoughby, Pek & Greenberg, 2012). Probably a certain mental health profile does not determine a child’s future mental health pathways, but having knowledge about the risk and protective factors at an individual level has implications for how to facilitate a child’s continued everyday functioning and development (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994; Magnusson & Stattin, 2006).

A novel aspect of the results from this study is the occurrence of young children having low engagement despite few behavior problems. This result indicates that it is important to study not only the occurrence of risk factors but also the absence of protective factors, such as engagement and positive social interaction. The result provides implicit support for Westerhof and Keyes et al. (2010, who argue that mental health exists partly independently of mental health problems or behavior problems. In terms of promoting mental health, low engagement has to be seen as an indicator of a need for intervention. When analyzing data from the same data set at the preschool unit level, it was found that some units promoted high engagement of children to a larger extent than other units (Beteinaki, 2020; Granlund et al. 2016).

A further aim was to study how these developmental pathways of mental health are related to the simultaneous occurrence of risk and protective factors at different ecological levels. The most frequently occurring typical pathway was characterized by children with high levels of engagement and social interaction with teachers and peers, and with low levels of hyperactivity and conduct problems. This pathway was associated with several protective indicators, but few risk indicators (Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008). The number of children in this cluster with high levels of engagement/interaction and no behavior problems increased over time, indicating that psychological functioning is age dependent. It is likely that children with high engagement over time develop positive social interaction with peers and teachers and that this generates positive spirals of good mental health and socio‐emotional development (Aydogan, 2012; Denham et al., 2012; Fuhs, Farran & Nesbitt, 2013; Hughes, Bullock & Coplan, 2014; Raspa, McWilliam, Maher & Ridley, 2001).

Two of the typical pathways in this study included children with more mixed mental health profiles, having above‐mean levels of engagement despite high levels of conduct problems. In one of these pathways, the children’s hyperactivity levels increased over time, while their level of conduct problems decreased. These children displayed moderate levels of most protective and risk indicators and their level of social interaction increased over time. Thus, mental health as engagement and positive social interaction may partly protect against the influence of hyperactivity on functioning. Different components of socioemotional development organize together to form distinct groups in young children (Denham et al., 2012). It might be, as suggested by Sjöman et al. (2015), that good social interaction sustains positive spirals by mutually reinforcing interactions between the child, peers and adults. This highlights the importance of promoting social interaction in preschool, especially for children with high‐risk patterns of behavior.

Lahey, Pelman, Loney, Lee, and Willcutt (2005) have demonstrated the importance of detecting a combination of hyperactivity and impulsiveness in preschool children in order to identify children who may later be diagnosed with ADHD (i.e., identifying children who need early support, including support for their skills in social interaction). In the second of the mixed‐pattern pathways, hyperactivity and conduct problems increased over time. These children displayed no protective indicators and several risk indicators. As in other studies focusing on these older children, risk indicators were present in both the environment and in their personal characteristics, together with few protective indicators (Andersson et al., 2016; Searle, Miller‐Lewis, Sawyer & Baghurst, 2013; Wille & Ravens‐Sieberer, 2008). Child engagement decreased over time (Searle et al., 2013). Other studies have shown that children with behavioral difficulties frequently sustain only low‐complexity social interactions and do not develop in their engagement (Rasmussen & Gillberg, 2000). To identify children at high risk of negative development, it is likely that both behavioral problems and engagement need to be screened (Aguiar & McWilliam, 2013; Romano et al., 2006). It is also important to identify and distinguish both hyperactivity and conduct problems, since these different behaviors affect each other (Gustafsson et al., 2018; Yu, Ziviani, Baxter & Haynes, 2012).

