Abstract
Psychosocial stress in childhood and adolescence is linked to stress system dysregulation, although few studies have examined the relative impacts of parental harshness and parental disengagement. This study prospectively tested whether parental harshness and disengagement show differential associations with overall cortisol output in adolescence. Associations between overall cortisol output and adolescent mental health problems were tested concurrently. Adolescents from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study provided hair samples for cortisol assay at 15 years (N = 171). Caregivers reported on parental harshness and disengagement experiences at 1, 3, 5, 9, and 15 years, and adolescents reported at 15 years. Both parent and adolescent reported depressive and anxiety symptoms and antisocial behaviors at 15. Greater parental harshness from 1–15 years, and harshness reported at 15 years in particular, was associated with higher overall cortisol output at 15. Greater parental disengagement from 1–15 years, and disengagement at 1 year specifically, was associated with lower cortisol output. There were no significant associations between cortisol output and depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, or antisocial behaviors. These results suggest that the unique variances of parental harshness and disengagement may have opposing associations with cortisol output at 15 years, with unclear implications for adolescent mental health.
Keywords: parental harshness, parental disengagement, hair cortisol, depressive symptoms, adolescence
Chronic or severe psychosocial stress in childhood and adolescence is linked to dysregulation in stress-mediating systems, including the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Doom & Gunnar, 2013; Koss & Gunnar, 2018). Dysregulation in cortisol, one of the HPA axis’s primary stress-mediating hormones, is associated with the onset of a number of mental and physical health problems (McEwen, 1998). However, most investigations of associations between psychosocial stress, the HPA axis, and mental health focus on diurnal cortisol regulation or cortisol reactivity and recovery in response to stress. It is largely unclear how chronic or severe psychosocial stress during childhood and adolescence prospectively affect overall output of cortisol over time, which may be particularly important in order to understand patterns of cortisol regulation and whether specific patterns predict the onset of psychopathology. The current investigation addresses this gap in the literature by testing whether parental harshness and parental disengagement measured prospectively during childhood and adolescence predict overall cortisol output at 15 years.
Although there is evidence that both heightened and reduced overall cortisol output are associated with a number of mental and physical health problems (Adam et al., 2017; Dockray et al., 2009; Shirtcliff & Essex, 2008), there is little empirical work that directly investigates whether heightened or reduced overall cortisol output may mediate associations between psychosocial stress and mental health problems. Only one study (White et al., 2017), to our knowledge, investigated whether overall cortisol output measured via hair (rather than diurnal cortisol levels or cortisol reactivity) may serve as a mediator. This cross-sectional study showed reduced hair cortisol concentrations in maltreated children at 9–16 years of age, compared to non-maltreated children (White et al., 2017). Reductions were related to chronicity and type of maltreatment. This reduction in cortisol output mediated the association between maltreatment and externalizing symptoms (White et al., 2017). It remains unclear, however, whether overall cortisol output can mediate associations between both earlier and later developmental stress and internalizing and externalizing symptomology in adolescence. Here we tested whether overall cortisol output measured via hair (reflecting 1–2 months of overall cortisol secretion) is a statistical mediator between childhood and adolescent parental harshness and disengagement (measured prospectively from age 1 year) and internalizing and externalizing problems at age 15.
Measuring overall cortisol output via hair
Advances in cortisol assessment have led to the development of cortisol assays using hair samples, which can reliably index overall cortisol output over time, retrospectively for at least 3 months (Gao et al., 2013; Kirschbaum et al., 2009). Hair cortisol is often easier to collect than salivary measures of cortisol as it does not rely on careful sample timing and participant compliance with strict collection rules. It also is able to capture overall cortisol exposure over months, providing an integrated measure that captures both day and night secretion. This may provide a better biomarker of brain exposure to cortisol over time, which might be a critical factor in shaping longer term consequences of early life stress experiences. This method has been validated in recently pregnant women, whose third trimester cortisol outputs are known to be higher (Kirschbaum et al., 2009) and in individuals showing higher hair cortisol concentration following major life stressors (Staufenbiel et al., 2013). Hair cortisol concentration is also positively correlated with diurnal salivary cortisol concentrations (D’Anna-Hernandez et al., 2011; Flom et al., 2017), though it requires a number of days of saliva collection to reliably see this association, likely because saliva cortisol is more strongly influenced by state-factors that vary day to day (e.g., meals, exercise, sleep patterns, compliance with sampling timing and rules). Thus, it likely provides a better integrated measure of overall secretion.
Stress and overall cortisol output
Chronic stress and contextual factors such as lower socioeconomic status (SES) are associated with overall cortisol output measured via hair in children and adults (Gray et al., 2018; Stalder et al., 2017). However, evidence is mixed with regard to the direction of these associations, with most studies reporting small positive associations between stress and hair cortisol concentration (Khoury et al., 2019). However, there are several studies reporting moderately-sized negative associations between stress and hair cortisol concentration (Khoury et al., 2019), including the study by White and colleagues (2017) described above. Timing of adversity is a significant moderator of hair cortisol concentration, with adult trauma showing stronger positive associations with overall cortisol output than childhood trauma (Khoury et al., 2019). Childhood stress is often broadly defined (e.g., maltreatment), and timing is frequently not accounted for in the wider literature on stress and HPA functioning. These issues likely contribute to discrepant findings that have been noted in studies of stress and other HPA measures such as cortisol reactivity or diurnal cortisol pattern. Prospective longitudinal studies, as reported here, could help us understand how factors like the type and timing of adversity influence overall cortisol output.
Threat vs. deprivation
Recent theoretical work argues that conceptualizations of early life stress as a unidimensional construct, with more stress leading broadly to more negative effects, are incomplete. This theoretical work suggests that stress should be conceptualized as having at least two dimensions: threat and deprivation (Humphreys & Zeanah, 2015; McLaughlin et al., 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Threat refers to experiences that threaten one’s physical integrity, such as physical abuse, neighborhood violence, or parental harshness. Greater threat exposure is related to atypical emotion processing and hippocampal functioning, as well as reduced ventromedial prefrontal cortex volume and/or thickness (McLaughlin et al., 2014). Deprivation refers to a lack of expected and needed environmental inputs, as seen with physical or emotional parental neglect/disengagement. These researchers argue, based on both human and non-human animal studies, that there are distinct neurobiological and behavioral signatures that result from experiences of threat versus deprivation. For instance, deprivation is associated with lower grey matter volume, changes in cortical activity, and difficulties with cognition, including executive function and language (McLaughlin et al., 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Non-human animal studies of environmental deprivation and enrichment also demonstrate neurobiological changes in response to the degree of environmental deprivation (Van Praag et al., 2000; Walsh, 1981).
These different neurobiological effects of threat versus deprivation may alter receptor distributions and sensitivities in brain regions that modulate HPA regulation, one consequence of which may be altered sensitivity to negative feedback at multiple levels of the HPA axis. This hypothesis has been tested in relation to cortisol reactivity to threat. In the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study, the cohort examined in the current analyses, childhood violence exposure—but not childhood social deprivation—was associated with blunted cortisol reactivity in adolescence (Peckins et al., 2020). Similarly, another study found that exposure to interpersonal violence (threat) was associated with blunted cortisol/DHEA reactivity to the Trier Social Stress Test in adolescents after controlling for poverty (which the authors conceptualized as deprivation in the study; Busso et al., 2017). Blunted HPA reactivity mediated the association between threat and externalizing behaviors. Similar mediational work has not yet been done using overall cortisol output (measured by hair cortisol concentration). White and colleagues (2017) tested hair cortisol concentration as a mediator between maltreatment and externalizing problems, but did not separate threat and deprivation within maltreatment. In the paper, early maltreatment and neglect each showed the strongest associations with lower hair cortisol concentration (White et al., 2017). This hypocortisolism could be a marker of allostatic load following the stress of neglect (McEwen, 1998), as neglect is typically a more chronic stressor occurring over longer time periods than abuse, which can be more intermittent over days and weeks, even if it occurs over long periods of time. This difference in time scales between neglect (deprivation) and abuse (threat) might explain the more frequent finding of hypocortisolism in those who have experienced chronic neglect versus abuse (Fisher, 2017). A recent study by Schalinski and colleagues (2019) separated maltreatment into neglect and abuse components and reported similar hair cortisol concentration findings. In this study, greater neglect at age 3 was associated with attenuated hair cortisol in adult inpatients with psychiatric conditions compared to patients with low neglect at age 3 and non-maltreated control participants (Schalinski et al., 2019). Patients with greater neglect at age 3 and low hair cortisol concentration were the most likely to show high trauma symptoms (Schalinski et al., 2019).
Research examining overall cortisol output as a mediator between threat and deprivation and later psychopathology would be valuable because overall cortisol secretion and acute reactivity may reflect different regulatory dynamics. Understanding both types of regulatory dynamics may help us better delineate this complex system and more fully characterize how threat and deprivation influence the HPA axis and lead to psychopathology. This may be particularly true in developmental studies as we may see changes in one measure of HPA functioning before another. Tracing the developmental sequence may be critical to fully illuminating the pathways from risk exposures to symptom expression.
Busso et al. (2017) linked altered acute cortisol reactivity to threat to earlier threat exposure, but not to deprivation, using poverty exposure as a proxy for deprivation. However, poverty is, itself, associated with neighborhood violence, domestic violence, and other types of threat (Evans, 2004). We were interested in better isolating the impact of deprivation as defined by a lack of needed care from the environment, so we chose to use measures of social, physical, and emotional parental disengagement as it would not be confounded by threatening experiences often associated with poverty. Previous investigations have statistically separated threat and deprivation although there are positive correlations between the two measures (Sumner et al., 2019). In this study, we focused on parental harshness as the measure of threat and parental disengagement as the measure of deprivation. Parental harshness reflects threatening experiences by the parent such as hitting, slapping, and spanking. Parental disengagement reflects a depriving environment, including experiences such as not providing food or medical care, being too drunk or high to care for the child, and not reading to or playing games with the child. Thus, these measures broadly cover experiences of threat and deprivation via parenting.
Parental harshness and disengagement often co-occur, which makes studying unique effects on HPA activity challenging, particularly in samples that are recruited for severe abuse experiences (e.g., White et al., 2017). Abuse is associated with higher cortisol levels closer to the onset of abuse, but may lead to down-regulation of the HPA axis in response to repeated cortisol elevations and result in lower levels of cortisol over time (Trickett et al., 2010). Thus, we may predict high cortisol levels would be predicted following recent-onset abuse, but lower cortisol levels years later. However, in a diverse sample of urban pregnant women, childhood physical and/or sexual abuse was associated with higher hair cortisol concentration in Black women (Schreier et al., 2015), suggesting that overall cortisol output may remain high for those who experienced abuse. A meta-analysis suggested that ongoing stress rather than past or absent stress is associated with higher cortisol output measured in hair (Stalder et al., 2017), which could suggest that individuals exposed to high levels of parental harshness in childhood with higher cortisol output could be experiencing continued stress. Thus, adolescents with high levels of recent parental harshness may have higher hair cortisol than adolescents without recent experiences of parental harshness, though adolescents with past but not recent experiences of parental harshness may show hair cortisol levels similar to or possibly lower than those without these experiences of harshness.
Studies of neglect in children typically report cortisol hyporeactivity to stress and a flattened diurnal cortisol rhythm (Bruce et al., 2009; Dozier et al., 2006; Koss et al., 2016; Reilly & Gunnar, 2019; van der Vegt et al., 2009). These studies usually involve children living in extreme situations such as foster care or children adopted from institutions (i.e., orphanages) into high-resource families. In the current study, we focused on depriving experiences termed parental disengagement, which are typically not as extreme as institutional care. Overall, associations between the HPA axis, parental harshness (threat), and parental disengagement (deprivation) are often not in the same direction, which supports the need to test parental harshness and disengagement separately in studies of HPA activity, particularly in families in the community facing high levels of adversity.
Overall cortisol output and psychopathology
Hair cortisol concentration is associated with several types of psychopathology, with different directions of effects depending on the type of psychopathology. For example, high overall cortisol output is more common in individuals with major depression and higher levels of depressive symptoms, while individuals with PTSD and externalizing problems typically show decreased overall cortisol output (Rietschel et al., 2016; Schalinski et al., 2015; Staufenbiel et al., 2013; White et al., 2017). Depression has been associated with higher cortisol reactivity to stress for individuals who have experienced maltreatment (Harkness et al., 2011; Heim et al., 2008) and in dysphoric post-pubertal adolescents (Hankin et al., 2010). However, some studies have reported blunted cortisol reactivity in adolescents with more severe depression regardless of maltreatment history (Harkness et al., 2011), while others show no increases in cortisol in response to negative daily events for depressed compared to non-depressed individuals (Peeters et al., 2003). In adolescents, higher levels of morning cortisol may predict onset of major depression (Goodyer et al., 2000), and flatter diurnal cortisol slopes are associated with past depressive episode, a recent depressive episode comorbid with an anxiety disorder, as well as current general distress (Doane et al., 2013).
Findings for associations between hair cortisol and anxiety are mixed. There is some evidence that individuals with generalized anxiety disorder have lower levels of hair cortisol compared to individuals without the disorder (Steudte et al., 2011), although results are inconsistent across studies and are likely complicated by comorbidities (Steudte-Schmiedgen et al., 2017). A higher cortisol awakening response may predict onset of anxiety disorders in adolescents (Adam et al., 2014). Comorbidity in psychopathology, particularly between depressive and anxiety symptoms, may explain some mixed findings in the literature, which is why the current study will examine several types of emotional and behavioral problems in the same model.
Meta-analytic evidence suggests that there is no association between externalizing problems and cortisol reactivity (Alink et al., 2008). However, externalizing problems are associated with a small but significant decrease in diurnal salivary cortisol (Alink et al., 2008). The association between overall cortisol output and symptomology thus appears to be complex, with different associations with differing types of psychopathology, and perhaps shaped by different pathways from early life adversity to differentially altered HPA axis regulation (perhaps not always visible in simple measures of cortisol levels) to different types of symptom profiles.
Developmental psychopathology framework
Considerations of how the type, timing, severity, and chronicity of stress impact adaptation are central to the developmental psychopathology framework (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 2009; Manly et al., 2001). Discrepancies in associations between childhood stress and heightened or reduced cortisol levels are puzzling, and these discrepancies are likely due, at least in part, to the heterogeneity and timing of stress, as well as different methods used in each study. If different profiles of childhood risk (i.e., parental harshness vs. disengagement) show unique associations with overall cortisol output over time, which then predict different types of adolescent symptomology (internalizing vs. externalizing), researchers will have a better understanding of how childhood stress can lead to a multitude of different outcomes (multifinality). The use of prospective, rather than retrospective measurements of parental harshness and disengagement, as done in the current study, is critical in efforts to determine if and how the timing of stress may lead to differing neurobiological and behavioral consequences. Careful neurobiological work in humans and in animal models provides evidence for sensitive periods of brain development that vary by brain region and neural networks (Knudsen, 2004). Differences in the timing of stress, intersecting with sensitive periods, may produce variable impacts on brain regions involved with HPA axis, and particularly on hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, which we know are influenced by development, rich in glucocorticoid receptors, and important in HPA axis regulatory control. These types of timing differences might help explain some of the discrepancies in the stress and HPA literature. The current study assessed whether prospectively-measured parental harshness and parental disengagement, and the timing of each, have differential associations with overall cortisol output in adolescence with implications for mental health.
This study seeks to contribute to the developmental psychopathology framework by testing a potential pathway by which childhood stress influences the development of psychopathology in adolescence, which is essential for understanding risk and resilience. Adolescence is a time of increased risk for psychopathology (Andersen & Teicher, 2008; Hankin et al., 1998; Lewinsohn et al., 2000), so establishing risk pathways across this developmental period is particularly important. By adding overall cortisol output as a potential mediator of associations between childhood and adolescent stress and adolescent mental health, this study will expand knowledge of how stress influences regulatory control of the HPA axis and the development of brain circuits that influence integrated cortisol output and associated behavioral proclivities that get reflected in “symptoms.”
The current study
The primary aim of the study was to test whether parental harshness and disengagement during childhood and adolescence are associated with hair cortisol concentration at 15 years. Our second aim was to test whether hair cortisol concentration at 15 years is a statistical mediator between parental harshness and disengagement during childhood and adolescence and depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behaviors at 15 years. A final, exploratory aim was to test whether the timing of parental harshness and disengagement between 1 and 15 years predicts hair cortisol concentration better than the average severity of parental harshness and disengagement from 1–15 years. The prospective assessment of parental harshness and disengagement starting in infancy and the multi-informant, multimodal assessment of psychopathology in adolescence make the current sample, a subset of the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study, ideal for asking these questions. The population of this study was oversampled for unwed mothers, so many more of these youth grew up in poverty and were exposed to more chronic and severe stressors compared to other studies while still having dimensional data on stress (e.g., not just maltreated versus controls).
We hypothesized that higher cumulative parental harshness from ages 1–15 years would be associated with higher hair cortisol concentration at age 15 based on the majority of papers showing positive associations between stress and overall cortisol output (Khoury et al., 2019). Due to previous research on post-institutionalized and foster care youth experiencing neglect, we predicted that higher cumulative parental disengagement from 1–15 would be associated with lower hair cortisol concentration at age 15 (Dozier et al., 2006; Koss et al., 2016). Based on previous literature, we predicted that higher hair cortisol concentration would be associated with higher depressive symptoms at age 15 and that lower hair cortisol concentration at age 15 would be associated with greater anxiety symptoms and antisocial behaviors at age 15. We hypothesized that there would be three significant indirect pathways: 1) from cumulative parental harshness to higher hair cortisol concentration to higher depressive symptoms, 2) from cumulative parental disengagement to lower hair cortisol concentration to higher anxiety symptoms, and 3) from cumulative parental disengagement to lower hair cortisol concentration to higher antisocial behaviors. We hypothesized that cumulative parental harshness and disengagement rather than parental harshness and disengagement at a specific time point (1, 3, 5, 9, or 15) would be better predictors of overall cortisol output.
Methods
Participants
The Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS; Reichman et al., 2001) is a population-based study of children who were born in large US cities, oversampling for non-marital births. At the beginning of the FFCWS, 42.2% of mothers reported their household income in the past 12 months was less than or equal to $25,000, while 60.5% of mothers reported their household income less than or equal to $50,000. FFCWS families were interviewed at the focal child’s birth and again at 1, 3, 5, 9, and 15 years of age. A subset of the FFCWS at the Detroit, Toledo, and Chicago sites underwent a more detailed in-person assessment at 15–17 years (M = 15.3 years) that included clinical assessments, neuroimaging, and collection of biological samples as part of the Study of Adolescent Neural Development (SAND; N = 237). A total of 175 participants provided hair samples at 15 years (the final analytic sample was 171 after removing participants with high cortisol values). Sixty-two participants did not provide hair samples for the following reasons: hair too short to collect (n = 33), youth or parent refused (n = 17), youth had braids, weave, dreadlocks or hairpiece that prevented collection (n = 6), or reason unclear (n = 6). The participants who provided a hair sample at 15 did not differ on maternal marital status at birth, depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, or antisocial behaviors (described below), medication use, classification as African American or multiracial vs. not African American or multiracial, maternal education, poverty status at birth, and poverty status at 15 compared to participants who did not provide a hair sample, ps > .05. Adolescent females, χ2 (1) = 13.22, p <. 001, and adolescents with older mothers at birth, M = 26.2 years vs. 24.1 years, t(234) = −2.44, p = .02, were more likely to provide hair samples within the SAND subset. The sample used in these analyses is economically high-risk (75.4% of mothers not married at birth; 40.9% reported household incomes below 100% of the poverty line at 15) and historically underrepresented in research (76% African American or Black; see Table 1 for descriptive statistics).
Table 1.
Measure | N (%) | Mean (SD) |
---|---|---|
Gender | ||
Female | 102 (59.6) | |
Male | 69 (40.4) | |
Race/Ethnicity | ||
Black/African American | 130 (76.0) | |
White | 24 (14.0) | |
Hispanic/Latinx | 8 (4.7) | |
Multiracial, non-Hispanic | 7 (4.1) | |
Other, non-Hispanic | 2 (1.2) | |
Mother married at child’s birth | 42 (24.6) | |
Mother’s age at child’s birth (years) | 26.2 (6.1) | |
Mother’s education at child’s birth | ||
Less than high school | 56 (32.9) | |
High school or equivalent | 44 (25.9) | |
Some college or technical school | 48 (28.2) | |
College grad | 22 (12.9) | |
Poverty level at 15y | ||
0–49% | 31 (18.1) | |
50–99% | 39 (22.8) | |
100–199% | 47 (27.5) | |
200–299% | 21 (12.3) | |
300%+ | 33 (19.3) | |
Child age at 15y (years) | 15.3 (0.4) |
N = 171. Calculated on participants with valid data values for overall cortisol output (within 3 SD of the mean).
Childhood parental harshness/disengagement
Our overarching goal was to create constructs that theoretically match threat and deprivation outlined in previous theoretical work (Humphreys & Zeanah, 2015; McLaughlin et al., 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Childhood experiences of parental harshness and disengagement were reported prospectively at ages 1, 3, 5, 9, and 15 years. The mother reported experiences at 1 year, and the primary caregiver reported experiences from 3–9 years. Both the primary caregiver and focal child reported experiences at 15. Variables that closely matched our constructs of interests were the same across the ages 3, 5, and 9 waves. However, variables were different for 1 and 15 years, and new variables were chosen to most closely match parental harshness and disengagement at those ages with the available variables. All variable names, respondents, questions, and response options are included in the supplemental methods including correlations between parental harshness and disengagement at each time point and means and standard deviations of each parental harshness and disengagement composite.
Parental harshness
At age 1 year, parental harshness included frequency of spanking by mother, father, and mother’s partner due to evidence from FFCWS that spanking at age 1 is developmentally inappropriate and a risk factor for later child protective services involvement (Lee et al., 2014). Spanking frequency was reverse-coded so that higher numbers indicate greater spanking frequency (see supplemental materials for specific variables). This variable was a sum of how often the child was spanked in the past month (0 = never spanked, 4 = spanked every day), which was summed across a total of the 3 individuals (possible range: 0–12). This sum was standardized to create the age 1 parental harshness variable. At ages 3, 5, and 9, a total of 10 items assessed frequency of parental harshness from the Conflict Tactics Scale corporal punishment and psychological aggression subscales (Straus et al., 1998), including items such as slapping, shaking, pinching, hitting, spanking, threatening, screaming at, and swearing at the child. Items were re-coded in the following way such that higher numbers indicate greater frequency of parental harshness: 0 = never happened in the past year, 1 = once, 2 = twice, 3 = 3–5 times, 4 = 6–10 times, 5 = 11–20 times, 6 = >20 times. All 10 items were summed within each time point to create parental harshness at 3, 5, and 9 years. These sums were then standardized. Reliability for the 10 items of parental harshness from the Conflict Tactics Scale was α = 0.81 at 3 and 5 years and was α = 0.96 at 9 years. At 15 years, the focal child was asked two questions from the Conflict Tactics Scale about how often their primary caregiver does the following: 1) shouts, yells, screams, swears or curses at you; 2) hits or slaps you. Replies were 1 = never, 2 = sometimes, and 3 = often. The primary caregiver was also asked the same two questions about their behavior towards the child with the same response options. These four variables were standardized and the mean was calculated. This mean was standardized for the parental harshness at 15 years variable. The cumulative parental harshness variable was a mean of the five standardized parental harshness variables from ages 1–15.
Parental disengagement
The neglect scale of the Conflict Tactics Scale that was measured at 3, 5, and 9 years was identified as being a valid measure of more severe parental disengagement in FFCWS. As this scale was not given at ages 1 and 15 years, we chose items relevant to parental disengagement that were in line with the Conflict Tactics Scale neglect scale described below. Parental disengagement at 1 year was a measure of seven variables chosen among all questions at this time point as being relevant to parental disengagement. Questions include six variables that have been previously used as part of a scale to assess maternal engagement in FFCWS that were also relevant to our measure of parental disengagement (α = .66; Turney & McLanahan, 2015) and one to assess lack of food for the child: 1) how often the mother played games like “peek-a-boo” and “gotcha” with the focal child (recoded as 0 = every day, 1 = several times per week, 2 = several times per month, 3 = 1–2 times per month, 4 = never); 2) how often the mother sang songs or nursery rhymes to the child (coded as above); 3) how often the mother read stores to the child (coded as above); 4) how often the mother told stories to the child (coded as above); 5) how often the mother played inside with toys such as blocks or Legos with the child (coded as above); 6) how frequently the mother hugged or showed physical affection toward the focal child (coded as above); 7) whether the child went hungry in the past year (coded as 0 = no, 1 = yes). These variables were standardized, and the mean was disengagement at age 1 year. Disengagement is in part defined as a lack of cognitive stimulation and sensory, motor, linguistic, and social input (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Nelson, 2017), which is in line with what the maternal engagement scale measures (e.g., no hugs, no reading or telling stories to child, no or playing games with child). Particularly at 1 year of age, parental engagement is a main source of cognitive, sensory, linguistic, and social input as it is more difficult for infants to navigate their world without parents. With the addition of the hunger question, this composite represents sensory, cognitive, linguistic, physical, emotional, and social disengagement at 1 year. At ages 3, 5, and 9, a total of 5 items assessed frequency of disengagement using the neglect scale of the Conflict Tactics Scale (Straus et al., 1998), including items such as not being able to express their love to their child, not able to give the child food or medical care, leaving the child alone, and being too drunk or high to care for their child. Items were re-coded in the following way such that higher numbers indicate greater frequency of disengagement: 0 = never happened in the past year, 1 = once, 2 = twice, 3 = 3–5 times, 4 = 6–10 times, 5 = 11–20 times, 6 = >20 times. All 5 items were summed within each time point to create disengagement at 3, 5, and 9 years. These sums were then standardized. The reliability for the neglect scale from age 3–9 years is low-to-moderate (α= .47 at 3 years; α= .15 at 5 years; α= .56 at 9 years), which is consistent with prior studies and is expected based on skewed distributions and reporting on low-baseline events (Guterman et al., 2009; Straus et al., 1998). However, the measure does have temporal consistency (r = .26-.36, p < .001, for correlations of the neglect scale from ages 3, 5, and 9 years). Parental disengagement at 15 years was measured by six items, five of which were reported by the primary caregiver, and one reported by the focal child. These items were not part of an established questionnaire but were chosen among all possible questions as relevant to disengagement at 15. Primary caregiver questions on substance use asked about 1) how often drinking interfered with responsibilities in past year; 2) how often the caregiver had problems with people because of drinking in past year; 3) how often illegal drug use interfered with responsibilities in past year; and 4) how often the caregiver had problems with people in the past year due to illegal drug use. Answers were coded as 0 = never, 1= one time, or 2 = more than one time. Parental substance abuse is a risk factor for poorer offspring mental health and drug and alcohol problems and is also a risk factor for child maltreatment (Osborne & Berger, 2009; Walsh et al., 2003), and the items used in the current study specifically assess functional impairment rather than substance use in general.1 The caregiver was also asked whether the youth had had a regular check-up or well visit with a health professional in the past 12 months, which was coded as 1 = yes and 2 = no. The child was asked how often he or she spends time alone in his or her home without an adult present. Answers were coded as 0 = never or sometimes, 1 = often. Being alone at home often can lead to the failure of the parent to provide for their youth’s physical and emotional needs (Hymel, 2006). These six items were z-scored, and the mean of the z-scored items was used for the age 15 disengagement variable. The cumulative parental disengagement variable was a mean of the five standardized disengagement variables from ages 1–15. This conceptualization of disengagement is consistent with previous tests of deprivation, with physical and emotional disengagement as well as food insecurity as measures of deprivation (Sumner et al., 2019).
Adolescent Mental Health
Depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behavior were assessed using a multi-informant, multi-method latent variable approach. Continuous measures of psychopathology rather than diagnoses of psychopathology were used to better capture the dimensional nature of psychopathology (Cuthbert, 2014). Latent variables represent shared variances in the observed variables to measure the underlying concept of interest (Bollen, 2002).
Depressive symptoms
We assessed depressive symptoms at 15 using a latent variable that consisted of the following scales: (1) youth-reported Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (MFQ) total (Wood et al., 1995) which asked about the youth’s feelings in the past 2 weeks and were rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = sometimes true, 2 = true; α= 0.91); (2) parent-reported MFQ total (α= 0.92); (3) the youth-report Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) total (Saylor et al., 1984) which used 3-point Likert scales to assess which option best described the youth’s feelings over the past 2 weeks (α= 0.86); (4) parent-reported CDI total which used 4-point Likert scales (0 = not at all, 1 = sometimes, 2 = often, 3 = most times) (α= 0.84); (5) total current symptom count including subclinical symptoms (0 = not present; 1 = present at subclinical levels; 2 = present at clinical threshold) of DSM-5 major depressive disorder from the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (K-SADS), which was completed by the parent and youth; and (6) total current symptom count of DSM-5 persistent depressive disorder (dysthymia) from the K-SADS. K-SADS scores were determined by clinician ratings on a modified version of the K-SADS (KSADS; Kaufman et al., 1997). The K-SADS is a semi-structured interview that has high test-retest reliability in establishing psychiatric diagnoses and has good validity with standard self-report measures of symptoms such as the Child Behavior Checklist (Kaufman et al., 1997). A trained clinical interviewer, typically a psychology doctoral student or post-baccalaureate staff member, administered the K-SADS separately to the youth and primary caregiver. Interviewers were trained using practice interviews and live supervision of interviews. All diagnostic interviews (100%) were reviewed during weekly team diagnostic meetings that included all clinicians and two faculty investigators who are licensed clinical psychologists with decades of experience with the K-SADS. Final current symptoms counts were derived by consensus of the clinical team using best-estimate procedures (Maziade et al., 1992) based on the parent and child report. Consensus diagnosis is the gold standard reliability check, and since the consensus diagnosis was performed on all cases and for every symptom, there was no need to calculate inter-rater reliability within the sample. The CDI shows good internal consistency and is able to identify children with emotional distress (Saylor et al., 1984). The MFQ shows acceptable reliability, is a satisfactory screen for major depressive disorder, and is able to identify clinical remission (Wood et al., 1995).
Latent factor scores were extracted in Mplus and used in the current analyses.2 Factor scores were calculated instead of calculating the latent variable within the model due to problems with model fit. There was a high (r >.95) correlation between the child report of the MFQ and the SCARED, which caused the depressive and anxiety symptoms latent variables to be correlated at greater than 1. Extracting latent factor scores in separate models eliminated this problem such that the final depressive and anxiety factor scores were correlated at 0.27, p < .01, in bivariate analyses (Table 2), which did not negatively impact model fit. The model fit for the depressive symptoms confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was excellent (CFI = .99, RMSEA = .03, SRMR = .03). All individual variables were significantly associated with the latent depressive symptoms variable, p < .01. Standardized factor loadings for each of the variables on the latent depressive symptoms factor follow: MFQ child report (0.54, p < .001), MFQ parent report (0.32, p < .001), CDI child report (0.83, p < .001), CDI parent report (0.33, p < .001), K-SADS major depressive episode (0.36, p < .001), and K-SADS persistent depressive disorder (0.17, p = .004).
Table 2.
Variable | 1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | 10. | 11. | 12. | 13. |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Cumulative Harshness (1–15y) | -- | ||||||||||||
2. Cumulative Disengagement (1–15y) | .32*** | ||||||||||||
3. Overall cortisol output (15y) | .25** | −.09 | -- | ||||||||||
4. Depressive Symptoms (15y) | −.09 | .02 | .12 | -- | |||||||||
5. Anxiety Symptoms (15y) | .05 | .07 | .07 | .27** | -- | ||||||||
6. Antisocial Behaviors (15y) | .20** | .14† | .00 | .14† | .20** | -- | |||||||
7. Female | −.19* | −.05 | .07 | .24** | .08 | −.11 | -- | ||||||
8. African American or Biracial | .30*** | .05 | .43*** | .01 | .02 | .16* | .07 | -- | |||||
9. Maternal Education at Child Birth | −.18* | −.18* | −.10 | .09 | −.14† | −.16* | .00 | −.18* | -- | ||||
10. Maternal Age at Child Birth | −.22** | −.10 | −.20* | .01 | −.04 | −.12 | .03 | −.22** | .39*** | -- | |||
11. Poverty ratio at birth | −.21** | −.11 | −.29*** | .07 | −.11 | −.12 | −.04 | −.37*** | .60*** | .43*** | -- | ||
12. Poverty ratio at 15y | −.22** | −.09 | −.26** | .05 | −.12 | −.17* | .05 | −.28*** | .48*** | .19* | .40*** | -- | |
13. Adolescent age | −.04 | −.03 | .17* | .05 | .04 | .11 | −.14† | .09 | −.13 | −.19* | −.09 | −.09 | -- |
14. Parents married at child’s birth | −.11 | −.05 | −.22** | .10 | −.09 | −.16* | .08 | −.26** | .42*** | .43*** | .55*** | .36*** | −.07 |
N = 171. Correlations computed using SPSS v25.
p < 0.10
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
Anxiety symptoms
We assessed anxiety symptoms at age 15 using a latent variable that consisted of the following scales: (1) youth-reported Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) (Hale et al., 2005) (α= 0.92); (2) parent-reported SCARED (α= 0.88); (3) anxiety symptoms scale from the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1991) where items were rated by parents on a 3-point scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, 2 = very true or often true) (α= 0.78); and (4) total current symptom count rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not present; 1 = present at subclinical levels; 2 = present at clinical threshold) of DSM-5 social phobia from the K-SADS. K-SADS scores were determined using the same consensus meetings as for depressive symptoms above. The SCARED demonstrates good internal consistency and discriminant validity between anxiety disorders and other disorders (Birmaher et al., 1997). The CBCL is a widely-used measure with good internal consistency, and the anxiety scale of the CBCL predicts DSM-IV diagnoses, demonstrating external validity (Achenbach & Edelbrock, 1991; Ferdinand, 2008).
Latent factor scores were extracted in Mplus and used in the analyses. Model fit for the anxiety symptoms CFA was excellent (CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .00, SRMR = .00). The SCARED child and parent report and the CBCL were all significantly associated with the anxiety factor, while the K-SADS had a p-value of 0.14. The K-SADS was left in the model due to its theoretical relevance to the construct. Standardized factor loadings for each of the variables on the latent anxiety symptoms factor follow: SCARED child report (0.17, p = .04), SCARED parent report (0.99, p < .001), CBCL (0.60, p < .001), K-SADS (0.11, p = .14).
Antisocial behavior
We assessed age 15 antisocial behavior via a latent variable approach using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén, 2014): (1) the aggression scale of the CBCL (α= 0.89) and (2) the rule-breaking scale of the CBCL (α= 0.82); (3) total score (excluding substance use items) from the youth-reported 62-item Self Report of Delinquency (SRD) (Elliott et al., 1985) (α= 0.87); and (4) total current symptom count of DSM-5 conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder rated on a 3-point scale (0 = not present; 1 = present at subclinical levels; 2 = present at clinical threshold). KSADS scores were determined using the same consensus meetings as for depressive and anxiety symptoms above. The SRD demonstrates good validity, with individuals who have been arrested or have gone to court showing higher SRD scores than those without these experiences (Huizinga & Elliott, 1986).
Latent factor scores were extracted in Mplus and used in the current analyses. Model fit for the antisocial behavior CFA was excellent (CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = .07, SRMR = .01). Standardized factor loadings for each of the variables on the latent antisocial behavior factor follow: CBCL aggression (0.70, p < .001), CBCL rule breaking (0.76, p < .001), SRD (0.49, p < .001), and K-SADS (0.87, p < .001).
Overall Cortisol Output via Hair
Hair samples were collected via standardized procedures at 15 years. A research assistant (RA) explained the procedure to participants, answered any questions they had to ensure understanding, and obtained consent. Participants then filled out a questionnaire about hair treatments such as straightening, coloring, and frequency of hair washing. A small section of hair at the posterior vertex of the head was tied with thread. The RA then cut the tied strands as close to the scalp as possible without pulling the hair. This procedure was repeated two more times in slightly different locations around the posterior vertex. The collected hair was placed in foil, and the thread was taped to the foil with the scalp-near ends together and clearly labeled. Foil was folded without folding any hair and stored at room temperature until all samples could be shipped together to Dr. Clemens Kirschbaum’s lab at Technische Universität Dresden.
A liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometry-based method was used to quantify hair cortisol concentration in hair samples (Gao et al., 2013). If there was enough hair volume in 1cm of hair for the assay, 1cm was used. If there was not enough volume within a 1cm segment, 2cm were used. We chose to include participants with at least 1cm of hair to avoid biasing the sample, which would have excluded any participants with short hair. Of the 171 with valid cortisol data, 126 samples used 1cm of hair and 45 samples used 2cm. Concentrations were calculated as picograms of cortisol per milligram of hair. All hair was washed with isopropanol. Steroid hormones were extracted by methanol incubation from 10 mg whole, non-pulverized hair. A column switching strategy was applied for on-line solid phase extraction. Then, analyte detection on an AB Sciex API 5000 QTrap mass spectrometer was conducted. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation (CVs) ranged from 3.7 and 9.1%. Quantification limits were below (or equal to) 0.1 pg/mg. All samples were run in a single batch. If lab personnel were concerned about a high or low value and there was enough hair left after the first assay, a second assay was conducted. Cortisol values were ln-transformed for analysis to correct for skewness and values above 3 SD were removed (4 values total, yielding 171 participants with valid cortisol data).
Data analytic plan
Cumulative (ages 1–15) parental harshness and disengagement model
A correlation table of study variables is shown in Table 2. Longitudinal structural equation modeling with bootstrapping (10,000 iterations) in Mplus was used to test whether hair cortisol concentration across 1–2 months mediated the association between parental harshness and disengagement from 1–15 years and depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behavior problems. Model fit was examined (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2015) using the comparative fit index (CFI; > .93), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; < .06), standardized root mean square residual (SRMR; < .08). The following were tested as covariates for parental harshness, disengagement, hair cortisol concentration, depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behavior: poverty ratio at focal child’s birth (mother’s household income/poverty threshold), sex, race/ethnicity (African American or multiracial vs. not African American or multiracial; initial analyses demonstrated that these groups showed higher cortisol levels than other groups), maternal education (1 = less than high school, 2 = high school or equivalent, 3 = some college, 4 = college degree or more), mother’s marital status (1 = married or 0 = not married), and mother’s age at focal child’s birth. For hair cortisol concentration, depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behavior, the following covariates were tested in addition to those above: poverty at 15 (1 = 0–49% of poverty line, 2 = 50–99%, 3 = 100–199%, 4 = 200–299%, 5 = 300%+), and current age. Guided by meta-analyses of determinants of hair cortisol concentration in children and adults (Gray et al., 2018; Stalder et al., 2017), we also tested whether hair treatments such as frequency of hair washing, use of chemical treatment on hair, or straightening, blow drying, and hair curling (1 = uses treatment, 0 = does not use treatment), medication use (0 = no medications or no medications with effects on cortisol, 1 = medication with possible effects on cortisol, and 2 = medication with known effects on cortisol [e.g., inhalers, birth control, psychotropic medications]) (Granger, Hibel, Fortunato, & Kapelewski, 2009), length of hair used (1cm vs. 2cm), and body mass index (BMI) z-score predicted hair cortisol concentration. To enhance model parsimony, a covariate was kept in the model if it showed an association with the variable at p < .10. A covariate that was associated with one variable may not be associated with other variables in the model and paths with p-values greater than .10 were removed (final covariates shown in Table 3).
Table 3.
β | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|
Cumulative harshness from 1–15y | ||
Female | −0.19** | −0.33, −0.05 |
Maternal age | −0.14* | −0.27, −0.01 |
African American or multiracial | 0.28*** | 0.15, 0.41 |
Cumulative disengagement from 1–15y | ||
Maternal education | −0.16** | −0.27, −0.04 |
Overall cortisol output at 15y | ||
Cumulative harshness from 1–15y | 0.20** | 0.05, 0.34 |
Cumulative disengagement from 1–15y | −0.17** | −0.27, −0.07 |
African American or multiracial | 0.31*** | 0.20, 0.42 |
Hair straightening | 0.21** | 0.08, 0.34 |
Poverty ratio at 15y | −0.11† | −0.23, 0.02 |
Hair quantity (2cm versus 1cm) | 0.16* | 0.03, 0.30 |
Poverty ratio at birth | −0.11† | −0.24, 0.01 |
Depressive symptoms at 15y | ||
Overall cortisol output at 15y | 0.14† | −0.02, 0.30 |
Cumulative harshness from 1–15y | −0.11 | −0.27, 0.05 |
Cumulative disengagement from 1–15y | 0.08 | −0.05, 0.20 |
Female | 0.21** | 0.08, 0.33 |
Anxiety symptoms at 15y | ||
Overall cortisol output at 15y | 0.06 | −0.07, 0.19 |
Cumulative harshness from 1–15y | −0.01 | −0.15, 0.14 |
Cumulative disengagement from 1–15y | 0.05 | −0.11, 0.20 |
Maternal education | −0.14* | −0.28, −0.01 |
Antisocial behaviors at 15y | ||
Overall cortisol output at 15y | −0.12 | −0.29, 0.06 |
Cumulative harshness from 1–15y | 0.15* | 0.02, 0.29 |
Cumulative disengagement from 1–15y | 0.07 | −0.10, 0.24 |
Parents married at child birth | −0.15** | −0.26, −0.04 |
African American or multiracial | 0.13* | 0.01, 0.26 |
Note. All estimates reported are standardized estimates and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for each of the direct pathways to behavior problems. Dependent variables are in bold with independent variables and associated standardized (β) coefficients. Note. Covariates were included in the final model if they were associated with the model variable at p < .10.
p < 0.10
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
Cross-lagged timing of parental harshness and disengagement model
Secondary analyses were conducted to understand whether parental harshness or disengagement at a particular time point (1, 3, 5, 9, or 15) was driving any associations between parental harshness or disengagement and hair cortisol concentration at 15 years. A cross-lagged model was conducted with parental harshness and disengagement at each of the time points predicting hair cortisol concentration as a mediator and depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behaviors as dependent variables. Paths from parental harshness and disengagement at the previous time point were tested, and within-time paths between parental harshness and disengagement were tested. Covariates were pared in the same manner as above, and parental harshness and disengagement variables not associated with hair cortisol concentration, depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behaviors were pared in the same way to understand unique associations with parental harshness and disengagement at a particular time point with hair cortisol concentration and mental health symptoms (final covariates shown in Table 4).
Table 4.
β | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|
Parental harshness at 1y | ||
African American or multiracial | 0.19*** | 0.09, 0.29 |
Parental disengagement at 1y | ||
Female | −0.14† | −0.29, 0.01 |
Parental harshness at 3y | ||
Parental harshness at 1y | 0.22* | 0.02, 0.42 |
Parental disengagement at 1y | −0.04 | −0.18, 0.10 |
Female | −0.13† | −0.28, 0.02 |
Poverty ratio at birth | −0.18* | −0.31, −0.04 |
Parental disengagement at 3y | ||
Parental harshness at 1y | 0.04 | −0.06, 0.15 |
Parental disengagement at 1y | 0.20 | −0.07, 0.46 |
Maternal education | −0.19** | −0.31, −0.08 |
Parental harshness at 5y | ||
Parental harshness at 3y | 0.46*** | 0.27, 0.65 |
Parental disengagement at 3y | 0.02 | −0.13, 0.16 |
Female | −0.12† | −0.27, 0.02 |
Maternal education | −0.14* | −0.26, −0.01 |
African American or multiracial | 0.15* | 0.01, 0.28 |
Parental disengagement at 5y | ||
Parental harshness at 3y | −0.06 | −0.30, 0.18 |
Parental disengagement at 3y | 0.44* | 0.08, 0.80 |
Maternal age | −0.12* | −0.24, −0.01 |
Parental harshness at 9y | ||
Parental harshness at 5y | 0.50*** | 0.38, 0.63 |
Parental disengagement at 5y | 0.00 | −0.16, 0.16 |
Female | −0.16* | −0.29, −0.03 |
Parents married at child’s birth | 0.16* | 0.03, 0.29 |
African American or multiracial | 0.15* | 0.04, 0.27 |
Parental disengagement at 9y | ||
Parental harshness at 5y | 0.17† | −0.03, 0.38 |
Parental disengagement at 5y | 0.21 | −0.12, 0.55 |
Poverty ratio at birth | −0.10** | −0.18, −0.03 |
Parental harshness at 15y | ||
Parental harshness at 9y | 0.45*** | 0.31, 0.58 |
Parental disengagement at 9y | −0.05 | −0.18, 0.08 |
Parents married at child’s birth | −0.17** | −0.30, −0.05 |
Parental disengagement at 15y | ||
Parental harshness at 9y | −0.07 | −0.20, 0.07 |
Parental disengagement at 9y | 0.06 | −0.14, 0.26 |
Overall cortisol output at 15y | ||
Parental disengagement at 1y | −0.13** | −0.20, −0.05 |
Parental harshness at 15y | 0.14* | 0.02, 0.26 |
African American or multiracial | 0.36*** | 0.26, 0.46 |
Hair straightening | 0.21** | 0.09, 0.34 |
Poverty ratio at 15y | −0.16** | −0.28, −0.04 |
Hair quantity (2cm versus 1cm) | 0.17* | 0.03, 0.31 |
Depressive symptoms at 15y | ||
Overall cortisol output at 15y | 0.15† | −0.02, 0.31 |
Parental disengagement at 1y | 0.11* | 0.003, 0.22 |
Female | 0.23*** | 0.11, 0.35 |
Parents married at child’s birth | 0.14† | −0.01, 0.30 |
Anxiety symptoms at 15y | ||
Overall cortisol output at 15y | 0.07 | −0.07, 0.20 |
Parental harshness at 3y | −0.21* | −0.38, −0.05 |
Parental disengagement at 3y | 0.20* | 0.03, 0.38 |
Parental harshness at 15 years | 0.17* | 0.02, 0.31 |
Antisocial behaviors at 15y | ||
Overall cortisol output at 15y | −0.16† | −0.33, 0.01 |
Parental harshness at 3y | −0.19† | −0.38, 0.01 |
Parental harshness at 15y | 0.40*** | 0.23, 0.56 |
Poverty ratio at 15y | −0.15* | −0.28, −0.02 |
African American or multiracial | 0.16** | 0.05, 0.27 |
Note. All estimates reported are standardized estimates and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for each of the direct pathways to behavior problems. Dependent variables are in bold with independent variables and associated standardized (β) coefficients. Note. Covariates were included in the final model if they were associated with the model variable at p < .10.
p < 0.10
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
Sex differences and curvilinear analyses
Sex differences in the cumulative model were tested. Curvilinear analyses were conducted in the cumulative model to understand whether high levels of parental harshness or disengagement were associated with either heightened or reduced hair cortisol concentration (quadratic) rather than with only high or low levels (linear). The ln-transformed hair cortisol concentration was centered and squared for this analysis. The model remained the same as the cumulative parental harshness and disengagement from 1–15 model described above except that the squared log-transformed cortisol variable was also added as a mediator in addition to the log-transformed cortisol variable (see supplemental information).
Results
Parental harshness, disengagement, and hair cortisol concentration
Greater cumulative parental harshness from 1–15 predicted higher hair cortisol concentration at 15 (β= 0.20, 95% CI: 0.05 to 0.34, p=.007), while greater cumulative disengagement from 1–15 predicted lower hair cortisol concentration (β= −0.17, 95% CI: −0.27 to −0.07, p=.001; Figures 1 and 2). This model demonstrated excellent fit (CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00, SRMR = .04). R2 values for the following key endogenous variables are as follows: hair cortisol concentration (0.33), depressive symptoms (0.08), anxiety symptoms (0.03), and antisocial behavior (0.09).
Hair cortisol concentration and mental health
In the same model, higher hair cortisol concentration at 15 was not significantly associated with depressive symptoms (β= 0.14, 95% CI: −0.02 to 0.30, p=.08), anxiety symptoms (β= 0.06, 95% CI: −0.07 to 0.19, p=.36) or antisocial behaviors (β= −0.12, 95% CI: −0.29 to 0.06, p=.18). None of the indirect paths from cumulative parental harshness or disengagement to depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, or antisocial behaviors through hair cortisol concentration were statistically significant, ps > .05. Cumulative parental harshness from 1–15 directly predicted greater age 15 antisocial problems (β=0.15, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.29, p=.03) above other paths in the model. Parental harshness and disengagement showed positive covariance (β= 0.30, 95% CI: 0.15 to 0.44, p<.001). Depressive symptoms were positively associated with anxiety symptoms (β=0.27, 95% CI: 0.12 to 0.41, p<.001), and antisocial behaviors (β=0.21, 95% CI: 0.06 to 0.36, p=.006). Anxiety symptoms were positively associated with antisocial behaviors (β=0.18, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.34, p=.03).
Timing
Results of the timing analysis demonstrated that parental harshness and disengagement at two time points were most highly associated with hair cortisol concentration at 15 years. Greater disengagement at 1 year was associated with lower hair cortisol concentration (β= −0.13, 95% CI: −0.20 to −0.05, p=.002), and greater parental harshness at 15 years was associated with higher hair cortisol concentration (β= 0.14, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.26, p=.02; Figure 3). Greater disengagement at 1 year was directly associated with greater depressive symptoms at 15 years (β= 0.11, 95% CI: 0.003 to 0.22, p=.04). Greater disengagement at 3 years (β= 0.20, 95% CI: 0.03 to 0.38, p=.02) and at 15 years (β= 0.17, 95% CI: 0.02 to 0.31, p=.02) was associated with greater anxiety at 15 years, though greater parental harshness at 3 years was associated with lower anxiety at 15 years (β= −0.21, 95% CI: −0.38 to −0.05, p=.01). Greater parental harshness at 15 years was directly associated with more antisocial behaviors at 15 years (β= 0.40, 95% CI: 0.23 to 0.56, p<.001). This model demonstrated good fit (CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.03, SRMR = .06). R2 values for the key endogenous variables are as follows: hair cortisol concentration (0.32), depressive symptoms (0.10), anxiety symptoms (0.08), and antisocial behavior (0.22). These findings suggest that disengagement at 1 year and parental harshness at 15 years may be driving the associations of ages 1–15 parental harshness and disengagement with hair cortisol concentration at 15 years. Results of the model also suggest that parental harshness at 15 years may be driving the association between ages 1–15 parental harshness and greater antisocial behaviors at 15 years.
Sex differences
Models where the five main pathways (parental harshness and disengagement to cortisol, cortisol to depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behaviors) were constrained between males and females one at a time, each in separate models, were compared to a model where all paths were free to vary by sex suggested that there were no differences in model fit, all p > .05. Thus, there were no significant sex differences between any of the main paths in the model (see supplemental information).
Curvilinear analyses
As standardized regression estimates cannot be interpreted in the same way for power terms, the unstandardized results were used. These analyses revealed no curvilinear associations between hair cortisol concentration and parental harshness from 1–15, p = .33, or disengagement from 1–15, p = 0.09. There were no curvilinear associations between hair cortisol concentration and depressive symptoms, p = .78, anxiety symptoms, p = .84, or antisocial behaviors, p = .88. There were also no increases in R2 values for any of the mental health variables with the addition of the squared cortisol value, and the R2 value for the squared cortisol value was not statistically significant. This evaluation of R2 values suggests that the addition of the quadratic cortisol term did not improve model fit (Cohen et al., 2003) (see supplemental information for more model results).
Discussion
The current study demonstrated that cumulative parental harshness from ages 1–15 years was associated with higher overall cortisol output at 15, while cumulative parental disengagement from 1–15 predicted lower overall cortisol output at 15. Higher hair cortisol concentration at 15 (representing overall cortisol output over the past 1–2 months) was not significantly associated with depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, or antisocial behaviors at 15 years. There was a timing effect such that age 15 parental harshness was particularly associated with higher hair cortisol concentration, while greater parental disengagement at age 1 uniquely predicted lower hair cortisol concentration at age 15. These findings add to knowledge of how different types of childhood stress are associated with overall cortisol output. The longitudinal nature of the study and prospective assessment of parental harshness and disengagement make these results particularly informative, given known discrepancies between prospective and retrospective reports of maltreatment (Baldwin et al., 2019).
Most studies of psychosocial stress and hair cortisol concentration show positive associations (Khoury et al., 2019; Staufenbiel et al., 2013), which are consistent with our finding of greater cumulative parental harshness from ages 1–15 predicting higher hair cortisol concentration at 15. Our analyses of timing effects suggest that age 15 parental harshness may have stronger associations with hair cortisol concentration than parental harshness at other time points, pointing to adolescence—or the past year—as periods during which parental harshness may shape adolescent HPA functioning. This finding is in line with the idea that a harsh and threatening environment over time may produce more occasions for the HPA axis to respond to and recover from stressors, which may shape the HPA axis and receptors over time to affect cortisol output at age 15. A harsh environment may also shape the brain’s responses to threat. For example, high levels of threat may lead to changes in threat detection and threat circuitry (Dannlowski et al., 2013; Pollak & Tolley-Schell, 2003), which could lead individuals to selectively attend to negative stimuli, process neutral stimuli as threatening, and respond accordingly. There is preliminary evidence that threat exposure is associated with increased stress perception in females, which is then associated with higher morning cortisol levels (LoPilato et al., 2019), suggesting a possible mechanism by which threat may lead to heightened cortisol secretion. Heightened threat may also alter emotion processing such that individuals may have more negative emotions or trouble regulating emotion (Kim & Cicchetti, 2010; Kim et al., 2013). A chronically harsh and threatening environment could lead to individuals having difficulties with returning to baseline after responding to frequent stressors which could lead to higher cortisol output. These more regular perturbations may also lead to problems with the system’s regulatory dynamics, which could lead to a chronic stress response if glucocorticoid receptors remain occupied (Gunnar et al., 2015; Gunnar & Vazquez, 2006). Heightened threat has been associated with changes in the hippocampus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and sometimes the amygdala (Gold et al., 2016; Hanson et al., 2015), all of which have indirect inputs to the HPA axis and could lead to chronic activation and higher cortisol output (Herman et al., 2016; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). There is evidence that high cortisol output over time can lead to down-regulation of the HPA axis at one or more levels of the system as an adaptation (De Bellis et al., 1994). Thus, it could be that continued parental harshness may be associated with lower cortisol output later in life in these same adolescents that are currently showing higher cortisol output.
Parental disengagement at 1 year showed negative associations with hair cortisol concentration at 15. This finding is consistent with literature on post-institutionalized and foster care youth who show decreased output of cortisol (Dozier et al., 2006; Koss et al., 2016; van der Vegt et al., 2009). Similarly, a recent meta-analysis reported that child maltreatment is associated with low morning cortisol output, which could partially contribute to lower overall secretion (Bernard et al., 2017). The negative association is also in line with a recent paper demonstrating that maltreatment in childhood is associated with lower overall cortisol output in middle childhood to adolescence (White et al., 2017). This study showed high rates of neglect (63.7%) in their maltreated sample, which is consistent with our findings for disengagement in this investigation (White et al., 2017). Thus, neglect may be driving the findings of lower cortisol output in their sample. Our findings are also in line with a recent study demonstrating that greater neglect at 3 years was the best predictor of attenuated hair cortisol concentration in adult inpatients with psychiatric conditions (Schalinski et al., 2019). The absence of expected sensitive and responsive caregiving may be a common theme among these similar HPA findings for neglected, foster care, and post-institutionalized youth (Bruce et al., 2009; Fisher, 2017). This type of care may precede neurobiological changes that produce low levels of cortisol output. Hypocortisolism in these samples may also be a marker of allostatic load (McEwen, 1998), and the long period of time between parental disengagement at 1 year and lower cortisol output at 15 years may be needed for the HPA axis to down-regulate to produce lower cortisol output following early disengagement.
There are multiple potential pathways from early disengagement to low cortisol output years later. First, there are complex genetic and epigenetic processes through which early experience may shape adult cognitive and emotional regulatory processes, and their impact on risk for psychopathology (Turecki et al., 2014), and these effects may be partly mediated by the HPA axis, with its dense receptor distribution in relevant brain regions like the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex (McEwen & Morrison, 2013; Wang et al., 2013). Why this would differ between parental disengagement and parental harshness experiences in not immediately clear, but it is possible that disengagement elicits specific behavioral, cognitive, and socioemotional adaptations that help children survive and navigate in their deprived environments. These adaptations could include simultaneous and linked changes in HPA axis regulatory control, leading to reduced overall cortisol secretion and alterations within hippocampal-prefrontal circuits involved in cognitive-emotional processing. Early disengagement may have particularly strong associations with the HPA axis as the absence of expected environmental input—in this case, a caregiver—to buffer stress reactivity before the infant is able to regulate their own biological and behavioral responses (Fisher, 2017). Diminished HPA responses may be an evolutionarily adaptive way of conserving metabolic resources in the face of stress even if diminished responses may have other negative consequences (Fisher, 2017). The consequences of these HPA alterations may have profound effects behaviorally and physiologically, with implications for risk for psychiatric disruptions (Holi et al., 2006). Low cortisol may be a predictor of future behavior and mental health problems even if there were no significant associations between cortisol and antisocial behavior at 15. In the current study, parental harshness and disengagement were correlated (r = .32), which is expected considering there is often co-occurrence of different stressors for the same individual. The model allowed us to account for this correlation to be able to identify unique associations between parental harshness, parental disengagement, hair cortisol concentration, and mental health. The zero-order correlations demonstrated a significant positive correlation between parental harshness from 1–15 and hair cortisol concentration at 15, while there was a negative (though not statistically significant) association between disengagement from 1–15 and hair cortisol concentration. There appears to be a suppressor effect where the negative but not statistically significant association between parental disengagement and hair cortisol concentration becomes statistically significant when added to the model with parental harshness and other variables. We hypothesize that this suppressor effect may result because only the aspects of disengagement that are not associated with parental harshness are associated with lower cortisol output. Including both parental harshness and disengagement together in the same model controls for the overlap between the variables, leading to suppression effects that increase the regression weights (Paulhus et al., 2004).
Another possible explanation for the differential associations of parental harshness and disengagement with overall cortisol secretion is that the experience of parental harshness and disengagement likely occurs on different time scales. For example, parental disengagement may be a more chronic, daily occurrence, while parental harshness may be a more unpredictable, intermittent occurrence that occurs as a single event or a string of individual events such as making threats or hitting the child intermittently. It could be that having a chronic stressor versus a repeated unpredictable stressor is what leads to differences in overall cortisol output. The finding that parental harshness at 15 years is associated with higher cortisol output is also consistent with the hypothesis that recent unpredictable or intermittent stress is associated with increases in cortisol output. Maltreatment that starts early in life and is chronic is predicted to transition from producing high cortisol output right away to downregulation of the HPA axis leading to low cortisol output over time. This down-regulation of the HPA axis following early life parental disengagement, which may be a chronic early life stressor, is consistent with our finding that parental disengagement in infancy rather than in recent years is associated with lower cortisol output. Down-regulation of cortisol output may be a marker of allostatic load (McEwen, 1998), and it may require longer intervals of time to see hypocortisolism.
The HPA axis has inputs from a number of limbic and cortical structures that help to regulate output of stress mediators. Early neglect has been associated with lower grey matter volume and changes in cortical activity (McLaughlin et al., 2014), which could indirectly change inputs to the HPA axis leading to low cortisol output across the day and night (Herman et al., 2005, 2016; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). Our data suggest that parental disengagement specifically during infancy was associated with low cortisol output. This could be due to the stage of rapid brain development occurring at the time of parental disengagement. Circuits involving the hippocampus and amygdala that are being sculpted by early experience are simultaneously involved in behavioral and emotional adaptations and HPA axis regulation. These circuits may be particularly sensitive to early parental disengagement. There is also evidence in childhood that disruptions in caregiver attachment may be another pathway to lower cortisol output (Dozier et al., 2008). Mouse models of early life stress include a protocol that limits nesting materials provided to the dam, which then leads to higher stress and more aberrant and fragmented maternal behaviors by the dam (Rice et al., 2008). This model has been associated with impaired HPA regulation in both dams and pups (Baram et al., 2012; Ivy et al., 2008; Rice et al., 2008). It will be helpful for translational researchers to use the findings in the animal literature to help disentangle why parental harshness versus disengagement (i.e., threat versus deprivation) may lead to different neuroendocrine and behavioral effects in both offspring and caregivers.
Although the association between hair cortisol concentration and age 15 depressive symptoms was positive (β= 0.14), as predicted based on previous literature (Dettenborn et al., 2012; Staufenbiel et al., 2013; Wei et al., 2015), it was not statistically significant (p = .08). Slower recovery of cortisol levels to baseline following a stressor and potentially higher baseline output of cortisol in the afternoon are reported in samples with depression (Burke et al., 2005). High morning cortisol can predict subsequent depression in adolescents (Goodyer et al., 2000). As with parental disengagement, adaptations important to survival in harsh environments may involve simultaneous changes in regulatory control of both stress hormones and cognitive-emotional processes, via their shared circuitry involving the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex, creating linkages across time, for those subjected to parental harshness, between later cortisol output, behavioral adaptations, and depressive symptoms. A tendency towards a depressive-withdrawn state may well be adaptive when trying to avoid parental harshness, but at the cost of a vulnerability to a depressive disorder later in life. As the association between hair cortisol concentration and depressive symptoms was not statistically significant, hair cortisol concentration could not statistically mediate the association between parental harshness from 1–15 and depressive symptoms at 15. However, this finding suggests that while parental harshness and parental disengagement may be predictive of hair cortisol output, hair cortisol levels do not appear to be predictive of mental health 1–2 months later. It is important to report these null indirect effects as they may guide researchers to examining other mediators by which stress from 1–15 years is associated with mental health. Researchers may also find that hair cortisol output following parental harshness and disengagement may mediate associations with outcomes such as physical health or may be associated with mental health over different time scales than those examined here. It is also likely that more nuanced measures of HPA axis function will be needed to tease out its true role in connecting stress exposures to health outcomes. Hair cortisol is an integrated measure that captures both day and night cortisol secretion over weeks to months, and though it shows promise as potentially informative, its failure to demonstrate mediation here does not mean that the HPA axis is not a key mediator. Replication of this finding is needed in larger samples to test whether there is a true null association between hair cortisol and mental health at age 15 or if more power is needed to detect a small effect. Research using multiple measures of HPA regulation and reactivity in relation to parental harshness and disengagement at different developmental time points is needed.
There was no association between age 15 hair cortisol concentration and anxiety symptoms in the current investigation. This is inconsistent with some evidence in the literature that anxiety is associated with decreased overall cortisol output (Staufenbiel et al., 2013). However, there are inconsistencies in the literature in regard to associations between anxiety and long-term versus short-term cortisol output (hair cortisol concentration versus diurnal and reactivity measures) (Staufenbiel et al., 2013). In general, there is little relationship, acutely, between cortisol levels and subjectively experienced anxiety (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004), but links to more chronic types of anxiety are less well-studied. One of the challenges is that anxiety is a heterogeneous phenomenon with multiple different types of associated disorders that may have different neurobiological underpinnings. Findings in the current study may also be impacted by the adolescent age of assessment.
Interestingly, although not statistically significant, there was a negative association (β = −0.12) between hair cortisol concentration and age 15 antisocial behavior, which is the direction we would expect based on previous literature. Although this sample has more participants than many studies with hair cortisol concentration measurement, there still may not be enough power to detect significant effects for weaker associations. For example, the low hair cortisol concentration and externalizing behavior association in the maltreatment study by White and colleagues (2017) had a sample size of over 500. This association may become more pronounced over time such that low cortisol output could be a predictor of later antisocial behaviors. This analysis also does not consider the contributions of other hormones that may interact with cortisol or vary by early experiences or type of psychopathology. However, in the current study, we conclude that there is no statistically significant association between hair cortisol concentration and antisocial behaviors at 15 years.
Additional potential contributors to our findings include the role of sleep and health behaviors in shaping cortisol output. Hair cortisol concentrations are determined by both daytime and nighttime cortisol secretion. Cumulative daytime cortisol secretion as measured in saliva samples collected daily (from waking to bedtime) for 30 days only explains 36% of the variance in hair cortisol concentration in the centimeter of hair thought to capture that same month’s cortisol secretion (Short et al., 2016). Other factors are likely in play, but nocturnal secretion rates may account for some of the unexplained variance, since they contribute to hair cortisol concentrations, but are not captured in daytime saliva collections. Sleep disruptions can influence nocturnal secretion rates (Leproult et al., 1997), and a large percentage of daily cortisol secretion occurs during the early morning rise that begins well before awakening (Hirotsu et al., 2015). If sleep patterns are altered, either chronically by early stress exposures or acutely by recent stressors, this could contribute to alterations in nocturnal cortisol secretion, which could then be reflected in hair cortisol concentrations. There is some evidence that chronic abuse can contribute to sleep disruption (McPhie et al., 2014). Further work is needed to explore sleep disturbances as a potential mediator between parental disengagement or parental harshness and adolescent cortisol secretion rates and patterns. Similarly, physical activity and dietary intake can each lead to changes in cortisol output (Gerber et al., 2013; Gibson et al., 1999), and both exercise and dietary habits may well be shaped by earlier stress exposures (Hanson & Chen, 2006; Michels et al., 2012). Future studies should also examine these types of potential behavioral mediators in efforts to identify pathways from developmental experiences to later in life HPA axis functioning.
There are limitations of the study that should be considered. First, parental disengagement and parental harshness were only reported by the mother or primary caregiver from 1–9 years. Due to social desirability biases and potential concerns about legal ramifications, caregivers likely underreport harsh or disengaged behaviors. Thus, the rates of parental harshness and disengagement in this sample are most likely an underreport of true experiences, and associations may be stronger than we were able to detect in this study, especially due to evidence that associations between maltreatment and cortisol are stronger for agency-referred samples (Bernard et al., 2017). It would be important to confirm these findings with objective reports of child maltreatment. Sexual abuse was not included in our parental harshness measure, which limits generalizability of the findings to emotional and physical dimensions of parental harshness. Further, we created broad measures of parental harshness and disengagement in this study rather than specific and validated measures of maltreatment. Though our goals were to make constructs that theoretically match threat and deprivation (Humphreys & Zeanah, 2015; McLaughlin et al., 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014) and to capture harsh and disengaged parent behavior dimensionally, the weakness of these measures is that by combining several idiographic measures, our work is less likely to be directly replicated by others. Thus, our findings may be specific to this sample and study. Second, hair was collected at the same visit as the age 15 symptomology variables. Hair cortisol concentrations in one or two centimeters of hair reflect secretion rates over the prior 1–2 months, which is before the symptomology variables were measured. However, far more temporally intensive measurement would be needed, of both cortisol and symptoms, to track their relationships to each other over time, and that was not feasible in a large-scale, long-term longitudinal study like this. Similarly, parental harshness and disengagement measures administered at 15 years either probed experiences in the past year or asked about an unspecified period (e.g., How often does your primary caregiver hit or slap you?). The exact temporal sequence cannot be established from our data. Third, the challenge with using data from studies spanning over a decade is that the original purpose of the study was not exactly in line with the goals of the current analysis. The funding and data collection challenges of collecting data on parental harshness and disengagement prevented more frequent collection of self-report, parent-report, and biological measures in the overall study. Thus, we attempt in this paper to leverage the available prospective data in a high-risk sample to be able to test approximate periods of risk. Another limitation is that hair samples were only collected once in this study and on a subset of participants. Although assessing cortisol output via hair was not well-established when the study began, future studies should incorporate multiple measures of overall cortisol output across development to better assess the possible role of HPA functioning in associations between parental harshness, parental disengagement, and psychopathology. Finally, factor loadings for some of the scales included in the depression and anxiety factors were low, and, as seen in many studies, there was more within-reporter correlation than within-construct correlation (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005), which could have influenced our inability to fit the latent factors together in the model.
Strengths of the study include the prospective nature of the parental harshness and disengagement assessments. With retrospective reports of childhood stress in adolescence and adulthood, those reports may be biased by current emotions and psychopathology, memory failures, or neuroticism. Likewise, it is extremely difficult to accurately understand timing effects of stress in early childhood using retrospective recall because adolescents and adults generally cannot remember specific events as early as 1 and 3 years of age. Thus, the prospective nature of the FFCWS makes questions of long-term timing effects more addressable. Though the measurements were fixed in this dataset, they fall at ideal time periods that are in line with known sensitive periods of brain development from infancy through early childhood. Negative experiences from 0–5 years have shown associations with HPA functioning and mental health years later during adolescence and adulthood (Gunnar et al., 2019; Humphreys et al., 2017; Koss et al., 2016; van der Vegt et al., 2009; White et al., 2017). Similarly, middle childhood and recent negative experiences are associated with adolescent HPA function and mental health (Bosch et al., 2012; Ge et al., 2001; Rao et al., 2008; Shirtcliff & Essex, 2008). Further, a proximal measure of overall cortisol output at 15 years is a strength given higher variability in mood during early adolescence compared to later adolescence (Maciejewski et al., 2015). Thus, cortisol output 1–2 months preceding the time of assessment may be more predictive of symptoms than cortisol output at an early time point, though this hypothesis should be tested empirically. As a result, the study allows us to better understand sensitive periods of risk using prospective measurement of parental harshness and parental disengagement.
Another strength is the focus on understudied populations, with high representation of low-income and racial/ethnic minority families. The FFCWS oversampled for unwed mothers and their children, who are at higher risk for mental and physical health problems due to lower SES and the myriad of stressors associated with poverty. Even within this high-risk sample, we found that parental harshness and disengagement had differential impacts on hair cortisol concentration. This result highlights the heterogeneity of risk in families even within high-risk populations. Future research should investigate how this heterogeneity, including the type and timing of stress exposure, impacts development. Even in this sample, parental harshness and disengagement were only modestly correlated (r = .32) which allowed us to somewhat disentangle their separate contributions. Finally, we were able to use a validated instrument to measure adolescent psychopathology and included both caregivers and youth as reporters. Thus, we are confident that our depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and antisocial behavior constructs provide a good measure of adolescent mental health in this model. This study design is methodologically stronger than those assessing mediation with only cross-sectional data as we have data from 1–15 years of age. Thus, this study should be weighed more heavily than analyses with only cross-sectional data and retrospective reports of parental harshness and parental disengagement.
The results of this study inform our understanding of the biology that may underlie the association between psychosocial stress and psychopathology. The literature has documented both positive and negative associations between cortisol and stress, and careful considerations of type of stress may be crucial to how heterogeneity and timing of stress may lead to different cortisol patterning. Parental harshness and parental disengagement may have unique characteristics that may be processed differently by the brain, leading to different inputs to the HPA axis. This difference in overall cortisol output between parental harshness and disengagement may help to clarify why there have been discrepant findings in the stress and cortisol literatures. Thus, these results may help researchers characterize stress in ways that may make more sense of discrepant biological findings in maltreated populations. Inconsistent results are notoriously common in cortisol studies, at least partly because measurement of cortisol levels does not capture the regulatory changes within this complex system that are likely the actual mediators of interest. The studies that will resolve this issue will have to follow large cohorts longitudinally over development using more sensitive measures of regulatory changes within the HPA axis. Ideally, we would like to identify specific epigenetic changes and alterations in glucocorticoid receptor distribution and sensitivity, which cannot yet be discerned from collection of saliva and hair. These cortisol patterns following parental harshness or parental disengagement may be specific to adolescence, or they may be seen in childhood and adulthood. Ongoing follow-up of this cohort and careful longitudinal work in other cohorts will help us to disentangle these developmental associations. These findings further support the hypothesis that HPA axis functioning may be differently affected by timing and type of maltreatment, though the implications for adolescent mental health are unclear.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We would like to acknowledge the following grants that supported this work: F32HD088029 (PI: Doom), K01HL143159 (PI: Doom), R01MH10376 (PI: Monk), T32HD007109 (McLoyd & Monk), R01HD36916 (PI: McLanahan), R25HD074544, and a Doris Duke Fellowship for the Promotion of Child Well-Being (Recipient: Hein). We thank the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study families for their continued participation in the study.
Footnotes
All results remained the same when the age 15 disengagement variable was calculated without the substance abuse questions and when the cumulative disengagement from ages 1-15 variable was calculated using the age 15 disengagement variable with no substance abuse items.
To be sure that using multimodal latent variables instead of only K-SADS variables would not change the results, we tested a model using only the K-SADS variables described in the methods section and found that all results in the cumulative model remained the same.
Conflicts of interest: None.
References
- Achenbach TM, & Edelbrock C (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ 4–18 and Revised Child Behavior Profile. University of Vermont. [Google Scholar]
- Adam EK, Quinn ME, Tavernier R, McQuillan MT, Dahlke KA, & Gilbert KE (2017). Diurnal cortisol slopes and mental and physical health outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.05.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adam EK, Vrshek-Schallhorn S, Kendall AD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, & Craske MG (2014). Prospective associations between the cortisol awakening response and first onsets of anxiety disorders over a six-year follow-up. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 44, 47–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alink LR, van Ijzendoorn MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Mesman J, Juffer F, & Koot HM (2008). Cortisol and externalizing behavior in children and adolescents: Mixed meta-analytic evidence for the inverse relation of basal cortisol and cortisol reactivity with externalizing behavior. Dev Psychobiol, 50(5), 427–450. 10.1002/dev.20300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen SL, & Teicher MH (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(4), 183–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin JR, Reuben A, Newbury JB, & Danese A (2019). Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 584–593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baram TZ, Davis EP, Obenaus A, Sandman CA, Small SL, Solodkin A, & Stern H (2012). Fragmentation and unpredictability of early-life experience in mental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 169(9), 907–915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard K, Frost A, Bennett CB, & Lindhiem O (2017). Maltreatment and diurnal cortisol regulation: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 78, 57–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birmaher B, Khetarpal S, Brent D, Cully M, Balach L, Kaufman J, & Neer SM (1997). The screen for child anxiety related emotional disorders (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(4), 545–553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bollen KA (2002). Latent variables in psychology and the social sciences. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 605–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bosch NM, Riese H, Reijneveld SA, Bakker MP, Verhulst FC, Ormel J, & Oldehinkel AJ (2012). Timing matters: Long term effects of adversities from prenatal period up to adolescence on adolescents’ cortisol stress response. The TRAILS study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(9), 1439–1447. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruce J, Fisher PA, Pears KC, & Levine S (2009). Morning cortisol levels in preschool-aged foster children: Differential effects of maltreatment type. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 51(1), 14–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke HM, Davis MC, Otte C, & Mohr DC (2005). Depression and cortisol responses to psychological stress: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30(9), 846–856. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.02.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busso DS, McLaughlin KA, & Sheridan MA (2017). Dimensions of adversity, physiological reactivity, and externalizing psychopathology in adolescence: Deprivation and threat. Psychosom Med, 79(2), 162. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cicchetti Dante, & Rogosch FA (2009). Adaptive coping under conditions of extreme stress: Multilevel influences on the determinants of resilience in maltreated children. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2009(124), 47–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, & Aiken LS (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences, 3rd ed. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Cuthbert BN (2014). The RDoC framework: Facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry, 13(1), 28–35. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Anna-Hernandez KL, Ross RG, Natvig CL, & Laudenslager ML (2011). Hair cortisol levels as a retrospective marker of hypothalamic–pituitary axis activity throughout pregnancy: Comparison to salivary cortisol. Physiol Behav, 104(2), 348–353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dannlowski U, Kugel H, Huber F, Stuhrmann A, Redlich R, Grotegerd D, Dohm K, Sehlmeyer C, Konrad C, & Baune BT (2013). Childhood maltreatment is associated with an automatic negative emotion processing bias in the amygdala. Human Brain Mapping, 34(11), 2899–2909. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Bellis MD, Chrousos GP, Dorn LD, Burke L, Helmers K, Kling MA, Trickett PK, & Putnam FW (1994). Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation in sexually abused girls. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 78(2), 249–255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Los Reyes A, & Kazdin AE (2005). Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulletin, 131(4), 483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dettenborn L, Muhtz C, Skoluda N, Stalder T, Steudte S, Hinkelmann K, Kirschbaum C, & Otte C (2012). Introducing a novel method to assess cumulative steroid concentrations: Increased hair cortisol concentrations over 6 months in medicated patients with depression. Stress, 15(3), 348–353. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson SS, & Kemeny ME (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol Bull, 130(3), 355–391. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doane LD, Mineka S, Zinbarg RE, Craske M, Griffith JW, & Adam EK (2013). Are flatter diurnal cortisol rhythms associated with major depression and anxiety disorders in late adolescence? The role of life stress and daily negative emotion. Development and Psychopathology, 25(3), 629–642. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dockray S, Susman EJ, & Dorn LD (2009). Depression, Cortisol Reactivity and Obesity in Childhood and Adolescence. The Journal of Adolescent Health : Official Publication of the Society for Adolescent Medicine, 45(4), 344–350. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doom JR, & Gunnar MR (2013). Stress physiology and developmental psychopathology: Past, present and future. Development and Psychopathology, 25(4 0 2). 10.1017/s0954579413000667 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dozier M, Manni M, Gordon MK, Peloso E, Gunnar MR, Stovall-McClough KC, Eldreth D, & Levine S (2006). Foster children’s diurnal production of cortisol: An exploratory study. Child Maltreatment, 11(2), 189–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dozier M, Peloso E, Lewis E, Laurenceau J-P, & Levine S (2008). Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), 845–859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott DS, Huizinga D, & Ageton SS (1985). Explaining delinquency and drug use. Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Evans GW (2004). The Environment of Childhood Poverty. American Psychologist, 59(2), 77–92. 10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferdinand RF (2008). Validity of the CBCL/YSR DSM-IV scales anxiety problems and affective problems. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(1), 126–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher PA (2017). Commentary: Is there a there there in hair? A reflection on child maltreatment and hair cortisol concentrations in White et al.(2017). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(9), 1008–1010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flom M, St. John AM, Meyer JS, & Tarullo AR (2017). Infant hair cortisol: Associations with salivary cortisol and environmental context. Dev Psychobiol, 59(1), 26–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao W, Stalder T, Foley P, Rauh M, Deng H, & Kirschbaum C (2013). Quantitative analysis of steroid hormones in human hair using a column-switching LC–APCI–MS/MS assay. Journal of Chromatography B, 928, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ge X, Conger RD, & Elder GH Jr (2001). Pubertal transition, stressful life events, and the emergence of gender differences in adolescent depressive symptoms. Developmental Psychology, 37(3), 404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerber M, Jonsdottir IH, Kalak N, Elliot C, Pühse U, Holsboer-Trachsler E, & Brand S (2013). Objectively assessed physical activity is associated with increased hair cortisol content in young adults. Stress, 16(6), 593–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson EL, Checkley S, Papadopoulos A, Poon L, Daley S, & Wardle J (1999). Increased salivary cortisol reliably induced by a protein-rich midday meal. Psychosom Med, 61(2), 214–224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold AL, Sheridan MA, Peverill M, Busso DS, Lambert HK, Alves S, Pine DS, & McLaughlin KA (2016). Childhood abuse and reduced cortical thickness in brain regions involved in emotional processing. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(10), 1154–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodyer IM, Herbert J, Tamplin A, & Altham PME (2000). Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(6), 499–504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray NA, Dhana A, Van Der Vyver L, Van Wyk J, Khumalo NP, & Stein DJ (2018). Determinants of hair cortisol concentration in children: A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 87, 204–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, DePasquale CE, Reid BM, & Donzella B (2019). Pubertal stress recalibration reverses the effects of early life stress in postinstitutionalized children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(48), 23984–23988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, Doom JR, & Esposito EA (2015). Psychoneuroendocrinology of stress. In Lamb ME & Lerner RM (Eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science (pp. 106–151). John Wiley & Sons Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Gunnar MR, & Vazquez D (2006). Stress neurobiology and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti D & Cohen DJ (Eds.), Developmental Psychopathology (Vol. 2). Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Guterman NB, Lee SJ, Taylor CA, & Rathouz PJ (2009). Parental perceptions of neighborhood processes, stress, personal control, and risk for physical child abuse and neglect. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33(12), 897–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hale WW, Raaijmakers Q, Muris P, & Meeus WIM (2005). Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) in the general adolescent population. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(3), 283–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Abramson LY, Moffitt TE, Silva PA, McGee R, & Angell KE (1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood: Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hankin BL, Badanes LS, Abela JRZ, & Watamura SE (2010). Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis Dysregulation in Dysphoric Children and Adolescents: Cortisol Reactivity to Psychosocial Stress from Preschool Through Middle Adolescence. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 484–490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson JL, Nacewicz BM, Sutterer MJ, Cayo AA, Schaefer SM, Rudolph KD, Shirtcliff EA, Pollak SD, & Davidson RJ (2015). Behavioral problems after early life stress: Contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala. Biological Psychiatry, 77(4), 314–323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanson M, & Chen E (2006). Socioeconomic status, race, and body mass index: The mediating role of physical activity and sedentary behaviors during adolescence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32(3), 250–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harkness KL, Stewart JG, & Wynne-Edwards KE (2011). Cortisol reactivity to social stress in adolescents: Role of depression severity and child maltreatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(2), 173–181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heim C, Newport DJ, Mletzko T, Miller AH, & Nemeroff CB (2008). The link between childhood trauma and depression: Insights from HPA axis studies in humans. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(6), 693–710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman JP, McKlveen JM, Ghosal S, Kopp B, Wulsin A, Makinson R, Scheimann J, & Myers B (2016). Regulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical stress response. Comprehensive Physiology, 6(2), 603. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman JP, Ostrander MM, Mueller NK, & Figueiredo H (2005). Limbic system mechanisms of stress regulation: Hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 29(8), 1201–1213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirotsu C, Tufik S, & Andersen ML (2015). Interactions between sleep, stress, and metabolism: From physiological to pathological conditions. Sleep Science, 8(3), 143–152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holi M, Auvinen-Lintunen L, Lindberg N, Tani P, & Virkkunen M (2006). Inverse correlation between severity of psychopathic traits and serum cortisol levels in young adult violent male offenders. Psychopathology, 39(2), 102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu L, & Bentler PM (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Huizinga D, & Elliott DS (1986). Reassessing the reliability and validity of self-report delinquency measures. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 2(4), 293–327. [Google Scholar]
- Humphreys KL, Fox NA, Nelson CA, & Zeanah CH (2017). Psychopathology following severe deprivation: History, research, and implications of the Bucharest Early Intervention Project. In Child maltreatment in residential care (pp. 129–148). Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Humphreys KL, & Zeanah CH (2015). Deviations from the expectable environment in early childhood and emerging psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(1), 154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hymel KP (2006). When is lack of supervision neglect? Pediatrics, 118(3), 1296–1298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ivy AS, Brunson KL, Sandman C, & Baram TZ (2008). Dysfunctional nurturing behavior in rat dams with limited access to nesting material: A clinically relevant model for early-life stress. Neuroscience, 154(3), 1132–1142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, & Ryan N (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khoury JE, Enlow MB, Plamondon A, & Lyons-Ruth K (2019). The Association between Adversity and Hair Cortisol Levels in Humans: A Meta-Analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 103, 104–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J, & Cicchetti D (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(6), 706–716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim P, Evans GW, Angstadt M, Ho SS, Sripada CS, Swain JE, Liberzon I, & Phan KL (2013). Effects of childhood poverty and chronic stress on emotion regulatory brain function in adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(46), 18442–18447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirschbaum C, Tietze A, Skoluda N, & Dettenborn L (2009). Hair as a retrospective calendar of cortisol production—Increased cortisol incorporation into hair in the third trimester of pregnancy. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(1), 32–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kline RB (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford publications. [Google Scholar]
- Knudsen EI (2004). Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1412–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koss KJ, & Gunnar MR (2018). Annual Research Review: Early adversity, the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis, and child psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(4), 327–346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koss KJ, Mliner SB, Donzella B, & Gunnar MR (2016). Early adversity, hypocortisolism, and behavior problems at school entry: A study of internationally adopted children. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 66, 31–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SJ, Grogan-Kaylor A, & Berger LM (2014). Parental spanking of 1-year-old children and subsequent child protective services involvement. Child Abuse Negl, 38(5), 875–883. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leproult R, Copinschi G, Buxton O, & Van Cauter E (1997). Sleep loss results in an elevation of cortisol levels the next evening. Sleep, 20(10), 865–870. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewinsohn PM, Striegel-Moore RH, & Seeley JR (2000). Epidemiology and natural course of eating disorders in young women from adolescence to young adulthood. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(10), 1284–1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LoPilato AM, Addington J, Bearden CE, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, Mathalon DH, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, & Tsuang MT (2019). Stress perception following childhood adversity: Unique associations with adversity type and sex. Development and Psychopathology, 1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maciejewski DF, van Lier PA, Branje SJ, Meeus WH, & Koot HM (2015). A 5-year longitudinal study on mood variability across adolescence using daily diaries. Child Development, 86(6), 1908–1921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manly JT, Kim JE, Rogosch FA, & Cicchetti D (2001). Dimensions of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology, 13(4), 759–782. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maziade M, Roy M-A, Fournier J-P, Cliche D, Mérette C, Caron C, Garneau Y, Montgrain N, Shriqui C, & Dion C (1992). Reliability of best-estimate diagnosis in genetic linkage studies of major psychoses: Results from the Quebec pedigree studies. The American Journal of Psychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 840(1), 33–44. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1998.tb09546.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McEwen BS, & Morrison JH (2013). The brain on stress: Vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron, 79(1), 16–29. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, & Lambert HK (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578–591. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McPhie ML, Weiss JA, & Wekerle C (2014). Psychological distress as a mediator of the relationship between childhood maltreatment and sleep quality in adolescence: Results from the Maltreatment and Adolescent Pathways (MAP) Longitudinal Study. Child Abuse Negl, 38(12), 2044–2052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michels N, Sioen I, Braet C, Eiben G, Hebestreit A, Huybrechts I, Vanaelst B, Vyncke K, & De Henauw S (2012). Stress, emotional eating behaviour and dietary patterns in children. Appetite, 59(3), 762–769. 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osborne C, & Berger LM (2009). Parental substance abuse and child well-being: A consideration of parents’ gender and coresidence. Journal of Family Issues, 30(3), 341–370. [Google Scholar]
- Paulhus DL, Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH, & Tracy JL (2004). Two replicable suppressor situations in personality research. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 39(2), 303–328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peckins MK, Roberts AG, Hein TC, Hyde LW, Mitchell C, Brooks-Gunn J, McLanahan SS, Monk CS, & Lopez-Duran NL (2020). Violence exposure and social deprivation is associated with cortisol reactivity in urban adolescents. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 111, 104426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peeters F, Nicholson NA, & Berkhof J (2003). Cortisol responses to daily events in major depressive disorder. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(5), 836–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak SD, & Tolley-Schell SA (2003). Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112(3), 323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rao U, Hammen C, Ortiz LR, Chen L-A, & Poland RE (2008). Effects of early and recent adverse experiences on adrenal response to psychosocial stress in depressed adolescents. Biological Psychiatry, 64(6), 521–526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reichman NE, Teitler JO, Garfinkel I, & McLanahan SS (2001). Fragile families: Sample and design. Children and Youth Services Review, 23(4–5), 303–326. [Google Scholar]
- Reilly EB, & Gunnar MR (2019). Neglect, HPA axis reactivity, and development. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 78, 100–108. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2019.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rice CJ, Sandman CA, Lenjavi MR, & Baram TZ (2008). A novel mouse model for acute and long-lasting consequences of early life stress. Endocrinology, 149(10), 4892–4900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rietschel L, Streit F, Zhu G, McAloney K, Kirschbaum C, Frank J, Hansell NK, Wright MJ, McGrath JJ, Witt SH, Rietschel M, & Martin NG (2016). Hair Cortisol and Its Association With Psychological Risk Factors for Psychiatric Disorders: A Pilot Study in Adolescent Twins. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 19(5), 438–446. 10.1017/thg.2016.50 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saylor CF, Finch AJ, Spirito A, & Bennett B (1984). The Children’s Depression Inventory: A systematic evaluation of psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(6), 955. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalinski I, Elbert T, Steudte-Schmiedgen S, & Kirschbaum C (2015). The cortisol paradox of trauma-related disorders: Lower phasic responses but higher tonic levels of cortisol are associated with sexual abuse in childhood. PLoS One, 10(8), e0136921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schalinski I, Teicher MH, & Rockstroh B (2019). Early neglect is a key determinant of adult hair cortisol concentration and is associated with increased vulnerability to trauma in a transdiagnostic sample. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 108, 35–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schreier HM, Enlow MB, Ritz T, Gennings C, & Wright RJ (2015). Childhood abuse is associated with increased hair cortisol levels among urban pregnant women. J Epidemiol Community Health, 69(12), 1169–1174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan MA, & McLaughlin KA (2014). Dimensions of early experience and neural development: Deprivation and threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(11), 580–585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shirtcliff EA, & Essex MJ (2008). Concurrent and longitudinal associations of basal and diurnal cortisol with mental health symptoms in early adolescence. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 50(7), 690–703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Short SJ, Stalder T, Marceau K, Entringer S, Moog NK, Shirtcliff EA, Wadhwa PD, & Buss C (2016). Correspondence between hair cortisol concentrations and 30-day integrated daily salivary and weekly urinary cortisol measures. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 71, 12–18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalder T, Steudte-Schmiedgen S, Alexander N, Klucken T, Vater A, Wichmann S, Kirschbaum C, & Miller R (2017). Stress-related and basic determinants of hair cortisol in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 261–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staufenbiel SM, Penninx BW, Spijker AT, Elzinga BM, & van Rossum EF (2013). Hair cortisol, stress exposure, and mental health in humans: A systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(8), 1220–1235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steudte S, Stalder T, Dettenborn L, Klumbies E, Foley P, Beesdo-Baum K, & Kirschbaum C (2011). Decreased hair cortisol concentrations in generalised anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research, 186(2), 310–314. 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steudte-Schmiedgen S, Wichmann S, Stalder T, Hilbert K, Muehlhan M, Lueken U, & Beesdo-Baum K (2017). Hair cortisol concentrations and cortisol stress reactivity in generalized anxiety disorder, major depression and their comorbidity. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 84, 184–190. 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.09.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Straus MA, Hamby SL, Finkelhor D, Moore DW, & Runyan D (1998). Identification of child maltreatment with the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scales: Development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse Negl, 22(4), 249–270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sumner JA, Colich NL, Uddin M, Armstrong D, & McLaughlin KA (2019). Early Experiences of Threat, but Not Deprivation, Are Associated With Accelerated Biological Aging in Children and Adolescents. Biological Psychiatry, 85(3), 268–278. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trickett PK, Noll JG, Susman EJ, Shenk CE, & Putnam FW (2010). Attenuation of cortisol across development for victims of sexual abuse. Development and Psychopathology, 22(1), 165–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turecki G, Ota VK, Belangero SI, Jackowski A, & Kaufman J (2014). Early life adversity, genomic plasticity, and psychopathology. The Lancet Psychiatry, 1(6), 461–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turney K, & McLanahan S (2015). The academic consequences of early childhood problem behaviors. Social Science Research, 54, 131–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich-Lai YM, & Herman JP (2009). Neural Regulation of Endocrine and Autonomic Stress Responses. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 10(6), 397–409. 10.1038/nrn2647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Vegt EJ, Van Der Ende J, Kirschbaum C, Verhulst FC, & Tiemeier H (2009). Early neglect and abuse predict diurnal cortisol patterns in adults: A study of international adoptees. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(5), 660–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Praag H, Kempermann G, & Gage FH (2000). Neural consequences of environmental enrichment. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 1(3), 191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh C, MacMillan HL, & Jamieson E (2003). The relationship between parental substance abuse and child maltreatment: Findings from the Ontario Health Supplement. Child Abuse Negl, 27(12), 1409–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walsh RN (1981). Effects of environmental complexity and deprivation on brain anatomy and histology: A review. International Journal of Neuroscience, 12(1), 33–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang Q, Van Heerikhuize J, Aronica E, Kawata M, Seress L, Joels M, Swaab DF, & Lucassen PJ (2013). Glucocorticoid receptor protein expression in human hippocampus; stability with age. Neurobiology of Aging, 34(6), 1662–1673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wei J, Sun G, Zhao L, Yang X, Liu X, Lin D, Li T, & Ma X (2015). Analysis of hair cortisol level in first-episodic and recurrent female patients with depression compared to healthy controls. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175, 299–302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White LO, Ising M, von Klitzing K, Sierau S, Michel A, Klein AM, Andreas A, Keil J, Quintero L, & Müller-Myhsok B (2017). Reduced hair cortisol after maltreatment mediates externalizing symptoms in middle childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(9), 998–1007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood A, Kroll L, Moore A, & Harrington R (1995). Properties of the mood and feelings questionnaire in adolescent psychiatric outpatients: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(2), 327–334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.