Skip to main content
GeroScience logoLink to GeroScience
. 2020 Oct 19;43(1):279–295. doi: 10.1007/s11357-020-00271-4

“Cerebral small vessel disease and other influential factors of cognitive impairment in the middle-aged: a long-term observational cohort PURE-MIND study in Poland”

Dorota Szcześniak 1, Joanna Rymaszewska 1, Anna Zimny 2,, Marek Sąsiadek 2, Katarzyna Połtyn-Zaradna 3, Eric E Smith 4, Katarzyna Zatońska 3, Tomasz Zatoński 5, Sumathy Rangarajan 6, Salim Yusuf 6,7, Andrzej Szuba 8
PMCID: PMC8050144  PMID: 33074422

Abstract

A complex picture of factors influencing cognition is necessary to be drawn for a better understanding of the role of potentially modifiable factors in dementia. The aim was to assess the prevalence and determinants of cognitive impairment, including the role of cerebral small vessel disease (CSVD) in Polish middle-aged cohort. A comprehensive set of clinical (hypertension, coronary heart disease, diabetes mellitus, hyperlipidaemia, body mass index, smoking status, alcohol intake) and socio-demographic data was collected in the PURE study in years 2007–2016, which was the basis for detailed analysis of risk factors of cognitive impairments in years 2016–2018 in the PURE-MIND sub-study. Five hundred forty-seven subjects (age range 39–65, mean 56.2 ± 6.5) underwent neuropsychological assessment with Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), Trail Making Test (TMT) and Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) followed by brain MRI. Mean MoCA score was 26.29 and 33% participants met criteria for mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (MoCA< 26). Seventy-three percent showed findings related to CSVD. Higher WMH burden and lacunar infarcts were associated with lower MoCA and DSST scores. Severe CSVD was associated with twofold incidence of MCI, and obesity increased its probability by 53% and hypertension by 37%. The likelihood of MCI was reduced in nonsmokers. One factor analysis showed the important role of lower level of education, older age, rural area of residence and hypertension. MCI and CSVD are highly prevalent in the middle-aged population in Poland. A greater importance should be given to potentially modifiable risk factors of dementia which are already present in mid-life.

Keywords: Cognitive impairment, Influential factors, Cerebral small vessel disease, Middle-aged population

Background

Most studies of age-related cognitive changes have focused on neurodegenerative diseases in the elderly. The rationale for this approach is that age is a significant risk factor for cognitive impairment [1]. The most common cause of cognitive deterioration in late life is progressive, irreversible neuronal damage associated with two dominant diseases, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and dementia due to cerebral small vessel disease (CSVD), i.e. vascular dementia (VaD). For many decades, these have been considered separate diseases with different causes. However, recent findings indicate that markers of CSVD are common in individuals diagnosed with AD [2], and, after accounting for non-independence between risk factors, about one-third of AD cases worldwide might be attributed to potentially modifiable factors [3]. Some of the potentially modifiable factors may depend on lifestyle and clinical status of an individual. Hypertension, diabetes mellitus and smoking have all been reported to be associated with covert infarcts and white matter lesions [4]. However, the effects of diabetes, obesity, diet, alcohol intake and stress have not been well established in population-based studies [5]. Nevertheless, the Alzheimer Association pointed out that good management of cardiovascular risk factors might be associated with reduced risk of dementia [6].

Therefore, it seems that regardless of the clinical classification, cerebrovascular contribution to neurodegenerative diseases will be relevant to the majority of the population living with dementia. So far, no efficient therapeutic solutions have been developed. Moreover, as with other pathological mechanisms in dementia, vascular risk factors can cause brain atrophy years before the onset of symptoms and before the diagnosis [7]. Brain lesions from CSVD may accumulate subclinically over decades; and by the time, they are recognized clinically it may be too late for effective intervention [8]. It is difficult to know how early these lesions may start to affect cognition, due to the scarcity of research into middle-aged cognitive impairment, and little is known about the prevalence of CSVD in people under 65 years of age [9, 10].

Given the above, there seems to be a complex relationship between the vascular risk factors and dementia throughout life. While large epidemiological studies have shown that traditional vascular risk factors are very important determinants of dementia, including AD, the magnitude of their importance appears to be greater in mid-life compared with late life [1113]. Hence, a detailed risk analysis of people under 65 years of age may identify why this life period is critical for establishing risk for dementia in later life.

Identifying risks in middle-aged people may enable the development of specific guidelines for the prevention of cognitive impairment, facilitating timely intervention in risk groups. As Norton et al. previously indicated, AD incidence might be limited through better access to education and the use of effective strategies to reduce the prevalence of vascular risk factors [3].

To obtain a better picture of risk factors and their influence on cognition in people under 65 years of age, it is necessary to analyse the early symptoms of the emerging neurodegenerative processes. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is defined as objective evidence of low cognitive performance in the absence of dementia and is a transition period through which persons pass when they develop dementia [14]. Comprehensive neuropsychological testing can be used to diagnose MCI, but it is not practical for widespread use in primary care. On the other hand, the Folstein Mini Mental Status Exam (MMSE) though practical and widely used is not sensitive for cognitive deficits in MCI and is heavily weighted towards memory and orientation and relatively less sensitive to executive dysfunctions which are prominent in cognitive impairment resulting from covert cerebral ischemia [15]. Thus, in this study, we used other tools of neuropsychological assessment such as Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) together with the Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) which are more sensitive to executive dysfunction, prominent in cognitive impairment related to CSVD [16, 17].

The aim of the study was to evaluate the prevalence of a wide range of socio-demographic, clinical and health-related factors, as well as CSVD in the middle-aged population and their early impact on cognition assessed using sensitive psychometric tools. In more details, the study aimed to (i) explore prevalent conditions that may contribute to increased risk of MCI during midlife, (ii) explore the contribution of cerebrovascular disease to midlife cognitive impairments and (iii) evaluate the sensitivity of different neuropsychological assessments to cognitive functioning associated with imaging markers of CSVD. In order to unravel complex longitudinal relationships between early exposure to risk factors and clinical outcome, we focused on a cohort of the relatively young people under 65 years of age, which is a unique aspect of this study.

Methods

Population

The presented results are part of the National Science Centre grant (NCN nr 2015/17/B/NZ7–02963) called the PURE-MIND study nested within the Polish fraction of the larger multinational PURE study (Prospective Urban Rural Epidemiology Study). The presented PURE-MIND sub-study was a cohort study carried out in 2016–2018, which aimed to explore relationships between socio-demographic and health-related factors, including CSVD and cognition.