When screening children with behavioral problems, it is especially important to identify patterns of dynamic risk factors (i.e., malleable risk factors) (Sideridis, Prock & Sheridan, 2014; Willoughby et al., 2012) and the stability of risk over time. Studies have demonstrated that 7% of preschool children displaying hyperactivity symptoms are stable over time (Leblanc et al., 2008; Romano et al., 2006). Lavigne, Arend, Rosenbaum, Binns, Christoffel & Gibbons (1998) showed that as many as 50% of 2‐ to 3‐year‐old preschool children with disruptive behavioral symptoms receive a diagnosis 42–48 months later. In the pathway with both high hyperactivity and conduct problems, a lower level of positive change in prosocial behavior was found compared to other pathways. This may indicate an increased risk of receiving more negative attention from teachers and experiencing more conflicts in relationships with both adults and children, leading to less focus on learning prosocial behavior. In the long run, social exclusion from preschool activities may appear (Barkley, 2014; Dodge et al., 2003; Nurmi, 2012). Early intervention could limit or reverse this negative trend. However, how and what support is provided is a key issue in interventions for children with behavioral problems. Almqvist et al. (2018), using the same population as in this study, showed that interventions were primarily focused on decreasing negative behavior, with little enhancement of positive engagement or social interaction. Routines are needed for early detection and support for children with behavioral problems in order to increase their engagement (Egger & Angold, 2006). Support focused on engagement and social interaction may influence the balance between risk factors, favoring a more positive outcome.

Two of the typical pathways displaying predominantly negative patterns were characterized by a high proportion of children in their preschool group with a mother tongue other than Swedish. This risk indicator may lead to difficulties in developing the nuances of the Swedish language, and the children may exhibit conduct problems as a consequence of not being understood by teachers and/or peers (D’Souza, Waldie, Peterson, Underwood & Morton, 2017). A Swedish cross‐sectional study on preschool unit level reports that, in units having a high proportion of children with another ethnic background, children were observed to exhibit a lower engagement level on average (Beneteaki, 2020). However, the proportion of children with a mother tongue other than Swedish in the group may also affect how children’s behavioral problems are perceived and dealt with. Other explanations might be related to differences in socioeconomic status between groups, and approaches to parenthood in different ethnic groups (Williams & Collins, 1995). Various risk factors are embedded in a child’s microsystem and affect the child’s development (Bronfenbrenner & Ceci, 1994). Thus, in order to obtain a better prognosis for individual children, it is important to focus on patterns of risk rather than single factors.

Children in cluster A2 (engagement, interaction and hyperactivity average, conduct problems below mean) (Table 1) were younger than the average for the total sample, and from this cluster there were no significant typical pathways. Younger children may more commonly change cluster profile, depending on the orchestrated effect of protective indicators, risk indicators and personal factors (Sameroff, Seifer, Barocas, Zax & Greenspan, 1987). The pathway for younger children with low engagement and social interaction together with multiple risk factors seemed to deteriorate over time, even where they did not initially exhibit behavioral problems. This stable negative pathway indicates that engagement is affected negatively if several risk factors are present (Sameroff et al., 1987). Improving engagement might be an important focus for early prevention and interventions.

El‐Radhi (2015) found that it was difficult to recognize children with emotional problems early, for both parents and preschool teachers. Many children attending preschool have not developed appropriate vocabulary or comprehension to express their emotions intelligibly (El‐Radhi, 2015). Preschool teachers, parents and also healthcare workers find it difficult to distinguish developmentally normal reactions (fears, crying) from severe and prolonged emotional distress that should be regarded as disorders requiring external help (Gardner & Shaw, 2009). In our study, preschool teachers generally scored children low on the SDQ subscale for emotional problems at 1–3 years old, even if they were able to identify emotional problems in combination with disruptive behavior (Granlund et al. 2016). It seems that teachers react more to externalizing behavioral problems that have an impact on the functioning of the whole group (Almqvist et al., 2018).

We predicted that the number of children per teacher would be a potential environmental risk indicator (Perlman, Falenchuk, Brunsek, McMullen & Shah, 2017). However, the child‐to‐teacher ratio was higher in the pathways that included children with high levels of engagement and social interaction who continued to function well over time. Pathways involving children with lower levels of engagement and social interaction had fewer children per teacher. Thus, it seems that preschools adjust the child‐to‐teacher ratio according to needs in different preschool groups, and thus this ratio is not a meaningful risk indicator in relation to engagement (Samuelsson, Williams & Sheridan, 2015; Swedish National Agency for Education, 2017; Swedish Schools Inspectorate, 2018).

The revised bioecological model developed by Bronfenbrenner and Evans (2000), in which the children are nested within different micro‐systems, such as the family, preschool and peer group, guided the approach of this study. Positive links between these systems are assumed to promote better functioning by affecting interactional processes. Preschool teachers reported that they collaborated better with the parents of engaged, interacting children without behavioral problems. Engagement may develop positively depending on the interrelationships between personal and environmental factors (Imms et al., 2017; Sommer, Pramling Samuelsson & Hundeide, 2013); for example, more engaged children may have more engaged parents. Preschool teachers’ ratings of parents as “engaged” may indicate stable and committed parents, or the fact that teachers prefer, or are better at managing collaboration with, highly engaged parents. According to, Woodard, Kim, Koenig, Yoon & Barry (2010), having engaged and present parents who can develop a secure attachment is an important factor for ensuring young children’s mental health and future positive socialization. The pathways in which children showed the lowest levels of engagement and interaction were also connected to low ratings in collaboration with parents. How to support preschool teachers in their collaboration with parents who need to develop their ability to engage with and attach to their child is therefore an important topic for future research. Almqvist et al. (2018) showed that preschool teachers work extensively to find strategies for providing individualized support to children with behavioral problems, before involving the parents. However, a first step might be to engage the parents, before the children’s issues are formally identified and receive special individualized support.

The present study has some limitations. According to Bergman et al. (2003), there is a restricted number of patterns of functioning, which can be organized into a classificatory system and analyzed longitudinally as typical pathways. The majority of children in this study did not follow a significant typical pathway. Despite high coverage in the cluster structure at each time point (10 residue cases over the three time points), only 37% of the children in sample year 1 followed a typical pathway. This is not uncommon, since it cannot be expected that all children in the studied age span will follow homogeneous pathways within the limits of a distinct cluster structure (Bergman, 1998). In a larger sample, the percentage of children included in significant typical pathways might have been larger. However, it is also important to examine how children’s memberships may shift along unique pathways of characteristics and behavior over time, due to the complex interaction between individual characteristics and environment (Bergman et al., 2003). The rather small sample size was partly due to a high proportion of parents who did not consent to the participation of their children. For ethical reasons, we required informed consent from both parents, which reduced the number of available children. This might have imposed a bias in the study, since it is possible that these children had a different symptomatology compared to those who were included. In the group that did not participate for all 3 years, significantly more children had a mother tongue other than Swedish, and here we may have lost important information. Preschool teachers were instructed to score the children based on their current age. Our interpretation of the children’s higher mean age in the well‐functioning cluster largely supports Berk’s (2013) notion that, in normal child development, function improves with age. For the children aged 1–3 years, the SDQ emotional scale has shown limited validity (Gustafsson et al., 2016), but it was still used in this study to enable a comparison of pathways. Another challenge to interpretation is that pathways involving younger children improved in both engagement and interaction over time, which was probably primarily an effect of maturation due to normal development. It can be difficult for preschool teachers to screen a child’s developmental age in mixed‐age groups, and children with different problems present together in the preschool group. This study is based upon children in preschool environments and the Swedish preschool teachers providing the ratings had professional knowledge (university education) of child development and had known each specific child for at least six months. However, it may also be a limitation that it was preschool teachers and not parents who judged the children’s behavior, and that we do not know if the same or different teachers estimated the ratings for several children in the preschool group.

Conclusions

The stability in young children’s pathways regarding mental health was quite high, both for groups identified as well‐functioning and for those with problems. Children high in engagement and interaction function well, even in the presence of hyperactivity, while children with low engagement and interaction, alone or in combination with exhibiting hyperactivity and conduct problems, continue to have problems over time. Stability was related to the existence of a larger number of protective or risk indicators, respectively. The results indicate that early interventions need to have a dual focus, including both interventions aimed at enhancing child engagement and interventions focused on decreasing behavior problems. From a long‐term perspective, interventions focusing on enhancing engagement seem to be particularly important.

Declaration of interest statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.

 

We would like to thank all the staff of the enrolled preschools for their time and support during the data collection process. We wish to express our gratitude to the National Board of Health and Welfare, FORTE, Sunnerdahls Handikappfond, FORSS Medical Research Council of Southwest Sweden (FORSS‐653271, FORSS‐930636) and the Futurum Academy for Health and Care Region Jönköping County for financial support.

Gustafsson, B. M. , Gustafsson, P. A. , Granlund, M. , Proczkowska, M. & Almqvist, L. (2021). Longitudinal pathways of engagement, social interaction skills, hyperactivity and conduct problems in preschool children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 62, 170–184.

References

  1. Aguiar, C. & McWilliam, R. A. (2013). Consistency of toddler engagement across two settings. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 102–110. [Google Scholar]
  2. Almqvist, L. (2006). Patterns of engagement in young children with and without developmental delay. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual Disabilities, 3, 65–75. [Google Scholar]
  3. Almqvist, L. , Sjöman, M. , Golsäter, M. & Granlund, M. (2018). Special support for behavior difficulties and engagement in Swedish preschools. Frontiers in Education, 3, 10.3389/feduc.2018.00035. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersson, A.‐K. , Martin, L. , Brodd, K. S. & Almqvist, L. (2016). Predictors for everyday functioning in preschool children born preterm and at term. Early Human Development., 103, 147–153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Augustine, L. , Lygnegård, F. , Granlund, M. & Adolfsson, M. (2018). Linking youths’ mental, psychosocial, and emotional functioning to ICF‐CY: lessons learned. Disability and Rehabilitation, 40, 2293–2299. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aydogan, C. (2012). Influences of instructional and emotional classroom environments and learning engagement on low‐income children’s achievement in the prekindergarten year. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt University. [Google Scholar]
  7. Barkley, R. A. (2014). Attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder: a handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  8. Beernink, A. C. E. , Swinkels, S. H. & Buitelaar, J. K. (2007). Problem behavior in a community sample of 14‐and 19‐month‐old children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 271–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bergman, L. R. (1988). You can’t classify all of the people all the time. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 23, 425–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bergman, L. R. (1998). A pattern‐oriented approach to studying individual development: snapshots and processes. In Cairns R. B., Bergman L. R. & Kagan J. (Eds.), Methods and models for studying the individual (pp. 83–122). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bergman, L. R. , Cairns, R. B. , Nilsson, L.‐G. & Nystedt, L. (2000). Developmental science and the holistic approach. Mahwah, NJ: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  12. Bergman, L. & El‐Khouri, B. (2002). SLEIPNER–a statistical package for pattern‐oriented data. (Version 2.1), Version. Department of Psychology. Stockholm, Sweden: Stockholm University, [Google Scholar]
  13. Bergman, L. R. , Magnusson, D. & El‐Khouri, B. M. (2003). Studying individual development in an interindividual context: A person‐oriented approach. New York: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Bergman, L. & Wångby, M. (2014) The person‐oriented approach: A short theoretical and practical guide. Estonia Journal of Education. Version. 2, 29–49. [Google Scholar]
  15. Berk, L. E. (2013). Child development. Boston, MA: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
  16. Bradley, R. H. & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology., 53, 371–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bronfenbrenner, U. & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature‐nature reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review., 101, 568–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bronfenbrenner, U. & Evans, G. (2000). Developmental science in the 21st century: Emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Social Development, 9, 115–125. [Google Scholar]
  19. Bronfenbrenner, U. & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Damon W., Lerner R. M., Renninger K. A. & Sigel I. E. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology Vol 4, Child psychology in practice, (pp. 795–825, 14 edn). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  20. Campbell, S. B. , Halperin, J. M. & Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. (2014). A developmental perspective on attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). In Lewis M. & Rudolph K. D. (Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 427–448). New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  21. Coolahan, K. , Fantuzzo, J. , Mendez, J. & McDermott, P. (2000). Preschool peer interactions and readiness to learn: relationships between classroom peer play and learning behaviors and conduct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 458–465. [Google Scholar]
  22. D’Souza, S. , Waldie, K. E. , Peterson, E. R. , Underwood, L. & Morton, S. M. (2017). Psychometric properties and normative data for the preschool Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in two‐year‐old children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45, 345–357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Denham, S. A. , Bassett, H. , Mincic, M. , Kalb, S. , Way, E. , Wyatt, T. & Segal, Y. (2012). Social–emotional learning profiles of preschoolers' early school success: A person‐centered approach. Learning and Individual Differences, 22, 178–189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Denham, S. A. , McKinley, M. , Couchoud, E. A. & Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development, 61, 1145–1152. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Dodge, K. A. , Lansford, J. E. , Burks, V. S. , Bates, J. E. , Pettit, G. S. , Fontaine, R. & Price, J. M. (2003). Peer rejection and social information‐processing factors in the development of aggressive behavior problems in children. Child Development, 74, 374–393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Egger, H. L. & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional and behavioral disorders in preschool children: Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 313–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Ellingsen, K . (2011). Deriving developmental code sets from the international classification of functioning, disability and health, for children and youth (ICF‐ CY). University of North Carolina Chapel Hill. [Google Scholar]
  28. El‐Radhi, A. S. (2015). Management of common behaviour and mental health problems. British Journal of Nursing, 24, 586–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Frønes, I. (1995). Among peers: On the meaning of peers in the process of socialization. Oslo: Scandinavian University Press. [Google Scholar]
  30. Fuchs, S. , Klein, A. M. , Otto, Y. & von Klitzing, K. (2013). Prevalence of emotional and behavioral symptoms and their impact on daily life activities in a community sample of 3 to 5‐year‐old children. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 44, 493–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Fuhs, M. W. , Farran, D. C. & Nesbitt, K. T. (2013). Preschool classroom processes as predictors of children’s cognitive self‐regulation skills development. School Psychology Quarterly, 28, 347–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Galéra, C. , Côté, S. M. , Bouvard, M. P. , Pingault, J.‐B. , Melchior, M. , Michel, G. et al. (2011). Early risk factors for hyperactivity‐impulsivity and inattention trajectories from age 17 months to 8 years. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 1267–1275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Gardner, F. & Shaw, D. S. (2009). Behavioral problems of infancy and preschool children (0–5). In Rutter M., Bishop D., Pine D., Scott S., Stevenson J., Taylor E. & Thapar A. (Eds.), Rutter’s Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (5th edn) (pp. 882–893). Malden, MA: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  34. Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Goodman, R. (2016). SDQ Information. Last modified 01/09/2016. Retrieved 27 May, 2019 from http://www.sdqinfo.org.
  36. Granlund, M. , Almqvist, L. , Gustafsson, P. , Gustafsson, B. , Golsäter, M. , Proczkowska‐Bjorklund, M. & Sjöman, M. (2016). Slutrapport Tidig upptäckt ‐ tidiga insatser (TUTI). Stockholm: National Board of Health and Welfare. [Google Scholar]
  37. Granlund, M. & Olsson, C. (1998). Familjen och habiliteringen. Stockholm: Ala. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gustafsson, M. B. (2019). Identifying patterns of emotional and behavioural problems in preschool children – facilitating early detection. Medical Dissertation No.1655 (CSAN) Linköping University Sweden. Retrieved on 26 Nov 2020 from http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva‐15358140.
  39. Gustafsson, M. B. , Danielsson, H. , Granlund, M. , Gustafsson, P. & Proczkowska, M. (2018). Hyperactivity precedes conduct problem in preschool children. British Journal of Psychiatry Open, 10.1192/bjo.2018.20:10.1192/bjo.2018.20.4 186–191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Gustafsson, M. B. , Gustafsson, P. A. & Proczkowska‐Bjorklund, M. (2016). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) for preschool children: A Swedish validation. Nord J Psychiatry. 31, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
  41. Gustafsson, M. B. , Proczkowska‐Björklund, M. & Gustafsson, P. A. (2017). Emotional and behavioural problems in Swedish preschool children rated by preschool teachers with the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). BMC Pediatrics, 17, 110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hatakenaka, Y. & Hirano, S. (2015). Training health professionals engaging in 18‐month checkup for early detection and early intervention of autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Social Policy and Social Work, 19, 45–57. [Google Scholar]
  43. Henricsson, L. & Rydell, A. M. (2006). Children with behaviour problems: The influence of social competence and social relations on problem stability, school achievement and peer acceptance across the first six years of school. Infant and Child Development, 15, 347–366. [Google Scholar]
  44. Hughes, K. , Bullock, A. & Coplan, R. J. (2014). A person‐centred analysis of teacher–child relationships in early childhood. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 253–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Imms, C. , Granlund, M. , Wilson, P. H. , Steenbergen, B. , Rosenbaum, P. L. & Gordon, A. M. (2017). Participation, both a means and an end: A conceptual analysis of processes and outcomes in childhood disability. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 59, 16–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Jones, D. J. & Forehand, G. (2003). The Stability of child problem behaviors: a longitudinal analysis of inner‐city African American children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 12, 215–227. [Google Scholar]
  47. Kochanska, G. , Woodard, J. , Kim, S. , Koenig, J. L. , Yoon, J. E. & Barry, R. A. (2010). Positive socialization mechanisms in secure and insecure parent–child dyads: Two longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 998–1009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Lahey, B. B. , Pelham, W. E. , Loney, J. , Lee, S. S. & Willcutt, E. (2005). Instability of the DSM‐IV subtypes of ADHD from preschool through elementary school. Archives of General Psychiatry., 62, 896–902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Lavigne, J. V. , Arend, R. , Rosenbaum, D. , Binns, H. J. , Christoffel, K. K. & Gibbons, R. D. (1998). Psychiatric disorders with onset in the preschool years: I. Stability of diagnoses. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 1246–1254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Leblanc, N. , Boivin, M. , Dionne, G. , Brendgen, M. , Vitaro, F. , Tremblay, R. E. & Pérusse, D. (2008). The development of hyperactive–impulsive behaviors during the preschool years: The predictive validity of parental assessments. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 977–987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Lewis, M. (2011). The origins and uses of self‐awarenesss or the mental representation of me. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 20, 120–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Lillvist, A. & Granlund, M. P. (2010). Preschool children in need of special support: Prevalence of traditional disability categories and functional difficulties. Acta Paediatrica, 99, 131–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Luttropp, A. & Granlund, M. (2010). Interaction – it depends – a comparative study of interaction in preschools between children with intellectual disability and children with typical development. Scandinavian Journal of Disability Research, 12, 151–164. [Google Scholar]
  54. Magnusson, D. & Stattin, H. (2006). The person in context: A holistic‐interactionistic approach. In Lerner R. M. & Damon W. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical Models of human development (pp. 400–464). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
  55. McWilliam, R. (1991). Children’s engagement questionnaire. Chapel Hill, NC: Frank Porter Graham Child Development Center, University of North Carolina. [Google Scholar]
  56. McWilliam, R. , Bailey, D. , Bailey, D. & Wolery, M. (1992). Promoting engagement and mastery. Teaching Infants and Preschoolers with Disabilities, 2, 230–255. [Google Scholar]
  57. Nurmi, J.‐E. (2012). Students’ characteristics and teacher–child relationships in instruction: A meta‐analysis. Educational Research Review, 7, 177–197. [Google Scholar]
  58. Perlman, M. F. B. , Falenchuk, O. , Brunsek, A. , McMullen, E. & Shah, P. S. (2017). Child‐staff ratios in early childhood education and care settings and child outcomes: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. PLoS One, 19;12(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Petrenko, C. L. (2013). A review of intervention programs to prevent and treat behavioral problems in young children with developmental disabilities. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 25, 651–679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Prior, M. , Bavin, E. , Cini, E. , Eadie, P. & Reilly, S. (2011). Relationships between language impairment, temperament, behavioural adjustment and maternal factors in a community sample of preschool children. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders., 46, 489–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rasmussen, P. & Gillberg, C. (2000). Natural outcome of ADHD with developmental coordination disorder at age 22 years: A controlled, longitudinal, community‐based study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry., 39, 1424–1431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Raspa, M. J. , McWilliam, R. & Maher Ridley, S. (2001). Child care quality and children’s engagement. Early Education and Development, 12, 209–224. [Google Scholar]
  63. Romano, E. , Tremblay, R. E. , Farhat, A. & Côté, S. (2006). Development and prediction of hyperactive symptoms from 2 to 7 years in a population‐based sample. Pediatrics, 117, 2101–2110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rutter, M. (1994). Family discord and conduct disorder: cause, consequence, or correlate? Journal of Family Psychology., 8, 170–186. [Google Scholar]
  65. Sameroff, A. J. , Seifer, R. , Barocas, R. , Zax, M. & Greenspan, S. (1987). Intelligence quotient scores of 4‐year‐old children: Social‐environmental risk factors. Journal of Pediatrics, 79, 343–350. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Samuelsson, I. P. , Williams, P. & Sheridan, S. (2015). Stora barngrupper i förskolan relaterat till läroplanens intentioner. Tidsskrift for Nordisk Barnehageforskning., 9.1–14. [Google Scholar]
  67. Searle, A. K. , Miller‐Lewis, L. R. , Sawyer, M. G. & Baghurst, P. A. (2013). Predictors of children’s kindergarten classroom engagement: Preschool adult–child relationships, self‐concept, and hyperactivity/inattention. Early Education & Development, 24, 1112–1136. [Google Scholar]
  68. Shaw, P. , Stringaris, A. , Nigg, J. & Leibenluft, E. (2015). Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Focus, 14, 127–144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sideridis, G. D. , Prock, L. A. & Sheridan, M. A. (2014). Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in young children: predictors of diagnostic stability. Pediatrics, 133, 659–667. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sjöman, M. , Granlund, M. & Almqvist, L. (2015). Interaction processes as a mediating factor between children’s externalized behaviour difficulties and engagement in preschool. Early Child Development and Care, 1–15. 186, 1649–1643. [Google Scholar]
  71. Sommer, D. , Pramling Samuelsson, I. & Hundeide, K. (2013). Early childhood care and education: A child perspective paradigm. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal., 21, 459–475. [Google Scholar]
  72. Sterba, S. K. & Bauer, D. J. (2010). Matching method with theory in person‐oriented developmental psychopathology research. Developmental and Psychopathology, 22, 239–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Swedish National Agency for Education . (2017). Children and staff in preschool [Barn och personal i förskolan hösten]. Swedish National Agency for Education. Last modified 2017. Retrieved 27 May, 2019 From: https://www.skolverket.se/publikationer?id=3949.
  74. Swedish Schools Inspectorate (2018). Preschool quality and goal achievement (Förskolans kvalitet och måluppfyllelse). Stockholm. The Swedish Schools Inspectorate. [Google Scholar]
  75. Van Lier, P. A. C. , der Ende, J. , Koot, H. K. & Verhulst, F. C. (2007). Which better predicts conduct problems? The relationship of trajectories of conduct problems with ODD and ADHD symptoms from childhood into adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 601–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Westerhof, G. J. & Keyes, L. M. (2010). Mental Illness and Mental Health: The Two Continua Model Across the Lifespan. Journal of Adult Development, 17, 110–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wille, B. S. & Ravens‐Sieberer, U. & BELLA Study Group (2008). Risk and protective factors for children’s and adolescents’ mental health: Results of the BELLA study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 17, 133–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Williams, D. R. & Collins, C. (1995). US socioeconomic and racial differences in health: Patterns and explanations. Annual Review of Sociology, 21, 349–386. [Google Scholar]
  79. Willoughby, M. T. , Pek, J. & Greenberg, M. T. (2012). Investigators FLP. Parent‐reported attention deficit/hyperactivity symptomatology in preschool‐aged children: Factor structure, developmental change, and early risk factors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 1301–1312. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Wlodarczyk, O. , Pawils, S. , Metzner, F. , Kriston, L. , Klasen, F. & Ravens‐Sieberer, U. (2017). Risk and protective factors for mental health problems in preschool‐aged children: cross‐sectional results of the BELLA preschool study. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 8, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. World Health Organization (2019). Mental Health. Retrieved 4 May 2020 from http://www.who.int/newsroom/fact‐sheets/detail/mental‐health‐strengthening‐our‐response. [Google Scholar]
  82. Yu, M. L. , Ziviani, J. , Baxter, J. & Haynes, M. (2012). Time use differences in activity participation among children 4–5 years old with and without the risk of developing conduct problems. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 490–498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Scandinavian Journal of Psychology are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES