Abstract
Surgical treatment of tumors, epileptic foci or of vascular origin, requires a detailed individual pre-surgical workup and intra-operative surveillance of brain functions to minimize the risk of post-surgical neurological deficits and decline of quality of life. Most attention is attributed to language, motor functions, and perception. However, higher cognitive functions such as social cognition, personality, and the sense of self may be affected by brain surgery. To date, the precise localization and the network patterns of brain regions involved in such functions are not yet fully understood, making the assessment of risks of related post-surgical deficits difficult. It is in the interest of neurosurgeons to understand with which neural systems related to selfhood and personality they are interfering during surgery. Recent neuroscience research using virtual reality and clinical observations suggest that the insular cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and temporo-parietal junction are important components of a neural system dedicated to self-consciousness based on multisensory bodily processing, including exteroceptive and interoceptive cues (bodily self-consciousness (BSC)). Here, we argue that combined extra- and intra-operative approaches using targeted cognitive testing, functional imaging and EEG, virtual reality, combined with multisensory stimulations, may contribute to the assessment of the BSC and related cognitive aspects. Although the usefulness of particular biomarkers, such as cardiac and respiratory signals linked to virtual reality, and of heartbeat evoked potentials as a surrogate marker for intactness of multisensory integration for intra-operative monitoring has to be proved, systemic and automatized testing of BSC in neurosurgical patients will improve future surgical outcome.
Keywords: Self, Consciousness, Social, Brain surgery, Neuropsychology, Bodily self
Introduction
What do neurosurgeons answer to patients, who are supposed to undergo elective intracranial procedures and who are asking “Doc, will I be the same person after surgery?” The traditional concerns of the neurosurgical community of course take into account potential post-operative personality changes in their patients, but compared to established feedback based on neuroscientific data in language, movement, and perception, feedback about potential changes to personality and the sense of self is not yet established. This is the case despite the widespread and important role of neuropsychologists in patient evaluation and increasing research on the sense of self. During consultations and consentment conversations, the issues of language or motor function, or of cranial nerve function, respectively, are regularly aborded. Usually, we come closest to the subject of personality, when we are explaining the various elements of language and speech, of receptive and expressive language centers, and the respective cortical and subcortical structures involved in its organization. Awake craniotomy may be suggested in order to peri-operatively assess language functions. The examination and mapping of personality and sense of self is not discussed on a routine basis, however, and not yet based on neuroscience data. The frequency of post-operative deficits of the sense of the self is difficult to quantify, as modern post-operative clinical neuropsychological evaluation does not incorporate specific dedicated tests of the sense of the bodily and the cognitive self. However, the authors are convinced that these deficits are underestimated in the post-operative period and may affect dramatically the social life and the working capacity of neurosurgical patients.
It is the goal of this perspective article to highlight the present state of knowledge about the underlying networks of brain structures contributing to a person’s sense of self and what deems possible to be added to the portfolio of pre-surgical evaluation and intra-operative surveillance of functions related to personality and of the human sense of self.
Self-consciousness
Self-consciousness is one of the most astonishing features of the human mind and has been approached in diverse fields such as philosophy, psychology, psychiatry, and more recently in cognitive neuroscience. However, self-consciousness is notoriously difficult to define, encompassing emotional, spatial self, verbal, conceptual, and social aspects among others [71, 83]. William James was one of the first who differentiated two critical notions of self: a so-called physical self and a mental self [65], and several current cognitive neuroscience approaches to the sense of self often keep a related dichotomy and target two distinct and separated notions of self (i.e., [39, 44, 50]). Here, we juxtapose a bodily self, based on mainly perceptual multisensory and sensorimotor mechanisms, with a cognitive self, based on language, memory, and other cognitive functions (i.e., [39, 44, 50]).
Bodily self and bodily self-consciousness
On the one hand, researchers have investigated a fundamental aspect of the self and its link with bodily perception: the bodily self. This implicit and pre-reflexive experience of being the subject of a given experience is proposed to be a low-level self-representation, based on perceptual mechanisms of multisensory bodily signals [83]. The term bodily self-consciousness (BSC) has recently been proposed [20, 47], and core BSC features are self-identification (i.e., the degree to which an organism identifies with the content of a global body representation), body ownership (i.e., the feeling that the physical body and its parts, such as its hands and feet, belong to “me” and are “my” body), self-location (i.e., a determinate volume in space, normally localized within the bodily boundaries as represented), first-person perspective (i.e., the experience of the position from where “I” experience to perceive the world), and the sense of agency (i.e., the feeling of being in control of one’s own actions) [15, 17, 18].
BSC research has investigated brain mechanisms that process body-related sensory signals originating from the space outside the body (i.e., exteroceptive bodily signals). However, the brain also receives other bodily, interoceptive, signals from the inner organs (i.e., cardiac, respiratory, and intestinal). Importantly, several authors have made an influential argument that the interoceptive brain mechanisms play a fundamental role in BSC [8, 11, 87]. Based on philosophical intuition [32, 33] and indirect evidence [8, 25, 34], this proposal for a neural BSC system based on interoceptive signals (i-BSC [87]) was developed by Antonio Damasio and AD Craig and has recently received experimental support [36, 38, 40, 41, 43]. Our own work was directly inspired by these proposals and driven by the ambition to investigate whether exteroceptive and interoceptive bodily signals contribute to BSC. For this, we developed a series of virtual reality setups that allowed to expose participants to multisensory stimulation including interoceptive as well as exteroceptive cues, revealing that this type of stimulation also allowed to manipulate BSC [1, 3, 7, 15, 87, 92]. Moreover, visual stimuli, which are presented synchronously or asynchronously with the frequency of the individual’s heartbeat, are interesting stimuli to investigate awareness [106]. Importantly, these findings suggest that BSC should be conceived as an integrated single system (x-BSC [87]), based on exteroceptive and interoceptive bodily signals. Merging behavioral, neuroimaging, neurological, and electrophysiological evidence, the x-BSC system has been proposed to reconcile the two largely separated views in terms of interoceptive and exteroceptive bodily contributions to BSC (for an extensive proposal why and how the integration between interoceptive and exteroceptive information is thought to be important for the sense of self, see the recent work from Park and Blanke [87]).
Cognitive self
The second notion of the self includes the many cognitive processes that are associated with the self (i.e., the cognitive self) [83, 99]. As opposed to the pre-reflective and perceptual lower-level bodily aspects of the bodily self, the cognitive self is rather of a reflective and integrated nature and involves cognitive functions such as language, memory, mental imagery, and symbolic and abstract aspects [83]. In other words, the cognitive self regroups higher-order cognitive processes allowing a subject to reflect and understand whether a specific mental content is related to themselves and their own person. Different cognitive components related to the cognitive self can be investigated such as the recognition of self-related cues, i.e., the visual recognition of one’s own face [129] or the auditory recognition of one’s own name or one’s own voice [12, 94, 124]. This also includes memory components such as the recall of personally relevant information, or of one’s own personality, by assessing episodic and autobiographical memories [25, 26, 53, 102]. The cognitive self, as defined here, overlaps with previous notions of such higher-level notions of self, such as James’ mental self, Damasio’s extended self [39, 65], or Gallagher’s [50] and Dennett’s narrative self [44]. It should also be noted that, although the conceptual distinction and neural mechanisms are distinct between bodily and cognitive self, both notions overlap. For example, multisensory stimulations have been shown to not only impact the bodily self, but also the cognitive self, including episodic memory [25], language processing [27], and spatial navigation [78]. Accordingly, we have recently shown that viewing the body during the encoding of scenes in VR facilitates later episodic memory for these events and impacts the neural correlates [25]. Moreover, an enhanced state of BSC for a self-avatar was found to improve spatial navigation performance and to alter activity in the medial temporal lobe [78]. During a body swap illusion, recent work showed flexibility of the self-concept in function of the strength of the illusory body ownership; such illusion was also modulating episodic memory [125]. The self, like many representations in humans, requires constant sub- or unconscious monitoring of the physical and the physiological state, i.e., by neural processing of incoming interoceptive (signals from the heart, respiration, and from viscera) signals and to match them with exteroceptive (i.e., audio-visual and tactile) data [21]. As the integrative target organ is the brain, in accordance with present-date concepts, neurosurgeons should be familiar with basic principles of the construction of the self and take potential impact of intracranial procedures on the self of their patients into consideration.
Experimental BSC paradigms
One approach to test the integrity of BSC is to investigate the perception of multisensory bodily stimuli by exposing people to conflicting sensorimotor inputs using different techniques (e.g., VR) and experimental paradigms. To highlight the importance that the multisensory integration processes are thought to play in BSC, we are describing below two classical paradigms.
The rubber hand illusion
In the so-called rubber hand illusion, the experimenter strokes a fake hand by hand or with a stick or another object simultaneously with the participant’s own yet hidden hand. This application of visuo-tactile stimulation is carried out for some time (1–2 min or longer) and leads to alterations of BSC, especially illusory touch and hand ownership for the seen hand. Such BSC changes are decreased or not observed when the visuo-tactile stimulations are asynchronous, when the orientation in space of the participant’s hand and the fake hand is misaligned, or when a control object is stroked (i.e., a box instead of a fake hand), suggesting the importance of multisensory integration in such phenomenon [22, 119]. These alterations in BSC are evaluated by using questionnaires, by “proprioceptive drift” measures (i.e., participants perceive their hand to be at a position that is displaced towards the fake hand [24]), skin conductance response changes [4], and multisensory measures such as cross-modal congruency effects [6] and peripersonal space measures [82]. The RHI paradigm has also been developed using VR [109, 127] and also allowed online manipulation of interoceptive feedback using cardio-visual stimulation (i.e., Suzuki et al. found that illusory ownership of a virtual hand is observed if a virtual hand is illuminated synchronously with respect to the participant’s online detected cardiac signals [123]).
The full-body illusion
The full-body illusion (FBI) paradigm generalizes principles from the rubber hand illusion and applies them to the entire body, usually using mixed reality. The test person is wearing a VR helmet with a head-mounted display, through which a virtual avatar of that very same person can be seen from the back and as projected at a distance of 2 m in front of the participant [72]. Then, the experimenter applies tactile stimulations on the subject’s back and such stimulations will be displayed to the participant in the VR helmet and as seen online on the back of the avatar in VR. This is done either synchronously or asynchronously (with an additional 500-ms delay), as in the rubber hand illusion paradigm. The synchronous condition gives rise to changes in BSC, including illusory self-identification, illusory self-location over the virtual body [6, 47, 72, 95], and changes in the first-person perspective [63, 97]. These alterations in BSC are evaluated by using questionnaires, by drift in self-location using walking responses [72] (i.e., participants perceive their body to be at a position that is displaced towards the position of the avatar) or mental imagery responses [63], skin conductance response changes [47], and multisensory measures such as cross-modal congruency effects [6] and peripersonal space measures [82, 115]. Several variants of the full-body illusion exist [47], and online manipulation of interoceptive feedback using cardio-visual stimulation has also been used to induce changes in self-identification and self-location [7] (see the section “Bodily self: full-body illusion based on exteroceptive and interoceptive signals”). A similar paradigm using a first-person perspective rather than a third one has been developed and led to the perceptual illusion of body swapping [95]. These BSC paradigms are of clinical relevance and have been automatized and translated to several clinical applications, leading to novel neuroprosthetics chronic leg pain therapies for patients with neuropathic pain in spinal cord injury [98], complex regional pain syndrome [120], bionic limbs and peripheral nerve stimulation [103], and severe leg pain and spinal cord stimulation [121].
Monitoring and mapping in neurosurgery
Direct cortical stimulation (DCS) via the application of electric currents, and stimulation of the cerebral white matter allow for functional allocation and for continuous peri-operative mapping and monitoring of the intactness of the concerned tracts and functions [45, 46, 59, 60, 84, 101, 105]. The introduction of phase reversal for the precise allocation of the central sulcus, in combination with transcranial or direct subsequent motor mapping and motor evoked potential (MEP) monitoring, has allowed to define entry zones for surgery in and around the central region and for the placement of subdural strip electrodes for continuous MEP monitoring during i.e. tumor surgery or during resective epilepsy surgical procedures in the vicinity of the somatosensory cortex [23, 28, 93]. Subsequent developments in monitoring and mapping of cerebral functions, of white matter fiber tracts, and cranial/peripheral nerves have since become a mainstay of functional preservation during neurosurgical procedures [29, 45, 46, 79, 81, 114]. Be it performed under general anesthesia, or during awake craniotomies, the development of these techniques has allowed to push the limit for resective intracranial (and intraspinal) procedures toward higher resection rates, prolonged progression-free survival (PFS), and better functional outcomes and quality of life (QOL) [43, 45, 80]. However, most attention is so far focused on brain regions controlling language, motor functions, and perception (i.e., mapping the so-called eloquent cortex) [29, 45]. Recent developments include the more routine use of monitoring of the visual pathways with visual evoked potentials (VEP) recording, olfactory monitoring, and the integration of stimulation-mapping tools into suction and resection devices [22, 58, 77, 101]. Language mapping under general anesthesia is under clinical evaluation still [73].
Finally, the Bispectral index™ (BIS) can be applied during neurosurgical procedures to monitor the hypnotic state of the patient during the surgery. It is based on various electroencephalogram (EEG) parameters recorded by the mean signals of scalp electrodes such as time and frequency domain components and returns a single BIS number [118]. This score represents the depth of anesthesia: the lower the score, the deeper the anesthesia. Typical values would be BIS > 97 for awake unsedated individuals, BIS = 60 for unconsciousness induced by drugs, BIS = 30 when EEG shows burst suppression, and BIS = 0 for a flat-line EEG. During intracranial surgeries, ideally, the BIS should range between 40 and 60. The online analyses of EEG are routinely performed using sensorimotor monitoring or visual evoked potentials, as aforementioned. The same type of analyses can be adapted to scrutinize residual cognitive cerebral functions like it has been done for measuring phonological processing of language [73] or analyses of epileptic discharges [128].
Anatomical substrates for the bodily and the cognitive self
Despite the abovementioned developments, stimulation of and/or damage to the insular cortex (i.e., direct physical, or due to ischemic lesions) due to resection of insular pathologies has been reported to be associated with changes in personality and self, but also autonomous regulation, emotional, and sexual functions [2, 33]. Given the role of the insula and related structures in BSC, and when aiming at mapping and preserving related aspects of sense of self and personality, it is thus mandatory to investigate how the human insula and related structures in the context of multisensory bodily signals and the role of such processing in the patient’s self-consciousness and how it relates to the two main aspects of the sense of self, the bodily self (BSC) and the cognitive self [35, 87]. Whereas the latter is based on different cognitive systems such as recognition, language, memory, or mental imagery, the former is based on multisensory perception, including interoceptive and exteroceptive information. However, it is currently unknown whether the potential post-surgical mental changes in personality and sense of self relate to the bodily or cognitive self or to both. Pre- and peri-operative investigation of the bodily self and the cognitive self and especially their dependence on interoceptive bodily cues as mediated by the insular cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and temporo-parietal junction (that all have been linked to self-related processing) would require the definition of measurable biomarkers, which are representative for various domains of the human self [41, 87]. The main neural relay structures for ascending interoceptive signals, i.e., from the viscera and glands, are located in the insula and in the cingulum [8, 87].
To maintain normal BSC, with normal self-identification with the body, self-location and first-person perspective multisensory perception and integration of exteroceptive and interoceptive information is important. Signals of such external or exteroceptive information may be recorded in a dedicated cortical network of multimodal neurons, spanning between the premotor cortex, the inter-parietal cortex, and the temporo-parietal junction [40, 57, 63, 64, 87, 96].
Interoceptive signals in BSC based on visceral afferents and their neural processing play a critical role in self-consciousness and traditionally relate to a different neural system [20, 31, 33, 36, 40, 88]. In particular, the insula and cingulate cortex have been proposed as primary cortical projection sites of interoceptive signals and critical for self-consciousness [34, 116]. Recent evidence supports these theoretical proposals [87, 106, 108]. For instance, Park and colleagues reported that spontaneous fluctuation of neural responses to heartbeats in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and angular gyrus predict participants’ visual detection performance [89]. Moreover, they suggested that such neural events time-locked to heartbeats might carry information related to the subjective dimension of conscious visual experience and may relate to the bodily self [88]. More recent studies directly investigated the link between neural responses to heartbeats and the bodily self [7, 21, 90]. Neural responses to heartbeats in the posterior cingulate cortex and insula have been shown to covary with experimentally induced changes in self-identification [90], suggesting that bodily signals and their neural processing in cortical midline structures and insular cortex are associated with the bodily self. There are other sources of continuous interoceptive information, however, that is of relevance for BSC, such as the cyclic breathing signals [1, 3, 15] and gastric signals [8, 126]. However, it is currently still unclear, if this neural system of bodily self (BSC), involving exteroceptive and interoceptive signals [87], serves as sort of a scaffold for higher level functions involved in the cognitive self.
Neural mechanisms of the bodily self
Recent cognitive neuroscience has aimed at revealing neural mechanisms of the bodily and cognitive self. A recent meta-analysis suggests that BSC recruits a network of multisensory brain areas located in the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) region, premotor cortex (PMC), and temporal-parietal junction (TPJ) [20, 25, 56]. These results are based on experimental manipulations of BSC using virtual reality (VR). For instance, tactile stimulation of a subject’s hand/body shown in immersive VR gives rise to illusory self-identification and illusory self-location over a virtual body (i.e., full-body illusion, FBI [6, 47, 72, 95]). In other recent work integrating fMRI with VR, synchronous visuo-tactile stroking of the real and virtual body, inducing illusory self-identification and self-location, was found to activate IPS, PMC, TPJ, and the putamen [56, 96]. Another VR-fMRI study found that TPJ activity (peaking in the posterior superior temporal gyri, the parietal operculum, and the posterior insula) was associated with changes in self-location and in the experienced direction of first-person perspective [63]. The involvement of the insula was recently confirmed by a 7T fMRI study [21] and by functional connectivity analysis [64] showing that the patterns of functional connectivity from the TPJ to the insula and medial prefrontal cortex reflected experimentally induced changes in self-location and first-person perspective.
Conscious access to interoceptive bodily signals has been proposed to inform the basis of selfhood [9, 37, 38, 116, 117]. One potential way to tackle the bodily self is to measure how accurately participants feel their internal body signals (i.e., signals coming from the glands and viscera), a measure also called interoceptive accuracy [51]. Different interoceptive channels can be objectively assessed (e.g., respiratory, rectal, or gastric), but the cardiac interoceptive channel (i.e., cardioception) is arguably the easiest to measure and, therefore, is a classic way to assess interoceptive processes [16, 51, 52]. The most popular tasks measuring cardioception are the heartbeat tracking and the heartbeat discrimination task [66, 110]. During the heartbeat tracking task, participants are asked to silently count their own heartbeats during a given period of time. Meanwhile, a pulse oximeter attached to their index finger (or ECG) tracks the participants’ actual heartbeat. The greater the overlap between the participant’s subjective heartbeat count and the objective recording, the higher the interoceptive accuracy [14, 52, 110]. In the heartbeat discrimination task, participants hear a series of sets of ten sounds, some of which are played synchronously with their heartbeat and others are played with a delay in their heartbeat. Participants are asked to detect whether or not the sounds were played in sync with their heartbeat and higher accuracy in this task suggests better cardiac interoception [14, 51, 66]. In the functional magnetic resonance imaging scanner (fMRI—using a modified version of the discrimination task), attention toward heartbeat classically elicits enhanced activity in the insula, sensorimotor, and cingulate cortices [38]. Moreover, the right anterior insula (AI) activity positively predicts subjects’ interoceptive accuracy on the task. In addition, right AI gray matter volume correlates with both interoceptive accuracy and sensibility, suggesting that this brain region supports a representation of body states, accessible to awareness and potentially contributing to the bodily self [38, 116].
The above anatomical regions are frequently subject to neurosurgical procedures, including the resection of intrinsic tumors, for epilepsy surgery, or during extra-axial approaches to the skull base and to vascular pathologies, with all of them carrying the risk of vascular damage to i.e. the insula or to the cingulum and others. Consideration of potential effects on bodily self, cognitive self, and personality when performing surgery in these regions thus seems not only important, but also possible, adapting the reviewed procedures to neurosurgical patient evaluations.
Effects of surgical resection on personality and mood
Cognitive, perceptual, and motor functions in candidates for brain surgery are usually assessed during pre-operative neuropsychological testing and have proved instrumental to plan, to predict, and to minimize surgery-induced damage and loss of function [69]. Recent research findings and clinical observations suggest that damage of the insular cortex (i.e., in the context of vascular or tumoral pathologies, or due to surgery) leads both to autonomous dysfunctions (e.g., changes in heart rate variability, intermittent tachycardia, hyperventilation) and to alterations of the sense of self (e.g., changes in personality and or self-identification) [42, 104]. Moreover, patients with insular pathologies may exhibit pre- or post-surgical psychological side effects, including mood changes or lack of stress resistance and empathy, which may relate to alterations in the sense of self and which often have important consequences in their daily life (e.g., divorce, professional and familial changes) [30]. Adapted and comprehensive patient information should also include the risk of post-surgical perturbation of those cognitive functions that might influence social behavior, personality, and the self.
We argue that post-surgical disturbances of the sense of self and personality, although complex and difficult to measure behaviorally and clinically, can be estimated by evaluations of the bodily self and cognitive self. One such condition following insula resection or damage that has been linked to personality and the sense of self is depersonalization. Depersonalization refers to the recurring and prolonged sensations of being detached from one’s body, as if being an outside observer, often associated with feelings of loss of control over one’s own body, actions, or thoughts. More work is needed on the brain mechanisms of depersonalization, but it has been argued frequently that they are linked to those of bodily and/or cognitive self [61, 119].
Assessment and mapping of the sense of self in neurosurgery
Bodily self: full-body illusion based on exteroceptive and interoceptive signals
One approach to test the integrity of BSC is to investigate the perception of multisensory bodily stimuli by exposing people to conflicting inputs using VR or other devices and setups.
Several variations of the full-body illusion paradigm have been developed (see the section “The full-body illusion”); one variation also includes interoceptive signals. Hence, in this cardio-visual full-body illusion, synchronous flashing of the silhouette of the avatar was shown to the participant, based on his or her own online detected heartbeat (ECG) or the respiratory pattern (respiration belt). These interoceptive full-body illusions also allowed to manipulate BSC [1, 3, 7, 15, 87, 92]. Moreover, visual stimuli, which are presented synchronously or asynchronously with the frequency of the individual’s heartbeat, are interesting stimuli to investigate awareness [106]. Here, again, the insular cortex has been confirmed to play a crucial role in filtering and submitting intero- and exteroceptive signals to a hierarchy, which is related to self-awareness [106, 130]. An extra-operative study on neurosurgical patients with and without insular tumors showed clear differences between the patients and the control group [106]: in the group with (anterior) insular tumors—as opposed to patients with lobar gliomas and to controls—the previously shown effect of cardio-visual suppression of visual stimuli, which were presented in synchrony with the heartbeat, was abolished. These findings underline the importance of the insula for the combined processing of interoceptive and exteroceptive information—and that is of relevance for the constitution of self-consciousness.
Cognitive self: self-other discrimination based on auditory and visual signals
Auditory signals involved in self-voice processing are processed via different cerebral networks and further differ whether they concern one’s own voice (= self-voice) or someone else’s voice (= other voice) [32, 54].
Recognizing our own voice as being our own is an essential feature contributing to the constitution of our own self. That applies to internal listening as well as to the spoken voice. Auditory testing by the use of dedicated earphones, based on bone-conduction, includes the presentation of a sentence or vocalization of the vocal /a/—once by the test person’s own voice, and once by someone else’s voice. Furthermore, artificially generated voice morphs allow a stepwise mix (i.e., 90%/10% or 30%/70) between the two voices [67] (Fig. 1). Then, the threshold for recognition of one’s own voice may be defined. The addition of structural brain imaging and co-registered high-resolution EEG to that auditory testing paradigm allows to examine the various underlying neural activation patterns, which are different for self-voice and other voices (Fig. 2) [32, 54]. Interesting advancements have been made towards elucidating the etiology of auditory verbal hallucinations (AVH) in schizophrenia [85, 86]. The authors used a robotic procedure able to engender mild hallucinations in healthy individuals [19] to experimentally induce specific misattributions of self-towards-other voices [86], thereby mimicking the AVH phenomenology [48, 49]. Interestingly, such robotically induced self-voice misperceptions were further related to breathing [85], demonstrating a relationship between self-voice perception, interoception, and sensorimotor integration.
Another type of external stimuli, which are part of the concept of multisensory integration of interoceptive and exteroceptive stimuli, are those of visual origin. Interestingly, visual stimuli, which are presented synchronously with the frequency of the individual’s heartbeat, do not reach the threshold of awareness [106]. Those visual stimuli, however, which are presented asynchronously are reaching awareness. That cardio-visual suppression effect could be shown in a series of ECG-controlled visual attention experiments in conjunction with simultaneous 7T fMRI. Here, again, the insular cortex has been confirmed to play a crucial role in filtering and submitting intero- and exteroceptive signals to a hierarchy, which is related to self-awareness [106, 130].
The combination of the above tests with high-resolution EEG source imaging (ESI), co-registered to structural MRI, is very precise for functional anatomical allocation [76]. It could be shown to reliably identify the central sulcus with ESI of somatosensory evoked potentials [70]. The experimental setup to achieve such high spatio-anatomical resolution is based on electrical source imaging (ESI) of high-density (EEG) recordings (256 channels) using individual head models and distributed inverse solutions [75].
Heartbeat evoked potentials (HEPs)
We think that especially HEPs are an important future measure of the sense of self in the pre-surgical evaluation or neurosurgical patients. HEPs—in analogy to MEPs or SEPs—are measurable brain activity related to muscular contractions of the heart (heartbeats). They reflect neural activity in cortical regions that process afferent cardiac signals within a time-window ranging from 200 to 650 ms following the R-peak [55, 68, 91] (Fig. 3). HEPs can be measured by high-resolution scalp EEG or by direct recording from intracranial electrodes [90, 91]. It is of importance to record averaged neural activity time-locked to ECG in order to identify the HEPs consistently. In a series of patients [91], who underwent implantation of SEEG electrodes (total, 599 intracranial contacts) for pre-surgical evaluation of intractable epilepsy, psychophysiological testing (neuropsychological exams, FBI test with head-mounted VR display as described above) was performed. Two principal anatomical regions, as the probable primary sources for HEPs, were found: the operculum and the insula. Experimentally induced alterations of self-identification were reflected by insular HEP pattern (Figs. 4 and 5). In MEG studies, recording of neural responses to cardiac signals in the so-called default network and in the insula was related to cognitive self involving spontaneous thoughts [9]. Thus, the amplitude of neural responses to heartbeats in the vmPFC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and insula has been shown to be associated with the cognitive self (i.e., self-relatedness of spontaneous thought fluctuations) [9–11]. It is also important to mention that the insula—especially its anterior part together with the anterior and middle cingulate cortices (ACC/MCC)—is playing a crucial role in salience attribution. In fact, as part of the “salience network” [113], the anterior insula is hypothesized to detect the most relevant bottom-up events among internal and external environments and to temporarily initiate attentional control inputs, which then are sustained by the ACC [74]. Moreover, the right anterior insula is involved in switching between the (externally oriented) central-executive and the (self-referential) default-mode networks [122]. Taking together, the insular cortex may integrate visceral afferent signals, external visual and auditory signals, and saliency-related signals, allowing the mobilization of sustained attention, also via the involvement of other regions. We speculate that HEPs in association with tasks associated with the bodily self and the cognitive self could be used as biomarkers to determine regions that if resected would lead to post-surgical alterations of the sense of self. Preliminary results obtained under general anesthesia are promising.
Processing of audio-tactile and peripersonal space (PPS)
The PPS is the space around a person, where direct human interaction with our environment takes place [56, 62]. To precisely allocate, i.e., audio-tactile stimuli in time and space, intactness of (unconscious) multisensory integration at the neural level is required [107]. Multisensory integration has been shown necessary to create and to maintain an intact PPS and BSC [20]. In a series of experiments in epilepsy patients with intracranial electrodes, such multisensory PPS responses were detected at distinct times after stimulus onset, which consisted of robotic-controlled application of a vibration stimulus to the chest of the patients. Separately, or simultaneously, white noise was presented via earphones, simulating to be approaching from the front [13]. Multisensory processing of auditive, tactile, and audio-tactile stimuli, respectively, involves the postcentral gyrus, the superior temporal gyrus, and the insula [13]. A recent fMRI study showed that spontaneous BOLD activity in the anterior and middle cingulate cortex (but also in the AI) can predict ascription of self-relatedness to the forthcoming noisy auditory stimuli [100]. An intra-operative feasibility study concerning PPS using an audio-tactile paradigm was recently conducted (J. Haemmerli et al., M.D. Thesis, still under production at the time of submitting this manuscript). Electrocorticography electrodes were placed on the two main regions of interest (i.e., parieto-temporal junction, fronto-basal region) during craniotomies and brain tumor removals. This primary study in anesthetized patients confirmed the feasibility to test PPS and multimodal integration on patients under narcosis with an intracranial recording.
A plea for extra- and intra-operative recording of functions underlying the sense of self
The methods described above for extensive psychophysiological testing of the sense of self and related aspects of personality have proved to be consistently and safely applicable in healthy volunteers and in neurosurgical patients—extra-operatively so far. Here, we propose to develop similar test paradigms for the application during intracranial surgery of i.e. paralimbic gliomas affecting the insula, or during epilepsy surgery. As the full test battery, including audio-tactile and visual testing combined with HEP recording may take more than 2 h, testing will need to be shortened and optimized to the surgical setting. We also note that elements of the above tests may become a routine part of the pre-surgical investigation of epilepsy patients, in whom SEEG electrodes or subdural grid electrodes were placed already, or during DBS procedures. Even if not all possible psychophysiological tests are performed, a more widespread use will contribute to the ongoing efforts to decipher the underlying mechanisms of the sense of self.
Intra-operatively, the portfolio of neuropsychological tests during awake craniotomy may be extended by some of the described test, i.e., by the addition of head-holder-mounted VR displays and the simultaneous recording of HEPs. HEPs on the other hand, once proved that stable intra-operative recording is feasible, even under general anesthesia, may serve as a biomarker for the capacity of multisensory integration as a surrogate for intactness of cortical cardiac processing and related aspects of BSC in patients undergoing brain surgery. Concurrently to these recent developments in psychophysics, new generations of ultra-thin and very flexible electrodes are under development and preclinical evaluation [111]. These visco-elastic electrodes, which have micro-electronical properties beyond simple recording, may be more easily placed on irregular cortical surface patterns than this is the case with traditional subdural strip and grid electrodes [112]. They interfere lesser with the course of surgery and thus will further ignite the ambition to broaden the perspectives of intra-operative surveillance during intracranial procedures [112].
Conclusions and outlook
Technical advances have always strongly impacted neurosurgical practice, especially over the past few decades: neuronavigation, intra-operative imaging and fluorescence, DBS, and the widespread acceptance of intra-operative neuromonitoring. All these tools or techniques require investment on the financial as well as on the personal side. Yet, their obvious clinical benefits outweigh the related expenses, at least in health care systems, where there is sufficient funding to pay for technological progress and surgical evolution. Whereas there is widespread acceptance to integrate cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology procedures in the pre-operative evaluation and in the post-operative follow-up of i.e. neuro-oncological patients, intra-operative neuromonitoring has long been limited to functions, such as movement during i.e. glioma surgery in the central region and i.e. CN VII and BAEP monitoring for surgery in and around the cerebello-pontine angle. The revival of awake craniotomy has brought along a surge of techniques for intra-operative awake mapping of white matter fiber tracts, i.e., for the protection of language and speech. Nowadays, olfactory monitoring, VEPs, and integrated suction-resection mapping devices complete the armamentarium for monitoring and mapping of precisely allocated functions.
Nevertheless, the present state of technology and the neuroscience of interoceptive and of exteroceptive aspects of the sense of self, as well as cognitive aspects of the self, allow to go one step further: Based on the above concepts and experimental paradigms, it is possible to test fully cooperative patients with i.e. low-grade gliomas, or with lesions in the frontal, the paracentral, the parietal, the temporal, and the insular lobes in the sense of self as based on auditory, visual, visuo-tactile, and interoceptive stimuli. Alterations of these brain functions may be followed by changes in the bodily self, the cognitive self, or other related aspects (i.e., personality). These pre- and post-operative behavioral, neural, and neuropsychological assessments require dedicated hardware and reseach staff. It is entirely feasible to conduct similar tests during awake craniotomy by the use of i.e. head-holder-mounted VR displays and by the use of newly developed ultra-thin cortical electrodes, which may follow the irregular geometry of the various brain regions. HEPs as biomarkers for the interoceptive component of the sense of self may be recorded and analyzed under the conditions of awake craniotomy and of general anesthesia. The role and the impact of that knowledge on patient counseling (i.e., the intra-operative decision to go for a more extensive tumor resection or not, if the risk to damage zones of multisensory integration is implied) has yet to be clarified. Systematically mapping psychophysiological alterations might however allow to quantify the risk of post-surgical perturbation of the sense of self.
Nowadays, intra-operative BSC monitoring is difficult to set-up in terms of time spent during the surgery and in terms of financial issue. BSC monitoring is, however and at least in part, technically feasible. Intra-operative electrophysiology setups have already most of these technical solutions. These setups have already been applied in different research domains (epilepsy, deep brain stimulation, vision, speech, olfaction, all under general anesthesia). Beyond clinical systems, they include several key elements: high-frequency electrophysiological brain signal recordings, captured from any kind of recording electrodes; internal or external open trigger sources for trigged recordings, diverse signal processing analyses including online analyses. Dedicated engineers have this particular expertise and experience to manage intra-operatively these setups and to analyze these signals. Intra-operative BSC monitoring represents also a potential risk for neurosurgical patients because of the additional time during which patients are anesthetized. Only a strict intra-operative coordination between neurosurgeons, electrophysiologists, and anesthesiologists can reduce the risk. Furthermore, the use of intra-operative MRI in the BSC monitoring will play a key role in recognition of the targeted brain areas. The whole intra-operative neuromonitoring setup should fit to intra-operative MR prerogatives. One main limitation has also to be highlighted, in that BSC monitoring requires specific technological instruments, which are not ubiquitously available even in modern neurosurgical centers.
Numerous open questions remain, of course, as many neuroimaging studies related to the self are correlative in nature. Thus, the causal role of the supposedly involved brain areas (e.g., insula and temporal-parietal junction) of the bodily self or cognitive self has to be determined yet. Then, the interactions or overlap between brain networks underlying these two notions of self is also unknown. Finally, it is not known whether bodily signals and their neural representations only impact the bodily self or whether they also modulate cortical networks of the cognitive self, impacting aspects of the self-related to personality traits. But joint efforts between neurosurgeons, clinical neuroscientists, engineers, and experts in signal analysis—inside and outside the operating room—will help allow to advance understanding of the human self and reduce unwarranted neurosurgical side effects.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Université de Genève.
Declarations
Ethics approval
For this type of study, formal consent is not required.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Functional Neurosurgery - Other
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Adler D, Herbelin B, Similowski T, Blanke O. Breathing and sense of self: visuo-respiratory conflicts alter body self-consciousness. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. 2014;203:68–74. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2014.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Afif A, Minotti L, Kahane P, Hoffmann D. Anatomofunctional organization of the insular cortex: a study using intracerebral electrical stimulation in epileptic patients. Epilepsia. 2010;51(11):2305–2315. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02755.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Allard E, Canzoneri E, Adler D, Morélot-Panzini C, Bello-Ruiz J, Herbelin B, Blanke O, Similowski T. Interferences between breathing, experimental dyspnoea and bodily self-consciousness. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):9990. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-11045-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Armel KC, Ramachandran VS. Projecting sensations to external objects: evidence from skin conductance response. Proc Biol Sci. 2003;270(1523):1499–1506. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2003.2364. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Armour JA, Ardell JL (eds) (2004) Basic and clinical neurocardiology. Oxford University Press, Oxford, New York
- 6.Aspell JE, Lenggenhager B, Blanke O. Keeping in touch with one’s self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness. PloS One. 2009;4(8):e6488. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Aspell JE, Heydrich L, Marillier G, Lavanchy T, Herbelin B, Blanke O. Turning body and self inside out: visualized heartbeats alter bodily self-consciousness and tactile perception. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(12):2445–2453. doi: 10.1177/0956797613498395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Azzalini D, Rebollo I, Tallon-Baudry C. Visceral signals shape brain dynamics and cognition. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23(6):488–509. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Babo-Rebelo M, Richter CG, Tallon-Baudry C. Neural responses to heartbeats in the default network encode the self in spontaneous thoughts. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2016;36(30):7829–7840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0262-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Babo-Rebelo M, Wolpert N, Adam C, Hasboun D, Tallon-Baudry C (2016) Is the cardiac monitoring function related to the self in both the default network and right anterior insula? Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 10.1098/rstb.2016.0004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 11.Babo-Rebelo M, Buot A, Tallon-Baudry C. Neural responses to heartbeats distinguish self from other during imagination. NeuroImage. 2019;191:10–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Berlad I, Pratt H. P300 in response to the subject’s own name. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1995;96(5):472–474. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(95)00116-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bernasconi F, Noel J-P, Park HD, Faivre N, Seeck M, Spinelli L, Schaller K, Blanke O, Serino A. Audio-tactile and peripersonal space processing around the trunk in human parietal and temporal cortex: an intracranial EEG study. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(9):3385–3397. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Betka S, Gould Van Praag C, Paloyelis Y, Bond R, Pfeifer G, Sequeira H, Duka T, Critchley H. Impact of intranasal oxytocin on interoceptive accuracy in alcohol users: an attentional mechanism? Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018;13(4):440–448. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Betka S, Canzoneri E, Adler D, Herbelin B, Bello-Ruiz J, Kannape OA, Similowski T, Blanke O. Mechanisms of the breathing contribution to bodily self-consciousness in healthy humans: lessons from machine-assisted breathing? Psychophysiology. 2020;57(8):e13564. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Betka S, Łukowska M, Silva M, King J, Garfinkel S, Critchley H (2020) Feeling the beat: temporal and spatial perception of heartbeat sensations. bioRxiv 2020.07.27.222810
- 17.Blanke O. Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(8):556–571. doi: 10.1038/nrn3292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Blanke O, Metzinger T. Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(1):7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Blanke O, Pozeg P, Hara M, et al. Neurological and robot-controlled induction of an apparition. Curr Biol CB. 2014;24(22):2681–2686. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Blanke O, Slater M, Serino A. Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness. Neuron. 2015;88(1):145–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Blefari ML, Martuzzi R, Salomon R, Bello-Ruiz J, Herbelin B, Serino A, Blanke O. Bilateral Rolandic operculum processing underlying heartbeat awareness reflects changes in bodily self-consciousness. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;45(10):1300–1312. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Boëx C, Momjian S, Schaller K. Letter to the Editor: Electric current application for motor tract mapping. J Neurosurg. 2016;124(6):1881–1883. doi: 10.3171/2015.12.JNS152830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Boex C, Haemmerli J, Momjian S, Schaller K. Prognostic values of motor evoked potentials in insular, precental, or postcentral resections. J Clin Neurophysiol Off Publ Am Electroencephalogr Soc. 2016;33(1):51–59. doi: 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Botvinick M, Cohen J. Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see. Nature. 1998;391(6669):756. doi: 10.1038/35784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bréchet L, Grivaz P, Gauthier B, Blanke O. Common recruitment of angular gyrus in episodic autobiographical memory and bodily self-consciousness. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:270. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bréchet L, Mange R, Herbelin B, Theillaud Q, Gauthier B, Serino A, Blanke O. First-person view of one’s body in immersive virtual reality: influence on episodic memory. PloS One. 2019;14(3):e0197763. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0197763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Canzoneri E, di Pellegrino G, Herbelin B, Blanke O, Serino A. Conceptual processing is referenced to the experienced location of the self, not to the location of the physical body. Cognition. 2016;154:182–192. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cedzich C, Taniguchi M, Schäfer S, Schramm J. Somatosensory evoked potential phase reversal and direct motor cortex stimulation during surgery in and around the central region. Neurosurgery. 1996;38(5):962–970. doi: 10.1097/00006123-199605000-00023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Chang EF, Clark A, Smith JS, Polley M-Y, Chang SM, Barbaro NM, Parsa AT, McDermott MW, Berger MS. Functional mapping-guided resection of low-grade gliomas in eloquent areas of the brain: improvement of long-term survival. Clinical article. J Neurosurg. 2011;114(3):566–573. doi: 10.3171/2010.6.JNS091246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chen P, Wang G, Ma R, Jing F, Zhang Y, Wang Y, Zhang P, Niu C, Zhang X. Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities in patients with insular glioma. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2016;16(5):962–975. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0445-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Christoff K, Gordon AM, Smallwood J, Smith R, Schooler JW. Experience sampling during fMRI reveals default network and executive system contributions to mind wandering. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009;106(21):8719–8724. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900234106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Conde T, Gonçalves ÓF, Pinheiro AP. Paying attention to my voice or yours: an ERP study with words. Biol Psychol. 2015;111:40–52. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Craig ADB. How do you feel--now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(1):59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Craig ADB. Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364(1525):1933–1942. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Craig ADB. Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011;1225:72–82. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Critchley HD, Harrison NA. Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron. 2013;77(4):624–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Critchley H, Seth A. Will studies of macaque insula reveal the neural mechanisms of self-awareness? Neuron. 2012;74(3):423–426. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Critchley HD, Wiens S, Rotshtein P, Ohman A, Dolan RJ. Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(2):189–195. doi: 10.1038/nn1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Damasio AR (1999) The feeling of what happens: body and emotion in the making of consciousness. NY: Harcourt Brace, New York
- 40.Damasio A, Carvalho GB. The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013;14(2):143–152. doi: 10.1038/nrn3403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Damasio A, Damasio H, Tranel D. Persistence of feelings and sentience after bilateral damage of the insula. Cereb Cortex N Y N. 2013;23(4):833–846. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.De Morree HM, Rutten G-J, Szabó BM, Sitskoorn MM, Kop WJ. Effects of insula resection on autonomic nervous system activity. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2016;28(2):153–158. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0000000000000207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.De Witt Hamer PC, Robles SG, Zwinderman AH, Duffau H, Berger MS. Impact of intraoperative stimulation brain mapping on glioma surgery outcome: a meta-analysis. J Clin Oncol Off J Am Soc Clin Oncol. 2012;30(20):2559–2565. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2011.38.4818. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Dennett DC (1993) Consciousness explained. Penguin Books, London
- 45.Duffau H, Capelle L, Denvil D, et al. Usefulness of intraoperative electrical subcortical mapping during surgery for low-grade gliomas located within eloquent brain regions: functional results in a consecutive series of 103 patients. J Neurosurg. 2003;98(4):764–778. doi: 10.3171/jns.2003.98.4.0764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Duffau H, Velut S, Mitchell M-C, Gatignol P, Capelle L. Intra-operative mapping of the subcortical visual pathways using direct electrical stimulations. Acta Neurochir (Wien) 2004;146(3):265–269. doi: 10.1007/s00701-003-0199-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ehrsson HH. The experimental induction of out-of-body experiences. Science. 2007;317(5841):1048. doi: 10.1126/science.1142175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ford JM, Mathalon DH. Corollary discharge dysfunction in schizophrenia: can it explain auditory hallucinations? Int J Psychophysiol Off J Int Organ Psychophysiol. 2005;58(2–3):179–189. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Frith CD. The positive and negative symptoms of schizophrenia reflect impairments in the perception and initiation of action. Psychol Med. 1987;17(3):631–648. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700025873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gallagher N. Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4(1):14–21. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01417-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Garfinkel SN, Seth AK, Barrett AB, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol Psychol. 2015;104:65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Garfinkel SN, Manassei MF, Hamilton-Fletcher G, In den Bosch Y, Critchley HD, Engels M (2016) Interoceptive dimensions across cardiac and respiratory axes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 10.1098/rstb.2016.0014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 53.Gauthier B, Bréchet L, Lance F, Mange R, Herbelin B, Faivre N, Bolton TAW, Ville DVD, Blanke O. First-person body view modulates the neural substrates of episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a functional connectivity study. NeuroImage. 2020;223:117370. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117370. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Graux J, Gomot M, Roux S, Bonnet-Brilhault F, Bruneau N. Is my voice just a familiar voice? An electrophysiological study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2015;10(1):101–105. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Gray MA, Taggart P, Sutton PM, Groves D, Holdright DR, Bradbury D, Brull D, Critchley HD. A cortical potential reflecting cardiac function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(16):6818–6823. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609509104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Grivaz P, Blanke O, Serino A. Common and distinct brain regions processing multisensory bodily signals for peripersonal space and body ownership. NeuroImage. 2017;147:602–618. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Guterstam A, Björnsdotter M, Gentile G, Ehrsson HH. Posterior cingulate cortex integrates the senses of self-location and body ownership. Curr Biol CB. 2015;25(11):1416–1425. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gutzwiller EM, Cabrilo I, Radovanovic I, Schaller K, Boëx C. Intraoperative monitoring with visual evoked potentials for brain surgeries. J Neurosurg. 2018;130(2):654–660. doi: 10.3171/2017.8.JNS171168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Hervey-Jumper SL, Berger MS. Technical nuances of awake brain tumor surgery and the role of maximum safe resection. J Neurosurg Sci. 2015;59(4):351–360. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Hervey-Jumper SL, Li J, Lau D, Molinaro AM, Perry DW, Meng L, Berger MS. Awake craniotomy to maximize glioma resection: methods and technical nuances over a 27-year period. J Neurosurg. 2015;123(2):325–339. doi: 10.3171/2014.10.JNS141520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Heydrich L, Blanke O. Distinct illusory own-body perceptions caused by damage to posterior insula and extrastriate cortex. Brain J Neurol. 2013;136(Pt 3):790–803. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws364. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Holt DJ, Cassidy BS, Yue X, Rauch SL, Boeke EA, Nasr S, Tootell RBH, Coombs G. Neural correlates of personal space intrusion. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2014;34(12):4123–4134. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0686-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ionta S, Heydrich L, Lenggenhager B, Mouthon M, Fornari E, Chapuis D, Gassert R, Blanke O. Multisensory mechanisms in temporo-parietal cortex support self-location and first-person perspective. Neuron. 2011;70(2):363–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ionta S, Martuzzi R, Salomon R, Blanke O. The brain network reflecting bodily self-consciousness: a functional connectivity study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9(12):1904–1913. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.James W (1890) The principles of psychology. Cosimo, Inc.
- 66.Katkin ES, Reed SD, DeRoo C (1983) A methodological analysis of three techniques for the assessment of individual differences in heartbeat detection. Psychophysiology 20:452
- 67.Kawahara H (2015) Temporally variable multi attribute morphing of arbitrarily many voices for exploratory research of speech prosody. Speech Prosody Speech Synth Model Gener Prosody High Qual Flex Speech Synth:109–120
- 68.Khalsa SS, Rudrauf D, Feinstein JS, Tranel D. The pathways of interoceptive awareness. Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(12):1494–1496. doi: 10.1038/nn.2411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Klein M, Duffau H, De Witt Hamer PC. Cognition and resective surgery for diffuse infiltrative glioma: an overview. J Neurooncol. 2012;108(2):309–318. doi: 10.1007/s11060-012-0811-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Lascano AM, Grouiller F, Genetti M, Spinelli L, Seeck M, Schaller K, Michel CM. Surgically relevant localization of the central sulcus with high-density somatosensory-evoked potentials compared with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurosurgery. 2014;74(5):517–526. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0000000000000298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Legrand D, Ruby P. What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychol Rev. 2009;116(1):252–282. doi: 10.1037/a0014172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Lenggenhager B, Tadi T, Metzinger T, Blanke O. Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science. 2007;317(5841):1096–1099. doi: 10.1126/science.1143439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Martin J, Tyrand R, Lopez U, Bernasconi Pertusio F, Lysakowski C, Laganaro M, Schaller K, Boëx C. Language monitoring in brain surgery under general anesthesia. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2020;32(3):268–272. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0000000000000602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5–6):655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Michel CM, Brunet D. EEG source imaging: a practical review of the analysis steps. Front Neurol. 2019;10:325. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Michel CM, He B (2012) EEG mapping and source imaging. Niedermeyers Electroencephalogr Basic Princ Clin Appl Relat Fields Sixth Ed:1179–1202
- 77.Momjian S, Tyrand R, Landis BN, Boëx C (2019) Intraoperative monitoring of olfactory function: a feasibility study. J Neurosurg:1–6 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 78.Moon H-J, Gauthier B, Park H-D, Faivre N, Blanke O (2020) Sense of self impacts spatial navigation and hexadirectional coding in human entorhinal cortex. bioRxiv 2020.09.13.295246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 79.Neuloh G, Pechstein U, Cedzich C, Schramm J. Motor evoked potential monitoring with supratentorial surgery. Neurosurgery. 2007;61(1 Suppl):337–346. doi: 10.1227/01.neu.0000279227.50826.6c. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Neuloh G, Pechstein U, Schramm J. Motor tract monitoring during insular glioma surgery. J Neurosurg. 2007;106(4):582–592. doi: 10.3171/jns.2007.106.4.582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Neuloh G, Bogucki J, Schramm J. Intraoperative preservation of corticospinal function in the brainstem. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80(4):417–422. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2008.157792. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Noel J-P, Pfeiffer C, Blanke O, Serino A. Peripersonal space as the space of the bodily self. Cognition. 2015;144:49–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J. Self-referential processing in our brain--a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage. 2006;31(1):440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Ojemann G, Ojemann J, Lettich E, Berger M. Cortical language localization in left, dominant hemisphere. An electrical stimulation mapping investigation in 117 patients. J Neurosurg. 1989;71(3):316–326. doi: 10.3171/jns.1989.71.3.0316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Orepic P, Park H-D, Rognini G, Faivre N, Blanke O (2020) Breathing affects self-other voice discrimination in a bodily state with increased otherness. 10.31234/osf.io/t9saq [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 86.Orepic P, Rognini G, Kannape OA, Faivre N, Blanke O (2020) Sensorimotor conflicts induce somatic passivity and louden quiet voices in healthy listeners. bioRxiv:2020.03.26.005843 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 87.Park H-D, Blanke O. Coupling inner and outer body for self-consciousness. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23(5):377–388. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Park H-D, Tallon-Baudry C. The neural subjective frame: from bodily signals to perceptual consciousness. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1641):20130208. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Park H-D, Correia S, Ducorps A, Tallon-Baudry C. Spontaneous fluctuations in neural responses to heartbeats predict visual detection. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17(4):612–618. doi: 10.1038/nn.3671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Park H-D, Bernasconi F, Bello-Ruiz J, Pfeiffer C, Salomon R, Blanke O. Transient modulations of neural responses to heartbeats covary with bodily self-consciousness. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2016;36(32):8453–8460. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0311-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Park H-D, Bernasconi F, Salomon R, Tallon-Baudry C, Spinelli L, Seeck M, Schaller K, Blanke O. Neural sources and underlying mechanisms of neural responses to heartbeats, and their role in bodily self-consciousness: an intracranial EEG study. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(7):2351–2364. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Park H-D, Barnoud C, Trang H, Kannape OA, Schaller K, Blanke O. Breathing is coupled with voluntary action and the cortical readiness potential. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):289. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13967-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Pechstein U, Cedzich C, Nadstawek J, Schramm J. Transcranial high-frequency repetitive electrical stimulation for recording myogenic motor evoked potentials with the patient under general anesthesia. Neurosurgery. 1996;39(2):335–343. doi: 10.1097/00006123-199608000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Perrin F, García-Larrea L, Mauguière F, Bastuji H. A differential brain response to the subject’s own name persists during sleep. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol. 1999;110(12):2153–2164. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(99)00177-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Petkova VI, Ehrsson HH. If I were you: perceptual illusion of body swapping. PLOS ONE. 2008;3(12):e3832. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003832. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Petkova VI, Björnsdotter M, Gentile G, Jonsson T, Li T-Q, Ehrsson HH. From part- to whole-body ownership in the multisensory brain. Curr Biol. 2011;21(13):1118–1122. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Pfeiffer C, Lopez C, Schmutz V, Duenas JA, Martuzzi R, Blanke O. Multisensory origin of the subjective first-person perspective: visual, tactile, and vestibular mechanisms. PloS One. 2013;8(4):e61751. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0061751. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Pozeg P, Palluel E, Ronchi R, Solcà M, Al-Khodairy A-W, Jordan X, Kassouha A, Blanke O. Virtual reality improves embodiment and neuropathic pain caused by spinal cord injury. Neurology. 2017;89(18):1894–1903. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Qin P, Northoff G. How is our self related to midline regions and the default-mode network? NeuroImage. 2011;57(3):1221–1233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Qin P, Grimm S, Duncan NW, Fan Y, Huang Z, Lane T, Weng X, Bajbouj M, Northoff G. Spontaneous activity in default-mode network predicts ascription of self-relatedness to stimuli. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11(4):693–702. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Raabe A, Beck J, Schucht P, Seidel K. Continuous dynamic mapping of the corticospinal tract during surgery of motor eloquent brain tumors: evaluation of a new method: clinical article. J Neurosurg. 2014;120(5):1015–1024. doi: 10.3171/2014.1.JNS13909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Rogers TB, Kuiper NA, Kirker WS. Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1977;35(9):677–688. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.35.9.677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rognini G, Petrini FM, Raspopovic S, et al. Multisensory bionic limb to achieve prosthesis embodiment and reduce distorted phantom limb perceptions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2019;90(7):833–836. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2018-318570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Ronchi R, Bello-Ruiz J, Lukowska M, Herbelin B, Cabrilo I, Schaller K, Blanke O. Right insular damage decreases heartbeat awareness and alters cardio-visual effects on bodily self-consciousness. Neuropsychologia. 2015;70:11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Sala F, Lanteri P. Brain surgery in motor areas: the invaluable assistance of intraoperative neurophysiological monitoring. J Neurosurg Sci. 2003;47(2):79–88. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Salomon R, Ronchi R, Dönz J, Bello-Ruiz J, Herbelin B, Martet R, Faivre N, Schaller K, Blanke O. The insula mediates access to awareness of visual stimuli presented synchronously to the heartbeat. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2016;36(18):5115–5127. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4262-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Salomon R, Noel J-P, Łukowska M, Faivre N, Metzinger T, Serino A, Blanke O. Unconscious integration of multisensory bodily inputs in the peripersonal space shapes bodily self-consciousness. Cognition. 2017;166:174–183. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Salomon R, Ronchi R, Dönz J, Bello-Ruiz J, Herbelin B, Faivre N, Schaller K, Blanke O. Insula mediates heartbeat related effects on visual consciousness. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav. 2018;101:87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Sanchez-Vives MV, Spanlang B, Frisoli A, Bergamasco M, Slater M. Virtual hand illusion induced by visuomotor correlations. PLOS ONE. 2010;5(4):e10381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Schandry R. Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology. 1981;18(4):483–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Schiavone G, Lacour SP (2019) Conformable bioelectronic interfaces: mapping the road ahead. Sci Transl Med. 10.1126/scitranslmed.aaw5858 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 112.Schiavone G, Fallegger F, Kang X, et al. Soft, implantable bioelectronic interfaces for translational research. Adv Mater Deerfield Beach Fla. 2020;32(17):e1906512. doi: 10.1002/adma.201906512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27(9):2349–2356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Senft C, Forster M-T, Bink A, Mittelbronn M, Franz K, Seifert V, Szelényi A. Optimizing the extent of resection in eloquently located gliomas by combining intraoperative MRI guidance with intraoperative neurophysiological monitoring. J Neurooncol. 2012;109(1):81–90. doi: 10.1007/s11060-012-0864-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Serino A, Noel J-P, Galli G, Canzoneri E, Marmaroli P, Lissek H, Blanke O. Body part-centered and full body-centered peripersonal space representations. Sci Rep. 2015;5(1):18603. doi: 10.1038/srep18603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Seth AK. Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends Cogn Sci. 2013;17(11):565–573. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Seth AK, Suzuki K, Critchley HD. An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Front Psychol. 2011;2:395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Sigl JC, Chamoun NG. An introduction to bispectral analysis for the electroencephalogram. J Clin Monit. 1994;10(6):392–404. doi: 10.1007/BF01618421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Simeon D, Kozin DS, Segal K, Lerch B, Dujour R, Giesbrecht T. De-constructing depersonalization: further evidence for symptom clusters. Psychiatry Res. 2008;157(1–3):303–306. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Solcà M, Ronchi R, Bello-Ruiz J, et al. Heartbeat-enhanced immersive virtual reality to treat complex regional pain syndrome. Neurology. 2018;91(5):e479–e489. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Solcà M, Krishna V, Young N et al (2020) Enhancing analgesic spinal cord stimulation for chronic pain with personalized immersive virtual reality. Pain. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002160 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 122.Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U A. 2008;105(34):12569–12574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Suzuki K, Garfinkel SN, Critchley HD, Seth AK. Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51(13):2909–2917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Tacikowski P, Nowicka A. Allocation of attention to self-name and self-face: an ERP study. Biol Psychol. 2010;84(2):318–324. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Tacikowski P, Weijs ML, Ehrsson HH. Perception of our own body influences self-concept and self-incoherence impairs episodic memory. iScience. 2020;23(9):101429. doi: 10.1016/j.isci.2020.101429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Tallon-Baudry C, Campana F, Park H-D, Babo-Rebelo M. The neural monitoring of visceral inputs, rather than attention, accounts for first-person perspective in conscious vision. Cortex J Devoted Study Nerv Syst Behav. 2018;102:139–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Tieri G, Gioia A, Scandola M, Pavone EF, Aglioti SM. Visual appearance of a virtual upper limb modulates the temperature of the real hand: a thermal imaging study in Immersive Virtual Reality. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;45(9):1141–1151. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Tyrand R, Momjian S, Pollo C, Lysakowski C, Lascano AM, Vulliémoz S, Schaller K, Boëx C. Continuous intraoperative monitoring of temporal lobe epilepsy surgery. Stereotact Funct Neurosurg. 2016;94(6):404–412. doi: 10.1159/000452842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Uddin LQ, Kaplan JT, Molnar-Szakacs I, Zaidel E, Iacoboni M. Self-face recognition activates a frontoparietal “mirror” network in the right hemisphere: an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2005;25(3):926–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Wiebking C, Duncan NW, Tiret B, Hayes DJ, Marjaǹska M, Doyon J, Bajbouj M, Northoff G. GABA in the insula - a predictor of the neural response to interoceptive awareness. NeuroImage. 2014;86:10–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]