The Pure-MIND study participants were recruited from the PURE study cohort of 1269 people, all of whom were inhabitants of the Lower Silesia region in Poland. The age and place of residence structure of this cohort corresponds to the structure of the general population in Poland. However, the dominance of women is visible in the PURE study sample. A total number of 824 individuals met the eligibility criteria and completed a full study protocol related to a cognitive assessment and underwent magnetic resonance (MR) examination. The PURE-MIND study exclusion criteria were the following: contraindications to MRI (including mainly a pacemaker and other contraindicated body implants, as well as severe claustrophobia), history of stroke or dementia, history of other neurological diseases of the brain, the presence of significant psychiatric diseases, residence in a skilled nursing facility and inability to participate in cognitive assessments (e.g. due to aphasia). Due to large motion artefacts on MRI and missing data, the PURE-MIND study sample consisted of 810 subjects (509 women and 301 men; mean age: 60.9 years; range: 39–81) (response rate 64%). Moreover, an additional criterion of age under 65 years was used in order to focus on middle-aged participants in the current analysis. Therefore, the final study sample consisted of 547 people (195 men and 352 women) with a mean age of 56.2 ± 6.5 years (aged 39–65 years). The flowchart diagram shows separate steps of the recruitment of the final study population (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Flowchart of the PURE study participants (N = 1269) and the final study population (N = 547)

All participants received complete information about the study protocol, their anonymity and the possibility to resign at any time. All participants were volunteers and signed an informed consent prior to the study enrolment. The study protocol was approved by the local Bioethical Committee (permission no.: KB – 32/2016).

Demographic and health-related factor assessment

A comprehensive set of socio-demographic and health-related data was collected as a part of the PURE study and was the basis for a detailed analysis of risk factors of cognitive impairment (Fig. 1.) All participants were examined according to the global PURE study protocol [18] using detailed questionnaires, physical examination and laboratory tests (at baseline, 3-year and 6-year follow-ups). This was followed by neuropsychological assessment and MRI of the brain (at 9-year follow-up) conducted according to the PURE-MIND sub-study protocol.

The baseline socio-demographic and health-related data was collected between years 2007 and 2010 from urban and rural populations of the Lower Silesia region in Poland. The participants were re-contacted every 3 years. Medical information used for this analysis was further collected between years 2007 and 2016 (at baseline, 3-year and 6-year follow-ups) and comprised the presence of hypertension, diabetes mellitus, hyperlipidaemia, coronary heart disease, smoking status, alcohol intake and body mass index (BMI). Clinical characteristics were assigned based on their presence over time from baseline to 3-year or 6-year follow-ups. Longitudinal data collection was used to determine the presence of prevalent conditions (and not the length of their presence) and how those contributed to the cognitive outcome.

Blood pressure measurements were carried out using an automated oscillometric device (Omron Corporation, Tokyo, Japan). The patients were advised to sit quietly and rest for 5 min before the measurements. Appropriate cuff size was selected. Three measurements were taken at 5-min intervals. The average of the measurements was used to diagnose hypertension, according to the ESC criteria (systolic blood pressure 140 or above and/or diastolic blood pressure 90 mmHg or above). Hypertension was considered to be controlled if the objectives of the ESH/ESC Guidelines were met (blood pressure < 140/90 mmHg) [19].

The participants were assigned to the diabetes group when their fasting plasma glucose was ≥ 126 mg/dL (7.0 mmol/L) or when they had been diagnosed with diabetes in the past and had been treated since then. Hyperlipidaemia was diagnosed when the level of total cholesterol was > 190 mg/dL, the level of LDL-C was > 115 mg/dL, the level of HDL-C was <50 mg/dL in women and < 40 mg/dL in men and the triglyceride level was > 150 mg/dL [20].

Coronary heart disease was based on self-reporting of angina, myocardial infarction, coronary artery bypass graft surgery, or percutaneous coronary angioplasty (each category was not separately identified).

Moreover, the smoking status and alcohol intake were assessed.

The body mass of the patients was measured with the use of Tanita Ironman Body Composition Monitor Model BC-554 with accuracy of 0.1 kg. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated as weight (kg) divided by height (m) squared. Subjects were classified into four BMI categories according to the WHO guidelines as being underweight (BMI < 18.5 kg/m2), normal weight (BMI 18.5–24.9 kg/m2), overweight (BMI 25.0–29.9 kg/m2) and obese (BMI ≥ 30.0 kg/m2).

Cognitive assessment

Cognitive evaluation was designed to efficiently assess multiple domains and to minimize the time and participants’ fatigue. It was based on the harmonization standards of the Canadian Stroke Network and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke used for the assessment of vascular cognitive impairment recommended in the consensus. The following standardized psychometric tools were used:

The MoCA (The Montreal Cognitive Assessment) scale in Polish adaptation was used to detect cognitive impairment. The published cut-off score (< 26 points) identifies MCI or dementia with high sensitivity and specificity [16]. For univariate analyses, a 1-point adjustment for subjects with less than 12 years of education was used. The MoCA assesses various cognitive domains: attention and concentration, executive functions, memory, language, visuo-constructional skills, conceptual thinking, calculations and orientation. Time to perform the test is about 10 min. The total possible score is 30 points with 26 points or above considered as cognitive health. In present statistical analyses, MoCA was a psychometric tool for measuring general cognitive functioning as a quantitative variable. Moreover, the score below 26 points was defined as mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and was used in the risk factor analysis as qualitative variable. Scores between 26 and 30 points were defined as the cognitive norm.

The DSST (Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edition) a 2-min test sensitive to covert cerebral ischemia that requires matching a symbol with a number according to the code [17]. The score is equal to the sum of correct symbols drawn in a limited time.

TMT (Trail Making Test) Parts A and B were used to measure cognitive flexibility and central executive functioning. TMT-A version requires combining numbers from 1 to 25, while TMT-B version requires combining numbers and letters alternately [21]. The result is execution time for both versions, number of hints and number of errors in order of digits. Higher scores reveal greater impairment.

The psychological examination was standardized in terms of the procedure and conditions to minimize the dependence of the test scores on the influence of external factors. Each study participant was assessed during an individual contact with a trained psychologist, in a quiet room, while maintaining silent conditions. The instructions were read in the same way each time. The examination was scheduled at a time convenient for the participants.

Functional assessment

The following tools were used to assess the overall functioning:

CES-D (The Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression) which is a scale in Polish adaptation to assess depression symptoms in epidemiological studies [22]. Higher scores reveal greater likelihood of depressive symptoms.

SAGE (The Standard Assessment of Global Activities in the Elderly) which is a screening tool to recognize and detect early loss of independence [23]. Higher scores reveal greater impairment.

Brain MRI acquisition and analysis

All brain MR examinations were performed using the same 1.5 Tesla MR scanner (GE, Signa Hdx). The MRI sequences included axial dual echo T2/PD-weighted images (TR = 2720 ms, TE = 88/8.8 ms, ET [Echo train length] = 12, FOV = 240 × 240 mm, slice thickness = 3.5 mm, matrix = 256 × 256, NEX = 1), axial fluid attenuated inversion recovery sequences (FLAIR) (TR = 8.800 ms, TE = 145 ms, TI = 2.200 ms, FOV = 240 × 240 mm, slice thickness = 3.5 mm, matrix = 256 × 256, NEX = 1), diffusion-weighted imaging (DWI) (SE/EPI, TR = 10,000 ms, TE = 107, FOV = 240 × 240 mm, slice thickness = 3.5 mm, matrix 256 × 256), susceptibility-weighted imaging (SWI) 3D (TR = 73.9 ms, TE = 47.4 ms, FOV = 240 × 168 mm, slice thickness = 3.5 mm, matrix 256 × 256, NEX = 0.7, flip angle = 20) and high-resolution T1-weighted images (FSPGR, TR = 8.3 ms, TE = 3.2 ms, TI = 650 ms, FOV 240 × 240 mm, slice thickness = 1.0 mm, matrix 256 × 256 mm, NEX = 1, flip angle = 12).

All MR scans were evaluated in relation to the findings characteristic of CSVD, such as lacunar infarcts, white matter hyperintensity (WMH) and microbleeds, according to the standard published criteria [10]. Lacunar infarcts were described as small focal lesions up to 15 mm with the same characteristics as cerebrospinal fluid on all sequences located in the subcortical white matter or within deep grey matter structures. White matter hyperintensities were graded on the FLAIR images using the Fazekas scale of 0–3 [24], separately for periventricular and subcortical locations. Burden of WMH was established using cut-off value of 4 for a combined periventricular and subcortical Fazekas grade (low WMH burden was defined as grade 1–3, while high WMH as grade ≥ 4). Cerebral microbleeds were described as small foci of signal loss with blooming effect on SWI.

Severe CSVD was defined as the presence of high WMH burden or lacunar infarcts or microbleeds. Mild CSVD was defined as low WMH burden without lacunar infarcts and microbleeds.

To compare cognitive test scores across lesion types, we grouped participants as follows: healthy (no brain lesions), infarcts (all subjects with lacunar infarcts with or without WMH or microbleeds), high WMH (high WMH burden with or without microbleeds), low WMH (low WMH burden with or without microbleeds) and microbleeds alone.

All MR scans were rated by two independent radiologists trained for CSVD assessment using the same criteria. The inter-rater reliability was established with kappa coefficient that ranged between 0.71 and 0.88 depending on the category of the assessed parameters. Despite the good inter-rater reliability, all scans that showed any discrepancy between the two raters were re-evaluated and the final score was established by consensus.

Statistical analysis

Differences between the groups of quantitative variables (MRI-related groups and MoCA<26 defined as MCI) were assessed using the Kruskal-Wallis test with Dunn’s non-parametric all-pair comparison and Holm correction or Mann Whitney test when appropriate.

Pearson chi-square test was used to assess differences in the qualitative variables. Correlation between cognitive outcomes and age was calculated using Spearman rank correlation. The optimal cut-off point for age in relation to MoCA below 26 points and 1.5 standard deviation below the mean of DSST score commonly used as clinical thresholds for MCI was determined by highest Youden index in ROC analysis. The analysis of risk factors of cognitive impairment, measured with the MoCA scale (MoCA used as a qualitative variable: < 26 defined as MCI), was calculated by estimation of prevalence ratios using the Poisson regression with robust variance. Moreover, the comparison of the study population to general population was done by comparing proportion confidence intervals of sample proportion to Statistics Poland data. The significance level was set at p < 0.05. Analysis was performed using R for windows (version 3.6.1) [25].

Results

Study sample

The study sample (n = 547) characteristics are shown in Table 1. The majority were women (64%), living in urban areas (74%), with tertiary (41%) or secondary (41%) education. All subjects were characterized as independent after being assessed by SAGE scale (mean score 2.33 ± 2.9, median 1). The average level of depressive symptoms was low (mean score 9.66 ± 8.0, median 8), indicating general lack of depression among the study participants.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the study population

Characteristics Female Male p value
9 year follow-up Age

Mean in years (SD)*

Range

56.24 (6.5)*

[39–65]

56.9 (6.3)* 55.1 (6.8)* 0.003
Sex, N (%) Female 352 (64)
Male 195 (36)
Education level, N (%) Primary 27 (5) 16 (4.5) 11 (5.6) 0.4
Trade school 68 (12) 41 (11.6) 27 (13.8)
Secondary/High school 227 (42) 155 (44.1) 72 (36.9)
College/University 225 (41) 140 (39.8) 85 (43.7)
Living location, N (%) Urban 403 (74) 255 (72.4) 148 (75.9) 0.4
Rural 144 (26) 97 (27.6) 47 (24.1)
Cognitive functioning, N (%) MoCA < 26 178 (33) 117 (33.2) 61 (31.3) 0.7
MoCA mean score (SD)* 26.29 (2.5)* 26.21 (2.6)* 26.43 (2.5)* 0.3
Brain pathology in structural MRI, N (%) No brain pathology 150 (27) 79 (22.4) 71 (36.4) 0.002
Low WMH burden 352 (64) 241 (68.5) 111 (56.9)
High WMH burden 15 (3) 12 (3.7) 2 (1.1)
Lacunar infarcts 16 (3) 8 (2.3) 8 (4.1)
Microbleeds 14 (3) 11 (3.1) 3 (1.5)
Depressive symptoms CES-d mean score (SD)* 9.66 (8.0)* 10.60 (8.7)* 7.97 (6.3)* 0.001
General activity SAGE mean score (SD)* 2.33 (2.9)* 2.47 (3.1)* 2.08 (2.4)* 0.4
Baseline, 3 & 6 year follow-up Hypertension, N (%) No 334 (61) 126 (35.8) 87 (44.6) 0.05
Yes 213 (39) 226 (64.2) 108 (55.4)
Diabetes Mellitus, N (%) No 467 (86) 46 (13.1) 30 (15.4) 0.5
Yes 76 (14) 306 (86.9) 161 (82.6)
Hyperlipidaemia, N (%) No 206 (38) 119 (34.0) 87 (45.3) 0.01
Yes 336 (62) 231 (66.0) 105 (54.7)
Coronary heart disease, N (%) No 511 (93) 330 (93.8) 181 (92.8) 0.8
Yes 36 (7) 22 (6.2) 14 (7.2)
Smoking, N (%) Current 101 (18) 72 (20.5) 29 (14.9) 0.02
Former 185 (34) 105 (29.8) 80 (41.0)
Never 261 (48) 175 (49.7) 86 (44.1)
Weight category, N (%) Normal weight 177 (33) 122 (34.7) 55 (28.5) 0.3
Overweight 149 (27) 135 (38.4) 84 (43.5)
Obese 219 (40) 95 (26.9) 54 (28.0)
Alcohol intake, N (%) No 136 (25) 106 (30.1) 30 (15.4) 0.0002
Yes 411 (75) 246 (69.9) 165 (84.6)

*data expressed in means and standard deviations (SD); Pearson chi-square test was used to assess gender differences in the qualitative variables and Mann-Whitney test to assess gender differences in the quantitative variables; Normal weight (BMI < 25), overweight (BMI 25.0–29.9), obese (BMI ≥30)

The total mean MoCA score was 26.29 ± 2.5 (median 27), and 33% (n = 178) of the study participants scored below the recommended cut-off for MCI or dementia (< 26 points). Only 12% (n = 67) of the subjects confirmed subjective cognitive impairment (SAGE scale).

In 27% (n = 150) of the participants, no brain pathology was detected on MRI, while in 73% (n = 397) of the subjects findings related to CSVD were revealed. Mild CSVD characterized by low WMH burden was found in 64%, while severe CSVD was detected in 9% of the participants, including high WMH burden (3%), lacunar infarcts (3%) and microbleeds (3%).

In addition, Table 1 shows the characteristics of the study group in terms of other health-related factors such as hypertension, diabetes mellitus, hyperlipidaemia, coronary heart disease, weight, smoking and alcohol intake. Moreover, gender differences are pointed out in terms of all characteristics. Detailed data are given in cases of significant differences between sexes.

Cerebral small vessel disease and cognitive functioning

Higher WMH burden was associated with lower MoCA score (rho = −0.17, p < 0.00009) and lower DSST score (rho = −0.19, p < 0.00001). Moreover, participants with high WMH burden needed more time to perform TMT part A (rho = 0.17, p < 0.00008) and part B (rho = 0.21, p < 0.00001).

Further analysis showed that participants categorized within 5 groups (healthy, microbleeds, low WMH, high WMH, infarcts) did not show any significant differences in the memory domain based on delayed recall in the MoCA scale (healthy: 3.1 ± 1.6, median: 4; microbleeds: 3.0 ± 1.2, median: 3; low WMH burden: 2.9 ± 1.5, median 3; high WMH burden: 2.5 ± 1.6, median 3; lacunar infarcts: 2.7 ± 1.7, median 3; p < 0.2). However, significant differences were observed in the total MoCA score (p < 0.004), DSST (p < 0.0009) and TMT parts A (p < 0.004) and B (p < 0.0001) performance (Fig. 2), and the subjects with high WMH or lacunar infarcts scored lower in all these tests. However, significant differences between healthy participants and those with high WMH were observed only in DSST (p = 0.02) and TMT B (p = 0.0124). In turn, for lacunar infarcts, TMT was the most sensitive psychometric test (part A: p = 0.035 and part B: p = 0.0453). Moreover, a statistically significant difference between healthy group and a group with a low WMH burden was already shown in the general cognitive functioning outcome (MoCA: p = 0.016) as well as in the TMT B (p = 0.0079) which stresses central executive processes of task-set inhibition and cognitive flexibility. Participants with microbleeds did not differ significantly from the healthy group.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

The differences between healthy participants and those with different lesion types in selected cognitive outcomes: (a) MoCA, (b) DSST, (c) TMT A and (d) TMT B. Data from participants grouped by lesion types: healthy (no lesions, n = 150), infarcts (with or without WMH or microbleeds, n = 16), high WMH (with or without microbleeds, n = 15), low WMH (with or without microbleeds, n = 352) and microbleeds alone (n = 14) are shown as mean and standard deviation (sd). The tables contain information about the median, q1 (quartile 1) and q2 (quartile 2). The charts show statistically significant (p < 0.05) differences in cognitive outcomes between healthy participants and the subjects with different cerebral lesions assessed by Kruskal-Wallis test with Dunn’s non-parametric all-pairs comparison and Holm correction

In addition, 21% (n = 37) of participants meeting the criteria for MCI (< 26 points in MoCA) had normal brain MRI without any CSVD lesions, while 10 participants with high WMH burden had scores corresponding to the cognitive norm in the neuropsychological tests (Fig. 3).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

The relationship between white matter hyperintensities (WMH) burden defined as healthy (0), low WMH burden (1–3), high WMH burden (≥ 4) and cognitive performance categorized based on the MoCA score as cognitive health (MoCA ≥ 26, n = 369) and cognitive impairment (MoCA < 26, n = 178)

Sociodemographic as well as health-related factors and cognitive functioning

Table 2 illustrates demographic and health-related variables in the context of cognitive functioning in the middle-aged population. The overall cognitive performance, measured with MoCA (rho = −0.29, p < 0.000001), and the ability to learn on new visual material (DSST: rho = −0.42, p < 0.000001) decreased with age. Moreover, with age, participants needed more time to complete the TMT A (rho = 0.42, p < 0.000001) and TMT B (rho = 0.41, p < 0.000001). Figure 4 shows the decline in cognitive function divided by age groups together with the illustration of the ROC curve, which determines the optimal cut-off point in years when this decrease appears to be the most significant. For both MoCA and DSST scales, the relevant cut-off points for age were 58 and 59 years, respectively. There was no significant difference between sexes in terms of general cognitive performance (MoCA: p < 0.3). However, speed processing, working memory, visuospatial processing and attention, as well as the ability to learn on new material were higher in women (DSST: p < 0.004). However, in terms of executive functions such as processes of task-set inhibition and cognitive flexibility, men showed better scores (TMT A: p = 0.02). Moreover, there was a clear effect of education—higher or secondary education level was associated with higher cognitive functioning (MoCA, DSST, TMT A and TMT B, p < 0.000001). However, there was no significant difference between people with education at primary and vocational level (p < 0.5). Urban residents scored higher on all cognitive tests compared with people living in rural areas.

Table 2.

Association between sociodemographic, health-related factors and cognitive functions

Characteristics MoCA Digit Symbol Substitution TMT A TMT B
rho p value rho p value rho p value rho p value
Age −0.29 <0.000001 −0.42 <0.000001 0.42 <0.000001 0.41 <0.000001
Total mean score (SD) p value Total mean score (SD) p value Total mean score (SD) p value Total mean score (SD) p value
Sex Female 26.21 (2.6) 0.3 65.90 (14.0) <0.004 37.18 (11.3) 0.06 82.89 (33.9) 0.02
Male 26.43 (2.5) 62.22 (14.9) 35.70 (12.2) 76.91 (31.2)
Education level Primary 24.12 (2.7) <0.000001 51.85 (13.5) <0.000001 44.81 (17.6) <0.000001 112.70 (44.2) <0.000001
Trade school 24.69 (2.7) 52.91 (12.7) 42.25 (13.4) 95.35 (39.5)
High school 25.91 (2.5) 62.56 (12.6) 37.47 (10.2) 84.58 (32.1)
College/University 27.41 (1.9) 71.70 (12.9) 33.14 (10.2) 68.60 (24.0)
Living location Urban 26.63 (2.3) <0.00001 67.12 (14.2) <0.00001 35.63 (11.2) 0.0004 78.15 (31.2) 0.0009
Rural 25.20 (2.7) 57.51 (12.7) 39.49 (12.3) 88.02 (36.8)
Hypertension Yes 25.69 (2.7) <0.00005 59.54 (13.0) <0.00001 39.97 (12.4) <0.000001 89.63 (34.9) <0.000001
No 26.67 (2.4) 67.81 (14.4) 34.54 (10.6) 75.12 (30.6)
Diabetes Mellitus Yes 25.75 (2.7) <0.05 57.50 (14.0) <0.000003 41.47 (13.4) 0.0007 93.68 (39.3) 0.00005
No 26.39 (2.5) 65.75 (14.3) 35.96 (11.1) 78.79 (31.6)
Hyperlipidaemia Yes 26.42 (2.4) 0.3 65.04 (13.8) 0.3 36.44 (11.3) 0.8 80.11 (31.4) 0.8
No 26.11 (2.7) 63.77 (15.7) 37.11 (12.1) 82.09 (35.9)
Coronary heart disease Yes 25.92 (2.0) 0.2 58.31 (15.2) 0.02 39.17 (13.5) 0.4 86.22 (31.6) 0.2
No 26.32 (2.6) 65.03 (14.3) 36.47 (11.4) 80.37 (33.2)
Smoking Current 25.94 (2.6) 0.1 60.66 (12.4) 0.0002 38.72 (12.8) 0.2 90.79 (43.4) 0.01
Former 26.19 (2.5) 63.26 (14.2) 35.94 (19.0) 81.57 (28.7)
Never 26.49 (2.5) 67.05 (14.9) 36.36 (12.1) 76.33 (30.6)
Weight category Normal weight 26.71 (2.4) 0.007 70.11 (13.3) <0.000001 34.74 (10.3) 0.03 74.57 (29.1) 0.0004
Overweight 26.24 (2.5) 63.31 (14.4) 37.22 (12.3) 82.07 (35.1)
Obese 25.88 (2.6) 59.85 (13.9) 38.23 (11.8) 86.33 (33.7)
Alcohol intake Yes 26.42 (2.5) 0.03 65.29 (14.0) 0.06 35.99 (11.1) 0.07 79.32 (31.8) 0.1
No 25.88 (2.7) 62.47 (15.5) 38.64 (12.9) 85.08 (36.4)

Kruskal-Wallis rank sum test or Mann-Whitney test were user to assess differences in the quantitative variables; Correlation between cognitive outcomes and age was calculated using Spearman rank correlation; Normal weight (BMI < 25), overweight (BMI 25.0–29.9), obese (BMI ≥ 30)

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

The decline in cognitive functioning with age and receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves for MoCA and DSST differentiating between cognitive health and cognitive impairment

Among health-related variables, hyperlipidaemia was not significantly associated with cognitive functioning (MoCA and DSST, p < 0.3; TMT A and B, p = 0.8). Other risk factors showed differences in the influence on cognition depending on the measured domains. MoCA as well as TMT A results showed that coronary heart disease and smoking were not related to the global cognitive functioning. However, the DSST task (more related to learning processes) was related to these factors (p < 0.02 and p < 0.0002, respectively). The results indicated that the participants who had never smoked showed significantly higher scores compared with the current (p < 0.0009) and former (p < 0.005) smokers. Participants who indicated abstinence from alcohol had lower scores on all tests, but this was only significant for the MoCA (p < 0.03). Hypertension, diabetes and obesity were associated with lower scores on all tests.

Mild cognitive impairment risk factors

Analysing variables independently of each other (Tab. 3) indicated that risk factors for decreased cognitive functioning which met the MCI criteria assessed by MoCA (< 26) were older age (p < 0.000001), rural area of residence (p < 0.000003) and hypertension (p < 0.0002). Participants with such characteristics significantly more often scored below 26 points in MoCA. On the other hand, significant protective factors were higher education level (p < 0.000001), normal body weight (p < 0.003) and alcohol intake (p < 0.05).

Table 3.

Association between sociodemographic, health-related factors and mild cognitive impairment versus cognitive health

Characteristics MoCA <26 MoCA >26 p value
Age Mean in years (SD) 58.77 (5.2) 55.02 (6.7) <0.000001
N (%)
Sex Female 117 (33.2) 235 (66.8) 0.7
Male 61 (31.3) 134 (68.7)
Education level Primary 18 (66.7) 9 (33.3) <0.000001
Trade school 42 (61.8) 26 (38.2)
High school 88 (38.8) 139 (61.2)
College/University 30 (13.3) 195 (86.7)
Living location Urban 108 (26.8) 295 (73.2) 0.000003
Rural 70 (48.6) 74 (51.4)
Hypertension Yes 90 (42.3) 123 (57.7) 0.0002
No 88 (26.3) 246 (73.7)
Diabetes Mellitus Yes 27 (35.5) 49 (64.5) 0.6
No 148 (31.7) 319 (68.3)
Hyperlipidaemia Yes 103 (30.7) 233 (69.3) 0.3
No 72 (34.9) 134 (65.1)
Coronary heart disease Yes 14 (38.9) 22 (61.1) 0.5
No 164 (31.9) 347 (68.1)
Smoking Current 40 (39.6) 61 (60.4) 0.07
Former 65 (35.1) 120 (64.9)
Never 73 (27.9) 188 (72.1)
Weight category Normal weight 41 (23.2) 136 (76.8) 0.003
Overweight 75 (34.2) 144 (65.8)
Obese 60 (40.3) 89 (59.7)
Alcohol intake Yes 124 (30.2) 287 (69.8) 0.05
No 54 (60.3) 82 (39.7)

Wilcoxon rank sum test was used to assess differences in age; Pearson chi-square test was used to assess differences in the qualitative variables; Normal weight (BMI < 25), overweight (BMI 25.0–29.9), obese (BMI ≥ 30)

Robust Poisson regression model was used for multivariable analysis (Fig. 5). Severe CSVD was associated with twofold higher incidence of MCI (PR 2.01; 95% CI: 1.31–3.09) and mild CSVD was associated with increased prevalence of MCI by 37% (RR 1.37; 95% CI: 0.99–1.89). Overweight was associated with higher rates of cognitive impairments (PR 1.37; 95% CI: 0.99–1.88), and obesity increased the probability of cognitive impairment by 53% (PR 1.53; 95% CI: 1.09–2.15). Among the participants who had never smoked, the likelihood of MCI was reduced by 31% (PR 0.69; 95% CI: 0.51–0.94). Participants who reported consuming alcohol were less likely to have cognitive deficits (PR 0.76; 95% CI: 0.59–0.98). Furthermore, hypertension was associated with the increased risk of cognitive impairment (PR 1.30; 95% CI: 1.01–1.69). A marginal but significant association was found with depressive symptoms (PR 1.02; 95% CI: 1.00–1.03), while no relationship was found with hyperlipidaemia, diabetes and coronary heart disease.

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5

Prevalence ratio (PR) of cognitive impairment (MoCA< 26) with regard to selected risk factors calculated by the Poisson regression with robust variance

Discussion

In one third of the community-representative study, including participants aged between 39 and 65 years, MCI range cognitive impairment was detected during the neuropsychological examinations. Only 12% of the participants were classified as having subjective deficits, which means that more than half of the study representatives with objective cognitive impairment were unaware of early signs of cognitive decline. Recent studies of MCI showed a prevalence of 15–20% in people aged 65 years or older [26]. Very limited data are available regarding people below 65 years of age. Other population-based studies included either mainly older adults or did not report specific findings in younger participants. However, the prevalence of cognitive impairment in this study population should be approached with some caution, given that situational factors such as fear of the psychological examination could worsen the results in the cognitive tests, despite maintaining all needed standardized conditions.

Magnetic resonance studies showed that over 73% of the middle-aged participants had CSVD, of which 9% had severe CSVD, defined as high WMH burden, lacunar infarcts or microbleeds. These results are in line with the conclusions of Smith et al. [27] who emphasized that CSVD was detected in a significant proportion of people in their 40s and 50s and that the brain changes were not silent even in relatively young individuals. What is more, Han et al. [28] in a cohort of 1211 stroke-free participants (55.6 ± 9.3 years) also showed the prevalence of 14% for lacunes, 72% for periventricular WMH, 65% for subcortical WMH and 10% for cerebral microbleeds, all of which increased with age. In the current study, higher WMH burden was associated with greater deficits in cognitive functions, and severe CSVD was one of the major risk factors for MCI increasing its incidence by 200%. Learning ability assessed with DSST, speed processing, cognitive flexibility and executive functions measured with TMT A and B tests were also significantly related to high WMH burden and lacunar infarcts. Nevertheless, it should be pointed out that in the neuropsychological assessment, TMT B seems to be of particular importance. It stresses central executive processes of task-set inhibition and cognitive flexibility and showed sensitivity in distinguishing between healthy people and people with all selected lesion types, even in those with low WMH burden. Interestingly, Smith et al. did not find significant associations between WMH and MoCA in the Canadian cohort, which they considered to be the result of too low sensitivity of the MoCA scale to the presence of covert cerebrovascular disease in a population-based setting [27]. However, a recent larger Canadian study demonstrated that associations between WMH and MoCA were present with sufficient sample size, concordant with the findings of this study [29]. Detailed analysis showed that participants without brain pathology on MRI and people with the presence of CSVD differed in all tasks measuring executive functions and cognitive flexibility in the MoCA scale, except for delayed recall. Due to the above, the first signs of morphological brain pathology might be visible primarily in the non-memory cognitive domains. Importantly, this relationship is observed in relatively young people who are expected to have “normal” cognitive productivity. It should also be emphasized that 21% of participants with MCI according to the MoCA test showed no brain pathology in MRI, which may be explained by a non-vascular origin of the symptoms, probably due to a purely degenerative process. On the other hand, 10 subjects with high burden of WMH showed normal results in the psychological tests, which may indicate their good compensatory abilities or cognitive reserve making the early symptoms of cognitive impairment not detectable using the performed tests.

Despite the fact that current analysis included people younger than 65 years of age, overall cognitive performance and learning ability on new material, reflected by performance on the DSST, as well as the speed processing (TMT) was found to decrease with age. The age-related decline in cognitive performance is consistent with findings among the middle-aged London civil servants, as well as many other studies conducted among older population [30]. Moreover, findings from the present study imply the clinically important suggestion that a significant decrease in cognitive functioning is visible already at age of 58 years, while clinical experience shows that in daily practice, specialists begin to deal with cognitive health usually in people around the age of 65 years or older.

The ambiguous conclusions relate to the gender effect in terms of the cognitive outcomes and CSVD. Mild cognitive impairment as well as general cognitive functioning is not related to gender, which is supported by the results of the MoCA test. However, our findings highlight the differences in terms of selected cognitive domain performance between women and men in the study sample. The results show better learning capacity (DSST) in women and better cognitive flexibility (TMT B) in men. Interestingly, the findings show a lower incidence of CSVD in men that could be seen as unexpected result as typically men are more at risk of cardiovascular disease, also in the present sample. Contradictory conclusions are presented by authors of the study from the United Kingdom who found that men were more prone to CSVD [31]. Male sex was a significant and independently associated factor for total CSVD score in that study. However, it should be emphasized that in the present study, women more often than men were classified as having low WMH and high WMH burden, but in men more often than in women lacunar infarcts were detected. Nevertheless, a larger proportion of men compared with women were characterized by a lack of pathological changes in the brain. An important observation that may be related to the data obtained is the gender difference present in depressive symptoms highlighting significantly higher scores on the depression scale in women in the study sample. This observation seems to be of great importance having regard to the significant relationship between depression and WMHs and cerebral atrophy as concluded in the systematic review and meta-analysis conducted by Rensma et al. [32]. Thus, gender differences in CSVD can be understood as a derivative of gender differences in terms of depressive symptoms in this particular study population. However, further analysis is necessary.

In this study, education level was found to be one of the most fundamental protective factors, and participants with higher education performed better in the cognitive assessment. The area of residence also turned out to be significant. In our study, urban living was associated with the better performance in the neuropsychological tests, as confirmed by some studies [33], while contradicting some other reports [34]. There is a likely relationship between education and the area of residence. In Poland, there are still disparities in access to education depending on where you live. Living in the city offers many different forms of cultural and educational participation, which may increase cognitive reserve phenomenon. This indicates the importance of lifestyle factors affecting the cognitive functioning among middle-aged people. It is consistent with Norton’s statement that many dementia cases worldwide might be attributable to potentially modifiable factors [3]. In this study, we found several risk factors for cognitive impairment meeting the MCI criteria that might be modifiable. One of them is obesity, found in our study to increase the probability of cognitive impairment by about 53%, which confirms recent findings on dementia among older adults [35]. Another important clinical risk factor in this study was hypertension, which disrupts the structure and function of cerebral blood vessels and may lead to ischemic damage of the white matter regions critical for cognitive function. Several reports emphasized the deleterious impact of mid-life hypertension on late-life cognitive decline [36, 37]. However, current findings have proved this harmful effect already in the middle-aged participants.

The obtained results confirmed the protective effect of not smoking. The likelihood of MCI was significantly reduced (by about 31%) in people who never smoked. This phenomenon may be related to the fact that these people generally lead a healthier lifestyle, a hypothesis that requires more in-depth examination. Our analysis found that alcohol drinkers were less likely to exhibit cognitive deficits compared with abstainers. This finding is consistent with the previously reported U-shaped relationship between regular alcohol consumption and cognitive function [38]. However, special care should be taken when formulating policy implications on this topic. Moreover, account should be taken of the fact that in this study assessment of alcohol consumption was carried out using interviews. The hard drinkers could deny drinking during interview, whereas people drinking occasionally could more openly confirm drinking alcohol. What is more, the reasons for not drinking alcohol have not been assessed. Which in the light of the results obtained may be of key importance assuming that non-drinkers make such a decision for health reasons and therefore remain in a much worse psychophysical health. In addition, hyperlipidaemia, diabetes and coronary heart disease were not associated with the occurrence of MCI. Nevertheless, there was a relationship found between diabetes and general cognitive performance. Participants who did not have diabetes scored significantly higher on the MoCA.

This study has several strengths. First, we focused on middle-aged participants, who are still an under-studied group. Second, this is the first large-scale study of this kind in Poland. Third, good quality data were obtained through the use of a research MRI with a strict protocol for the assessment of cerebrovascular pathology and a sensitive set of psychometric tools that were used to assess cognitive functioning. Another very important aspect was an access to longitudinal data on a wide range of lifestyle and clinical factors which are more powered than cross-sectional studies. Finally, due to the similarities in age structure and place of residence of the studied cohort at the baseline compared with the general population of Poland, these findings can be extrapolated to the Polish population, nevertheless with some caution. Discrepancies in the population structure such as women’s dominance in the PURE-MIND sub-study sample could result from the exclusion criteria, as well as the greater activity of female volunteers in a scientific research.

On the other hand, the biggest limitation of this study was the relatively small sample size as for a cohort study, since the participants were recruited from only one region in Poland. Moreover, the role of genetics was not analysed in the present study and the importance of genetic factors in the development of dementia and cerebrovascular pathology should not be overlooked. Heritability studies, including twin studies, show that most of the variance in a white matter hyperintensity volume is explained by genetic factors [39, 40]. Nevertheless, the role of the main genetic determinant of AD, apolipoprotein ɛ4 allele (APOE4), is still unclear in vascular cognitive deficits. A study from Singapore emphasized that APOE4 is significantly associated with dementia and cognitive deficits related to AD pathology, but not with vascular dementia [41]. According to Clark et al. [42], APOE genotype did not moderate relationship between risk factors and cerebral perfusion. Thus, despite the important role of genetic factors, the authors suggested that there is a great need to continue exploration of the relationship between potentially modifiable risk factors, cerebrovascular health and AD risk in underrepresented populations.

Conclusions

In this community-based study conducted on the Polish rural and urban population of people aged between 39 and 65 years, a relatively high prevalence of cognitive impairment was observed. The major clinical risk factors of MCI that already appeared in the middle-life were found to be severe CSVD, obesity and hypertension. Moreover, socio-demographic factors such as place of residence, age and education, as well as lifestyle factors, such as smoking also played an important role in cognitive impairment. In addition, it turned out that people with diabetes achieved significantly lower results in the neuropsychological tests, indicating the importance of this disease for cognitive performance, although it was not ultimately identified as a risk factor for MCI. This probably means that diabetes reduces global cognition, but in people up to 65 years of age, there are no clinically relevant consequences visible in cognitive impairment. Moreover, our findings indicate that among some study participants, the phenomenon of cognitive reserve is observed recognized by proper cognitive functioning despite serious morphological pathologies visible in MRI. Therefore, subsequent research should focus mainly on potential protective factors that improve compensatory possibilities and cognitive reserve of an individual person.

In the light of these findings emphasizing the important influence of lifestyle factors on cognition, improving access to higher education and applying effective strategies targeted at reducing the prevalence of vascular risk factors should become a major focus of health providers and policy makers. However, further research and data analyses are necessary to determine whether such preventive actions will really translate into a reduction in mid-life cognitive impairment and dementia at the later stages of life. Dissemination of knowledge that the first signs of cognitive impairment may be associated with domains other than memory, and information that cognitive impairment is visible before the age of 60 years, needs to be considered in everyday clinical practice.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Science Centre grant (NCN nr 2015/17/B/NZ7-02963). The authors have not entered into an agreement with the funding organizations that have limited their ability to complete the research as planned and publish the results. The authors have full control of the primary data as well as are willing to allow the journal to review their data if requested. The PURE-Poland project is a part of the global PURE study funded by the Population Health Research Institute, the Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Heart and Stroke Foundation of Ontario and through unrestricted grants from several pharmaceutical companies; the Poland sub-study was partially funded by: Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education grant no. 290/W-PURE/2008/0.

Authors’ contribution

D.S. contributed to the study design, conducted the psychometric examinations, analysed and interpreted data, conducted literature search, designed figures and wrote the manuscript; J.R. contributed to the study design, supervised psychometric data collection and critically reviewed the paper; A.Z. corresponding author, contributed to the study design, analysed and interpreted MRI data and contributed to writing the paper; M.S. contributed to the study design, supervised MRI data collection and critically reviewed the paper; K.P-Z. contributed to the study design and critically reviewed the paper; E.S. contributed to the study design and critically reviewed the paper; K.Z. contributed to the study design, supervised epidemiological data collection and critically reviewed the paper; T.Z. contributed to the study design and critically reviewed the paper; S.R. contributed to the study design and coordinated the PURE project; S.Y. contributed to the study design and critically reviewed the paper; A.Sz. contributed to the study design, critically reviewed the paper and supervised the research project.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Legdeur N, Heymans MW, Comijs HC, Huisman M, Maier AB, Visser PJ. Age dependency of risk factors for cognitive decline. BMC Geriatr. 2018;18:187. doi: 10.1186/s12877-018-0876-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Scott TM, Bhadelia RA, Qiao Qiu W, Folstein MF, Rosenberg IH. Small vessel cerebrovascular pathology identified by magnetic resonance imaging is prevalent in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: a potential target for intervention. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;65(1):293–302. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(8):788–794. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Das RR, Seshadri S, Beiser AS, Kelly-Hayes M, Au R, Himali JJ, Kase CS, Benjamin EJ, Polak JF, O'Donnell CJ, Yoshita M, D'Agostino RB, Sr, DeCarli C, Wolf PA. Prevalence and correlates of silent cerebral infarcts in the Framingham offspring study. Stroke. 2008;39(11):2929–2935. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.516575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Furie KL, Smith EE. Metabolic syndrome: a target for preventing leukoaraiosis and age-related dementia? Neurology. 2007;69:951–952. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000271094.43789.0e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Baumgart M, Snyder HM, Carrillo MC, Fazio S, Kim H, Johns H. Summary of the evidence on modifiable risk factors for cognitive decline and dementia: a population-based perspective. Alzheimers Dement. 2015;11(6):718–726. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jack CR, Knopman DS, Jagust WJ, et al. Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: an updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:207–216. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70291-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Knopman D, Boland LL, Mosley T, Howard G, Liao D, Szklo M, McGovern P, Folsom AR. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in middle-aged adults. Neurology. 2001;56(1):42–48. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.1.42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Pantoni L. Cerebral small vessel disease: from pathogenesis and clinical characteristics to therapeutic challenges. Lancet Neurol. 2010;9:689–701. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(10)70104-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Vermeer SE, Longstreth WT, Jr, Koudstaal PJ. Silent brain infarcts: a systematic review. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:611–619. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70170-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kivipelto M, Helkala EL, Hanninen T, et al. Midlife vascular risk factors and late-life mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study. Neurology. 2001;56:1683–1689. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.12.1683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kivipelto M, Ngandu T, Fratiglioni L, et al. Obesity and vascular risk factors at midlife and the risk of dementia and Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2005;62:1556–1560. doi: 10.1001/archneur.62.10.1556. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Tervo S, Kivipelto M, Hanninen T, et al. Incidence and risk factors for mild cognitive impairment: a population-based three-year follow- up study of cognitively healthy elderly subjects. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2004;17:196–203. doi: 10.1159/000076356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment: transition between aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neurologia. 2000;15(3):93–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dong Y, Sharma VK, Chan BPL, Venketasubramanian N, Teoh HL, Seet RCS, Tanicala S, Chan YH, Chen C. The Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA) is superior to the mini-mental state examination (MMSE) for the detection of vascular cognitive impairment after acute stroke. J Neurol Sci. 2010;299(1–2):15–18. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2010.08.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, et al. The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale-third edition. Pearson: San Antonio, TX; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zatońska K, Zatoński WA, Szuba A. Prospective urban and rural epidemiology Poland – study design. J Heal Inequalities. 2016;2:136–141. doi: 10.5114/jhi.2016.65353. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mancia G, Laurent S, Agabiti-Rosei E, Ambrosioni E, Burnier M, Caulfield MJ, Cifkova R, Clément D, Coca A, Dominiczak A, Erdine S, Fagard R, Farsang C, Grassi G, Haller H, Heagerty A, Kjeldsen SE, Kiowski W, Mallion JM, Manolis A, Narkiewicz K, Nilsson P, Olsen MH, Rahn KH, Redon J, Rodicio J, Ruilope L, Schmieder RE, Struijker-Boudier HAJ, van Zwieten PA, Viigimaa M, Zanchetti A. Reappraisal of European guidelines on hypertension management: a European Society of Hypertension Task Force document. Blood Press. 2009;18(6):308–347. doi: 10.3109/08037050903450468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Catapano AL, Graham I, De Backer G, et al. ESC/EAS guidelines for the management of dyslipidaemias: the task force for the management of dyslipidaemias of the european society of cardiology (ESC) and European atherosclerosis society (EAS) developed with the special contribution of the European Assocciation for Cardiovascular Prevention & Rehabilitation (EACPR) Atherosclerosis. 2016;253:281–344. doi: 10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2016.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Gordon NG. The trail making test in neuropsychological diagnosis. J Clin Psychol. 1972;28:167–169. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(197204)28:2&#x0003c;167::AID-JCLP2270280212&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hautzinger M, Bailer M (1993) Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CES-D). Weinheim: Beltz.
  • 23.Marzona I (2011) The standard assessment of global activities in the elderly (SAGE) scale: validation process of a new tool for the assessment of disability in older adults (doctoral dissertation).
  • 24.Wardlaw JM, Smith EE, Biessels GJ, Cordonnier C, Fazekas F, Frayne R, Lindley RI, O'Brien JT, Barkhof F, Benavente OR, Black SE, Brayne C, Breteler M, Chabriat H, Decarli C, de Leeuw FE, Doubal F, Duering M, Fox NC, Greenberg S, Hachinski V, Kilimann I, Mok V, Oostenbrugge Rv, Pantoni L, Speck O, Stephan BC, Teipel S, Viswanathan A, Werring D, Chen C, Smith C, van Buchem M, Norrving B, Gorelick PB, Dichgans M, STandards for ReportIng Vascular changes on nEuroimaging (STRIVE v1) Nueroimaging standards for research into small vessel disease and its contribution to aging and neurodegeneration. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:822–838. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70124-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.R Core Team R (2019) A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URL https://www.R-project.org/ [access on 31st December 2019].
  • 26.Roberts R, Knopman DS. Classification and epidemiology of MCI. Clin Geriatr Med. 2013;29(4):753–772. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2013.07.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Smith EE, O'Donnell M, Dagenais G, Lear SA, Wielgosz A, Sharma M, Poirier P, Stotts G, Black SE, Strother S, Noseworthy MD, Benavente O, Modi J, Goyal M, Batool S, Sanchez K, Hill V, McCreary CR, Frayne R, Islam S, DeJesus J, Rangarajan S, Teo K, Yusuf S, on behalf of the PURE Investigators Early cerebral small vessel disease and brain volume, cognition, and gait. Ann Neurol. 2015;77(2):251–261. doi: 10.1002/ana.24320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Han F, Zhai F-F, Wang Q, Zhou LX, Ni J, Yao M, Li ML, Zhang SY, Cui LY, Jin ZY, Zhu YC. Prevalence and risk factors of cerebral small vessel disease in a Chinese population-based sample. J Stroke. 2018;20(2):239–246. doi: 10.5853/jos.2017.02110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Anand SS, Friedrich MG, Desai D, Schulze KM, Awadalla P, Busseuil D, Dummer TJB, Jacquemont S, Dick A, Kelton D, Kirpalani A, Lear SA, Leipsic J, Noseworthy MD, Parker L, Parraga G, Poirier P, Robson P, Tardif JC, Teo K, Vena J, Yusuf S, Moody AR, Black SE, Smith EE. Canadian Alliance for healthy hearts and minds. Reduced Cognitive Assessment Scores Among Individuals With Magnetic Resonance Imaging-Detected Vascular Brain Injury Stroke. 2020;51:1158–1165. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.119.028179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Singh-Manoux A, Kivimaki M, Glymour MM, Elbaz A, Berr C, Ebmeier KP, Ferrie JE, Dugravot A. Timing of onset of cognitive decline: results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study. BMJ doi. 2012;344:d7622. doi: 10.1136/bmj.d7622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Staals J, Makin SD, Doubal FN, Dennis MS, Wardlaw JM. Stroke subtype, vascular risk factors, and total MRI brain small-vessel disease burden. Neurology. 2014;83(14):1228–1234. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000837. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rensma SP, van Sloten TT, Launer LJ, Stehouwer CD. Cerebral small vessel disease and risk of incident stroke, dementia and depression, and all-cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;90:164–173. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Saenz JL, Downer B, Garcia MA, Wong R. Cognition and context: rural–urban differences in cognitive aging among older Mexican adults. J Aging Health. 2018;30(6):965–986. doi: 10.1177/0898264317703560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Lorenzo-López L, Millán-Calenti JC, López-López R, Diego-Diez C, Laffon B, Pásaro E, Valdiglesias V, Maseda A (2017) Effects of degree of urbanization and lifetime longest-held occupation on cognitive impairment prevalence in an older Spanish population. Front Psychol: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 35.Singh-Manoux A, Dugravot A, Shipley M, Brunner EJ, Elbaz A, Sabia S, Kivimaki M. Obesity trajectories and risk of dementia: 28 years of follow-up in the Whitehall II study. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(2):178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2017.06.2637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Iadecola C, Yaffe K, Biller J, Bratzke LC, Faraci FM, Gorelick PB, Gulati M, Kamel H, Knopman DS, Launer LJ, Saczynski JS, Seshadri S, Zeki al Hazzouri A, American Heart Association Council on Hypertension; Council on Clinical Cardiology; Council on Cardiovascular Disease in the Young; Council on Cardiovascular and Stroke Nursing; Council on Quality of Care and Outcomes Research; and Stroke Council Impact of hypertension on cognitive function: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Hypertension. 2016;68(6):e67–e94. doi: 10.1161/HYP.0000000000000053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Walker KA, Sharrett AR, Wu A, Schneider ALC, Albert M, Lutsey PL, Bandeen-Roche K, Coresh J, Gross AL, Windham BG, Knopman DS, Power MC, Rawlings AM, Mosley TH, Gottesman RF. Association of midlife to late-life blood pressure patterns with incident dementia. Jama. 2019;322(6):535–545. doi: 10.1001/jama.2019.10575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gutwinski S, Schreiter S, Priller J, Henssler J, Wiers CE, Heinz A. Drink and think: impact of alcohol on cognitive functions and dementia–evidence of dose-related effects. Pharmacopsychiatry. 2018;51(04):136–143. doi: 10.1055/s-0043-118664. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Atwood LD, Wolf PA, Heard-Costa NL, Massaro JM, Beiser A, D’Agostino RB, DeCarli C. Genetic variation in white matter hyperintensity volume in the Framingham study. Stroke. 2004;35:1609–1613. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000129643.77045.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Turner ST, Jack CR, Fornage M, at al (2004) Heritability of leukoaraiosis in hypertensive sibships. Hypertension 43: 483–487. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 41.Chai YL, Yeo HKH, Wang J, Hilal S, Ikram MK, Venketasubramanian N, Wong BS, Chen CLH. Apolipoprotein ɛ4 is associated with dementia and cognitive impairment predominantly due to Alzheimer’s disease and not with vascular cognitive impairment: a Singapore-based cohort. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;51(4):1111–1118. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Clark LR, Zuelsdorff M, Norton D, et al (2020) Association of cardiovascular risk factors with cerebral perfusion in whites and African Americans. J Alzheimers Dis (Preprint), 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Articles from GeroScience are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES