Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 Apr 17.
Published in final edited form as: Addiction. 2020 Apr 27;115(11):1989–2007. doi: 10.1111/add.15055

Associations between solitary drinking and increased alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, and drinking to cope motives in adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Carillon J Skrzynski 1, Kasey G Creswell 1
PMCID: PMC8053066  NIHMSID: NIHMS1691417  PMID: 32196794

Abstract

Background and aims

Emerging evidence suggests that solitary drinking may be an important early risk marker for alcohol use disorder. The current paper is the first meta-analysis and systematic review on adolescent and young adult solitary drinking to examine associations between solitary drinking and increased alcohol consumption, alcohol problems, and drinking to cope motives.

Methods

PsychINFO, PubMed, and Google Scholar were searched using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) methodology and a pre-registered International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) protocol (no. CRD42020143449). Data from self-report questionnaires regarding negative correlates of solitary drinking (e.g. alcohol problems) and solitary drinking motives (e.g. drinking to cope) were pooled across studies using random-effects models. Studies included adolescents (aged 12–18 years) and young adults (mean age between 18 and 30 years or samples with the majority of participants aged 30 years or younger).

Results

Meta-analytical results from 21 unique samples including 28,372 participants showed significant effects for the associations between solitary drinking and the following factors: increased alcohol consumption, r = 0.23, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.12, 0.33; drinking problems, r = 0.23, 95% CI = 0.13, 0.32; negative affect, r = 0.21, 95% CI = 0.16, 0.26; social discomfort, r = 0.17, 95% CI = 0.06, 0.27; negative reinforcement, r = 0.28, 95% CI = 0.24, 0.31; and positive reinforcement, r = 0.10, 95% CI = 0.03, 0.17. These associations were not moderated by age group (i.e. adolescent versus young adult), study quality, or differing solitary drinking definitions. Accounting for publication bias increased the effect sizes from r = 0.23 to 0.34 for alcohol consumption and from r = 0.23 to 0.30 for drinking problems, and lowered it from r = 0.10 to 0.06 and r = 0.17 to 0.11 for positive reinforcement and social discomfort, respectively.

Conclusions

Solitary drinking among adolescents and young adults appears to be associated with psychosocial/alcohol problems and drinking to cope motives.

Keywords: Adolescents, alcohol use disorder risk, drinking alone, drinking to cope, meta-analysis, social drinking context, solitary drinking, systematic review, tension reduction, young adults

INTRODUCTION

Most adolescents and young adults who drink alcohol do so in social settings [14], with far fewer drinking alcohol while alone (e.g. [5,6]). However, an emerging literature suggests that solitary drinking in younger individuals may represent an informative divergence from normative behavior, with important implications for understanding risk for alcohol use disorder (AUD). For example, adolescent and young adult solitary (versus social-only) drinkers often report increased alcohol consumption and more alcohol-related problems (e.g. [79]). While solitary drinking appears to be a risky drinking pattern for adolescents and young adults, no prior studies have meta-analyzed results across studies. The current study is the first, to our knowledge, to do so, providing a critical evaluation of the strength and reliability of these effects across studies.

If solitary drinking is a reliable marker for alcohol problems among adolescents and young adults, it will be important to understand why individuals drink alone, as such information may help to inform future intervention/prevention programs. One theory put forth in the literature is that adolescents and young adults drink alone to self-medicate with alcohol to alleviate or cope with negative affect (NA) (e.g. [7,8,10,11]). Consistent with this motivational model of alcohol use [4,1113] in which individuals drink alcohol to regulate NA, studies suggest that drinking to cope motives are associated with solitary drinking in both adolescents (e.g. [11]) and young adults (e.g. [9]). In fact, among these individuals, increased solitary drinking frequency is associated with increased NA [14] and NA-inducing experiences/states (e.g. loneliness) [15], which may make these individuals particularly likely to drink alcohol to cope.

A recent narrative review on solitary drinking in US adolescents emphasized the association between solitary drinkingand coping with NA [16]. It is unclear how reliable these effects are across studies, however, as quantitative analyses were not conducted. In addition, to lend credence to the theory that solitary drinking is motivated by the desire to ameliorate NA, it is important to evaluate whether solitary drinking is not also associated with positive reinforcement (e.g. enhancement motives). Specifying the exact motives underlying solitary drinking (i.e. negative versus positive reinforcement) is necessary to develop effective treatment approaches. Finally, this prior narrative review [16] focused exclusively on adolescents, but young adulthood is also a period of particular risk for alcohol misuse [17] and solitary drinking has been associated with problematic outcomes in this group, as well (e.g. [15,18]). A meta-analytical strategy will allow us to determine the relative importance of solitary drinking in predicting problematic alcohol use throughout these two age periods.

The current meta-analysis is the first, to our knowledge, to provide quantitative analyses on (1) whether adolescent and young adult solitary drinking is associated with increased alcohol involvement and more alcohol-related problems and (2) whether the desire to cope with NA [versus a desire to enhance positive affect (PA)] is associated with solitary drinking. Because adolescence and young adulthood are unique periods of development with differences across a variety of domains (e.g. physically, socially [19,20]), including differences in drinking experiences (e.g. legal access to alcohol), age group was included as a moderator in analyses. We also conducted a systematic review examining the prevalence rates of and demographic factors associated with solitary drinking in adolescents and young adults and other related negative psychosocial outcomes. This information will increase our understanding of which adolescents and young adults are especially vulnerable to drink alone.

We hypothesized that solitary drinking would be associated with greater alcohol consumption and alcohol-related problems. Consistent with motivational models of alcohol use [4,1113], we also predicted that solitary drinking would be associated with greater NA (e.g. depressive symptoms), more NA-inducing social experiences (e.g. loneliness, social anxiety) and negative (but not positive) reinforcement (e.g. drinking to cope). We begin by defining solitary drinking and reviewing how it has been measured. Next, we present prevalence rates and associated demographics of solitary drinking in adolescents and young adults. We finally review and meta-analyze the negative correlates and consequences of solitary drinking and motives associated with drinking alone, specifically highlighting the association between drinking to cope motives and solitary drinking.

METHOD

We adhered to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) methodology [21]. The full review protocol is available in International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO); http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO/), registration no. CRD42020143449.

The following electronic databases were searched for studies in December 2018 and again in June 2019: Google Scholar, psycINFO, and PubMed. Search terms were (alcohol or drinking) and (solitary, alone, or non-social) and (young adult, emerging adult, adolescent). The reference lists of identified studies were also scanned, and reverse searches were generated and scanned for appropriate studies. Articles were included in both the systematic review and meta-analysis if solitary drinking was assessed in a sample of adolescents (aged 12–18 years) or young adults (mean ages = 18–30 or samples with the majority of participants ≤ age 30).

To be included in the meta-analysis, studies were required to measure negative correlates or consequences of solitary drinking or motives for drinking alone (see below for more information). To be included in the systematic review, studies had to report prevalence rates of drinking alone, associated demographic factors or negative psychosocial outcomes, or examine in-the-moment affective experiences while drinking alone. Exclusionary criteria for both reviews included non-human animals and non-English language.

Data extraction, coding, and statistical analysis

For the meta-analysis, we extracted statistics (i.e. correlations, means, odds ratios) relevant for understanding the relationships between solitary drinking and the following superordinate factors: ‘alcohol consumption’, ‘drinking problems’, ‘negative affect’, ‘social discomfort’, ‘negative reinforcement’, and ‘positive reinforcement’ (see Table 1). When these statistics were not available, we requested them from authors.1 A second member of the study team independently extracted these data, which resulted in excellent reliability (inter-rater agreement = 97%). The few discrepancies that existed were reconciled by team discussion. Analyses were run using Comprehensive Meta Analysis (CMA version 2.0) software [22], and final effect sizes are reported as Pearson’s r.

Table 1.

Superordinate factors and underlying variables of interest.

Study authors Superordinate factor Variables of interest included Scale/questionnaires
Alcohol consumption
Anderson & Brown 2010 Binge drinking Frequency 5+ drinks/past month
General drinking frequency Times/past month
Life-time drinking frequency Times/life
Maximum drinking quantity Max. drinks/occasion/past month
Armeli et al. 2016a Drinking frequency Days/past month
Heavy drinking frequency Heavy drinking days/past month
Armeli et al. 2018 Drinking quantity Drinks/day
Blevins et al. 2018 Drinking quantity DDQ
Drinking frequency DDQ
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 Drinking frequency DDQ
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 Drinking frequency CDDR
Heavy drinking frequency Max. drinks past 3 months
Drinking quantity CDDR
Creswell et al. 2014 Drinking quantity LDHM
Drinking frequency LDHM
Creswell et al. 2015 Drinking frequency LDHM
Drinking quantity LDHM
Falcon et al. 2014 Binge drinking Presence/absence 5+ drinks on last drinking occasion
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 Drinking quantity NIAAA
Keough et al. 2015 Quantity × frequency Days/week × drinks/occasion
Keough et al. 2016 Quantity × frequency Days/week × drinks/occasion
O’Hara et al. 2014 Drinking quantity Drinks/day
Heavy drinking frequency Presence/absence 4/5+ drinks/day
O’Hara et al. 2015 Drinking frequency Days/past month
Drinking quantity Drinks/day/past 3 months
Binge drinking Days 4/5+ drinks consumed/past month
Skrzynski et al. 2018 Drinking frequency NIAAA
Drinking quantity NIAAA
Stewart & Powers 2003 Drinking frequency Days/past month and days/per year
Heavy drinking frequency Number of days large quantity consumed
Drinking quantity Typical drinks/occasion
Terry-McElrath, Stern & Patrick 2017 Maximum drinking quantity Max drinks/occasion
Tucker et al. 2006 Drinking frequency Times/year and times/month
Drinking quantity Drinks/day
Tucker et al. 2014 Drinking quantity At least 1 drink/past month
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 Drinking problems Drinking quantity CDDR
Anderson & Brown 2010 Drinking problems CHKS
Armeli et al. 2018 Drinking problems B-YAACQ
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 Drinking problems Driving after drinking
Drinking problems Drinking after drinking too much
Bilevicious et al. 2018 Drinking problems AUDIT
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 Drinking problems RAPI
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 Drinking problems Questions adapted from studies by Wechslerc
Creswell et al. 2014 Drinking problems AUD symptoms at age 25 from SCID-IV
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 Drinking problems YAACQ
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 Drinking problems SADD
Keough et al. 2015 Drinking problems RAPI
Keough et al. 2016 Drinking problems RAPI
Keough et al. 2018 Drinking problems YAACQ
Drinking problems AUDIT-C
Skrzynski et al. 2018 Drinking problems AUDIT
Drinking problems B-YAACQ
Stewart & Powers 2003 Drinking problems Negative consequences from drinking
Tucker et al. 2014 Drinking problems Negative consequences from drinking
West, Drummond & Eames 1990 Drinking problems Experiencing/perpetrating trouble
Drinking problems MAST
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 Negative affect (NA) Drinking problems CDDR DSM-IValcohol dependence
Anderson & Brown 2010 Negative temperament Negative Temperament Scale
Armeli et al. 2016a Anger/anxiety/depressive affect PANAS
Daily stress Question rating day’s overall stressfulness
Neuroticism NEO-PI-R
Armeli et al. 2018 Anger/anxiety/depressive affect PANAS/Larsen & Diener mood circumplex
Life stress PERI life events scale
Bilevicious et al. 2018 Depression CES-D
Anxiety GAD-7
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 Depression CES-D
Creswell et al. 2015 Aggression MPQ
Alienation MPQ
Stress reaction MPQ
Gonzalez 2012 Previous suicidal behavior SBQ-R
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 Depression BDI-II
Hopelessness BHS
Suicidal ideation ASIQ
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 Depression BDI-II
Suicidal ideation ASIQ
Keough et al. 2015 Depression MASQ-AD
O’Hara et al. 2015 Social discomfort Anger/anxiety/depressive affect PANAS
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 Social anxiety SIAS
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 Loneliness UCLA
Social competence SCQ
Social network satisfaction Individual question
Social support-seeking CSI-SS
Social withdrawal CSI-SW
Keough et al. 2016 Social anxiety SIAS
Skrzynski et al. 2018 Loneliness UCLA
Perceived social support—appraisal ISEL
Perceived social support—belonging ISEL
Social anxiety SIAS
Negative reinforcement
Armeli et al. 2016 Drinking to cope DMQ-Rb
Armeli et al. 2018 Drinking to cope DMQ-Rb
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 Drinking to cope with emotional pain Social context of drinking (Beck et al. 2008)
Blevins Abrantes & Stephens 2018 Drinking to cope DMQ-R
Drinking during unpleasant emotion IDS
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 Drinking refusal self-efficacy during NA DRSE-ER
Personal coping expectancies AEQ
Social coping expectancies AEQ
Creswell et al. 2014 Drinking during unpleasant emotion IDS
Creswell et al. 2015 Drinking refusal self-efficacy during NA SCQA
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 Drinking to cope Drinking context scale
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 Drinking to cope DMQ-R
O’Hara et al. 2014 Drinking to cope DMQ-Rb
O’Hara, Armeli & Tennen 2015 Drinking to cope DMQ-Rb/MDMQ-R
Skrzynski et al. 2018 Drinking to cope DMQ-R
Drinking refusal self-efficacy during NA DRSE-ER
Stewart & Powers 2003 Drinking to cope Drinking reasons related to coping
Terry-McElrath et al. 2017 Drinking to cope Drinking reasons related to coping
Tucker et al. 2014 Negative outcome expectancies 2 items from drinking expectancies scale (Ellickson et al. 2003)
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 Positive reinforcement Drinking to cope DMQ-R
Armeli et al. 2016 Drinking to enhance DMQ-Rb
Armeli et al. 2018 Drinking to enhance DMQ-Rb
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 Drinking to socialize/have a good time Social context of drinking (Beck et al. 2008)
Blevins, Abrantes & Stephens 2018 Drinking to socialize DMQ-R
Drinking to enhance DMQ-R
Drinking during pleasant emotion IDS
Drinking during pleasant times with others IDS
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 Personal enhancement expectancies AEQ
Social enhancement expectancies AEQ
Creswell et al. 2014 Drinking during pleasant emotion IDS
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 Drinking to enhance DMQ-R
Drinking to socialize DMQ-R
O’Hara et al. 2014 Drinking to enhance DMQ-Rb
Drinking to socialize DMQ-Rb
O’Hara, Armeli & Tennen 2015 Drinking to socialize DMQ-Rb
Drinking to enhance DMQ-Rb
Stewart & Powers 2003 Drinking to socialize Drinking reasons related to being social
Terry-McElrath et al. 2017 Drinking to socialize Drinking reasons related to being social
Tucker et al. 2014 Positive outcome expectancies 1 item from drinking expectancies scale (Ellickson et al. 2003)

DDQ = Daily drinking questionnaire; CDDR = customary drinking and drug use record; LDHM = life-time drinking history method; NIAAA = National Institute of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism alcohol consumption question set; CHKS = California Healthy Kids Survey; (B-)YAACQ = (Brief) Young adult alcohol consequences questionnaire; AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; RAPI = Rutgers Alcohol Problem Index; SCID-IV = Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV; SADD = short alcohol dependence data; MAST = Michigan Alcohol Screening Test; PANAS = Positive and Negative Affective Scale; NEOPI-R = neuroticism extroversion openness–personality inventory—revised; CES-D = Center for Epidemiologic Studies—depression; GAD-7 = Generalized Anxiety Disorder scale; MPQ = Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire; SBQ-R = the Suicidal Behaviors Questionnaire—Revised; BDI-II = Beck Depression Inventory-II; BHS = Beck Hopelessness Scale; ASIQ = Adult Suicidal Ideation Questionnaire; MASQ-AD = Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire—anhedonic depression subscale; SIAS = Social Interaction and Anxiety Scale; UCLA = UCLA Loneliness Scale; SCQ = Social Competence Questionnaire; CSI-SS/SW = Coping strategies inventory—social support/social withdrawal; ISEL = Interpersonal support evaluation list; DMQ-R = Drinking Motives Questionnaire—revised; IDS = Inventory of Drinking Situations; DRSE-ER = Drinking Refusal Self-Efficacy—emotional relief; SCQA = Situational Confidence Questionnaire–alcohol; MDMQ-R = Modified Drinking Motives Questionnaire; AEQ = Alcohol Expectancies Questionnaire.

a

This paper contained two samples; all data referenced in this table come from sample 2.

b

This was an adapted version of the DMQ-R using seven items for coping (‘to forget my ongoing problems/worries’, ‘to feel less depressed’, ‘to feel less nervous’, ‘to avoid dealing with my ongoing problems’, ‘to cheer up’, ‘because I was angry’ and ‘to feel more confident/sure of myself’), two items for enhancement (‘Because I like the pleasant feeling’ and ‘to have fun’) and two items for social motives (‘to make party/gathering more fun’ and ‘to improve party/gathering’) on a three-point scale (0 = no, 1 = somewhat, 2 = definitely).

c

Wechsler et al. 1994; Wechsler & Isaac, 1992; Wechsler & McFadden, 1979.

Each value contributing to an aggregate effect size was independent of all other values. When studies included multiple ways of assessing solitary drinking (e.g. frequency and quantity [15]), to be conservative we included the solitary drinking variable with the weakest association. When studies reported associations between solitary drinking and multiple variables categorized within the same superordinate factor (e.g. depression and hopelessness, which were both included in the ‘negative affect’ superordinate factor), we computed an average effect size across the variables [22,23]. Finally, results from studies reporting on the same sample were averaged over and treated as a single study.

Random-effects models were used for all analyses [24]. The heterogeneity of effect sizes across studies for each superordinate factor was tested with the Q statistic [22,23]. When the heterogeneity test was significant, we tested for potential moderation by (1) age group (i.e. adolescent versus young adult), (2) differing solitary drinking definitions, and (3) study quality. Studies were coded as ‘alone’ if solitary drinking was defined as drinking while physically alone and ‘all others’ if it was defined as drinking with non-drinking or non-interacting others.2 Study quality was assessed using the system outlined by Mason [16]. Each study was coded on three study features (i.e. representative sampling, standardized measurement, and prospective longitudinal analysis) and received a score of 0–3, depending on the absence/presence (0/1) of each feature. The average study quality rating across studies was 1.44 [standard deviation (SD) = 0.78], with the majority of studies receiving a score of 1 (37%) or 2 (46%) (see Table 2). Publication bias was evaluated by Begg’s rank correlation test [25], funnel plots to visualize bias, and trim-and-fill methods [26].

Table 2.

Characteristics and quality assessments of reviewed articles.

Citation Population Design Measure of solitary drinking Rating
Anderson & Brown 2010 US population: middle school students (n = 1171) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where on drinks 2b,c
Armeli et al. 2014 US population: college students (n = 1421) Cross-sectional Number of drinks previous night ‘alone/not interacting with others’ versus ‘with others/in a social setting’ over 30 days 1b
Armeli et al. 2016 (sample 2) US population: urban historically black US college/university (n = 452) Cross-sectional Number of drinks previous night ‘alone/not interacting with others’ versus ‘with others/in a social setting’ over 30 days 0
Armeli et al. 2018 US population: college students (n = 927) Cross-sectional Number of drinks previous night ‘alone/not interacting with others’ versus ‘with others/in a social setting’ over 30 days 2b,c
Bailey & Rachal 1993 USpopulation:middle and high schoolstudents (n = 2771) Cross-sectionald Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one drank in the past year 1c
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 USpopulation: Maryland motorists who had an alcohol-impaired driving citation (n = 161) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 1c
Beck, Summons & Thombs 1991 US population: high school students (n = 1797) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Beck, Summons & Thombs 1993 US population: high school students (n = 1360) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Bilevicious et al. 2018 Canadian population: college students (n = 308) Prospective longitudinal Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 3a,b,c
Blevins, Abrantes & Stephens 2018 US population: college students (n = 303) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 US population: college students (n = 776) Cross-sectional Number of days on which drinking occurred in the past year in social (i.e. with others) and in solitary (i.e. alone) settings 1b
Falcon et al. 2014 European population: Roma adolescents in three Spanish areas (n = 569) Cross-sectional Drinks alone often (not specified further) 0
Chalder, Elgar & Bennett 2006 European population: community sample of adolescents (n = 1744) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 US population: college students (n = 464) Cross-sectional Heavy drinking alone and/or being the only person drinking vs not in the previous 3 months 1b
Cooper 1994 US population: community sample of adolescents (n = 1243) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Creswell et al. 2014 US population: clinical and community sample of adolescents (n = 709) Prospective longitudinal Life-time percentage of drinking spent without others present 2a,c
Creswell et al. 2015 US population: clinical and community sample of adolescents (n = 761) Cross-sectional Life-time percentage of drinking spent without others present 1c
Dauber et al. 2009 US population: black and white female middle school students (n = 2948) Cross-sectional High risk drinking context questions (e.g. drinking while drunk), including drinking alone 2b,c
Degenhardt et al. 2015 Australian population: Victorian Adolescent Health cohort study (44 schools) (n = 1943) Prospective longitudinal Drinking contexts questions asking with whom and where one drinks 3a,b,c
Gibbons et al. 1986 US population: rural middle and high school students (n = 650) Cross-sectional Drinking contexts questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 1c
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 US population: heavy drinking underage college students (n = 91) Cross-sectional Number of typical days drinking alone or whilenoone else wasdrinking versus not in a typical month 0
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 US population: heavy drinking college students (n = 90) Cross-sectional Number of typical days drinking alone or whilenoone else wasdrinking versus not in a typical month 0
Gonzalez 2012 US population: heavy drinking college students (n = 182) Cross-sectional Number of typical days drinking alone or while nooneelsewas drinkingversusnotina typical month 0
Kablicek et al. 2018 European clinical population: adolescents from inpatient wards in the Czech Republic (30 wards) (n = 1838) Cross-sectional Drinking contexts questions asking with whom one drank at time of admission 1c
Kask & Markina 2014 European population: adolescents from 25 European countries (n = 57771) Cross-sectional Drinking contexts question asking with whom one drank on last drinking occasion 2b,c
Keough et al. 2015 US population: vollege students (n = 295) Cross-sectional Drinking contexts question asking with whom one usually drank in the past 6 months 2b,c
Keough et al. 2016 US population: college students (n = 293) Cross-sectional Drinking contexts question asking with whom one usually drank in the past 6 months 2b,c
Keough et al. 2018 US population: college students (n = 118) Cross-sectional Drinking contexts question asking with whom one usually drank in the past 6 months 2b,c
Koposov et al. 2002 Russian population: adolescents from 25 secondary schools (n = 387) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Kouzis & Labouvie 1992 US population: community sample of adolescents (n = 437) Prospective longitudinal Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 3a,b,c
Lowman 1981 US population: national probability survey of 10–12th graders (n = not specified) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Mayer et al. 1998 US population: 9th and 12th graders (n = 4646) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one drank on last drinking occasion 1b
McCabe et al. 2014 US population: High school seniors (N = 24809) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one drinks 2b,c
McGee et al. 2011 US population: high school students (n = 6748) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one drinks 1c
O’Hara et al. 2015 US population: urban historically black US college/university (n = 452) Cross-sectional Number of drinks previous night ‘alone/not interacting with others’ versus ‘with others/in a social setting’ over 30 days 0
O’Hara et al. 2014 US population: urban historically black US college/university (n = 462) Cross-sectional Number of drinks previous night ‘alone/not interacting with others’ versus ‘with others/in a social setting’ over 30 days 0
O’Hara, Armeli, & Tennen, 2015 US population: college students (n = 722) Cross-sectional Number of drinks previous night ‘alone/not interacting with others’ versus ‘with others/in a social setting’ over 30 days 1b
O’Hare, 1990 US population: college students Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Okulicz-Kozaryn & Borucka 2013 European population: 15 -year-old Polish students (n = 1229–3918) Longitudinal Drinking context question asking with whom one drank on the last drinking occasion 1b
Pederson, LaBrie, & Kilmer, 2009 US population: college students (n = 444) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom one usually pre-parties 1b
Rivers & Noret 2008 European population: adolescents from 14 schools in England (n = 106) Cross-sectional Drinking context question asking how often one drank alone 1b
Russell et al. 2002 US population: middleand high school students Prospective longitudinal Single item on ever being drunk while alone 2a,b
Skrzynski et al. 2018 US population: underage drinkers aged 18–20 years (n = 664) Cross-sectional Life-time percentage of drinking spent without others present 2b,c
Stewart & Power 2003 US population: high school students (n = 1874) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one drinks 1b
Strunin & Demissie 2001 US population: middle school students (n = 314) Cross-sectional Item asking if participants ever drank alcohol alone 1b
Swahn & Donovan 2004 US population: nationally representative sample of adolescents from middle and high school (n = 8885) Prospective longitudinal Drinking context question asking if one ever drank alcohol alone 2a,b
Swahn et al. 2004 US population: nationally representative sample of adolescents from middle and high school (n = 8885) Cross-sectional Drinking context question asking if one ever drank alcohol alone 1b
Swahn & Donovan 2005 US population: nationally representative sample of adolescents from middle and high school (n = 6041) Prospective longitudinal Drinking context question asking if one ever drank alcohol alone 2a,b
Swahn & Donovan 2006 US population: nationally representative sample of adolescents from middle and high school (n = 8866) Cross-sectional Drinking context question asking if one ever drank alcohol alone 1b
Terry-McElrath et al. 2017 US population: nationally representative sample of high school seniors (n = 16902) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom one often drinks 2b,c
Thombs et al. 1994 US population: high school students (n = 1484) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions asking with whom and where one usually drinks 2b,c
Tomlinson & Brown 2012 US population: 8th grade students (n = 406) Cross-sectional Drinking context questions with whom and how often one drank in the past month 2b,c
Tucker et al. 2006 US population: 8th grade students and later, same students at age 23 [n = 2003 (drinkers)] Prospective longitudinal Item asking if participants ever drank alone in their life-time 2a,b
Tucker et al. 2008 US population: 6th and 7th grade students and later, same students at age 23 (n = 1633) Prospective longitudinal Item asking if participants ever drank alone in their life-time 2a,b
Tucker et al. 2014 US population: 6th and 7th grade students (n = 818) Cross-sectional Item asking if participants ever drank alone in their life-time 1b
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 US population: college students (n = 134) Cross-sectional Heavy drinking alone and/or being the only person drinking versus not in the previous 3 months 1b
West et al. 1990 European population: college students (n = 270) Cross-sectional Drinking context question asking if one drank alone (not further specified) 1b
a

Prospective longitudinal design;

b

representative sample;

c

standardized measure of solitary use.

d

This study was a longitudinal design overall, but data from the third wave was used for an exploratory factor analysis including an item on drinking alone.

RESULTS

A total of 528 articles were identified in the search. Fifty-eight articles were included in the systematic review; 28 articles with 21 unique samples (n = 28, 372 individuals) were included in the meta-analysis (see Fig. 1 for a PRISMA diagram).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) diagram for papers selected for the systematic review and meta-analysis.

Definitions and measures of solitary drinking

Definitions and measures of solitary drinking varied across studies (see Table 2). Solitary drinking was defined in most studies as drinking while alone (i.e. drinking without others present) (e.g. [7,8,10]). In some studies, solitary drinking also included drinking with non-drinking others (e.g. [6,18,27]) and/or among non-interacting others (e.g. [2831]). Measures of solitary drinking also differed across studies. Researchers often dichotomized solitary drinking as presence or absence of this versus social-only drinking (e.g. [5,6]), but some also reported solitary drinking quantity (e.g. [15]), frequency (e.g. [14,32]) and/or frequency of heavy solitary drinking (e.g. [9,15]). Alternatively, other researchers assessed solitary drinking as a percentage of total drinking episodes (e.g. [7,8,10]). Importantly, the latter may decrease confounding overall drinking frequency with solitary drinking (i.e. those who drink more often, have more opportunities to drink in social and solitary settings), which can be problematic when only inquiring about solitary drinking frequency.

Prevalence rates

Prevalence rates of solitary drinking varied among studies, but generally the majority of adolescents and young adults did not report drinking alone. According to a recent review [16], 14% of the general US adolescent population reported drinking alone, but this increased to almost 40% among high-risk subgroups (i.e. clinical). Interestingly, some studies reported lower prevalence rates (< 14%) of adolescent solitary drinking [3338], but this may be due to the use of non-US samples [34,38], or the measurement utilized (e.g. last drinking context) [33,3537].

Among young adults, prevalence rates of solitary drinking also varied, ranging from approximately 15 to 24%, regardless of how solitary drinking was defined (e.g. [6,18,39]). However, prevalence rates of solitary drinking in on-line samples [7], as well as samples that included heavy drinkers [9,15,27] and those with suicidal ideation [9] or depressive symptomology [27] were higher, ranging from 24 to 74%.

Demographics

Findings on demographic variables associated with solitary drinking were mixed. Some studies found that adolescents and young adults who endorsed drinking alone were more likely to be older relative to social-only drinkers [6,15,35,39]3 while others found no differences or suggested opposite findings [10,34,36]. Data on adolescent solitary drinking among different ethnicities/races were also inconsistent, despite using similar definitions/time-frames to assess solitary drinking [5,8,16,4043]. We are unaware of studies investigating ethnic/racial differences in young adult solitary drinking. Evidence of sex differences among solitary drinkers was similarly equivocal. Some data suggested that males were more likely to drink alone as young adults [30,39,44,45] and adolescents [8,10,16,46], while others found no differences across sex [5,14,15,43,47]. Finally, regarding sexual orientation, three studies indicated that adolescents with same-sex attractions were more likely to drink alone [4850].

Negative correlates of solitary drinking

For each correlate below, findings from the systematic review are presented first, followed by a summary of meta-analytical results.

Alcohol consumption and alcohol problems

Several cross-sectional studies found that adolescent and young adult solitary drinkers reported greater alcohol use than their social-only drinking peers [5,810,15,18,51], and solitary drinking frequency was positively associated with alcohol use and problems in these individuals [5254]. Further, solitary (versus social-only) young adult drinkers reported more alcohol-related problems [9], greater alcohol dependence severity [15], and less likelihood to change problem drinking [18], and adolescent solitary (versus social-only) drinkers reported more AUD symptoms [8]. Notably, in analyses controlling for alcohol use quantity/frequency, the association between solitary drinking (measured as both a continuous variable and a categorical yes/no variable) and alcohol problems held for both adolescents and young adults [5,8,53,54], suggesting that this association was not due to greater alcohol involvement but was specific to solitary drinking (although see [15] for null findings). Corroborating this, the meta-analytical results revealed significant small effect sizes for the relationships between solitary drinking and both increased ‘alcohol consumption’ and more ‘drinking problems’ (see Table 3).

Table 3.

Meta-analysis of superordinate factors and underlying variables of interest and solitary drinking.

Heterogeneity
Study authors Superordinate factor n r 95% CI P Q P
Alcohol consumption
Anderson & Brown 2010 1160 0.66 0.63, 0.70 < 0.001
Armeli et al. 2016a 452 0.07 −0.03, 0.16 0.16
Armeli et al. 2018b 927 0.29 0.23, 0.35 < 0.001
Blevins, Abrantes & Stephens 2018 301 0.00 −0.11, 0.11 0.99
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 776 0.26 0.19, 0.32 < 0.001
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002c 262 0.17 0.05, 0.28 < 0.01
Creswell et al. 2014d 761 0.26 0.19, 0.32 < 0.001
Creswell et al. 2015d
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 91 0.36 0.17, 0.53 < 0.001
Falcón et al. 2014 566 0.17 −0.04, 0.37 0.12
Keough et al. 2015e 295 0.02 −0.09, 0.13 0.73
Keough et al. 2016e 293
O’Hara, Armeli & Tennen 2015b 722
O’Hara et al. 2014a 462
O’Hara et al. 2015a 452
Skrzynski et al. 2018 664 0.16 0.08, 0.23 < 0.001
Stewart & Powers 2003 1227 0.20 0.15, 0.26 < 0.001
Terry-McElrath, Stern & Patrick 2017 16902 0.04 −0.02, 0.10 0.17
Tucker et al. 2006c 1996 0.41 0.38, 0.45 < 0.001
Tucker et al. 2014 818 0.28 0.22, 0.34 < 0.001
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 134 0.14 −0.03, 0.30 0.09
Overall estimate 0.23 0.12, 0.33 < 0.001 522.43 < 0.001
Drinking problems
Anderson & Brown 2010 1118 0.59 0.56, 0.63 < 0.001
Armeli et al. 2018 927 0.13 0.07, 0.19 < 0.001
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 73 0.22 −0.01, −0.43 0.06
Bilevicious et al. 2018 308 0.09 −0.03, 0.20 0.14
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 776 0.17 0.10, 0.23 < 0.001
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 262 0.24 0.13, 0.35 < 0.001
Creswell et al. 2015 535 0.16 0.07, 0.24 < 0.001
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 91 0.26 0.06, 0.44 < 0.05
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 90 0.42 0.23, 0.58 < 0.001
Keough et al. 2015e 295 0.09 −0.02, 0.20 0.12
Keough et al. 2016e 293
Keough et al. 2018 118 0.14 −0.04, 0.32 0.12
Skrzynski et al. 2018 664 0.26 0.19, 0.33 < 0.001
Stewart & Powers 2003 1227 0.25 0.19, 0.30 < 0.001
Tucker et al. 2014 818 0.28 0.22, 0.34 < 0.001
West, Drummond & Eames 1990 270 0.14 0.02, 0.25 0.02
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 134 0.12 −0.05, 0.29 0.16
Overall estimate 0.23 0.13, 0.32 < 0.001 256.36 < 0.001
Negative affect
Anderson & Brown 2010 1159 0.26 0.20, 0.31 < 0.001
Armeli et al. 2016a 451 0.22 0.13, 0.31 < 0.001
Armeli et al. 2018 927 0.16 0.09, 0.22 < 0.001
Bilevicious et al. 2018 308 0.13 0.01, 0.23 < 0.05
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 262 0.30 0.19, 0.40 < 0.001
Creswell et al. 2015 709 0.12 0.05, 0.19 < 0.01
Gonzalez 2012f 182 0.26 0.07, 0.43 < 0.005
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013f 90
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 91 0.20 −0.01, 0.39 0.06
Keough et al. 2015 295 0.31 0.20, 0.41 < 0.001
O’Hara et al. 2015a 452
Overall estimate 0.21 0.16, 0.26 < 0.001 220.17 < 0.05
Social discomfort
Buckner & Terlecki 2016 776 0.06 −0.01, 0.13 0.08
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 90 0.16 −0.05, 0.36 0.13
Keough et al. 2016 293 0.31 0.20, 0.41 < 0.001
Skrzynski et al. 2018 664 0.15 0.08, 0.23 < 0.001
Overall estimate 0.17 0.06, 0.27 < 0.001 14.22 0.01
Negative reinforcement
Armeli et al. 2016a 451 0.35 0.27, 0.43 < 0.001
Armeli et al. 2018b 927 0.18 0.12, 0.24 < 0.001
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 162 0.26 0.11, 0.40 < 0.001
Blevins Abrantes & Stephens 2018 301 0.29 0.18, 0.39 < 0.001
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 262 0.20 0.09, 0.32 < 0.001
Creswell et al. 2014e 698 0.24 0.17, 0.31 < 0.001
Creswell et al. 2015e 709
Gonzalez & Skewes 2013 90 0.46 0.28, 0.61 < 0.001
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 91 0.27 0.07, 0.45 0.01
O’Hara et al. 2014a 462
O’Hara, Armeli & Tennen 2015b 722
Skrzynski et al. 2018 664 0.31 0.24, 0.38 < 0.001
Stewart & Powers 2010 1227 0.34 0.29, 0.39 < 0.001
Terry-McElrath, Stern & Patrick 2017 16 902 0.29 0.28, 0.31 < 0.001
Tucker et al. 2014 818 0.24 0.17, 0.30 < 0.001
Williams, Vik & Wong 2015 134 0.19 0.02, 0.35 < 0.05
Overall estimate 0.28 0.24, 0.31 < 0.001 30.64 < 0.005
Positive reinforcement
Armeli et al. 2016a 452 −0.03 −0.12, 0.06 0.54
Armeli et al. 2018b 927 −0.05 −0.11, 0.01 0.13
Beck, Ahmad & Farkas 2011 162 0.07 −0.08, 0.22 0.37
Blevins, Abrantes & Stephens 2018 303 −0.03 −0.14, 0.09 0.63
Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 262 0.24 0.13, 0.35 < 0.001
Creswell et al. 2014 698 0.19 0.12, 0.26 < 0.001
Gonzalez, Collins & Bradizza 2009 91 0.10 −0.11, 0.30 0.37
O’Hara et al. 2014a 462
O’Hara, Armeli & Tennen 2015b 722
Stewart & Powers 2003 1227 0.22 0.16, 0.27 < 0.001
Terry-McElrath, Stern & Patrick 2017 16902 0.06 0.04, 0.07 < 0.001
Tucker et al. 2014 818 0.21 0.14, 0.27 < 0.001
Overall estimate 0.10 0.03, 0.17 < 0.001 86.56 < 0.001

For studies with multiple variables of interest per superordinate factor, these variables are combined and generate an overall r, 95% confidence interval (CI), and P-value. When the relationships between variables of interest and solitary drinking were negatively correlated but still in the direction of the superordinate factor, the effect directions were coded positively so that effects aligned (e.g. solitary drinking was negatively correlated with perceived social support, but this relationship was coded so it contributed a positive value to the ‘social discomfort’ factor given that this signifies greater social problems).

a,b,d,e,f

These studies used the same sample and thus the statistics were averaged over them to avoid problems from contributing multiple weights to the same superordinate factor.

c

These sample sizes include only alcohol drinkers in the case of Tucker et al. 2006 (the whole sample size was 3303, which included students who used other substances) and only heavy social-only and heavy solitary drinkers in the case of Christiansen, Vik & Jarchow 2002 (the whole sample size was 325, which included students who were non-heavy drinkers).

Negative psychosocial outcomes

Beyond alcohol-related problems, adolescents and young adults who reported drinking alone earned poorer grades and missed more classes [5], were more likely to engage in risky behavior (e.g. unplanned sex4) and experienced more problems with authorities (i.e. getting arrested) than those who reported only drinking in social settings [18]. Further, being an adolescent or young adult solitary drinker was associated with violence and deviant acts (e.g. assault) [45,5557], even after controlling for drinking frequency and binge drinking [55].

Negative affectivity

Solitary drinking in adolescents and young adults was also associated with NA. Adolescent solitary drinkers reported more NA than adolescent social-only drinkers [8], and young adult heavy solitary drinkers reported more depressive symptoms than their social-only drinking counterparts [9,15,18]. Additionally, solitary drinking frequency was positively associated with depressive symptoms among both adolescents and young adults [14,53], and quantity of alcohol consumed in solitary settings was related to suicidal ideation and attempts [27] and NA among young adults [58]. These associations may be bi-directional, given that NA has been shown to predict later solitary drinking in longitudinal research [54], and laboratory findings show that drinking in a solitary context increases NA [5961]. Meta-analytical results revealed a small effect size between solitary drinking and ‘negative affect’ (see Table 3).

Social discomfort

Finally, adolescent and young adult solitary drinking was associated with social discomfort. For instance, among young adults, solitary drinking frequency and the percentage of drinking episodes that occurred while alone were both positively associated with social anxiety [7,32,62]. Solitary drinking percentage was also positively associated with loneliness and negatively associated with perceived social support in young adults [7]. Additionally, young adult heavy solitary drinkers reported lower perceived social competence than social-only heavy drinkers [15], and adolescent solitary drinkers reported less time participating in school or other organized activities than social-only drinkers [5]. Mixed findings have been reported, however. Some researchers have found no differences between heavy solitary and social-only college drinkers on their social network size or their drinking network satisfaction [15]. Similarly, others have found that adolescent solitary drinking status (yes/no), as well as the quantity of alcohol adolescents consume while alone, were associated with spending more time on social activities (e.g. hangingout with friends) [5,46]. To reconcile this, some have speculated that solitary drinkers, while being socially active, may not be as comfortable in social settings as social-only drinkers [7,15]. Consistent with this hypothesis, our meta-analytical results revealed a small effect size between solitary drinking and ‘social discomfort’ (see Table 3).

Consequences of solitary drinking

We are aware of six longitudinal studies among adolescents and young adults that tested whether solitary drinking prospectively predicts alcohol problems and other negative outcomes after controlling for baseline risk factors [5,8,30,54,56,63,64].5 Findings generally corroborated cross-sectional links between solitary drinking and negative outcomes. For instance, solitary drinkers in eighth grade went on to have more physical health problems, deviant behavior, and academic problems at age 23 than their social-only drinking peers [5]. Solitary drinking in adolescence (aged 12–18), measured as a percentage of total drinking episodes, also predicted age 25 AUD symptoms among both clinical and community samples [8]. Additionally, among university students, frequency of solitary drinking at the beginning of first semester predicted increased harmful drinking at the end of first semester, and solitary drinking mediated the link between increased NA and harmful drinking [54]. Finally, adolescent solitary drinkers had a higher incidence of risky drinking in later adolescence compared to social-only drinkers [63]. Of note, these findings held even after controlling for baseline alcohol use/problems [5,8,54] and other risk factors (e.g. NA, other drug use) [63], suggesting that solitary drinking accounts for unique variance in alcohol use and problems and may be an early warning sign for the development of AUD.

In two studies, solitary drinking failed to predict later psychosocial problems, but this may be because it was entered into regression models that included several other predictor variables. Specifically, Swahn & Donovan [56,64] found that adolescent solitary drinking (yes/no) was not a longitudinal predictor of violent behavior when it was entered into a regression model with demographic variables and nine other drinking variables (e.g. drinking quantity/frequency, unsupervised drinking, receiving drug and alcohol abuse treatment). Armeli and colleagues [30] found a significant, positive correlation between quantity of drinks consumed in solitary settings and drinking problems 5 years later in young adults, but drinking alone was no longer significant when entered into a regression model that included several other predictors (e.g. adult social roles, physical ailment symptoms, NA).

Reasons for solitary drinking

To our knowledge, no studies have assessed drinking motives specifically for solitary alcohol use. In other words, all of the studies reviewed below examined associations between drinking motives (with no context specified) and drinking that is social versus solitary (or differences across solitary versus social-only drinkers).

Negative reinforcement

Many studies have linked drinking to cope motives with adolescent and young adult solitary drinking (measured as both a continuous variable and a categorical yes/no variable) [6,7,9,15,28,30,31,65,66]. Importantly, the association between solitary drinking quantity/frequency and drinking to cope motives held even after controlling for social, enhancement, and conformity motives [9,11,31,66]. Further, young adult solitary drinkers reported greater beliefs in alcohol’s ability to reduce NA than social-only drinkers [18], and being a solitary drinker was associated with drinking in the context of NA but not PA in adolescents [8]. Additionally, the perceived inability to refuse alcohol during NA completely mediated the relationship between NA and solitary drinking status (yes/no) in a large sample of adolescents [10]. This is consistent with prior research [18] showing that college student heavy solitary drinkers had less confidence in their ability to resist drinking during NA than their social-only drinking peers. Finally, several studies have found that solitary drinking frequency loaded onto factors that include drinking to cope items (e.g. to get rid of depression) in adolescents and young adults [6771]. Notably, our meta-analysis revealed a significant small-to-medium effect size between solitary drinking and ‘negative reinforcement’ (see Table 3).

Positive reinforcement

While most research supports an association between negative reinforcement processes and solitary drinking in adolescents and young adults, some studies suggest that positive reinforcement (e.g. drinking to enhance PA) may also be associated with drinking alone. Specifically, in four studies, solitary drinking was related to both positive and negative reinforcement [18,41,42,51]. However, in two of the four studies, alcohol expectancies were assessed, which are thought to be more distal to drinking behavior than drinking motives. Individuals may hold certain expectancies about drinking but still not drink, while motivations are thought to be necessary for drinking to occur [4,1113]. Indeed, several studies showed that social and enhancement motives were either unrelated to drinking alone [9,30,65] or negatively associated with it [11,31]. Furthermore, solitary drinking was not related to drinking during PA [8]. While the meta-analytical results between solitary drinking and the ‘positive reinforcement’ factor were significant, the effect size was nearly three times smaller than that of the ‘negative reinforcement’ factor (see Table 3).

Heterogeneity in effect sizes and moderator variables

As shown in Table 3, the Q values for heterogeneity, which represent variation in the true effect size across studies, were significant for all factors. Table 4 shows results for moderator analyses. As can be seen, none of the moderators (i.e. age group, study quality, and differing solitary drinking definitions) were significant.

Table 4.

Moderation analyses.

Moderator n Point estimate Confidence interval Q P
Superordinate factor Definition
Alcohol consumption Alone 10 0.24 0.08, 0.38 0.08 0.77
All others 6 0.21 0.13, 0.29
Drinking problems Alone 10 0.23 0.10, 0.36 0.24 0.62
All others 6 0.20 0.13, 0.26
Negative affect Alone 4 0.20 0.11, 0.29 0.03 0.87
All others 5 0.21 0.15, 0.27
Social discomfort Alone 2 0.23 0.07, 0.38 3.07 0.08
All others 2 0.07 0.01, 0.14
Negative reinforcement Alone 7 0.29 0.26, 0.31 0.24 0.62
All others 6 0.27 0.18, 0.35
Positive reinforcement Alone 6 0.13 0.04, 0.21 0.72 0.40
All others 4 0.06 −0.08, 0.19
Age groupa
Alcohol consumption Adolescents 7 0.31 0.12, 0.47 2.14 0.14
Young adults 9 0.16 0.08, 0.24
Drinking problems Adolescents 4 0.33 0.11, 0.53 1.85 0.17
Young adults 12 0.18 0.13, 0.22
Negative affect Adolescents 2 0.19 0.06, 0.32 0.14 0.71
Young adults 7 0.22 0.16, 0.27
Negative reinforcement Adolescents 4 0.29 0.25, 0.32 0.13 0.72
Young adults 9 0.27 0.22, 0.33
Positive reinforcement Adolescents 4 0.17 0.06, 0.27 3.07 0.08
Young adults 6 0.04 −0.05, 0.14
Quality levelb Z-value
Alcohol consumption 16 0.03 −0.12 to 0.18 0.40 0.69
Drinking problems 16 −0.04 −0.17 to 0.10 −0.52 0.60
Negative affect 9 −0.01 −0.07 to 0.04 −0.45 0.65
Social discomfort 4 0.06 −0.06 to 0.18 0.80 0.42
Negative reinforcement 13 −0.04 −0.09 to 0.01 −1.43 0.15
Positive reinforcement 10 −0.02 −0.12 to 0.08 −0.41 0.69
a

Social discomfort only contained young adult samples, and thus was not included in these analyses.

b

Quality level analyses were run using meta-regression with quality level designated as a continuous variable.

Publication bias

Although Begg’s rank correlation test resulted in null findings, there was evidence of publication bias for the factors of ‘alcohol consumption’, ‘drinking problems’, ‘social discomfort’, and ‘positive reinforcement’ based on visual inspection of funnel plots and trim-and-fill analyses. These analyses suggested the possibility of six unpublished studies for ‘alcohol consumption’ and ‘drinking problems’, and two unpublished studies for ‘social discomfort’ and ‘positive reinforcement’. Imputation of these studies increased the effect sizes from r = 0.23 to 0.34 for ‘alcohol consumption’ and from r = 0.23 to r = 0.30 for ‘drinking problems’, and lowered it from r = 0.10 to 0.06 for ‘positive reinforcement’ and from r = 0.17 to 0.11 for ‘social discomfort’ (see Supporting information).

DISCUSSION

The current study provides results of a systematic review along with findings from the first meta-analysis to evaluate the strength and reliability of presumed solitary drinking effects. Findings demonstrated that solitary drinking is a reliable indicator of increased risk for AUD in adolescents and young adults. Correlational studies showed that it is associated with increased alcohol use and problems, as well as issues in academic, legal, interpersonal, emotional, and physical health domains (e.g. [5,7,8,18]). Importantly, longitudinal studies showed that solitary drinking predicts future alcohol problems after controlling for baseline risk factors (e.g. alcohol consumption/problems) [5,8,54,63]. Meta-analytical results showed small but significant effect sizes between solitary drinking and increased alcohol consumption, drinking problems, NA, and social discomfort. Notably, after accounting for publication bias, there were medium effect sizes between solitary drinking and increased alcohol consumption and drinking problems. The strengths of all relationships varied significantly across studies for all factors, but this heterogeneity was not explained by age group, suggesting that solitary drinking is problematic in both adolescents and young adults. The heterogeneity across studies was also not explained by differing solitary drinking definitions or study quality. Future studies are indicated to further explore this heterogeneity.

Understanding why individuals drink alone is essential for developing effective interventions. Results suggested that the most compelling theory to date is a motivational model in which adolescents and young adults drink alone to cope with NA. Solitary drinking in these individuals has been linked to coping motives over and above other drinking motives (i.e. social, enhancement, and conformity motives), beliefs in alcohol’s ability to mitigate NA, the inability to resist drinking during NA, and problems coping with discomfort or regulating emotions (e.g. [6,8,9,11]). Indeed, our meta-analytical results revealed a small-to-medium effect size for studies examining these relationships. Further, adolescent and young adult solitary drinkers reported more NA and NA-inducing experiences (e.g. social discomfort) than social-only drinkers (e.g. [8,15]), perhaps making it more likely for them to use alcohol to cope with NA. There is research to support this: a recent longitudinal study demonstrated that NA (e.g. depression) prospectively predicted solitary drinking in young adults, even after accounting for baseline solitary drinking [54]. While all studies to date are correlational, taken together these results suggest that negative reinforcement processes may be a primary mechanism driving solitary drinking among both adolescents and young adults.

Conversely, positive reinforcement was not as strongly related to solitary drinking. Most studies showed that enhancement and social motives were unrelated [9,30,65] or negatively associated with drinking alone [11,31], and two of the four studies that found a positive relationship assessed alcohol expectancies [18,41], which are more distally related to drinking than motives [12,13]. While the meta-analytical results indicated a significant relationship between solitary drinking and the ‘positive reinforcement’ factor, the effect size was nearly three times smaller than that of the ‘negative reinforcement’ factor. Experimental data are needed, but the existing correlational data converge to support drinking to cope motives as a likely predictive and contributing factor for solitary drinking while positive reinforcement is a weaker motive.

In summary, while solitary drinking is less common than social drinking, it has substantial negative implications for adolescents and young adults who engage in it. The most compelling theory for why individuals drink alone is that they are doing so to cope with NA. However, a limitation of this review and the literature more generally on solitary drinking is that nearly all studies use correlational designs. More rigorous tests are needed to understand the mechanisms underlying solitary drinking and the pathways by which drinking alone leads to adverse outcomes. For instance, analysis of longitudinal repeated-measures data would permit the study of dynamic changes in the sequence of events leading to solitary drinking and from solitary drinking to negative outcomes. These study designs, although still correlational, can provide stronger information about the causal processes operating in the day-to-day lives of young drinkers by demonstrating temporal precedence. They would be particularly helpful with adolescent populations, as it is not possible to conduct alcohol administration studies with underage drinkers. Experimental designs using vignettes or alcohol administration protocols could be conducted with young adults to test the hypothesized motivational model of solitary alcohol consumption. For example, mood manipulations could be used to induce NA to determine whether it increases the preference to drink alone among individuals with a history of solitary drinking. We are currently conducting such a study (https://osf.io/e7yxn/register/5771ca429ad5a1020de2872e) to test for a causal relationship between NA, drinking to cope motives, and solitary drinking among young adults. In general, intensive longitudinal designs and experimental protocols will aid in our understanding of the factors that evoke solitary drinking and help establish the necessary directional and causal relationships between negative reinforcement and drinking alone.

It is interesting to note that while drinking to cope seems to be the primary motivation for solitary drinking among adolescents and young adults, drinking alone may not be effective in ameliorating NA. For instance, in laboratory studies, young adults reported increased NA and decreased PA in solitary versus social drinking contexts [5961,7274]. It will be important to replicate these laboratory findings in samples that include only solitary drinkers, however, as drinking alone in these laboratory studies might be an aversive experience for social-only drinkers. It is noteworthy that solitary drinking dampened the pleasurable effects of alcohol in ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies among individuals who chose to drink alone [75,76]. If future studies corroborate this, the findings could have important treatment implications. For instance, it might be helpful to challenge adolescents’ and young adults’ beliefs about the effectiveness of solitary drinking in ameliorating NA by describing studies that have examined in-the-moment affective consequences of drinking alone, and provide instruction on more effective ways to cope with NA.

Some other considerations also may improve future research. First, standardizing the definition of solitary drinking as drinking while alone would create a more consistent measure for establishing associations between solitary drinking and other variables. Second, more research is needed to replicate initial findings showing that early solitary drinking predicts increased alcohol involvement and the development of alcohol problems over time beyond the influence of other related risk factors. Third, to our knowledge, no studies have assessed drinking motives specifically for solitary alcohol use but rather have examined associations between solitary drinking and drinking motives with no context specified. Future studies are indicated that explicitly ask about solitary drinking motives. Fourth, while problems associated with solitary drinking are well established, other areas, such as associated demographic variables, remain under-investigated or equivocal. Studies investigating demographic variables and other individual differences linked to solitary drinking are indicated, so that intervention programs can be focused on those who need it most. Finally, solitary use of other drugs has also been linked to negative outcomes (e.g. [5,77]), and future studies should continue to explore the role of negative reinforcement in this drug use behavior. In general, more research is needed to fully understand the mechanisms driving solitary drinking and who may be most at risk to engage in this risky drinking pattern. Such research will shed light on a minority of adolescents and young adults who appear to be especially vulnerable to heavy drinking and the development of alcohol problems.

Supplementary Material

Supp Material

Table S1 PRISMA checklist

Figure S1 Example of a full electronic search method

Figure S2 Funnel plots of superordinate factors including imputed studies

Table S1 Duvall and Tweedie’s Trim-and-fill analyses

Table S2 Begg’s Rank correlation test

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (R01 AA025936 to Kasey Creswell).

Footnotes

1

Data were obtained for 12 independent samples, data no longer existed for 13 samples, two authors did not respond to multiple queries, and two authors stated it was too difficult to extract the requested data.

2

We also conducted the meta-analysis omitting studies that defined solitary drinking as anything other than drinking while alone, and results were similar.

3

Gonzalez & Skewes (2013) found age differences by gender, such that solitary heavy drinking men were significantly older than social heavy-drinking men.

4

This is assumed to be a general association, not unplanned sex within a drinking event.

5

Swahn & Donovan 2006 [56] and Swahn & Donovan 2005 [64] used the same sample.

Declaration of interests

None.

References

  • 1.Fairbairn CE, Sayette MA A social-attributional analysis of alcohol response. Psychol Bull 2014; 140: 1361–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Johnston LD, Bachman JG, O’Malley PM Monitoring the Future: Questionnaire Responses from the Nation’s High School Seniors, 2011. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan, Institute for Social Research; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Johnston LD, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, Schulenberg JE Monitoring the Future: National Results on Adolescent Drug Use. Overview of Key Findings, 2005. Bethesda, MD: National Institutes of Health, US Department of Health and Human Services; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engels R Why do young people drink? A review of drinking motives. Clin Psychol Rev 2005; 25: 841–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Tucker JS, Ellickson PL, Collins RL, Klein DJ Does solitary substance use increase adolescents’ risk for poor psychosocial and behavioral outcomes? A 9-year longitudinal study comparing solitary and social users. Psychol Addict Behav 2006; 20: 363–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Williams CL, Vik PW, Wong MM Distress tolerance in social versus solitary college student drinkers. Addict Behav 2015; 50: 89–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Skrzynski C, Creswell KG, Bachrach RL, Chung T Social discomfort moderates the relationship between drinking in response to negative affect and solitary drinking in underage drinkers. Addict Behav 2018; 78: 124–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Creswell KG, Chung T, Clark DB, Martin CS Solitary alcohol use in teens is associated with drinking in response to negative affect and predicts alcohol problems in young adulthood. Clin Psychol Sci 2014; 2: 602–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gonzalez VM, Collins RL, Bradizza CM Solitary and social heavy drinking, suicidal ideation, and drinking motives in underage college drinkers. Addict Behav 2009; 34: 993–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Creswell KG, Chung T, Wright AG, Clark DB, Black JJ, Martin CS Personality, negative affect coping, and drinking alone: a structural equation modeling approach to examine correlates of adolescent solitary drinking. Addiction 2015; 110: 775–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Cooper ML Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: development and validation of a four-factor model. Psychol Assess 1994; 6: 117–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: a motivational model of alcohol use. J Pers Soc Psychol 1995; 69: 990–1005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cox WM, Klinger E A motivational model of alcohol use. J Abnorm Psychol 1988; 97: 168–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tomlinson KL, Brown SA Self-medication or social learning? A comparison of models to predict early adolescent drinking. Addict Behav 2012; 37: 179–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gonzalez VM, Skewes MC Solitary heavy drinking, social relationships, and negative mood regulation in college drinkers. Addict Res Theory 2013; 21: 285–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mason WA, Stevens AL, Fleming CB A systematic review of research on adolescent solitary alcohol and marijuana use in the United States. Addiction 2020; 115: 19–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Windle M Alcohol use among adolescents and young adults. Population 2003; 45: 15–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Christiansen M, Vik PW, Jarchow A College student heavy drinking in social contexts versus alone. Addict Behav 2002; 27: 393–404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bjork JM, Knutson B, Fong GW, Caggiano DM, Bennett SM, Hommer DW Incentive-elicited brain activation in adolescents: similarities and differences from young adults. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 1793–802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Markiewicz D, Lawford H, Doyle AB, Haggart N Developmental differences in adolescents’ and young adults’ use of mothers, fathers, best friends, and romantic partners to fulfill attachment needs. J Youth Adolesc 2006; 35: 121–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med 2009; 151: 264–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR Computer Software. Comprehensive Meta-analysis, Version 2. Englewood, NJ: Biostat, Inc.; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Segerstrom SC, Miller GE Psychological stress and the human immune system: a meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychol Bull 2004; 130: 601–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Raudenbush SW Random effects models. In: Cooper H, Hedges LV, editors. The Handbook of Research Synthesis. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation; 1994, pp. 301–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Begg CB, Mazumdar M Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics 1994; 50: 1088–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Duval S, Tweedie R Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2000; 56: 455–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Gonzalez VM Association of solitary binge drinking and suicidal behavior among emerging adult college students. Psychol Addict Behav 2012; 26: 609–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Armeli S, O’Hara RE, Ehrenberg E, Sullivan TP, Tennen H Episode-specific drinking-to-cope motivation, daily mood, and fatigue-related symptoms among college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2014; 75: 766–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Armeli S, O’Hara RE, Covault J, Scott DM, Tennen H Episode-specific drinking-to-cope motivation and next-day stress-reactivity. Anxiety Stress Coping 2016; 29: 673–84. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Armeli S, Covault J, Tennen H Long-term changes in the effects of episode-specific drinking to cope motivation on daily well-being. Psychol Addict Behav 2018; 32: 715–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.O’Hara RE, Armeli S, Tennen H College students’ drinking motives and social-contextual factors: comparing associations across levels of analysis. Psychol Addict Behav 2015; 29: 420–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Buckner JD, Terlecki MA Social anxiety and alcohol-related impairment: the mediational impact of solitary drinking. Addict Behav 2016; 58: 7–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Chalder M, Elgar FJ, Bennet P Drinking and motivations to drink among adolescent children of parents with alcohol problems. Alcohol Alcohol 2005; 41: 107–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kask K, Markina A With whom did you drink last time? An analysis of adolescents’ alcohol use. Annu Res Rev Biol 2014; 4: 174–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kouzis AC, Labouvie EW Use intensity, functional elaboration, and contextual constraint as facets of adolescent alcohol and marijuana use. Psychol Addict Behav 1992; 6: 188–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Lowman C Facts for planning no. 3: US teenage alcohol use in unsupervised social settings. Alcohol Res Health 1981; 6: 46–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mayer RR, Forster JL, Murray DM, Wagenaar AC Social settings and situations of underage drinking. J Stud Alcohol 1998; 59: 207–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Okulicz-Kozaryn K, Borucka A The patterns of alcohol use among Warsaw adolescents across 20 years-from 1988 to 2008. Int J Alcohol Drug Res 2013; 2: 37–44. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.O’Hare TM Drinking in college: consumption patterns, problems, sex differences and legal drinking age. J Stud Alcohol 1990; 51: 536–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dauber S, Hogue A, Paulson JF, Leiferman JA Typologies of alcohol use in white and African American adolescent girls. Subst Use Misuse 2009; 44: 1121–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tucker JS, Pedersen ER, Miles JN, Ewing BA, Shih RA, D’Amico EJ Alcohol and marijuana use in middle school: comparing solitary and social-only users. J Adolesc Health 2014; 55: 744–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stewart C, Power TG Ethnic, social class, and gender differences in adolescent drinking: examining multiple aspects of consumption. J Adolesc Res 2003; 18: 575–98. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Strunin L, Demissie S Cultural identification and alcohol use among ‘Black’ adolescents. Subst Use Misuse 2001; 36: 2025–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Pedersen ER, LaBrie JW, Kilmer JR Before you slip into the night, you’ll want something to drink: exploring the reasons for prepartying behavior among college student drinkers. Issues Ment Health Nurs 2009; 30: 354–63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.West R, Drummond C, Eames K Alcohol consumption, problem drinking and antisocial behaviour in a sample of college students. Br J Addict 1990; 85: 479–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gibbons S, Wylie ML, Echterling L, French J Situational factors related to rural adolescent alcohol use. Int J Addict 1986; 21: 1183–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Falcón CM, García EG, Berrio AR Daily tobacco consumption and binge drinking in Roma adolescents in three Spanish areas. Rev Cercet si Interv So 2014; 44: 132–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Russell ST, Driscoll AK, Truong N Adolescent same-sex romantic attractions and relationships: implications for substance use and abuse. Am J Public Health 2002; 92: 198–202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tucker JS, Ellickson PL, Klein DJ Understanding differences in substance use among bisexual and heterosexual young women. Womens Health Issues 2008; 18: 387–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Rivers I, Noret N Well-being among same-sex- and opposite-sex-attracted youth at school. Sch Psychol Rev 2008; 37: 174–87. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Terry-McElrath YM, Stern SA, Patrick ME Do alcohol use reasons and contexts differentiate adolescent high-intensity drinking? Data from US high school seniors, 2005–2016. Psychol Addict Behav 2017; 31: 775–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Bailey SL, Rachal JV Dimensions of adolescent problem drinking. J Stud Alcohol 1993; 54: 555–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Keough MT, O’Connor RM, Sherry SB, Stewart SH Context counts: solitary drinking explains the association between depressive symptoms and alcohol-related problems in undergraduates. Addict Behav 2015; 42: 216–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Bilevicius E, Single A, Rapinda KK, Bristow LA, Keough MT Frequent solitary drinking mediates the associations between negative affect and harmful drinking in emerging adults. Addict Behav 2018; 87: 115–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Swahn MH, Simon TR, Hammig BJ, Guerrero JL Alcohol-consumption behaviors and risk for physical fighting and injuries among adolescent drinkers. Addict Behav 2004; 29: 959–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Swahn MH, Donovan JE Correlates and predictors of violent behavior among adolescent drinkers. J Adolesc Health 2004; 34: 480–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Swahn MH, Donovan JE Alcohol and violence: comparison of the psychosocial correlates of adolescent involvement in alcohol-related physical fighting versus other physical fighting. Addict Behav 2006; 31: 2014–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.O’Hara R, Armeli S, Scott D, Covault J, Tennen H Perceived racial discrimination and negative-mood-related drinking among African American college students. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2015; 76: 229–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Doty P, de Wit H Effect of setting on the reinforcing and subjective effects of ethanol in social drinkers. Psychopharmacology 1995; 118: 19–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Del Porto JA, Masur J The effects of alcohol, THC and diazepam in two different social settings: a study with human volunteers. Res Comm Psychol Psychiatr Behav 1984; 9: 201–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Lindman R Social and solitary drinking: effects on consumption and mood in male social drinkers. Physiol Behav 1982; 28: 1093–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Keough MT, Battista SR, O’Connor RM, Sherry SB, Stewart SH Getting the party started—alone: solitary predrinking mediates the effect of social anxiety on alcohol-related problems. Addict Behav 2016; 55: 19–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Degenhardt L, Romaniuk H, Coffey C, Hall WD, Swift W, Carlin JB, et al. Does the social context of early alcohol use affect risky drinking in adolescents? Prospective cohort study. BMC Public Health 2015; 15: 1137–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Swahn MH, Donovan JE Predictors of fighting attributed to alcohol use among adolescent drinkers. Addict Behav 2005; 30: 1317–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Blevins CE, Abrantes AM, Stephens RS The relationship between situational determinants of use and drinking motives. Addict Res Theory 2018; 26: 28–34. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.O’Hara RE, Boynton MH, Scott DM, Armeli S, Tennen H, Williams C, et al. Drinking to cope among African American college students: an assessment of episode-specific motives. Psychol Addict Behav 2014; 28: 671–81. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Beck KH, Ahmed A, Farkas ZA A descriptive analysis of the social context of drinking among first-time DUI offenders. Traffic Inj Prev 2011; 12: 306–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Beck KH, Summons TG, Thombs DL A factor analytic study of social context of drinking in a high school population. Psychol Addict Behav 1991; 5: 66–77. [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Beck KH, Thombs DL, Summons TG The social context of drinking scales: construct validation and relationship to indicants of abuse in an adolescent population. Addict Behav 1993; 18: 159–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Koposov RA, Ruchkin VV, Eisemann M, Sidorov PI Alcohol use in adolescents from northern Russia: the role of the social context. Alcohol Alcohol 2002; 37: 297–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Thombs DL, Beck KH, Mahoney CA, Bromley MD, Bezon KM Social context, sensation seeking, and teen-age alcohol abuse. J Sch Health 1994; 64: 73–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Pliner P, Cappell H Modification of affective consequences of alcohol: a comparison of social and solitary drinking. J Abnorm Psychol 1974; 83: 418–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Kirkpatrick MG, De Wit H In the company of others: social factors alter acute alcohol effects. Psychopharmacology 2013; 230: 215–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Lindman R, Lindfors B, Dahla E, Toivola H Alcohol and ambience: social and environmental determinants of intake and mood. Alcohol Alcohol 1987; 1: 385–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Piasecki TM, Wood PK, Shiffman S, Sher KJ, Heath AC Responses to alcohol and cigarette use during ecologically assessed drinking episodes. Psychopharmacology 2012; 223: 331–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Ray LA, Miranda R Jr., Tidey JW, McGeary JE, MacKillop J, Gwaltney CJ, et al. Polymorphisms of the μ-opioid receptor and dopamine D receptor genes and subjective responses to alcohol in the natural environment. J Abnorm Psychol 2010; 119: 115–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Creswell KG, Chung T, Clark DB, Martin CS Solitary cannabis use in adolescence as a correlate and predictor of cannabis problems. Drug Alcohol Depend 2015; 156: 120–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supp Material

Table S1 PRISMA checklist

Figure S1 Example of a full electronic search method

Figure S2 Funnel plots of superordinate factors including imputed studies

Table S1 Duvall and Tweedie’s Trim-and-fill analyses

Table S2 Begg’s Rank correlation test

RESOURCES