Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Apr 15;16(4):e0249831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249831

Association between early viral LRTI and subsequent wheezing development, a meta-analysis and sensitivity analyses for studies comparable for confounding factors

Sebastien Kenmoe 1,*, Arnol Bowo-Ngandji 2, Cyprien Kengne-Nde 3, Jean Thierry Ebogo-Belobo 4, Donatien Serge Mbaga 2, Gadji Mahamat 2, Cynthia Paola Demeni Emoh 2, Richard Njouom 1,*
Editor: Bernadette van den Hoogen5
PMCID: PMC8049235  PMID: 33857215

Abstract

Introduction

Consideration of confounding factors about the association between Lower Respiratory Tract Infections (LRTI) in childhood and the development of subsequent wheezing has been incompletely described. We determined the association between viral LRTI at ≤ 5 years of age and the development of wheezing in adolescence or adulthood by a meta-analysis and a sensitivity analysis including comparable studies for major confounding factors.

Methods

We performed searches through Pubmed and Global Index Medicus databases. We selected cohort studies comparing the frequency of subsequent wheezing in children with and without LRTI in childhood regardless of the associated virus. We extracted the publication data, clinical and socio-demographic characteristics of the children, and confounding factors. We analyzed data using random effect model.

Results

The meta-analysis included 18 publications (22 studies) that met the inclusion criteria. These studies showed that viral LRTI in children ≤ 3 years was associated with an increased risk of subsequent development of wheezing (OR = 3.1, 95% CI = 2.4–3.9). The risk of developing subsequent wheezing was conserved when considering studies with comparable groups for socio-demographic and clinical confounders.

Conclusions

When considering studies with comparable groups for most confounding factors, our results provided strong evidence for the association between neonatal viral LRTI and the subsequent wheezing development. Further studies, particularly from lower-middle income countries, are needed to investigate the role of non-bronchiolitis and non-HRSV LRTI in the association between viral LRTI in childhood and the wheezing development later. In addition, more studies are needed to investigate the causal effect between childhood viral LRTI and the wheezing development later.

Trial registration

Review registration: PROSPERO, CRD42018116955; https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42018116955.

Introduction

Epidemiological studies have shown that up to 50% of children have at least one episode of wheezing before their third birthday [13]. This wheezing is responsible for hospitalization rates in children of up to 8% [1].

Several studies have shown that hospitalization for early Human Respiratory Syncytial Virus (HRSV) bronchiolitis was a predictor of subsequent wheezing episodes [4,5]. The association between HRSV-bronchiolitis and long-term wheezing is now clearly established by multiple original studies and meta-analyses [4,5]. The current residual questions are to known whether the HRSV-LRTI are for subsequent wheezing: 1) a marker of susceptibility, 2) a causal agent, or 3) both a marker of susceptibility and a causal agent. The responses to these residual shadow points now relate to the multiple confounding factors or ideally the response to the causal effect between the HRSV-bronchiolitis and wheezing which can only be addressed by interventional studies with prophylactic means such as palivizumab or very soon with anti-HRSV vaccines approval [6]. A recent meta-analysis failed to provide acceptable evidence for the causal relationship between childhood HRSV infections and long-term respiratory morbidity [7]. This question of wheezing after bronchiolitis in childhood have also been incompletely explored in multiple aspects including the age of children at the time of bronchiolitis development, bronchiolitis due to non-HRSV viruses, non-bronchiolitis Lower Respiratory Tract Infections (LRTI), and the influence of confounding factors [810]. The introduction of molecular diagnostic tests has led to better definition of the role of respiratory virus in bronchiolitis [11]. It is now recognized that Human Metapneumovirus (HMPV), Human Bocavirus (HBoV) or even Rhinovirus (RV) are among common viruses involved in bronchiolitis [1216]. It has also been shown that childhood RV infections are associated with subsequent wheezing [17,18]. Apart from HRSV and RV, there is no systematic review with respect to the association between childhood LRTI due to other common respiratory virus and later wheeze. In addition to bronchiolitis, other low respiratory infections are very common in childhood [19,20]. To date, however, there is no systematic review on the influence of non-bronchiolitis respiratory infections in infancy on long-term wheezing. There are several confounding factors that have not yet been explored concerning their effect on post bronchiolitis wheezing [2125]. These confounding factors include atopic predisposition [2634], reduced premorbid lung function [35], smoking exposure, breastfeeding [34,36], premature birth, low birth weight, or overcrowding [36,37]. These confounding factors may act synergistically with low respiratory infections in childhood and all these mechanisms remain to be elucidated [38].

Early recognition of children at risk of developing long-term wheezing may be important for many prevention and management policies [39,40]. The aim of this study was to evaluate, in a meta-analysis and sensitivity analyses including studies comparable for sociodemographic and clinical confounding factors, wheezing as a long-term sequela of viral LRTI in children.

Methods

Study design

This systematic review was prepared according to the Centre for Reviews and Dissemination guidelines [41]. The PRISMA declaration served as a model for the presentation of this review (S1 Table) [42]. The protocol of this review has been registered in the PROSPERO database under number CRD42018116955. Obtaining ethical clearance was not required for this study.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Due to the multiple limitations of cross-sectional (non-comparable groups) and case control (misclassification and recall bias) studies in estimating the association between exposure and outcome, we include only cohort studies that compare children with LRTI with controls in regards of wheezing as long-term respiratory sequelae.

We considered studies with healthy controls (with no history of LRTI or non-respiratory conditions) compared to LRTI cases with criteria as similar as possible (gender, age, geographic origin, geographic location, social class and time of inclusion). The study participants were children with severe respiratory infections and independently of the respiratory virus detected. The definitions of LRTI were adapted as described by the authors of the primary studies.We excluded studies with high-risk people (preterm, chronic respiratory disease, heart disease or immunodeficiency) to reduce the potentially confounding effect of co-morbidities in these individuals on the association between LRTI and wheezing.

Study outcomes and case definition

The main outcome was the link between LRTI in children at ≤ 5 years and the wheezing development later. The secondary results were to determine factors associated with the development of wheezing after LRTI in infancy. Wheezing was considered as any whistling nose at the expiration declared by the included study. Wheezing during the interview was defined as current wheezing. Wheezing in the last 12 months was defined as wheezing episodes during the previous year. Recurrent wheezing was considered for persistent wheezing between the diagnosis of LRTI and the interview. Recurrent wheezing was also considered as ≥3 episodes of bronchial obstruction. If there were multiple wheezing phenotypes in the same study, we chose the one that spanned the longest time. We separately considered studies with multiple effect data depending on interview time, respiratory viruses detected or types of LRTI.

Search strategy

Research was conducted in the following databases: Pubmed and Global Index Medicus. The search strategy in Pubmed is shown in S2 Table. This search strategy was adapted for the second database. The search was performed without any language restrictions since the creation of the databases until the 28 August 2020. The reference lists selected and the relevant review was deeply checked to include additional articles.

Study selection

Two investigators (JTEB and SK) independently selected potentially relevant studies based on the titles and abstracts from the list of references in Rayyan website [43]. Full versions of selected articles were uploaded. The selection process was summarized in a PRISMA flowchart.

Data extraction

Six authors (SK, ABN, JTEB, DSM, GM, and CPDE) evaluated independently all included studies. The following information was collected: 1) title, first author, year of publication, time of data collection, country and participants interview period of article; 2) the type, rank, period, age, hospitalization and type of infection associated with the LRTI; 3) the age and gender of controls; 4) the total number of cases and controls and numbers with wheezing or confounders at follow-up. All data on confounding factors collected were defined as presented in the included article. We have harmonized the names of the confounding factors collected in several articles according to their similarity.

Appraisal of the methodological quality of included studies and risk of bias

Six authors (SK, ABN, JTEB, DSM, GM, and CPDE) independently assessed the quality of each study using an adapted version of Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (S3 Table) [44]. Discrepancies in the study selection, study quality evaluation, and data extraction were resolved by discussion between the authors involved in the process or by consultation of another author of the review as appropriate (SK).

Data synthesis and analysis

The odds ratio (OR) and the 95% confidence interval (95% CI) and prediction interval were used as a measure of the association between LRTI and long-term wheezing. These parameters were calculated using a random-effects model by the Der Simonian and Laird method [45,46]. Heterogeneity was assessed by the Q test p-value and the I2 statistic [47,48]. The heterogeneity between the studies was considered significant for the values of P <0.1 and I2> 50%. The robustness of the results was assessed by a sensitivity analysis including only the first episode of LRTI, physician-diagnosed wheezing and studies with proportions of confounding factors comparable between cases and controls. The comparability of confounders between cases and controls were estimated using the Chi-square and Fisher tests for qualitative variables and Student’s T-test for continuous variables. The primary confounding factors data for continuous variables expressed as median and/or range were converted to mean and standard deviation [49]. Univariate subgroup analyses were performed using random effect meta-analyses based on sampling method, time of exposure collection, age range during LRTI, age at time of interview for wheezing, hospitalization of controls, viruses responsible for LRTI, type of LRTI, and type of wheezing [45]. We analyzed the data using the “meta: version 4.15–0” and “metafor: version 2.4–0” packages in version 3.5.1 of software R [50,51].

Results

Study selection

The search strategy identified 1962 articles. We eliminated 1425 articles unrelated to the objectives of the study. We reviewed a total of 139 complete articles to determine their eligibility for meta-analysis. Among these items, we excluded 121 for multiple reasons (S4 Table) and included 18 in this study (Fig 1) [5269]. The 18 citations represented 22 individual studies since 3 references included data collected at 2 different ages and one study included data for 2 different viruses [54,55,61,68].

Fig 1. Flow chart for the systematic literature search.

Fig 1

Characteristics of studies included for the meta-analysis

S5 Table summarizes the characteristics of the 22 included studies. The included cohort studies were published between 1978 and 2017 and most were conducted in Northern Europe (68.2%) and in high income countries (95.5). Most of the included studies had prospective (77.3%), non-probabilistic (95.5%) recruitment and follow-up duration ranged from 1.5 to 10 years. All children with LRTI were hospitalized, had LRTI from 1960 to 2005, and most were < 1 year (59.1%) with their first episode (68.2%) of bronchiolitis (68.2%) due to HRSV (90.9%). Wheezing in children was reported between 2 and 20 years and most studies were between 2 and 10 years (77.2%). The most reported wheezing phenotype in the included studies was recurrent wheezing (59.1%). All included studies were at low risk of bias (S6 Table). S7 Table depicts individual data of included studies.

Meta-analysis results

Association between LRTI in infancy and wheezing later

Findings from the meta-analysis showed that the rate of wheezing was significantly higher in patients with childhood LRTI than in controls: OR = 3.0, 95% CI = 2.3–3.9 (Fig 2). All wheezing categories (any wheezing: OR = 3.2, 95% CI = 1.9–5.3, I2 = 0.0%; current wheezing: OR = 3.1, 95% CI = 1.5–6.2, I2 = 72.3%; recurrent wheezing: OR = 3.0, 95% CI = 2.2–4.2, I2 = 44.0%; wheezing in the last 12 months: OR = 3.0, 95% CI = 1.8–5.0 were significantly more common in older LRTI patients compared to the control group.

Fig 2. Forest plot of wheezing in children with and whitout LRTI in infancy.

Fig 2

Sensitivity analysis

We assessed the constancy of our results through multiple sensitivity analyses including studies reporting only the first episode of LRTI, physician-diagnosed wheezing, and studies comparable between cases and controls for multiples confounding factors (S8 and S9 Tables). Table 1 presents the results of these different sensitivity analyses. The results of the sensitivity analyses were consistent with those of the overall findings for studies reporting only the first episode of LRTI and studies with studies comparable between cases and controls for almost all confounding factors. We found no publication bias in the overall analyses (p-value Egger test = 0.282). Funnel graph showed no asymmetry (S1 Fig). Significant heterogeneity was observed in overall analyses (I2: 47.4% 95% = 13.5–68; p-value heterogeneity: 0.008).

Table 1. Wheezing in children with LRTI in infancy and control without respiratory diseases.

Wheezing OR (95%CI) 95% Prediction interval N Studies N LRTI cases N controls H (95%CI) I2 (95%CI) P heterogeneity P Egger test
Overall 3 [2.4–3.9] [1.4–6.7] 22 1239 18649 1.4 [1.1–1.8] 47.4 [13.5–68] 0.008 0.282
Sensitivity analyses
First episode of LRTI 2.8 [2.1–3.7] [1.3–6.1] 15 958 18316 1.4 [1–1.9] 46.7 [2.8–70.8] 0.024 0.582
Physician-diagnosed wheezing 3 [1.9–5] [0.9–10.8] 7 363 340 1.5 [1–2.3] 55.4 [0–80.9] 0.036 0.993
Studies comparable for allergic rhinitis 3.4 [1.6–7.4] [0–7574.6] 3 131 136 1.7 [1–3.1] 64.5 [0–89.8] 0.06 0.094
Studies comparable for antiasthmatic treatment 3.1 [1.7–5.7] NA 1 130 111 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for asthma admissions 4.5 [1.5–13.3] NA 1 32 30 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for asthma in children 1.3 [0.7–2.5] NA 1 74 74 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for asthma in father 6 [2.6–14.3] NA 2 55 60 1 0 0.38 NA
Studies comparable for asthma in mother 6 [2.6–14.3] NA 2 55 60 1 0 0.38 NA
Studies comparable for asthma in parents 3.4 [2.3–5.1] [1.4–8.4] 4 187 371 1 [1–2.6] 5.2 [0–85.5] 0.367 0.555
Studies comparable for asthma in siblings 6 [2.6–14.3] NA 2 55 60 1 0 0.38 NA
Studies comparable for atopic dermatitis 3.1 [2–4.8] [1.1–8.6] 7 295 361 1.3 [1–2] 39.1 [0–74.4] 0.131 0.131
Studies comparable for atopy in children 3 [1.4–6.4] [0.2–53.4] 4 149 154 1.7 [1–2.9] 64.8 [0–88] 0.037 0.168
Studies comparable for atopy in father 6 [2.6–14.3] NA 2 55 60 1 0 0.38 NA
Studies comparable for atopy in mother 3.6 [2.4–5.6] [0.2–59.1] 3 210 576 1.1 [1–3.5] 22.6 [0–91.9] 0.275 0.415
Studies comparable for atopy in parents 3.3 [2.3–4.7] [2–5.3] 7 273 417 1 [1–1.8] 0 [0–70.1] 0.438 0.357
Studies comparable for atopy in siblings 10 [2.4–41.2] NA 1 23 30 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for current asthma 5.8 [2.2–15.2] NA 1 35 64 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for current atopy 5.9 [3.1–11.3] [0.1–382.6] 3 90 124 1 [1–1.9] 0 [0–73.2] 0.679 0.794
Studies comparable for current eczema 5.8 [2.2–15.2] NA 1 35 64 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for family history of atopy 2 [0.6–7.1] NA 1 17 25 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for heredity for asthma 3.3 [2–5.2] [0.2–66.1] 3 141 279 1 [1–1] 0 [0–0] 0.97 0.605
Studies comparable for heredity for atopy 3.3 [2–5.2] [0.2–66.1] 3 141 279 1 [1–1] 0 [0–0] 0.97 0.605
Studies comparable for history of asthma 2.9 [1.3–6.2] NA 2 111 140 2 [1–4.1] 74 [0–94.1] 0.05 NA
Studies comparable for history of atopy 4.6 [2.8–7.5] [0.2–110.8] 3 192 17039 1.3 [1–2.4] 42.2 [0–82.5] 0.177 0.652
Studies comparable for history of eczema 2.9 [1.3–6.2] NA 2 111 140 2 [1–4.1] 74 [0–94.1] 0.05 NA
Studies comparable for history of pertussis 3.3 [1.9–5.6] NA 2 94 186 1 0 0.812 NA
Studies comparable for male gender 3.2 [2.4–4.3] [1.4–6.9] 15 768 1279 1.3 [1–1.8] 45 [0–70] 0.03 0.307
Studies comparable for maternal smoking 4.7 [2.5–8.9] [1.2–19] 4 127 101 1 [1–1.9] 0 [0–71.5] 0.657 0.466
Studies comparable for maternal smoking during pregnancy 1.8 [0.9–3.4] NA 1 76 76 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for parental smoking 5.8 [2.2–15.2] NA 1 35 64 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for paternal smoking 3.9 [2.4–6.3] [0.2–90.4] 3 185 171 1.1 [1–3.3] 12.3 [0–90.9] 0.32 0.247
Studies comparable for pets at home 3.1 [2.4–4.1] [2.3–4.3] 9 520 1116 1.1 [1–1.5] 13 [0–54.9] 0.326 0.18
Studies comparable for premature birth 3.9 [1.9–8] [0–381.1] 3 104 71 1 [1–1.2] 0 [0–26.8] 0.868 0.315
Studies comparable for siblings in the house 5.8 [2.2–15.2] NA 1 35 64 NA NA 1 NA
Studies comparable for smoke exposure 2.6 [1.9–3.5] [1.3–5.1] 9 580 1189 1.2 [1–1.8] 34.4 [0–69.8] 0.142 0.246
Studies comparable for age at interview (years) 7.8 [3.5–17.3] [0–1362.5] 3 90 95 1.3 [1–2.2] 36.6 [0–79.8] 0.207 0.671
Studies comparable for age at recrutment (months) 4.5 [2.4–8.4] [0.1–256.7] 3 142 111 1 [1–1.7] 0 [0–64.9] 0.744 0.291
Studies comparable for birth weight (grams) 5.2 [2.2–12.2] [0–1284.3] 3 107 76 1.4 [1–2.7] 51 [0–85.8] 0.13 0.333
Studies comparable for breastfeeding period (months) 3.3 [1.9–5.6] NA 2 94 186 1 0 0.812 NA
Studies comparable for height at interview (cm) 3.4 [2.5–4.6] [2.3–5] 7 424 1015 1.2 [1–1.8] 27.1 [0–68.4] 0.221 0.328
Studies comparable for number of siblings 3 [2–4.5] [1.2–7.2] 4 188 372 1 [1–1.2] 0 [0–34.1] 0.874 0.961
Studies comparable for weight at interview (kg) 3.2 [2.3–4.3] [2–4.9] 6 378 923 1.1 [1–2.2] 16.4 [0–78.8] 0.308 0.492

CI: Confidence interval; OR: Odds ratio; NA: Not applicable.

Subgroup analysis of the occurrence of wheezing in children with and without LRTI in infancy

The type of LRTI was associated with the occurrence of wheezing (p = 0.007) (S10 Table). History of bronchiolitis (OR = 3.8, 95% CI = 3.4–4.7) and LRTI unspecified (OR = 1.9, 95% CI = 1.4–2.7) were associated with the occurrence of wheezing in childhood. Age rage at recruitment was significantly associated with the occurrence of wheezing later (p <0.045). We did not record any difference in the occurrence of wheezing by the children age range at recruitment (p = 0.113) and the type of virus screened (p = 0.249). However, the only study available with Adenovirus type 7 was not associated with the occurrence of wheezing according to the history of LRTI (OR = 2.8, IC 95% = 0.8–10.0). No difference was recorded in the occurrence of wheezing depending on the age range at the interview (p = 0.053), the sampling approach (non-probabilistic vs probabilistic, p = 0.505), the time of exposure collection (prospective vs retrospective, p = 0.990), the hospitalization of controls (p = 0.478), and the type of wheezing (p = 0.996). However, all age groups at interview up to 20 years were associated with an increased risk of developing wheezing in former cases of LRTI compared to controls.

Discussion

This systematic review of studies with LRTI patients in nearly half a century gives four main results. Children who experienced an episode of viral LRTI at ≤ 3 years of age were 3 times more likely than controls to develop wheezing later. This increased risk of wheezing in children with a history of viral LRTI was not influenced by the rank of LRTI episode, physician-diagnosed wheezing and studies comparable for the investigated confounding factors.

These results are consistent with previous systematic reviews that had already demonstrated an increased risk of wheezing among old bronchiolitis patients [810]. Kneyber et al have demonstrated in a quantitative analysis that hospitalized children with bronchiolitis episodes due to HRSV at <1 year of age had an increased risk of wheezing compared to controls [70]. Fauroux et al in a systematic review without meta-analysis of data published between 1995 and 2015 in Western countries reported that children hospitalized for HRSV infections were at high risk of developing recurrent wheezing until age 25 years old [71]. Shi et al in a recent meta-analysis showed that childhood HRSV infection was significantly associated with wheezing later in former LRTI cases compared to healthy individuals or controls without respiratory infection in infancy [8].

Although represented by a single study in this review, pneumonia was not associated with a risk of subsequent wheezing. A previous meta-analysis showed that childhood pneumonia mainly linked to adenoviruses was associated with respiratory sequelae in hospitalized and non-hospitalized children, including restrictive pulmonary disease, obstructive pulmonary disease, bronchiectasis and chronic bronchitis [72]. Asthma, considered by most authors as ≥ 3 episodes of wheezing, were however not associated with pneumonia in the meta-analysis [72]. Although the type of LRTI was a significant source of heterogeneity during this work, we are unable to draw a definitive conclusion about this parameter because the pneumonia category was represented by a single study and the category LRTI not specified is very heterogeneous to allow an objective interpretation. Previous reports have shown that the strength of the association between LRTI in childhood and wheezing later diminished with age with persistence into adulthood [18,70,71,7377]. These previous reports suggest that the factor responsible for wheezing loses its influence with age, which could be small airways. Unfortunately, in this report, we have only recorded 2 studies with patients > 10 and are therefore unable to comment on the decrease in the strength of the association between LRTI in childhood and the subsequent wheezing risk.

The development of molecular diagnostic tests has made it possible to reassess the role of respiratory viruses in bronchiolitis and the description of new viruses [11]. Systematic reviews have shown that infections with no-HRSV respiratory viruses in childhood are also associated with an increased risk of developing wheezing sequelae [18,71]. Liu et al. showed the involvement of RV infections during the first 3 years of life in the subsequent wheezing in a meta-analysis including cohorts with and without controls [18]. The results of this systematic review, also, indicate that viral LRTI generally was associated with a risk of subsequent wheezing.

Studies have contributed to show that the index children with a family history of asthma had a greater risk of developing post-bronchiolitis wheezing [5]. Kneyber et al. in a quantitative analysis showed that family history of atopy/asthma were similar between index cases and controls [70]. In this systematic review, studies with confounding factor comparable in index cases and controls showed concordant results in the incidence of subsequent wheezing (Table 1). Studies have shown that male sex is a predictive factor of wheezing as a result of bronchiolitis [78,79]. In this systematic review, in the 15 studies with gender comparable between cases and controls, there were no difference in the association between early LRTI and subsequent wheezing. Studies have shown that maternal smoking was a predictor of subsequent wheezing [80]. In this meta-analysis, 4 included studies comparable for maternal smoking exposure in index cases compared to controls did not appear to affect the global effect observed. In this meta-analysis also studies comparable for the family atopy, family asthma, preponderance of overcrowding, daycare attendance, premature birth or breastfeeding did not affect the incidence of wheezing after bronchiolitis. Several plausible alternative hypotheses may be involved in post-bronchiolitis wheezing [31,81,82]. The decrease in pulmonary function very early in life has also been reported by several authors as a predisposing factor for bronchiolitis and post-bronchiolitis wheezing [35,8385]. Despite these multiple other factors, bronchiolitis has always been shown to be a very important predictor of subsequent wheezing.

The main limitation of this systematic review is the small number of studies in some categories of our subgroup analyses. These categories included non-bronchiolitis LRTI and non-HRSV LRTI. The results of this subgroup analyses should therefore be considered with caution. The statistical significance of the symmetric or asymmetric distribution of confounders between cases and controls is largely a function of sample size. Although we used both parametric and nonparametric tests, studies with large samples have the power to detect much smaller differences between cases and controls than small studies do. We were not able to evaluate the influence of premorbid pulmonary function, the presence of co-morbidities or co-infections on the development of subsequent wheezing, as this information was missing in the majority of studies included. Despite these weaknesses, this systematic review has several strengths that include overall results consistent with multiple sensitivity analyses (consideration of the first episode of LRTI, physician-diagnosed wheezing and studies comparable for various confounding factors). No publication bias was recorded in global analyses. We conducted a comprehensive search strategy with no restriction in geographic, temporal, LRTI type, and LRTI virus type that significantly increased the number of studies and increased the statistical power of the analyses. We have adopted a transparent protocol, pre-recorded and intervention of two investigators at every stage of the process. We conducted a thorough data collection and provided an exhaustive characteristic of individual included studies. We have assessed for the first time studies comparable for confounding factors for the determination of the association between neonatal viral LRTI and subsequent wheezing. We have transparently documented our method of defining the categories of multiple wheezing case definitions and confounding factors recorded in the included studies and the definitions considered in each category for this systematic review.

Evidence from this meta-analysis strongly suggests that viral LRTI at ≤ 3 years is one of the major predictors of wheezing. Despite the multiple studies conducted on the subject, the interaction between bronchiolitis and other predictors of subsequent wheezing is unknown. More controlled prospective studies are needed to resolve these confusions. Nearly all studies evaluating the long-term sequelae of bronchiolitis have focused primarily on hospitalized children [78,86,87]. One study demonstrated the involvement of mild infections induced by RV early life in the development of wheezing sequelae later [88]. It is important to implement other cohort studies to clarify the role of ambulatory bronchiolitis in developing long-term wheezing symptoms. It is also necessary to conduct additional longitudinal studies to further evaluate the role of non-HRSV respiratory viruses, especially RV and HMPV, in post-bronchiolitis wheezing. The above additional studies are expected especially from lower-middle income countries where data are very scarce.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Funnel plot for publication for wheezing in children with and without LRTI in infancy.

(PDF)

S1 Table. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

(PDF)

S2 Table. Search strategy in Medline (PubMed).

(PDF)

S3 Table. Items for risk of bias assessment.

(PDF)

S4 Table. Main reasons of exclusion of eligible studies.

(PDF)

S5 Table. Baseline characteristics of studies meeting inclusion criteria.

(PDF)

S6 Table. Risk of bias assessment.

(PDF)

S7 Table. Individual characteristics of included studies.

(PDF)

S8 Table. P-value of Khi-2 and Fisher exact tests for qualitative confounding factors.

(PDF)

S9 Table. P-value of student test for quantitative confounding factors.

(PDF)

S10 Table. Subgroup analyses of wheezing in children with LRTI in infancy and control without respiratory diseases.

(PDF)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

The authors received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Dawood FS, Fry AM, Goswami D, Sharmeen A, Nahar K, Anjali BA, et al. Incidence and characteristics of early childhood wheezing, Dhaka, Bangladesh, 2004–2010. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2015; n/a-n/a. 10.1002/ppul.23343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hopp RJ. Recurrent wheezing in infants and young children and bronchial hyperresponsiveness: a perspective. Clin Rev Allergy Immunol. 2003;24: 7–18. 10.1385/CRIAI:24:1:7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Piippo-Savolainen E, Korppi M. Wheezy babies—wheezy adults? Review on long-term outcome until adulthood after early childhood wheezing. Acta Paediatr. 2008;97: 5–11. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00558.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Openshaw PJ, Hewitt C. Protective and harmful effects of viral infections in childhood on wheezing disorders and asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2000;162: S40–43. 10.1164/ajrccm.162.supplement_1.maic-11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Rooney JC, Williams HE. The relationship between proved viral bronchiolitis and subsequent wheezing. The Journal of Pediatrics. 1971;79: 744–747. 10.1016/s0022-3476(71)80385-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Rossey I, Saelens X. Vaccines against human respiratory syncytial virus in clinical trials, where are we now? Expert Rev Vaccines. 2019. 10.1080/14760584.2019.1675520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Brunwasser SM, Snyder BM, Driscoll AJ, Fell DB, Savitz DA, Feikin DR, et al. Assessing the strength of evidence for a causal effect of respiratory syncytial virus lower respiratory tract infections on subsequent wheezing illness: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Respir Med. 2020;8: 795–806. 10.1016/S2213-2600(20)30109-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Shi T, Ooi Y, Zaw EM, Utjesanovic N, Campbell H, Cunningham S, et al. Association Between Respiratory Syncytial Virus-Associated Acute Lower Respiratory Infection in Early Life and Recurrent Wheeze and Asthma in Later Childhood. J Infect Dis. 2019. [cited 2 Aug 2019]. 10.1093/infdis/jiz311 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kneyber MCJ, Steyerberg EW, Groot R de, Moll HA. Long-term effects of respiratory syncytial virus (RSV) bronchiolitis in infants and young children: a quantitative review. Acta Paediatrica. 2000;89: 654–660. 10.1080/080352500750043945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fauroux B, Simões EAF, Checchia PA, Paes B, Figueras-Aloy J, Manzoni P, et al. The Burden and Long-term Respiratory Morbidity Associated with Respiratory Syncytial Virus Infection in Early Childhood. Infect Dis Ther. 2017;6: 173–197. 10.1007/s40121-017-0151-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kenmoe S, Kengne-Nde C, Ebogo-Belobo JT, Mbaga DS, Modiyinji AF, Njouom R. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the prevalence of common respiratory viruses in children < 2 years with bronchiolitis in the pre-COVID-19 pandemic era. PLOS ONE. 2020;15: e0242302. 10.1371/journal.pone.0242302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Amat F, Henquell C, Verdan M, Roszyk L, Mulliez A, Labbé A. Predicting the severity of acute bronchiolitis in infants: Should we use a clinical score or a biomarker?: Is a Biomarker Useful in Acute Bronchiolitis? Journal of Medical Virology. 2014;86: 1944–1952. 10.1002/jmv.23850 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Huguenin A, Moutte L, Renois F, Leveque N, Talmud D, Abely M, et al. Broad respiratory virus detection in infants hospitalized for bronchiolitis by use of a multiplex RT-PCR DNA microarray system. J Med Virol. 2012;84: 979–985. 10.1002/jmv.23272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McNamara PS, Flanagan BF, Smyth RL, Hart CA. Impact of human metapneumovirus and respiratory syncytial virus co-infection in severe bronchiolitis. Pediatric pulmonology. 2007;42: 740–3. 10.1002/ppul.20649 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ricart S, Garcia-Garcia JJ, Anton A, Pumarola T, Pons M, Muñoz-Almagro C, et al. Analysis of Human Metapneumovirus and Human Bocavirus Viral Load: The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal. 2013;32: 1032–1034. 10.1097/INF.0b013e3182932f4f [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Uyar M, Kuyucu N, Tezcan S, Aslan G, Tasdelen B. [Determination of the frequency of human bocavirus and other respiratory viruses among 0–2 years age group children diagnosed as acute bronchiolitis]. Mikrobiyoloji bulteni. 2014;48: 242–58. 10.5578/mb.7575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lemanske RF Jr, Jackson DJ, Gangnon RE, Evans MD, Li Z, Shult PA, et al. Rhinovirus illnesses during infancy predict subsequent childhood wheezing. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology. 2005;116: 571–577. 10.1016/j.jaci.2005.06.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Liu L, Pan Y, Zhu Y, Song Y, Su X, Yang L, et al. Association between rhinovirus wheezing illness and the development of childhood asthma: a meta-analysis. BMJ Open. 2017;7: e013034. 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-013034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Nair H, Simões EAF, Rudan I, Gessner BD, Azziz-Baumgartner E, Zhang JSF, et al. Global and regional burden of hospital admissions for severe acute lower respiratory infections in young children in 2010: a systematic analysis. Lancet. 2013;381: 1380–1390. 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61901-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lukšić I, Kearns PK, Scott F, Rudan I, Campbell H, Nair H. Viral etiology of hospitalized acute lower respiratory infections in children under 5 years of age—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Croat Med J. 2013;54: 122–134. 10.3325/cmj.2013.54.122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Feldman AS, He Y, Moore ML, Hershenson MB, Hartert TV. Toward Primary Prevention of Asthma. Reviewing the Evidence for Early-Life Respiratory Viral Infections as Modifiable Risk Factors to Prevent Childhood Asthma. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2015;191: 34–44. 10.1164/rccm.201405-0901PP [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Long CE, McBride JT, Hall CB. Sequelae of respiratory syncytial virus infections. A role for intervention studies. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 1995;151: 1678–1680; discussion 1680–1681. 10.1164/ajrccm/151.5_Pt_1.1678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Martinez FD. The Origins of Asthma and Chronic Obstructive Pulmonary Disease in Early Life. Proc Am Thorac Soc. 2009;6: 272–277. 10.1513/pats.200808-092RM [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Stein RT. Long-term airway morbidity following viral LRTI in early infancy: recurrent wheezing or asthma? Paediatr Respir Rev. 2009;10 Suppl 1: 29–31. 10.1016/S1526-0542(09)70013-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Amat F, Plantard C, Mulliez A, Petit I, Rochette E, Verdan M, et al. RSV-hRV co-infection is a risk factor for recurrent bronchial obstruction and early sensitization 3 years after bronchiolitis. J Med Virol. 2018;90: 867–872. 10.1002/jmv.25037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gurwitz D, Mindorff C, Levison H. Increased incidence of bronchial reactivity in children with a history of bronchiolitis. J Pediatr. 1981;98: 551–555. 10.1016/s0022-3476(81)80758-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Henry RL, Hodges IG, Milner AD, Stokes GM. Respiratory problems 2 years after acute bronchiolitis in infancy. Arch Dis Child. 1983;58: 713–716. 10.1136/adc.58.9.713 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kattan M, Keens TG, Lapierre JG, Levison H, Bryan AC, Reilly BJ. Pulmonary function abnormalities in symptom-free children after bronchiolitis. Pediatrics. 1977;59: 683–688. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kuikka L, Reijonen T, Remes K, Korppi M. Bronchial asthma after early childhood wheezing: a follow-up until 4.5–6 years of age. Acta Paediatr. 1994;83: 744–748. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1994.tb13131.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Laing I, Reidel F, Yap PL, Simpson H. Atopy predisposing to acute bronchiolitis during an epidemic of respiratory syncytial virus. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1982;284: 1070–1072. 10.1136/bmj.284.6322.1070 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Martinez FD, Wright AL, Taussig LM, Holberg CJ, Halonen M, Morgan WJ. Asthma and wheezing in the first six years of life. The Group Health Medical Associates. N Engl J Med. 1995;332: 133–138. 10.1056/NEJM199501193320301 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Mok JY, Simpson H. Symptoms, atopy, and bronchial reactivity after lower respiratory infection in infancy. Arch Dis Child. 1984;59: 299–305. 10.1136/adc.59.4.299 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Price JF. Acute and long-term effects of viral bronchiolitis in infancy. Lung. 1990;168 Suppl: 414–421. 10.1007/BF02718159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rylander E, Eriksson M, Freyschuss U. Risk factors for occasional and recurrent wheezing after RSV infection in infancy. Acta Paediatr Scand. 1988;77: 711–715. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1988.tb10735.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Martinez FD, Morgan WJ, Wright AL, Holberg CJ, Taussig LM. Diminished lung function as a predisposing factor for wheezing respiratory illness in infants. N Engl J Med. 1988;319: 1112–1117. 10.1056/NEJM198810273191702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Carlsen KH, Larsen S, Bjerve O, Leegaard J. Acute bronchiolitis: predisposing factors and characterization of infants at risk. Pediatric pulmonology. 1987;3: 153–60. 10.1002/ppul.1950030308 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wittig HJ, Glaser J. The relationship between bronchiolitis and childhood asthma; a follow-up study of 100 cases of bronchiolitis. J Allergy. 1959;30: 19–23. 10.1016/0021-8707(59)90054-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Holt PG, Sly PD. Interactions between RSV Infection, Asthma, and Atopy. J Exp Med. 2002;196: 1271–1275. 10.1084/jem.20021572 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Simoes EAF, Groothuis JR, Carbonell-Estrany X, Rieger CHL, Mitchell I, Fredrick LM, et al. Palivizumab prophylaxis, respiratory syncytial virus, and subsequent recurrent wheezing. J Pediatr. 2007;151: 34–42, 42.e1. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.02.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Yoshihara S, Kusuda S, Mochizuki H, Okada K, Nishima S, Simões EAF, et al. Effect of palivizumab prophylaxis on subsequent recurrent wheezing in preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2013;132: 811–818. 10.1542/peds.2013-0982 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Centers for Reviews and Dissemination. CRD’s guidance for undertaking reviews in 301 healthcare: centers for reviews and dissemination. England: York Associates; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6. 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ouzzani M, Hammady H, Fedorowicz Z, Elmagarmid A. Rayyan—a web and mobile app for systematic reviews. Syst Rev. 2016;5. 10.1186/s13643-015-0182-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hoy D, Brooks P, Woolf A, Blyth F, March L, Bain C, et al. Assessing risk of bias in prevalence studies: modification of an existing tool and evidence of interrater agreement. J Clin Epidemiol. 2012;65: 934–939. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2011.11.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.DerSimonian R, Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials revisited. Contemp Clin Trials. 2015;45: 139–145. 10.1016/j.cct.2015.09.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Higgins JPT, Thompson SG, Spiegelhalter DJ. A re-evaluation of random-effects meta-analysis. J R Stat Soc Ser A Stat Soc. 2009;172: 137–159. 10.1111/j.1467-985X.2008.00552.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Higgins JPT, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21: 1539–1558. 10.1002/sim.1186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Cochran WG. The Combination of Estimates from Different Experiments. Biometrics. 1954;10: 101–129. 10.2307/3001666 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wan X, Wang W, Liu J, Tong T. Estimating the sample mean and standard deviation from the sample size, median, range and/or interquartile range. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2014;14. 10.1186/1471-2288-14-14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Schwarzer G. meta: An R package for meta-analysis. R News. 2007;7: 40–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Balduzzi S, Rücker G, Schwarzer G. How to perform a meta-analysis with R: a practical tutorial. Evid Based Ment Health. 2019;22: 153–160. 10.1136/ebmental-2019-300117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bertrand P, Lay MK, Piedimonte G, Brockmann PE, Palavecino CE, Hernández J, et al. Elevated IL-3 and IL-12p40 levels in the lower airway of infants with RSV-induced bronchiolitis correlate with recurrent wheezing. Cytokine. 2015;76: 417–423. 10.1016/j.cyto.2015.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Fjærli H-O, Farstad T, Rød G, Ufert GK, Gulbrandsen P, Nakstad B. Acute bronchiolitis in infancy as risk factor for wheezing and reduced pulmonary function by seven years in Akershus County, Norway. BMC Pediatr. 2005;5: 31. 10.1186/1471-2431-5-31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.García-García ML, Calvo C, Casas I, Bracamonte T, Rellán A, Gozalo F, et al. Human metapneumovirus bronchiolitis in infancy is an important risk factor for asthma at age 5. Pediatr Pulmonol. 2007;42: 458–464. 10.1002/ppul.20597 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Henderson J, Hilliard TN, Sherriff A, Stalker D, Al Shammari N, Thomas HM. Hospitalization for RSV bronchiolitis before 12 months of age and subsequent asthma, atopy and wheeze: a longitudinal birth cohort study. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2005;16: 386–392. 10.1111/j.1399-3038.2005.00298.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Juntti H, Kokkonen J, Dunder T, Renko M, Niinimäki A, Uhari M. Association of an early respiratory syncytial virus infection and atopic allergy. Allergy. 2003;58: 878–884. 10.1034/j.1398-9995.2003.00233.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Kristjánsson S, Wennergren D, Eriksson B, Thórarinsdóttir H, Wennergren G. U-EPX levels and wheezing in infants and young children with and without RSV bronchiolitis. Respir Med. 2006;100: 878–883. 10.1016/j.rmed.2005.08.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Osundwa VM, Dawod ST, Ehlayel M. Recurrent wheezing in children with respiratory syncytial virus (RSV) bronchiolitis in Qatar. Eur J Pediatr. 1993;152: 1001–1003. 10.1007/BF01957225 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Poorisrisak P, Halkjaer LB, Thomsen SF, Stensballe LG, Kyvik KO, Skytthe A, et al. Causal direction between respiratory syncytial virus bronchiolitis and asthma studied in monozygotic twins. Chest. 2010;138: 338–344. 10.1378/chest.10-0365 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Pullan CR, Hey EN. Wheezing, asthma, and pulmonary dysfunction 10 years after infection with respiratory syncytial virus in infancy. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed). 1982;284: 1665–1669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Sigurs N, Bjarnason R, Sigurbergsson F, Kjellman B, Björkstén B. Asthma and immunoglobulin E antibodies after respiratory syncytial virus bronchiolitis: a prospective cohort study with matched controls. Pediatrics. 1995;95: 500–505. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sigurs N, Aljassim F, Kjellman B, Robinson PD, Sigurbergsson F, Bjarnason R, et al. Asthma and allergy patterns over 18 years after severe RSV bronchiolitis in the first year of life. Thorax. 2010;65: 1045–1052. 10.1136/thx.2009.121582 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sigurs N, Bjarnason R, Sigurbergsson F, Kjellman B. Respiratory Syncytial Virus Bronchiolitis in Infancy Is an Important Risk Factor for Asthma and Allergy at Age 7. American Journal of Respiratory and Critical Care Medicine. 2000;161: 1501–1507. 10.1164/ajrccm.161.5.9906076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Sigurs N, Gustafsson PM, Bjarnason R, Lundberg F, Schmidt S, Sigurbergsson F, et al. Severe respiratory syncytial virus bronchiolitis in infancy and asthma and allergy at age 13. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2005;171: 137–141. 10.1164/rccm.200406-730OC [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sims DG, Downham MA, Gardner PS, Webb JK, Weightman D. Study of 8-year-old children with a history of respiratory syncytial virus bronchiolitis in infancy. Br Med J. 1978;1: 11–14. 10.1136/bmj.1.6104.11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Singleton RJ, Redding GJ, Lewis TC, Martinez P, Bulkow L, Morray B, et al. Sequelae of severe respiratory syncytial virus infection in infancy and early childhood among Alaska Native children. Pediatrics. 2003;112: 285–290. 10.1542/peds.112.2.285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Sly PD, Soto-Quiros ME, Landau LI, Hudson I, Newton-John H. Factors predisposing to abnormal pulmonary function after adenovirus type 7 pneumonia. Arch Dis Child. 1984;59: 935–939. 10.1136/adc.59.10.935 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Stensballe LG, Simonsen J, Breindahl M, Winding L, Kofoed P-E. Wheeze after Hospitalization for Respiratory Syncytial Virus Infection in Children. Journal of Pediatric Infectious Diseases. 2018;13: 46–50. 10.1055/s-0037-1606565 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Zomer-Kooijker K, van der Ent CK, Ermers MJJ, Uiterwaal CSPM, Rovers MM, Bont LJ. Increased Risk of Wheeze and Decreased Lung Function after Respiratory Syncytial Virus Infection. PLoS One. 2014;9. 10.1371/journal.pone.0087162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Kneyber MCJ, Steyerberg EW, Groot R de, Moll HA. Long-term effects of respiratory syncytial virus (RSV) bronchiolitis in infants and young children: a quantitative review. Acta Paediatrica. 2000;89: 654–660. 10.1080/080352500750043945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Fauroux B, Simões EAF, Checchia PA, Paes B, Figueras-Aloy J, Manzoni P, et al. The Burden and Long-term Respiratory Morbidity Associated with Respiratory Syncytial Virus Infection in Early Childhood. Infect Dis Ther. 2017. 10.1007/s40121-017-0151-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Edmond K, Scott S, Korczak V, Ward C, Sanderson C, Theodoratou E, et al. Long Term Sequelae from Childhood Pneumonia; Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. PLoS One. 2012;7. 10.1371/journal.pone.0031239 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Régnier S, Huels J. Association between Respiratory Syncytial Virus Hospitalizations in Infants and Respiratory Sequelae: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal. 2013; 1. 10.1097/INF.0b013e31829061e8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Stensballe LG, Ravn H, Kristensen K, Agerskov K, Meakins T, Aaby P, et al. Respiratory syncytial virus neutralizing antibodies in cord blood, respiratory syncytial virus hospitalization, and recurrent wheeze. J Allergy Clin Immunol. 2009;123: 398–403. 10.1016/j.jaci.2008.10.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Strachan DP, Butland BK, Anderson HR. Incidence and prognosis of asthma and wheezing illness from early childhood to age 33 in a national British cohort. BMJ. 1996;312: 1195–1199. 10.1136/bmj.312.7040.1195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Rhodes HL, Thomas P, Sporik R, Holgate ST, Cogswell JJ. A birth cohort study of subjects at risk of atopy: twenty-two-year follow-up of wheeze and atopic status. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2002;165: 176–180. 10.1164/ajrccm.165.2.2104032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Stein RT, Sherrill D, Morgan WJ, Holberg CJ, Halonen M, Taussig LM, et al. Respiratory syncytial virus in early life and risk of wheeze and allergy by age 13 years. Lancet. 1999;354: 541–545. 10.1016/S0140-6736(98)10321-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Mikalsen IB, Halvorsen T, Øymar K. The outcome after severe bronchiolitis is related to gender and virus. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. 2012;23: 391–398. 10.1111/j.1399-3038.2012.01283.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Wang X, Dockery DW, Wypij D, Fay ME, Ferris BG. Pulmonary function between 6 and 18 years of age. Pediatr Pulmonol. 1993;15: 75–88. 10.1002/ppul.1950150204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Stein RT, Holberg CJ, Sherrill D, Wright AL, Morgan WJ, Taussig L, et al. Influence of parental smoking on respiratory symptoms during the first decade of life: the Tucson Children’s Respiratory Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1999;149: 1030–1037. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Openshaw PJ. Potential mechanisms causing delayed effects of respiratory syncytial virus infection. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2001;163: S10–13. 10.1164/ajrccm.163.supplement_1.2011111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Balfour-Lynn IM. Why do viruses make infants wheeze? Arch Dis Child. 1996;74: 251–259. 10.1136/adc.74.3.251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Martinez FD, Morgan WJ, Wright AL, Holberg C, Taussig LM. Initial airway function is a risk factor for recurrent wheezing respiratory illnesses during the first three years of life. Group Health Medical Associates. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1991;143: 312–316. 10.1164/ajrccm/143.2.312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Young S, O’Keeffe PT, Arnott J, Landau LI. Lung function, airway responsiveness, and respiratory symptoms before and after bronchiolitis. Arch Dis Child. 1995;72: 16–24. 10.1136/adc.72.1.16 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Tager IB, Hanrahan JP, Tosteson TD, Castile RG, Brown RW, Weiss ST, et al. Lung function, pre- and post-natal smoke exposure, and wheezing in the first year of life. Am Rev Respir Dis. 1993;147: 811–817. 10.1164/ajrccm/147.4.811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Backman K, Piippo-Savolainen E, Ollikainen H, Koskela H, Korppi M. Increased asthma risk and impaired quality of life after bronchiolitis or pneumonia in infancy: Asthma Risk After Early Bronchiolitis or Pneumonia. Pediatric Pulmonology. 2014;49: 318–325. 10.1002/ppul.22842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Jeng M-J, Lee Y-S, Tsao P-C, Yang C-F, Soong W-J. A Longitudinal Study on Early Hospitalized Airway Infections and Subsequent Childhood Asthma. Schildgen O, editor. PLOS ONE. 2015;10: e0121906. 10.1371/journal.pone.0121906 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.de Winter JJH, Bont L, Wilbrink B, van der Ent CK, Smit HA, Houben ML. Rhinovirus wheezing illness in infancy is associated with medically attended third year wheezing in low risk infants: results of a healthy birth cohort study. Immun Inflamm Dis. 2015;3: 398–405. 10.1002/iid3.77 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Bernadette van den Hoogen

29 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-36324

Influence of confounding factors on the association between early viral LRTI and subsequent wheezing development, a systematic review and meta-analysis

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Njouom,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

One of the reviewers has a number of major comments and has provided an extensive review rapport to improve the manuscript. Therefore, it is important that a revised version of the manuscript has all these issues addressed and contains all improvements as suggested by this reviewer. 

Please submit your revised manuscript by Feb 05 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bernadette van den Hoogen

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

"The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

At this time, please address the following queries:

  1. Please clarify the sources of funding (financial or material support) for your study. List the grants or organizations that supported your study, including funding received from your institution.

  2. State what role the funders took in the study. If the funders had no role in your study, please state: “The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.”

  3. If any authors received a salary from any of your funders, please state which authors and which funders.

  4. If you did not receive any funding for this study, please state: “The authors received no specific funding for this work.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This is a very-well written manuscript that proposes an update in the available results about LRTI and the risk of subsequent asthma

I would highlight in the text the mean age of evaluation, and also stress this point in the discussion (what is expected in term of respiratory prognosis in adulthood for these children?)

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity tor review this manuscript describing a meta-analytic review of studies evaluating the association between infant LRTI and subsequent wheezing illness. The study had a number of strengths, including a broad literature search, rating of study quality, consideration of a broad range of potential confounders, and duplicate coding of studies for inclusion. However, as described in detail below, I did not find the procedure for evaluating the influence of potential confounders compelling. Additionally, I found the descriptions for some critical aspects of the study procedures (e.g., operational definitions, statistical methods) to be unclear or incomplete. As a reader, I had to work hard to understand critical study procedures. For example, understanding how the authors defined “symmetrically” vs. “asymmetrically” distributed confounders required careful reading of the supplementary materials. This critical part of the study procedures should not be relegated to supplements that most readers won’t read. Finally, I do not agree with some of the authors’ interpretations of the study findings. I hope that these comments are helpful.

1. Abstract: “Our result supports absence of the influence of the main confounding factors” Frequentist null hypothesis testing can only provide evidence against the null hypothesis or inconclusive evidence. Lack of evidence for an effect is not evidence of absence. Therefore, I would recommend reworking this statement to say that the analyses did not provide evidence against the null hypothesis and avoid saying that there was evidence against the influence of the confounding factors.

2. Abstract: As PLOS One is a general scientific journal and not specifically focused on infectious airway disease, I think it’s important to define the acronyms HSRV and HMPV in the abstract rather than assuming that readers know what these acronyms mean.

3. lines 109-110: “We include only cohort studies that compare children with LRTI with controls in regards of wheezing as long-term respiratory sequelae.” I think it would be helpful to reader to have an explanation of why other studies (e.g., case-control studies) were excluded. Cohort studies have advantages over case-control studies that could potentially justify this decision, but I think it’s important to explain the rationale clearly.

4. lines 115-117: “Studies with only high-risk participants (preterm, chronic respiratory disease, heart disease or immunodeficiency) were not considered.” Again, why were these studies excluded? Is it not of interest whether LRTIs have a causal effect on chronic respiratory illness in these high-risk populations?

5. lines 122-123: “Wheezing was considered as any whistling nose at the expiration declared by the included study.” This doesn’t seem like a clear enough operationalization of the study outcome. Did you include any wheezing outcomes, including parental report?

6. Did the authors require a washout period between the LRTI and measurement of the wheezing outcomes to ensure that the wheezing was due to a subsequent illness and was not merely a symptom of the exposure-defining LRTI? That is, can we be fairly certain that the exposure and outcome variables were due to separate illnesses?

7. line 123: “Wheezing during follow up was defined as current wheezing.” This again is not clear to me. Does current wheezing mean that the child was wheezing during the evaluation or assessment?

8. I don’t think the statistical analysis section had sufficient detail. Did the authors use a fixed-effects models? Random effects models? If the authors used random-effects meta-analysis (this seems more appropriate than fixed effects), what method was used to estimate the between-study variable (e.g., DerSimonian Laird, Paule-Mandel, Hartung-Knapp)? How did the authors deal with the fact that many studies report more than one relevant estimate? For example, some studies report effect estimates for the same construct (e.g., current wheeze) at multiple time points or using different measures of the same construct. Did they average across estimates? Did they select specific estimates to include and, if so, how was it determined which estimates to include? Did the authors calculate naïve odds ratios or did they use adjusted odds ratios when they were available?

9. lines 156-157: “The validity of results was evaluated by a sensitivity analysis including only first episode of LRTI and studies with confounding factors distributed symmetrically between cases and controls.” How are the authors defining “symmetrically distributed”? What is the criterion used to determine whether confounders were symmetrically distributed? I gather from the S7 Table that the authors conducted chi-square tests evaluating whether the confounding factors were independent of LRTI exposure and used the p value to determine “symmetrically” vs. “asymmetrically” distributed confounding factors. This approach should be described clearly in the Method section. If this is the method that what used, I don’t think it is a compelling approach. Statistical significance is largely a function of sample size. Studies with large samples have the power to detect much smaller discrepancies between LRTI+ and LRTI- groups than small studies do. So, a small study could have an equal or larger discrepancy on a confounder between the LRTI+ and LRTI- groups than a much larger study, yet be labeled as “symmetric” simply because it had less power to detect the discrepancy. Relatedly, the fact that there is no significant difference between the exposed and non-exposed groups on levels of a confounder does not mean that the confounder cannot bias the estimate of the effect of the exposure on the outcome. Again, with small sample sizes, there could be important imbalances that are not statistically significant. In sum, I am not convinced that the variable coding studies as having “asymmetrically” vs. “symmetrically” distributed confounders provides a reasonable approximation for the level of balance on the confounders. Therefore, I am not sure if the sensitivity analysis including only symmetrical studies has great value.

10. Table 1 is not adequately labeled or described. Do the rows represent subgroup analyses? For example, does the row labeled “Heredity for asthma” provide an estimate for the subgroup of estimates from samples that were positive for asthma heredity? Or does this row provide an aggregate estimate among the subgroup of studies for which there was no significant difference between the LRTI+ and LRTI- groups on heredity for asthma? How was heredity for asthma defined?

11. line 160: “We analyzed the data using the R software version 3.5.1.” It would be good to also provide the version of the meta package used to conduct the analyses.

12. line 181: “All included studies were at low risk of bias.” This statement seems too vague. In the supplement, the authors note that a score of 6-9 on the Newcastle-Ottawa scale was indicative of low bias. Were there any areas of concern across the studies? For example, did all of the studies have low risk of bias due to missingness? This seems unlikely.

13. In Table S9, the authors provide the “P-value subgroup difference” in the final column. But there I did not see any information about how this p-value was obtained. Did the authors run multiple meta-analytic models with these study characteristics entered as covariates to obtain the p-values? Was one covariate entered per model or were multiple covariates entered in the same model? Generally speaking, I think more information is needed about the modeling approach. Why did the authors include 95% prediction intervals in the table? How should the readers interpret this and why is it needed in addition to the 95% confidence interval?

14. lines 212-214: “The type of LRTI was associated with the occurrence of wheezing (p= 0.007) (S9 Table). History of bronchiolitis (OR= 3.8, 95% CI= 3.4-4.7) and LRTI unspecified (OR= 1.9, 95% CI= 1.4-2.7) were associated with the occurrence of wheezing in childhood.” If I understand correctly, the type of LRTI accounted for significant heterogeneity in the effect estimates. This means that the strength of the estimated effect of LRTI on wheezing outcomes differed depending on the type of LRTI. But the authors don’t explain which comparison was driving this effect. They note that studies using both bronchiolitis and unspecified LRTI found significant weighted mean effect sizes. The third category (studies evaluating pneumonia as the exposure) did not yield a significant effect size, though this is likely due to the fact that there was only one pneumonia exposure study. The point estimate for the pneumonia study (OR=2.8) was larger than the point estimate and the upper bound of the 95% CI for the unspecified bronchiolitis studies. So, we shouldn’t make much of the fact that there was too much uncertainty in the pneumonia study to reject the null hypothesis. My guess is that the weighted mean OR for the bronchiolitis studies was significantly larger than the weighted mean OR for the unspecified studies and this is why LRTI type accounts for significant heterogeneity. The 95% Cis for these two study groups (bronchiolitis and unspecified) do not overlap at all. But the meaning of the significant effect of LRTI type does not come across in the authors’ description at all.

15. lines 216-218: “The type of LRTI was associated with the occurrence of wheezing (p= 0.007) (S9 Table). History of bronchiolitis (OR= 3.8, 95% CI= 3.4-4.7) and LRTI unspecified (OR= 1.9, 95% CI= 1.4-2.7) were associated with the occurrence of wheezing in childhood.” The authors present what appears to be an analysis of whether “Age range at recruitment” accounts for significant between-study heterogeneity in effect estimates. Again, however, the authors follow up a significant effect of this covariate with an analysis of which subgroups (levels of the covariate) yielded significant weighted mean ORs. There is no focus on which levels of the covariate differ from each other (e.g., Is there a significant difference in the mean OR for the studies beginning recruitment before 6 months compared to studies beginning recruitment before 1 year of age?), making the effect of the covariate hard to interpret for readers. Furthermore, the authors created a categorical “Age at recruitment” variable with four levels. However, two cells of this variable are highly sparse, containing one and two studies. I don’t think the authors have enough data to support a four-level factor for this analysis.

16. lines 218-219: “We did not record any difference in the occurrence of wheezing by type of virus screened (p= 0.249).” I don’t think that this analysis was very meaningful as 20 of the 22 studies evaluated HRSV. There is simply not enough information to conduct a meaningful analysis.

17. lines 221-222: “No difference was recorded in the occurrence of wheezing depending on the sampling approach (non-probabilistic vs probabilistic, p= 0.505)” Again, I don’t think that this was a meaningful analysis as there was only one study that used probabilistic sampling.

18. lines 228-229: “Children who experienced an episode of HMPV or HRSV bronchiolitis at ≤ 3 years of age were 3 times more likely than controls to develop wheezing later.” It’s unclear to me why the main conclusion is limited to HMPV and HRSV bronchiolitis rather than LRTI more broadly. The main hypothesis was about LRTI more generally. Why are we drawing conclusions about specific subgroups rather than LRTI altogether?

19. lines 229-232: “This increased risk of wheezing in children with a history of HMPV or HRSV bronchiolitis was not influenced by the rank of bronchiolitis episode and any of the investigated confounding factors.” It’s not clear to me what “rank of bronchiolitis episode” means. Additionally, I don’t think that the authors truly tested whether confounding factors influenced the estimates. As noted in an earlier comment, I don’t think the procedure for determining “symmetrically” and “asymmetrically” distributed confounders was compelling. It would have been far more interesting to evaluate whether estimates based on adjustment for specific covariates believed to represent major confounders (e.g., genetic confounding) were smaller than estimates that did not adjust for these factors.

20. lines 248-249: “Asthma, considered by most authors as ≥ 3 episodes of wheezing, were however not associated with pneumonia in the meta-analysis.” This comment must be put in context. According to Table S9, there was only one pneumonia study with 40 participants. This meta-analysis does not provide us any new information beyond what the one pneumonia study has already provided because it simply reports the results of this one trial – there is not combining of information across pneumonia studies.

21. lines 255-257: “The results of this systematic review, also, indicate that apart of HRSV, HMPV bronchiolitis was associated with a risk of subsequent wheezing.” Again, according to Table S9, there was only one HMPV study. There was no need for meta-analytic aggregation because there was only a single estimate, from what I can tell. In which case, this review did not provide any new information about the association between HMPV and subsequent wheezing above what was already reported from the single study that evaluated the association between HMPV LRTI and wheezing illness. There is no unique contribution of the meta-analysis in relation to the estimated effect of HMPV LRTI on subsequent wheezing illness.

22. line 261: “In this systematic review, studies with confounding factor similarly distributed” This is an important point, so I will make it again. The fact that there was no statistically significant imbalance across the LRTI+ and LRTI- groups does not necessarily mean that the confounding variable was similarly distributed across the two groups.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Apr 15;16(4):e0249831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249831.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


6 Jan 2021

Review Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1: This is a very-well written manuscript that proposes an update in the available results about LRTI and the risk of subsequent asthma

Authors: Thank you for appreciation and summary.

I would highlight in the text the mean age of evaluation, and also stress this point in the discussion (what is expected in term of respiratory prognosis in adulthood for these children?)

Authors: Thank you for this suggestion. We added to the manuscript a subgroup analysis by age of participants at interview in the results and discussion sections. See below corresponding text:

“Wheezing in children was reported between 2 and 20 years and most studies were between 2 and 10 years (77.2%).

No difference was recorded in the occurrence of wheezing depending on the age range at the interview (p= 0.053)

However, all age groups at interview up to 20 years were associated with an increased risk of developing wheezing in former cases of LRTI compared to controls.

Previous reports have shown that the strength of the association between LRTI in childhood and wheezing later diminished with age with persistence into adulthood [18,67,68,70–74]. These previous reports suggest that the factor responsible for wheezing loses its influence with age, which could be small airways. Unfortunately, in this report, we have only recorded 2 studies with patients > 10 and are therefore unable to comment on the decrease in the strength of the association between LRTI in childhood and the subsequent wheezing risk.”

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity tor review this manuscript describing a meta-analytic review of studies evaluating the association between infant LRTI and subsequent wheezing illness. The study had a number of strengths, including a broad literature search, rating of study quality, consideration of a broad range of potential confounders, and duplicate coding of studies for inclusion. However, as described in detail below, I did not find the procedure for evaluating the influence of potential confounders compelling. Additionally, I found the descriptions for some critical aspects of the study procedures (e.g., operational definitions, statistical methods) to be unclear or incomplete. As a reader, I had to work hard to understand critical study procedures. For example, understanding how the authors defined “symmetrically” vs. “asymmetrically” distributed confounders required careful reading of the supplementary materials. This critical part of the study procedures should not be relegated to supplements that most readers won’t read. Finally, I do not agree with some of the authors’ interpretations of the study findings. I hope that these comments are helpful.

Authors: Thank you for this summary.

1. Abstract: “Our result supports absence of the influence of the main confounding factors” Frequentist null hypothesis testing can only provide evidence against the null hypothesis or inconclusive evidence. Lack of evidence for an effect is not evidence of absence. Therefore, I would recommend reworking this statement to say that the analyses did not provide evidence against the null hypothesis and avoid saying that there was evidence against the influence of the confounding factors.

Authors: Thank you for these suggestions. The abstract has been modified and can now be read as below:

“The risk of developing subsequent wheezing was conserved when considering studies with comparable groups for socio-demographic and clinical confounders.

When considering studies with comparable groups for most confounding factors, our results provided strong evidence for the association between neonatal HRSV or HMPV bronchiolitis and the subsequent wheezing development.”

2. Abstract: As PLOS One is a general scientific journal and not specifically focused on infectious airway disease, I think it’s important to define the acronyms HSRV and HMPV in the abstract rather than assuming that readers know what these acronyms mean.

Authors: Thank you, the text has been corrected as suggested

3. lines 109-110: “We include only cohort studies that compare children with LRTI with controls in regards of wheezing as long-term respiratory sequelae.” I think it would be helpful to reader to have an explanation of why other studies (e.g., case-control studies) were excluded. Cohort studies have advantages over case-control studies that could potentially justify this decision, but I think it’s important to explain the rationale clearly.

Authors: Thank you for this suggestion. The text has been modified and can now be read as below:

“Due to the multiple limitations of cross-sectional (non-comparable groups) and case control (misclassification and recall bias) studies in estimating the association between exposure and outcome, we include only cohort studies that compare children with LRTI with controls in regards of wheezing as long-term respiratory sequelae.”

4. lines 115-117: “Studies with only high-risk participants (preterm, chronic respiratory disease, heart disease or immunodeficiency) were not considered.” Again, why were these studies excluded? Is it not of interest whether LRTIs have a causal effect on chronic respiratory illness in these high-risk populations?

Authors: We thank the reviewer for this excellent comment. The long-term sequelae of lower respiratory infections in childhood in subjects with underlying medical conditions have been the subject of numerous observational and interventional studies and are of considerable interest. This aspect is not, however, part of the objectives of this review. Given the critical importance of this research question, we have already recorded a second protocol at Prospero (CRD42019131343) to more specifically address the concern in another ongoing review.

5. lines 122-123: “Wheezing was considered as any whistling nose at the expiration declared by the included study.” This doesn’t seem like a clear enough operationalization of the study outcome. Did you include any wheezing outcomes, including parental report?

Authors: Thank you for this excellent comment. We included all wheezing including those defined by parents. We added all wheezing definitions from included studies in S6 Table. We also conducted a sensitivity analysis including only studies with physician-diagnosed wheezing, and it does not reflect any major differences from overall results.

6. Did the authors require a washout period between the LRTI and measurement of the wheezing outcomes to ensure that the wheezing was due to a subsequent illness and was not merely a symptom of the exposure-defining LRTI? That is, can we be fairly certain that the exposure and outcome variables were due to separate illnesses?

Authors: As shown in S6 Table, the minimum time between the onset of the lower respiratory infection and the development of wheezing was 1.5 years. We also considered from the inclusion criteria predefined in the protocol declared in Prospero to include only studies with a minimum follow-up duration of one year, which suggests a sufficiently long duration for wheezing (outcome) recorded to not be a symptom of lower respiratory disease (exposure). Thank you.

7. line 123: “Wheezing during follow up was defined as current wheezing.” This again is not clear to me. Does current wheezing mean that the child was wheezing during the evaluation or assessment?

Authors: Thank you for this suggestion. The text has been modified and can now be read as below:

“Wheezing during the interview was defined as current wheezing. Wheezing in the last 12 months was defined as wheezing episodes during the previous year. Recurrent wheezing was considered for persistent wheezing between the diagnosis of LRTI and the interview.”

8. I don’t think the statistical analysis section had sufficient detail. Did the authors use a fixed-effects models? Random effects models? If the authors used random-effects meta-analysis (this seems more appropriate than fixed effects), what method was used to estimate the between-study variable (e.g., DerSimonian Laird, Paule-Mandel, Hartung-Knapp)? How did the authors deal with the fact that many studies report more than one relevant estimate? For example, some studies report effect estimates for the same construct (e.g., current wheeze) at multiple time points or using different measures of the same construct. Did they average across estimates? Did they select specific estimates to include and, if so, how was it determined which estimates to include? Did the authors calculate naïve odds ratios or did they use adjusted odds ratios when they were available?

Authors: Thank you for this suggestion. The text has been modified and can now be read as below:

“If there were multiple wheezing phenotypes in the same study, we chose the one that spanned the longest time. We separately considered studies with multiple effect data depending on interview time, respiratory viruses detected or types of LRTI.

The odds ratio (OR) and the 95% confidence interval (95% CI) were used as a measure of the association between LRTI and long-term wheezing. These parameters were calculated using a random-effects model by the Der Simonian and Laird method [45]. Heterogeneity was assessed by the Q test p-value and the I² statistic [46,47]. The heterogeneity between the studies was considered significant for the values of P <0.1 and I²> 50%. The robustness of the results was assessed by a sensitivity analysis including only the first episode of LRTI, physician-diagnosed wheezing and studies with proportions of confounding factors comparable between cases and controls. The comparability of confounders between cases and controls were estimated using the Chi-square and Fisher tests for qualitative variables and Student's T-test for continuous variables. The primary confounding factors data for continuous variables expressed as median and/or range were converted to mean and standard deviation [48]. Subgroup analyses were performed based on sampling method, time of exposure collection, age range during LRTI, age at time of interview for wheezing, hospitalization of controls, viruses responsible for LRTI, type of LRTI, and type of wheezing. We analysed the data using version 3.5.1 of software R [49,50].”

9. lines 156-157: “The validity of results was evaluated by a sensitivity analysis including only first episode of LRTI and studies with confounding factors distributed symmetrically between cases and controls.” How are the authors defining “symmetrically distributed”? What is the criterion used to determine whether confounders were symmetrically distributed? I gather from the S7 Table that the authors conducted chi-square tests evaluating whether the confounding factors were independent of LRTI exposure and used the p value to determine “symmetrically” vs. “asymmetrically” distributed confounding factors. This approach should be described clearly in the Method section.

Authors: Thank you for this comment. We have now described in full detail our definition of “symmetric distribution” and the methodological approach we used to obtain the results. The text has been modified and can now be read as below:

“The robustness of the results was assessed by a sensitivity analysis including only the first episode of LRTI, physician-diagnosed wheezing and studies with proportions of confounding factors comparable between cases and controls. The comparability of confounders between cases and controls were estimated using the Chi-square and Fisher tests for qualitative variables and Student's T-test for continuous variables. The primary confounding factors data for continuous variables expressed as median and/or range were converted to mean and standard deviation [48].”

If this is the method that what used, I don’t think it is a compelling approach. Statistical significance is largely a function of sample size. Studies with large samples have the power to detect much smaller discrepancies between LRTI+ and LRTI- groups than small studies do. So, a small study could have an equal or larger discrepancy on a confounder between the LRTI+ and LRTI- groups than a much larger study, yet be labeled as “symmetric” simply because it had less power to detect the discrepancy. Relatedly, the fact that there is no significant difference between the exposed and non-exposed groups on levels of a confounder does not mean that the confounder cannot bias the estimate of the effect of the exposure on the outcome. Again, with small sample sizes, there could be important imbalances that are not statistically significant. In sum, I am not convinced that the variable coding studies as having “asymmetrically” vs. “symmetrically” distributed confounders provides a reasonable approximation for the level of balance on the confounders. Therefore, I am not sure if the sensitivity analysis including only symmetrical studies has great value.

Authors: We greatly thank the reviewer for these clarifications. We used a parametric test (chi-square test) and a non-parametric test (fisher test) to try to attenuate the effect of this small population size on the statistical significance of the distribution of confusing factors between cases and controls. We have now pointed out this aspect in the limits section of our discussion. We fully agree with the Reviewer that a symmetrical allocation of the confounding factor between cases and controls does not guarantee the exclusion of its influence on the effect of exposure on outcome. However, it is known that comparable samples on the majority of confounding factors give a result with a higher level of proof than those which are not. In this regard, the randomized trials which are known to have comparable sample according to most of the confounding factors. Studies paired for certain sociodemographic confounding factors such as sex and age are also accepted as comparable for these aspects. However, the examination of the comparability of the remaining confounders is most often done on the basis of the statistical significance of the tests performed, as we have done in the present work.

10. Table 1 is not adequately labeled or described. Do the rows represent subgroup analyses? For example, does the row labeled “Heredity for asthma” provide an estimate for the subgroup of estimates from samples that were positive for asthma heredity? Or does this row provide an aggregate estimate among the subgroup of studies for which there was no significant difference between the LRTI+ and LRTI- groups on heredity for asthma? How was heredity for asthma defined?

Authors: Thank you for these comments. We have now improved the label of row entities. All confounding factors presented have been defined as presented in the included article. We have harmonized the names of the confounding factors collected in several articles according to their similarity. We have now clarified this in the section “data extraction”.

11. line 160: “We analyzed the data using the R software version 3.5.1.” It would be good to also provide the version of the meta package used to conduct the analyses.

Authors: Thank you for the suggestion the packages used are now specified.

12. line 181: “All included studies were at low risk of bias.” This statement seems too vague. In the supplement, the authors note that a score of 6-9 on the Newcastle-Ottawa scale was indicative of low bias. Were there any areas of concern across the studies? For example, did all of the studies have low risk of bias due to missingness? This seems unlikely.

Authors: We added S6 Table which presents the individual score allocations for the included studies and the total scores to rule out this ambiguity. Thank you.

13. In Table S9, the authors provide the “P-value subgroup difference” in the final column. But there I did not see any information about how this p-value was obtained. Did the authors run multiple meta-analytic models with these study characteristics entered as covariates to obtain the p-values? Was one covariate entered per model or were multiple covariates entered in the same model? Generally speaking, I think more information is needed about the modeling approach. Why did the authors include 95% prediction intervals in the table? How should the readers interpret this and why is it needed in addition to the 95% confidence interval?

Authors: Thank you for the feedback, we performed the subgroup analyses using random-effect meta-analysis with single-covariate analyses. Although the interpretation of the prediction interval can be difficult, this parameter provides a convenient format for expressing any uncertainty surrounding the effect size because it takes into account the magnitude and consistency of the effects. The prediction interval thus represents one of the most important results of a meta-analysis because the aim of the work is to put the acquired knowledge into future application. We have now added the relevant references in the manuscript and modify the text accordingly.

14. lines 212-214: “The type of LRTI was associated with the occurrence of wheezing (p= 0.007) (S9 Table). History of bronchiolitis (OR= 3.8, 95% CI= 3.4-4.7) and LRTI unspecified (OR= 1.9, 95% CI= 1.4-2.7) were associated with the occurrence of wheezing in childhood.” If I understand correctly, the type of LRTI accounted for significant heterogeneity in the effect estimates. This means that the strength of the estimated effect of LRTI on wheezing outcomes differed depending on the type of LRTI. But the authors don’t explain which comparison was driving this effect. They note that studies using both bronchiolitis and unspecified LRTI found significant weighted mean effect sizes. The third category (studies evaluating pneumonia as the exposure) did not yield a significant effect size, though this is likely due to the fact that there was only one pneumonia exposure study. The point estimate for the pneumonia study (OR=2.8) was larger than the point estimate and the upper bound of the 95% CI for the unspecified bronchiolitis studies. So, we shouldn’t make much of the fact that there was too much uncertainty in the pneumonia study to reject the null hypothesis. My guess is that the weighted mean OR for the bronchiolitis studies was significantly larger than the weighted mean OR for the unspecified studies and this is why LRTI type accounts for significant heterogeneity. The 95% Cis for these two study groups (bronchiolitis and unspecified) do not overlap at all. But the meaning of the significant effect of LRTI type does not come across in the authors’ description at all.

Authors: Thank you for these relevant comments. We added the following sentence in the discussion section:

“Although the type of LRTI was a significant source of heterogeneity during this work, we are unable to draw a definitive conclusion about this parameter because the pneumonia category was represented by a single study and the category LRTI not specified is very heterogeneous to allow an objective interpretation.”

15. lines 216-218: “The type of LRTI was associated with the occurrence of wheezing (p= 0.007) (S9 Table). History of bronchiolitis (OR= 3.8, 95% CI= 3.4-4.7) and LRTI unspecified (OR= 1.9, 95% CI= 1.4-2.7) were associated with the occurrence of wheezing in childhood.” The authors present what appears to be an analysis of whether “Age range at recruitment” accounts for significant between-study heterogeneity in effect estimates. Again, however, the authors follow up a significant effect of this covariate with an analysis of which subgroups (levels of the covariate) yielded significant weighted mean ORs. There is no focus on which levels of the covariate differ from each other (e.g., Is there a significant difference in the mean OR for the studies beginning recruitment before 6 months compared to studies beginning recruitment before 1 year of age?), making the effect of the covariate hard to interpret for readers. Furthermore, the authors created a categorical “Age at recruitment” variable with four levels. However, two cells of this variable are highly sparse, containing one and two studies. I don’t think the authors have enough data to support a four-level factor for this analysis.

Authors: We totally agree with the Reviewer, thanks for the comment. We combined categories <2 and <3, consisting of only 3 studies, and redone the analyses. We have modified the result section accordingly. Thank you.

16. lines 218-219: “We did not record any difference in the occurrence of wheezing by type of virus screened (p= 0.249).” I don’t think that this analysis was very meaningful as 20 of the 22 studies evaluated HRSV. There is simply not enough information to conduct a meaningful analysis.

Authors: We initially pointed out this major limitation of our work in the discussion (see sentence below). We have also withdrawn the conclusions of our work drawn from this subgroup analysis and defined the corresponding research implications in the discussion section. Thank you.

“The main limitation of this systematic review is the small number of studies in some categories of our subgroup analyses. These categories included non-bronchiolitis LRTI and non-HRSV LRTI. The results of this subgroup analyses should therefore be considered with caution.”

17. lines 221-222: “No difference was recorded in the occurrence of wheezing depending on the sampling approach (non-probabilistic vs probabilistic, p= 0.505)” Again, I don’t think that this was a meaningful analysis as there was only one study that used probabilistic sampling.

Authors: We initially pointed out this major limitation of our work in the discussion (see sentence below). We have also withdrawn the conclusions of our work drawn from this subgroup analysis and defined the corresponding research implications in the discussion section. Thank you.

“The main limitation of this systematic review is the small number of studies in some categories of our subgroup analyses. These categories included non-bronchiolitis LRTI and non-HRSV LRTI. The results of this subgroup analyses should therefore be considered with caution.”

18. lines 228-229: “Children who experienced an episode of HMPV or HRSV bronchiolitis at ≤ 3 years of age were 3 times more likely than controls to develop wheezing later.” It’s unclear to me why the main conclusion is limited to HMPV and HRSV bronchiolitis rather than LRTI more broadly. The main hypothesis was about LRTI more generally. Why are we drawing conclusions about specific subgroups rather than LRTI altogether?

Authors: Subgroup analyses do obviously present a major weakness due to the low number of studies in most categories. We thus consider the global null hypothesis throughout the manuscript. Thank you.

19. lines 229-232: “This increased risk of wheezing in children with a history of HMPV or HRSV bronchiolitis was not influenced by the rank of bronchiolitis episode and any of the investigated confounding factors.” It’s not clear to me what “rank of bronchiolitis episode” means. Additionally, I don’t think that the authors truly tested whether confounding factors influenced the estimates. As noted in an earlier comment, I don’t think the procedure for determining “symmetrically” and “asymmetrically” distributed confounders was compelling. It would have been far more interesting to evaluate whether estimates based on adjustment for specific covariates believed to represent major confounders (e.g., genetic confounding) were smaller than estimates that did not adjust for these factors.

Authors: We reviewed our initial misleading consideration of confounding factors throughout the manuscript. Rather, our sensitivity analysis takes into account studies that are comparable according to the confounding factors collected. In our work the effects are calculated from primary numbers collected in the included studies. We are therefore unable to collect directly adjusted versus unadjusted effects. Thank you.

20. lines 248-249: “Asthma, considered by most authors as ≥ 3 episodes of wheezing, were however not associated with pneumonia in the meta-analysis.” This comment must be put in context. According to Table S9, there was only one pneumonia study with 40 participants. This meta-analysis does not provide us any new information beyond what the one pneumonia study has already provided because it simply reports the results of this one trial – there is not combining of information across pneumonia studies.

Authors: We totally agree with the Reviewer. This sentence was the description of a result of the review cited in the previous sentence “A previous meta-analysis showed that childhood pneumonia mainly linked to adenoviruses was associated with respiratory sequelae in hospitalized and non-hospitalized children, including restrictive pulmonary disease, obstructive pulmonary disease, bronchiectasis and chronic bronchitis [72]”. We repeated the citation to rule out the confusion. Thank you.

21. lines 255-257: “The results of this systematic review, also, indicate that apart of HRSV, HMPV bronchiolitis was associated with a risk of subsequent wheezing.” Again, according to Table S9, there was only one HMPV study. There was no need for meta-analytic aggregation because there was only a single estimate, from what I can tell. In which case, this review did not provide any new information about the association between HMPV and subsequent wheezing above what was already reported from the single study that evaluated the association between HMPV LRTI and wheezing illness. There is no unique contribution of the meta-analysis in relation to the estimated effect of HMPV LRTI on subsequent wheezing illness.

Authors: Subgroup analyses do obviously present a major weakness due to the low number of studies in most categories. We thus consider the global null hypothesis throughout the manuscript. Thank you.

22. line 261: “In this systematic review, studies with confounding factor similarly distributed” This is an important point, so I will make it again. The fact that there was no statistically significant imbalance across the LRTI+ and LRTI- groups does not necessarily mean that the confounding variable was similarly distributed across the two groups.

Authors: We fully agree with the Reviewer that a symmetrical allocation of the confounding factor between cases and controls does not guarantee the exclusion of its influence on the effect of exposure on outcome. However, it is known that comparable samples on the majority of confounding factors give a result with a higher level of proof than those which are not. In this regard, the randomized trials which are known to have comparable sample according to most of the confounding factors. Studies paired for certain sociodemographic confounding factors such as sex and age are also accepted as comparable for these aspects. However, the examination of the comparability of the remaining confounders is most often done on the basis of the statistical significance of the tests performed, as we have done in the present work. Other best study designs also include adjustment of the estimated effect on major confounders. However, we calculate the effects in this work using primary data from the included studies. We do not collect the estimated effects in the study. Thank you.

Decision Letter 1

Bernadette van den Hoogen

19 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-36324R1

Association between early viral LRTI and subsequent wheezing development, a meta-analysis and sensitivity analyses for studies comparable for confounding factors.

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Njouom,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by May 03 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bernadette van den Hoogen

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: I thank the editors for the opportunity to review this manuscript.

This manuscript is clearly written and have adequately addressed the comments from the previous reviewers. I agree with the points made by reviewer 2 and have additional minor comments stated below.

1) Please explain rationale on exclusion of high risk population in the paper

2) I am not sure why proportions of confounders similar in both cases and controls were used in sensitivity analysis. Is this to ensure that the effects seen is not due to confounding? But I will assume that the studies have corrected for confounders in their analysis?

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #3: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Apr 15;16(4):e0249831. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249831.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


21 Mar 2021

Review Comments to the Author

Reviewer #3: I thank the editors for the opportunity to review this manuscript.

This manuscript is clearly written and have adequately addressed the comments from the previous reviewers. I agree with the points made by reviewer 2 and have additional minor comments stated below.

Authors: We thank Reviewer 1 for these favorable comments.

1) Please explain rationale on exclusion of high risk population in the paper

Authors: Studies often recruit high-risk cases (eg preterm) and not for the controls (eg apparently healthy individuals). This type of study is therefore likely to lead to a confounding bias in attributing the observed effect to exposure and yet it is rather the comorbidity in high-risk subjects that is linked to the effect. We have now clarified in the manuscript that we have excluded studies with high-risk individuals to reduce the potentially confounding effect associated with these studies. Thank you

2) I am not sure why proportions of confounders similar in both cases and controls were used in sensitivity analysis. Is this to ensure that the effects seen is not due to confounding? But I will assume that the studies have corrected for confounders in their analysis?

Authors: We claim that studies with similar proportions of confounding factors in both cases and controls are associated with a higher level of proof for the association between exposure (LRTI) and outcome (wheezing). Thank you.

Decision Letter 2

Bernadette van den Hoogen

26 Mar 2021

Association between early viral LRTI and subsequent wheezing development, a meta-analysis and sensitivity analyses for studies comparable for confounding factors.

PONE-D-20-36324R2

Dear Dr. Njouom,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Bernadette van den Hoogen

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Bernadette van den Hoogen

30 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-36324R2

Association between early viral LRTI and subsequent wheezing development, a meta-analysis and sensitivity analyses for studies comparable for confounding factors.

Dear Dr. Njouom:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Bernadette van den Hoogen

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Funnel plot for publication for wheezing in children with and without LRTI in infancy.

    (PDF)

    S1 Table. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

    (PDF)

    S2 Table. Search strategy in Medline (PubMed).

    (PDF)

    S3 Table. Items for risk of bias assessment.

    (PDF)

    S4 Table. Main reasons of exclusion of eligible studies.

    (PDF)

    S5 Table. Baseline characteristics of studies meeting inclusion criteria.

    (PDF)

    S6 Table. Risk of bias assessment.

    (PDF)

    S7 Table. Individual characteristics of included studies.

    (PDF)

    S8 Table. P-value of Khi-2 and Fisher exact tests for qualitative confounding factors.

    (PDF)

    S9 Table. P-value of student test for quantitative confounding factors.

    (PDF)

    S10 Table. Subgroup analyses of wheezing in children with LRTI in infancy and control without respiratory diseases.

    (PDF)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES