Abstract
Objective
To determine whether memory tasks with demonstrated sensitivity to hippocampal function can detect variance related to preclinical Alzheimer disease (AD) biomarkers, we examined associations between performance in 3 memory tasks and CSF β-amyloid (Aβ)42/Aβ40 and phosopho-tau181 (p-tau181) in cognitively unimpaired older adults (CU).
Methods
CU enrolled in the Stanford Aging and Memory Study (n = 153; age 68.78 ± 5.81 years; 94 female) completed a lumbar puncture and memory assessments. CSF Aβ42, Aβ40, and p-tau181 were measured with the automated Lumipulse G system in a single-batch analysis. Episodic memory was assayed using a standardized delayed recall composite, paired associate (word–picture) cued recall, and a mnemonic discrimination task that involves discrimination between studied “target” objects, novel “foil” objects, and perceptually similar “lure” objects. Analyses examined cross-sectional relationships among memory performance, age, and CSF measures, controlling for sex and education.
Results
Age and lower Aβ42/Aβ40 were independently associated with elevated p-tau181. Age, Aβ42/Aβ40, and p-tau181 were each associated with (1) poorer associative memory and (2) diminished improvement in mnemonic discrimination performance across levels of decreased task difficulty (i.e., target–lure similarity). P-tau mediated the effect of Aβ42/Aβ40 on memory. Relationships between CSF proteins and delayed recall were similar but nonsignificant. CSF Aβ42 was not significantly associated with p-tau181 or memory.
Conclusions
Tests designed to tax hippocampal function are sensitive to subtle individual differences in memory among CU and correlate with early AD-associated biomarker changes in CSF. These tests may offer utility for identifying CU with preclinical AD pathology.
Identifying cognitively unimpaired older adults (CU) who harbor Alzheimer disease (AD) pathology is critical for developing disease-modifying treatments, which may be most effective during the asymptomatic (preclinical) stage of the disease.1 Decreases in CSF β-amyloid (Aβ42) and increases in phospho-tau181 (p-tau181) may be the earliest detectable changes in the AD pathophysiologic cascade.2,3 However, detecting cross-sectional associations between CSF and cognition using traditional standardized cognitive tests has posed a challenge.4–8
Tests designed to tax core functions of the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex—areas of the medial temporal lobe (MTL) affected early on by tangle pathology9,10—may be sensitive to subtle variations in memory that are associated with biomarker abnormalities, particularly elevations in CSF p-tau181, which is known to associate with tangle pathology.11 Associative memory (figure 1A) and mnemonic discrimination of studied target stimuli and perceptually similar lure stimuli12 (figure 1B), tasks in which performance is tightly linked with hippocampal function,13–15 show initial promise.16–19 However, the ability of these tasks to detect CSF biomarker abnormalities in CU remains unclear.
Figure 1. Memory Paradigms and Discrimination Performance.
(A) Schematic of the associative memory paradigm, reproduced from reference 15 (creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Participants intentionally studied word–picture pairs. At test, they viewed studied words intermixed with new words, and were asked to recall the picture associated with each word, if old. Participants responded “face” or “place” if they remembered the associated picture or picture category; “old” if they remembered the word but could not recollect the associate; “new” if they did not remember the word as studied. (B) Schematic of the mnemonic discrimination paradigm. Participants incidentally encoded objects and memory was assessed using a modified recognition memory test with perceptually similar lures, ranging from high (L1) to low similarity (L5), as well as novel (non-lure) foils. Correct responses are indicated next to each stimulus. (C) Performance on the associative memory task. Memory for studied words, irrespective of memory for the associate (old/new d′), was higher than memory for the associations (associative d′). (D, E) Mnemonic discrimination performance by target–lure similarity. Both lure/new d′ (D) and old/lure d′ (E) increased as target–lure similarity decreased. ISI = interstimulus interval.
This study leverages critical developments in CSF protein analysis, including fully automated20,21 measurement of p-tau181, Aβ42, and Aβ4022–25 to quantify preclinical AD burden in CU. We examine associations between CSF Aβ42/Aβ40, p-tau181, and memory performance on standardized memory tests and specialized hippocampal-dependent tests: associative memory and mnemonic discrimination. In an exploratory analysis, relationships between CSF proteins and MTL tau, measured by 18F-PI-2620,26,27 were examined. We predicted that performance on specialized hippocampal-dependent tests would be associated with CSF biomarkers of AD, particularly p-tau181.
Methods
Participants
This study includes data from 153 CU (table 1; aged 60–88 years) of an initial 212 enrolled in the Stanford Aging and Memory Study (SAMS; see figure e-1 for participant flowchart, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). SAMS is a fluid and neuroimaging biomarker study focused on neuronal and behavioral measures of the MTL.15 SAMS eligibility included normal or corrected-to-normal vision/hearing, right-handedness, native English speaking, no history of neurologic or psychiatric disease, a Clinical Dementia Rating28 global score of zero, and performance within the normal range on a standardized neuropsychological test battery.15 All participants were deemed cognitively unimpaired during a clinical consensus meeting consisting of neurologists and neuropsychologists.
Table 1.
Participant Demographics and Biomarker Summary
CSF Data
Participants underwent lumbar puncture at 8 or 9 am following overnight fasting. CSF was stored in either 1.0 or 0.5 mL aliquots at −80°C. A single aliquot for each participant was used to measure Aβ42, Aβ40, p-tau181, and total tau using the fully automated Lumipulse G system (Fujirebio US, Inc., Malvern, PA) in a single-batch analysis using procedures previously described29 by the Stanford Alzheimer's Disease Research Center Biomarker Core. Our primary measures of amyloid and tau were the Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio, due to greater specificity and sensitivity for detecting AD-related amyloid pathology than Aβ42 alone,22–25 and p-tau181, due to greater specificity for AD than total tau.30 For comparison, we also report CSF Aβ42 levels.
We primarily examined CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and p-tau181 as continuous variables, but also examined amyloid by binary status. We used a Gaussian mixture modeling approach (R package mclust v4.131) to classify CU as Aβ+ or Aβ− based on CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 values (figure 2D; see figure e-2 for more detail, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). Briefly, this procedure identified a 2-distribution model as optimal, yielding an Aβ42/Aβ40 cutoff of 0.0752. Participants were classified as Aβ+ if they had a greater than 0.5 probability of belonging to the Aβ+ distribution (Aβ42/Aβ40 <0.0752) or as Aβ− if they had a greater than 0.5 probability of belonging to the Aβ− distribution (Aβ42/Aβ40 > 0.0752). For visualization only, we also categorized participants into tau+ and tau− groups. The top quartile of CSF p-tau181 concentration (>42 pg/mL) was used to define tau+, as the distribution showed a continuum of values. This binary classification was used to plot performance as a function of combined Aβ/tau status (Aβ−/tau−, Aβ+/tau−, Aβ−/tau+, Aβ+/tau+), as described in the biomarker framework of AD.32
Figure 2. CSF Protein Distributions in Cognitively Unimpaired Older Adults.
Distributions of (A) CSF β-amyloid (Aβ)42, (B) Aβ40, (C) phospho-tau181 (p-tau181), and (D) Aβ42/Aβ40 in cognitively unimpaired older adults. Probability density functions for the estimated Gaussian distributions for the best fit model (a 2-cluster solution; see figure e-2, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp) are superimposed on the Aβ42/Aβ40 distribution in (D); the turquoise curve represents the Aβ distribution and the coral curve represents the Aβ+ distribution.
Tau PET Data
An exploratory analysis of the relationship between CSF proteins and MTL tau, measured by 18F-PI-2620 PET, was conducted in 32 participants who had both CSF and tau PET data available (table 1). The data collection and image processing procedures, along with the exploratory outcomes, are reported in the supplement (method e-1; figure e-3, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp).
Episodic Memory Measures
Standardized Neuropsychological Delayed Recall
The composite delayed recall score reflected delayed recall performance across (1) the logical memory subtest of the Wechsler Memory Scale, (2) the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised, and (3) the Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised. Composite scores were computed by first z-scoring individual subtest scores using the full SAMS sample as reference and then averaging. Data were available from all 153 participants.
Associative Memory
The associative memory task (figure 1A) was administered concurrent with fMRI as previously described.15 Briefly, this task assessed memory for word–picture pairs comprising concrete nouns (e.g., “banana,” “violin”) paired with pictures of famous faces (e.g., “Queen Elizabeth,” “Ronald Reagan”) or well-known places (e.g., “Golden Gate Bridge,” “Niagara Falls”). The task consisted of 5 alternating study and test blocks. Each study block included 12 word–face and 12 word–place pairs; participants were instructed to form a link between the word and picture presented. In each test block, participants saw a mix of 24 studied words and 6 novel (foil) words. Memory was assessed using an associative cued-recall test accompanied by a button response: participants selected “face” or “place” if they remembered the word and could recall the associated picture or picture category; they selected “old” if they remembered the word but could not recall the associated picture or picture category; they selected “new” if they did not remember studying the word.
Associative memory performance was estimated using a sensitivity index, associative d′, where hits were defined as correct associative category responses to studied words and false alarms were defined as incorrect category responses to new words. Thus, associative d′ = Z(“correct associate category”|old) − Z(“associate category”|new). To assess basic task comprehension and the ability to make discriminations between studied and novel words, we also calculated an old/new sensitivity index. Here, hits were defined as correct old responses to studied words, irrespective of successful memory for the associate, and false alarms rate was defined as any incorrect old response to novel words. Thus, old/new d ′= Z(“old” + “face” + “place”|old) − Z(“old” + “face” + “place”|new). Analyses included data from 128 participants (table 1; figure e-1, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp).
Mnemonic Discrimination
The mnemonic discrimination task (figure 1B) was administered using previously described measures and instructions12 (task and stimuli are available at github.com/celstark/MST). During an incidental encoding phase, participants made indoor/outdoor judgments for 128 pictures of everyday objects. Participants then performed a surprise memory test, in which half of the studied objects (64) were intermixed with 64 perceptually similar lure objects and 64 novel (dissimilar) objects. Participants were to respond “old” if they remembered the object as having been studied, “similar” if they remembered the object as similar, but not identical, to a studied object, or “new” if they remembered the object as not having been studied. Trials with a biologically implausible reaction time (<400 ms; M 1.68 trials/participant; SD 3.36) and trials in which participants did not respond (M 10.50 trials/participant; SD 9.47) were excluded from analysis.
Of particular interest in this task is the ability to correctly identify lures as “similar,” avoiding the tendency to label lures as “old”; this ability is thought to be hippocampal-dependent. Due to the 3-response task design, there are 2 measures of memory sensitivity (d′) that can been calculated to quantify lure discrimination ability: (1) lure/new d′—the ability to correctly classify perceptually similar lures and differentiate them from novel objects, as Z(“similar”|lure) − Z(“similar”|novel foil); and (2) old/lure d′—the ability to correctly endorse studied objects and avoid the propensity to incorrectly endorse lures as old, as Z(“old”|target) − Z(“old”|lure). Although related (r = 0.53), the former measure may be particularly sensitive to hippocampal function.14
The 64 lures systematically varied in perceptual similarity to the studied targets (figure 1B), ranging from level 1 (high perceptual similarity; most difficult) to 5 (low perceptual similarity; least difficult). For each of the 5 similarity levels, 13 lures were presented, except for level 1, in which 12 lures were presented. Thus, each lure discrimination measure was computed overall, as described above, as well as at each level of target–lure similarity (for example, Z[“similar”|lure bin 1] − Z[“similar”|novel foil]; Z[“old”|target] − Z[“old”|lure bin 1]). In addition to these lure discrimination measures of primary interest, we also computed old/new d′—the ability to differentiate between studied objects and novel objects, which is not selectively dependent on hippocampal function33—as Z(“old”|target) − Z(“old”|novel foil). Analyses included data from 133 participants (table 1; figure e-1, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp).
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted in R version 3.3.1. Multiple linear regression was used to examine the relationship between CSF proteins and memory. Prior to analysis, all continuous predictors were z scored across participants; standardized coefficients are reported. All models included age, sex, and years of education as nuisance regressors. Linear mixed-effects models were used to examine the relationship between CSF proteins and mnemonic discrimination as a function of target–lure similarity (5 levels, treated as ordinal variable and centered), with the inclusion of (1) an interaction term of CSF protein by similarity, (2) interaction terms for age, sex, and education by similarity, and (3) a random intercept and slope for each participant. To visualize interactions, we extracted the slope across lure bins for each participant and plotted against CSF proteins.
To mitigate the effect of influential data points, such as individuals with high p-tau181, on the outcomes, we used bootstrap resampling with 5,000 iterations of data sampled with replacement to determine effect significance. Thus, for all analyses relating continuous CSF proteins to memory measures, we report 95% confidence intervals (CIs), and consider effects significant only if 0 does not fall within the 95% CI of the bootstrapped estimate of the effect. For mediation analyses, the coefficient of the indirect path was calculated as the product of direct effects a × b, and considered significant if 0 does not fall within the 95% CI of the bootstrapped estimate. All findings were replicated when log-transforming CSF p-tau181 values as an alternative approach to mitigating the influence of extreme values (data not shown).
Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents
All participants provided informed consent in accordance with a protocol approved by the Stanford institutional review board.
Data Availability
Anonymized data will be made available to any qualified investigator upon request.
Results
Sample Characteristics
Data subsamples did not differ from the full CSF sample with respect to demographics or CSF values (all p > 0.260; table 1). CSF Aβ42, Aβ40, and p-tau181 distributions are plotted in figure 2, A–C. Gaussian mixture models fit to the Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio (figure 2D) identified a 2-distribution, equal variance model as optimal; the resulting Aβ42/Aβ40 cutoff was 0.0752 (figure e-2, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). This resulted in 40 CU classified as Aβ+ (Mage 70.45; SD 6.25) and 113 as Aβ− (Mage 68.19; SD 5.56). The Aβ+ group was significantly older than the Aβ− group (t[62.16] = 2.02, p = 0.048); groups did not differ in years of education (p = 0.461), sex (p = 0.727), or Mini-Mental State Examination score (p = 0.241).
CSF Protein Characteristics
Age was associated with lower CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 (β = −0.233, p = 0.004; figure 3A) and higher p-tau181 (β = 0.342, p = 10−5; figure 3B), but not Aβ42 (β = −0.029, p = 0.727; figure e-4A, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). Including age as a covariate, p-tau181 was marginally greater in women (β = −0.302, p = 0.059), whereas CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 (β = 0.153, p = 0.353) and Aβ42 (β = −0.253, p = 0.133) did not vary by sex.
Figure 3. CSF Protein Sample Characteristics.
Data are plotted for β-amyloid (Aβ)+ (coral) and Aβ− (turquoise) participants. (A) Age is associated with lower CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and (B) higher phospho-tau (p-tau). (C) CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 is associated with p-tau. Plots show linear model predictions (black line) and 95% confidence intervals (shaded area).
CSF Aβ42 and Aβ40 were correlated (r = 0.64, p < 10−16; figure e-4B, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). A lower Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio was associated with elevated p-tau181 (β = −0.494, p < 10−11; figure 3C), but did not account for the effect of age on p-tau181, which remained significant (β = 0.227, p = 0.001). Similarly, the Aβ+ group had elevated p-tau181 relative to the Aβ− group (β = −1.146, p < 10−12). In contrast, CSF Aβ42 was not significantly related to p-tau181 (β = 0.027, p = 0.732; figure e-4C, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp).
Standardized Neuropsychological Delayed Recall
Delayed recall composite score declined with age (table 2 and figure 4A), was lower in men (β = −0.447, p = 0.004), and was positively associated with education (β = 0.246, p = 0.001). Including these variables as covariates, delayed recall score did not vary as a function of CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 (table 2 and figure 4B) or amyloid status (β = −0.229, p = 0.182). An association with p-tau181 was observed, but the bootstrapped effect was nonsignificant (table 2 and figure 4C). CSF Aβ42 was not associated with delayed recall score (β = 0.103, p = 0.172). Similar outcomes were observed when analyses were restricted to individual delayed recall tests (table e-1, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp).
Table 2.
Summary of Model Results Across Primary Episodic Memory Measures
Figure 4. Effects of Age, Amyloid, and Phospho-Tau (P-Tau) on Hippocampal-Dependent Memory.
Data are plotted for β-amyloid (Aβ)+ (coral) and Aβ− (turquoise) participants. (A) Delayed recall declined with age, but did not significantly vary with (B) Aβ42/Aβ40 or (C) p-tau181 (bootstrapped effect nonsignificant). Associative d′ declined with (D) age, (E) lower Aβ42/Aβ40, and (F) p-tau181. (G–I) Linear mixed effects models assessed the relationship between target–lure similarity and mnemonic discrimination performance. Each participant's slope (adjusted for sex and education), plotted on the y-axis, reflects the magnitude of the increase in performance as target–lure similarity moved from high to low. (G) The relationship between similarity and performance did not vary with age (bootstrapped effect nonsignificant), whereas (H) lower Aβ42/Aβ40 and (I) greater p-tau181 are each associated with a diminished performance improvement as similarity decreased. Plots show linear model predictions (black line) and 95% confidence intervals (shaded area).
Associative Memory
The primary measure of interest from the associative memory task is associative d′—the ability to remember the category of the image initially paired with the cue word (figure 1C; see figure e-5 for results as a function stimulus category—i.e., face associations and place associations, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). Associative d′ declined with age (table 2 and figure 4D), but did not vary by sex or education (all p > 0.175). Including these variables as covariates, we found lower levels of CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 (table 2 and figure 4E) and Aβ+ status (β = −0.537, p = 0.007) were associated with poorer associative d′, whereas Aβ42 was not (β = 0.049, p = 0.570). Similarly, p-tau181 was negatively related to associative d′ and the bootstrapped effect was significant (table 2 and figure 4F). When CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and p-tau181 were combined in the same model, p-tau181 remained a significant predictor of associative d′, while age and amyloid were no longer significant (table 2). A mediation analysis indicated that p-tau181 mediated the relationship between Aβ42/Aβ40 and associative d′ (indirect effect: β = 0.165, CI = 0.019–0.323). To visualize this pattern another way, we plot performance as a function of binary amyloid (A)/tau (T) status (where T+ is the top 25% of the distribution; figure 5B), which demonstrates that pronounced deficits in performance are observed primarily in the A+/T+ group.
Figure 5. Memory Performance as a Function of Combined Amyloid (A) and Tau (T) Status.
Cognitively unimpaired individuals (CU) in the upper quartile of the phospho-tau181 (p-tau181) distribution (≥42 pg/mL; n = 38; 27 F) were classified as p-tau+, and the remainder as p-tau− (n = 125; 67 F), yielding 4 groups: A−T− (n = 98), A−T+ (N = 15), A+T− (n = 17), A + T+ (n = 23). Qualitatively, (A) while delayed recall performance was similar across groups, (B) associative d′, (C) lure/new d′, and (D) old/lure d′ revealed performance decrements predominantly in the A+/T+ group. Colors in C and D reflect target–lure similarity levels ranging from highest perceptual similarity (red) to lowest perceptual similarity (magenta).
The same pattern was observed with respect to old/new d′—the ability to discriminate studied from novel words irrespective of associative recall accuracy (figure 1C), which was highly correlated with associative d′ (r = 0.86). Performance declined with age (β = −0.262, p = 0.003, CI −0.45 to −0.09), with lower Aβ42/Aβ40 (β = 0.176, p = 0.049, CI 0.01–0.36) and Aβ+ status (β = −0.402, p = 0.044), but not Aβ42 (β = 0.058, p = 0.510). An association with p-tau181 was observed, and the bootstrapped effect was significant (β = −0.214, p = 0.020, CI −0.37 to −0.01).
Mnemonic Discrimination
Participants were able to successfully identify studied items as “old” (M 0.85, SD 0.10) and novel foils as “new” (M 0.83, SD 0.11). In contrast, the ability to identify lures as “similar,” avoiding the propensity to call lures “old,” varied systematically as a function of target–lure similarity, as reflected in the lure/new d′ and old/lure d′ scores (figure 1, D and E). Specifically, the probability of incorrectly calling a lure “old” (i.e., a false alarm) decreased as lures went from high (M 0.76, SD 0.16) to low similarity (M 0.35, SD 0.20). Likewise, the probability of correctly calling a lure “similar” was least likely when similarity was high (M 0.18, SD 0.14) and systematically improved as similarity decreased (M 0.49, SD 0.25).
We modeled each d′ measure in a linear mixed model context to determine whether there was a linear relationship between lure similarity and performance and whether this relationship varied as a function of demographics and CSF variables. Target–lure similarity significantly affected performance (lure/new d′: β = 0.304, p < 10−16; old/lure d′: β = 0.435, p < 10−16), suggesting that each similarity bin was associated with a d′ increase of 0.30–0.43 (in z-score units) across the entire group (figure 1, D and E). We also observed an education × similarity interaction (lure/new d′: β = 0.056, p = 0.001; old/lure d′: β = 0.081, p < 10−5), such that as target–lure similarity decreased, fewer years of education were associated with a smaller enhancement in performance. An age × similarity interaction was also observed, but the bootstrapped effect was not significant for lure/new d′ (table 2 and figure 4G) or old/lure d′ (β = −0.044, p = 0.018, CI −0.11 to 0.02).
Turning first to lure/new d′, models including interactions of age, sex, and education with similarity revealed significant Aβ42/Aβ40 × similarity (table 2 and figure 4H), amyloid status × similarity (β = −0.083, p = 0.032), and p-tau181 × similarity (table 2 and figure 4I) interactions. When combined in a single model, only the p-tau181 × similarity interaction remained significant (table 2). A mediation analysis indicated that the p-tau181 × similarity effect mediated the Aβ42/Aβ40 × similarity effect on lure/new d′ (indirect effect: β = 0.022, CI 0.004–0.043). To visualize this relationship, we plot performance as a function of similarity bin, with participants grouped based on binary amyloid/tau status (figure 5C). Examining the pattern of performance across similarity levels, these interactions reflect that the A+/T+ group shows the smallest increase in performance as target–lure similarity decreases, whereas the A−/T− group shows a clear increase in performance.
While a qualitatively similar pattern of results was observed for old/lure d′ (figure 5D), only the p-tau181 × similarity effect was significant (β = −0.046, p = 0.017, CI −0.08 to −0.01), whereas nonsignificant interactions of Aβ42/Aβ40 (β = 0.032, p = 0.086, CI −0.002 to 0.066) and amyloid status (β = −0.061, p = 0.157) with similarity were observed. Across discrimination measures, Aβ42 × similarity effects were nonsignificant (lure/new d′: β = 0.027, p = 0.107; old/lure d′: β = 0.032, p = 0.091).
While the preceding primary analyses considered how performance changes as a function of target–lure similarity (i.e., bins 1 through 5; see Statistical Analyses), similar results were observed when performance was analyzed as a difference in performance between low similarity (bins 3–5) and high similarity (bins 1–2) discrimination (see figure e-6, doi:10.5061/dryad.ngf1vhhrp). In contrast, when lure/new d′ or old/lure d′ were averaged across all target–lure similarity bins, we did not observe a significant effect of CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 (lure/new d′: β = −0.019, p = 0.829; old/lure d′: β = −0.038, p = 0.658) or p-tau181 (lure/new d′: β = −0.030, p = 0.744; old/lure d′: β = −0.009, p = 0.919) on performance. Similarly, performance at any single level of target–lure similarity was not significantly associated with CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 or p-tau181 (all p > 0.089). Finally, the ability to discriminate between studied and novel objects—old/new d′—did not significantly vary as a function of Aβ42/Aβ40 (β = 0.072, p = 0.384), Aβ status (β = −0.267, p = 0.155), or p-tau181 (β = −0.134, p = 0.118). CSF Aβ42 did not exhibit significant associations with any of these measures (all p > 0.557).
Behavioral Predictors of Preclinical AD Pathology
Given that qualitatively similar relationships were observed between memory performance and CSF biomarkers across multiple memory measures, we next assessed which measures might be most sensitive to individual differences in CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and p-tau181 using stepwise regression. The set of possible predictors entered into the model included age, sex, education, delayed recall composite score, associative d′, lure/new d′ similarity slope, and old/lure d′ similarity slope, with CSF variables as the outcome variables. The final model retained only significant predictors (at p < 0.05). For Aβ42/Aβ40, the final model (R2adj = 0.101, F3,112 = 7.28, p = 0.001) included age (β = −0.225, p = 0.018, CI −0.411 to −0.039) and associative d′ (β = 0.200, p = 0.035, CI 0.014–0.385). For p-tau181, the final model (R2adj = 0.228, F3,112 = 12.05, p < 0.001) included age (β = 0.200, p = 0.035, CI 0.025–0.384), associative d′ (β = −0.250, p = 0.006, CI −0.426 to −0.074), and lure/new d′ similarity slope (β = −0.224, p = 0.015, CI −0.403 to −0.044). Thus, associative memory and mnemonic discrimination are stronger predictors, relative to delayed recall, of variance in CSF biomarkers in CU. Moreover, these tasks are not redundant, but explain unique variance in p-tau181.
Discussion
This study tested the hypothesis that in a CU population, memory assays designed to tax hippocampal function are sensitive to variations in performance associated with underlying preclinical AD pathology. In the present cohort, over a quarter of individuals were identified as Aβ+ based on CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 distribution. We observed significant associations between CSF Aβ42/Aβ40, p-tau181, and 2 specialized assays of hippocampal-dependent memory, where effects of Aβ42/Aβ40 were mediated by associated increases in p-tau181. Although relationships were qualitatively similar, a standardized clinical memory measure—delayed recall composite score—did not exhibit significant associations with CSF proteins. Together, these results suggest that assays that are designed to incisively tax hippocampal function have promise for detecting variance in memory related to the presence of preclinical AD pathology in CU.
These findings have relevance for efforts to identify specialized cognitive screening tools for detecting biomarker positivity in CU, which are gaining increasing interest and popularity18,34–36 due to limitations of standardized neuropsychological assays for detecting subtle variations in cognition among CU, especially when examining cross-sectional associations. Our results indicate that both associative cued recall and mnemonic discrimination of perceptually similar objects outperform standardized delayed recall tests with respect to detecting variance related to CSF biomarkers of preclinical AD in CU. Thus, although all 3 memory tests explored here are supported by the hippocampus and surrounding medial temporal lobe structures, tasks designed to tax hippocampal computations (e.g., pattern separation, pattern completion) may offer enhanced sensitivity to detect initial changes in performance related to AD pathology. Notably, we found that associative memory and mnemonic discrimination measures explained unique variance in p-tau181, suggesting that these tasks may be used in combination to improve detection of preclinical AD pathology in older adults.
Critically, however, such relationships within the mnemonic discrimination task were observed only as a function of change in performance (i.e., slope) across levels of target–lure similarity: whereas all participants performed poorly on the most difficult discriminations (i.e., high lure–target similarity), only biomarker-positive individuals failed to systematically improve as discriminations became easier (i.e., lower lure–target similarity; figure 5). The sensitivity of an individual's slope across similarity bins to variance in AD biomarkers replicates recent work using a spatial mnemonic discrimination task and amyloid PET to measure preclinical AD burden.19 Notably, this pattern also parallels boundary conditions of the ability of the hippocampus to differentiate similar inputs: at extremely high levels of perceptual overlap, even a functional hippocampus will often fail to distinguish between events37; this level therefore lacks utility for measuring differences across CU. As overlap decreases across lure bins, but events nevertheless share overlapping features, performance improves in a linear fashion, reflecting hippocampal-dependent computations supporting performance. The magnitude of improvement across similarity levels, or lack thereof, may provide an index of hippocampal functional integrity. This pattern highlights important boundary conditions regarding the use of mnemonic discrimination tasks for detecting variance related to AD biomarkers in CU, suggesting it may be optimal to measure change in performance across successive levels of difficulty.
More broadly, this pattern is consistent with current hypotheses that the sensitivity of these tasks to biomarker levels in CU is related to links between performance and functional integrity of the hippocampus–entorhinal circuit, areas that are particularly vulnerable to early tangle pathology in CU. For example, we previously demonstrated in the SAMS cohort that the magnitude of hippocampal activity was tightly coupled with the likelihood of accurate associative cued recall on individual trials, and predicted associative d′ across individuals.15 Similarly, prior work indicates that discrimination of perceptually similar lures from studied objects engages the hippocampus and anterolateral entorhinal cortex, and that functional imbalances of this circuit are associated with worse performance.13,14 Thus, performance may also be sensitive to alterations in hippocampal–entorhinal functional integrity, such as those arising due to tangle pathology. Consistent with this possibility, prior work in CU has observed associations between MTL tau, altered activity,18 and functional connectivity38,39 in these areas and memory performance. The present observation that CSF p-tau181 was more proximal to behavior, mediating relationships between Aβ42/Aβ40 and performance, is compatible with these hypotheses.
CSF p-tau181 is an indirect measure of tangle pathology, and longitudinal data indicate that CSF p-tau181 becomes abnormal relatively early in the disease course, years prior to significant regional uptake using tau PET imaging,3,40 which provides a measure of focal tangle accumulation.41 The present results therefore build on findings from Aβ-PET and tau-PET imaging suggesting sensitivity of associative cued recall tasks16,17 and mnemonic discrimination tasks18,19,36 to preclinical AD pathology, and provide novel evidence for such relationships using CSF to measure biomarker abnormality in a large sample of CU. Moreover, by measuring amyloid and tau simultaneously, they also provide insights into how these 2 proteins relate to one another and behavior. While the present data are cross-sectional, they suggest that amyloid-dependent increases in p-tau181 are necessary to observe decrements in memory performance using the present measures (i.e., they are not observed in Aβ+ alone or T+ alone, and overt tau elevations are not present in the Aβ− group). This observation is consistent with prior work highlighting a correlation between amyloid plaque and neurofibrillary tangle deposition, as well as work showing that tangles are a closer proxy of cognitive decline and clinical status compared to amyloid plaques.42–44
Beyond the adoption of specialized tasks, the ability to detect meaningful variability in CSF protein levels within CU may be particularly affected by methodologic precision, given the more limited range in analyte values. We employed fully automated CSF analysis, which reduces experimenter-introduced noise and intralaboratory and interlaboratory variability45 in the data through incorporation of standardized reference material.46 We also normalized Aβ42 by Aβ40, which improves sensitivity and specificity for detecting Aβ burden related to AD22–25 by adjusting for individual differences in Aβ production. Notably, the Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio provided a basis for establishing Aβ positivity within CU in the absence of Aβ-PET or a patient comparison group, yielding a cutoff (<0.0752) that corresponds remarkably well with that from an independent CU cohort (<0.075),47 despite the use of a different analysis platform (Elecsys). Furthermore, the ratio enabled detection of relationships between amyloid and both p-tau181 and memory, neither of which was achieved using CSF Aβ42 alone. These results demonstrate the value of Aβ40 measurement and suggest it may be particularly critical for detecting initial changes in Aβ in CU.
Importantly, while the present results provide evidence for significant relationships between CSF proteins and specialized tests of hippocampal-dependent memory, we observed qualitatively similar, though nonsignificant, relationships with standardized clinical memory tests. Future work is needed to further define the task characteristics that optimize sensitivity to preclinical AD pathology in CU. Moreover, although these tasks are readily implemented in laboratory contexts, there may be challenges for integration into clinical contexts in their current form. Nevertheless, the present findings add to a growing body of evidence encouraging further exploration of assays that tax hippocampal function for early detection of biomarker positivity in cognitively unimpaired older adults.
Glossary
- Aβ
β-amyloid
- AD
Alzheimer disease
- CI
confidence interval
- CU
cognitively unimpaired older adults
- MTL
medial temporal lobe
- p-tau181
phospho-tau181
- SAMS
Stanford Aging and Memory Study
Appendix. Authors
Study Funding
Supported by the National Institute on Aging (R01AG048076, R21AG058111, R21AG058859, and P30AG066515), Stanford Wu Tsai Neurosciences Institute, and Stanford Center for Precision Health and Integrated Diagnostics (PHIND).
Disclosure
A.N. Trelle, V.A. Carr, E.N. Wilson, M.S. Swarovski, M.P. Hunt, T.N. Toueg, T.T. Tran, D. Channappa, N.K. Corso, M.K. Thieu, M. Jayakumar, A. Nadiadwala, W. Guo, N.J. Tanner, J.D. Bernstein, C.P. Litovsky, S.A. Guerin, A.M. Khazenzon, M.B. Harrison, B.K. Rutt, G.K. Deutsch, F.T. Chin, G.A. Davidzon, J.N. Hall, S.J. Sha, C.A. Fredericks, and K.I. Andreasson report no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. G.A. Kerchner is a full-time employee and shareholder of F. Hoffmann-La Roche, Ltd. A.D. Wagner reports no disclosures relevant to the manuscript. E.C. Mormino has served as a paid consultant to F. Hoffmann-La Roche and Alector. Go to Neurology.org/Nhttps://n.neurology.org/lookup/doi/10.1212/WNL.0000000000011477 for full disclosures.
References
- 1.Sperling R, Mormino E, Johnson K. The evolution of preclinical Alzheimer's disease: implications for prevention trials. Neuron 2014;84:608–622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Palmqvist S, Mattsson N, Hansson O. Cerebrospinal fluid analysis detects cerebral amyloid-β accumulation earlier than positron emission tomography. Brain 2016;139:1226–1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Meyer PF, Pichet Binette A, Gonneaud J, Breitner JCS, Villeneuve S. Characterization of Alzheimer disease biomarker discrepancies using cerebrospinal fluid phosphorylated tau and AV1451 positron emission tomography. JAMA Neurol 2020;77:508–516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fagan AM, Roe CM, Xiong C, Mintun MA, Morris JC, Holtzman DM. Cerebrospinal fluid tau/β-amyloid42 ratio as a prediction of cognitive decline in nondemented older adults. Arch Neurol 2007;64:343–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Stomrud E, Hansson O, Zetterberg H, Blennow K, Minthon L, Londos E. Correlation of longitudinal cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers with cognitive decline in healthy older adults. Arch Neurol 2010;67:217–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Soldan A, Pettigrew C, Cai Q, et al. Hypothetical preclinical Alzheimer disease groups and longitudinal cognitive change. JAMA Neurol 2016;73:698–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Schindler SE, Jasielec MS, Weng H, et al. Neuropsychological measures that detect early impairment and decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurobiol Aging 2017;56:25–32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Clark LR, Berman SE, Norton D, et al. Age-accelerated cognitive decline in asymptomatic adults with CSF β-Amyloid. Neurology 2018;90:e1306–e1315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991;82:239–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Braak H, Braak E. Staging of Alzheimer’s disease-related neurofibrillary changes. Neurobiol Aging 1995;16:271–278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Buerger K, Ewers M, Pirttilä T, et al. CSF phosphorylated tau protein correlates with neocortical neurofibrillary pathology in Alzheimer's disease. Brain 2006;129:3035–3041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Stark SM, Yassa MA, Lacy JW, Stark CEL. A task to assess behavioral pattern separation (BPS) in humans: data from healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychol 2013;51:2442–2449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Yassa MA, Lacy JW, Stark SM, Albert MS, Gallagher M, Stark CEL. Pattern separation deficits associated with increased hippocampal CA3 and dentate gyrus activity in nondemented older adults. Hippocampus 2011;21:968–979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Reagh ZM, Noche JA, Tustison NJ, Delisle D, Murray EA, Yassa MA. Functional imbalance of anterolateral entorhinal cortex and hippocampal dentate/CA3 underlies age-related object pattern separation deficits. Neuron 2018;97:1187–1198.e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Trelle AN, Carr VA, Guerin SA, et al. Hippocampal and cortical mechanisms at retrieval explain variability in episodic remembering in older adults. Elife 2020;9:1–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rentz DM, Amariglio RE, Becker JA, et al. Face-name associative memory performance is related to amyloid burden in normal elderly. Neuropsychologia 2011;49:2776–2783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Papp KV, Amariglio RE, Mormino EC, et al. Free and cued memory in relation to biomarker-defined abnormalities in clinically normal older adults and those at risk for Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia 2015;73:169–175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Maass A, Berron D, Harrison TM, et al. Alzheimer's pathology targets distinct memory networks in the ageing brain. Brain 2019;142:2492–2509. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Webb CE, Foster CM, Horn MM, Kennedy KM, Rodrigue KM. Beta-amyloid burden predicts poorer mnemonic discrimination in cognitively normal older adults. Neuroimage 2020;221:117199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Leitão MJ, Silva-Spínola A, Santana I, et al. Clinical validation of the Lumipulse G cerebrospinal fluid assays for routine diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Res Ther; 2019;11:1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Paciotti S, Sepe FN, Eusebi P, et al. Diagnostic performance of a fully automated chemiluminescent enzyme immunoassay for Alzheimer's disease diagnosis. Clin Chim Acta 2019;494:74–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lewczuk P, Matzen A, Blennow K, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid Aβ42/40 corresponds better than Aβ42 to amyloid PET in Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis 2017;55:813–822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Alcolea D, Pegueroles J, Muñoz L, et al. Agreement of amyloid PET and CSF biomarkers for Alzheimer's disease on Lumipulse. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2019;6:1815–1824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Janelidze S, Zetterberg H, Mattsson N, et al. CSF Aβ42/Aβ40 and Aβ42/Aβ38 ratios: better diagnostic markers of Alzheimer disease. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2016;3:154–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Hansson O, Lehmann S, Otto M, Zetterberg H, Lewczuk P. Advantages and disadvantages of the use of the CSF amyloid β (Aβ) 42/40 ratio in the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Res Ther; 2019;11:1–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kroth H, Oden F, Molette J, et al. Discovery and preclinical characterization of [18F]PI-2620, a next-generation tau PET tracer for the assessment of tau pathology in Alzheimer's disease and other tauopathies. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging 2019;46:2178–2189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Mormino EC, Toueg TN, Azevedo C, et al. Tau PET imaging with 18F-PI-2620 in aging and neurodegenerative diseases. Eur J Nucl Med Mol Imaging Epub 2020 Jun 23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 28.Morris JC. The clinical Dementia rating (CDR): current version and scoring rules. Neurol 1993;43:2412–2414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wilson EN, Swarovski MS, Linortner P, et al. Soluble TREM2 is elevated in Parkinson's disease subgroups with increased CSF tau. Brain 2020;143:932–943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Koopman K, Le Bastard N, Martin JJ, Nagels G, De Deyn PP, Engelborghs S. Improved discrimination of autopsy-confirmed Alzheimer's disease (AD) from non-AD dementias using CSF P-tau181P. Neurochem Int 2009;55:214–218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Mormino EC, Betensky RA, Hedden T, et al. Amyloid and APOE e4 interact to influence short-term decline in preclinical Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2014;82:1760–1767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jack CR, Bennett DA, Blennow K, et al. NIA-AA Research Framework: toward a biological definition of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement 2018;14:535–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Norman KA, O'Reilly RC. Modeling hippocampal and neocortical contributions to recognition memory: a complementary-learning-systems approach. Psychol Rev 2003;110:611–646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rentz DM, Parra Rodriguez MA, Amariglio R, Stern Y, Sperling R, Ferris S. Promising developments in neuropsychological approaches for the detection of preclinical Alzheimer's disease: a selective review. Alzheimers Res Ther 2013;5:58. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Berron D, Cardenas-Blanco A, Bittner D, et al. Higher CSF tau levels are related to hippocampal hyperactivity and object mnemonic discrimination in older adults. J Neurosci 2019;39:8788–8797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Leal SL, Ferguson LA, Harrison TM, Jagust WJ. Development of a mnemonic discrimination task using naturalistic stimuli with applications to aging and preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Learn Mem 2019;26:219–228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Norman KA. Revisiting the complementary learning systems model. Hippocampus 2010;20:1217–1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Harrison TM, Maass A, Adams JN, Du R, Baker SL, Jagust WJ. Tau deposition is associated with functional isolation of the hippocampus in aging. Nat Commun 2019;10:4900. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Berron D, van Westen D, Ossenkoppele R, Strandberg O, Hansson O. Medial temporal lobe connectivity and its associations with cognition in early Alzheimer's disease. Brain 2020;143:1233–1248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Mattsson-Carlgren N, Andersson E, Janelidze S, et al. Aβ deposition is associated with increases in soluble and phosphorylated tau that precede a positive tau PET in Alzheimer's disease. Sci Adv 2020;6:eaaz2387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Lowe VJ, Lundt ES, Albertson SM, et al. Tau-positron emission tomography correlates with neuropathology findings. Alzheimers Dement 2020;16:561–571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Arriagada PV, Growdon JH, Hedley-Whyte ET, Hyman BT. Neurofibrillary tangles but not senile plaques parallel duration and severity of Alzheimer's disease. Neurology 1992;42:631–639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Wilson RS, Bienias JL, Arnold SE. Neurofibrillary tangles mediate the association of amyloid load with clinical Alzheimer disease and level of cognitive function. Arch Neurol 2004;61:378–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nelson PT, Alafuzoff I, Bigio EH, et al. Correlation of Alzheimer disease neuropathologic changes with cognitive status: a review of the literature. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2012;71:362–381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mattsson N, Andreasson U, Persson S, et al. CSF biomarker variability in the Alzheimer's Association quality control program. Alzheimers Dement 2013;9:251–261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Andreasson U, Kuhlmann J, Pannee J, et al. Commutability of the certified reference materials for the standardization of β-Amyloid 1-42 assay in human cerebrospinal fluid: lessons for tau and β-amyloid 1-40 measurements. Clin Chem Lab Med 2018;56:2058–2066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Schindler SE, Gray JD, Gordon BA, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers measured by Elecsys assays compared to amyloid imaging. Alzheimers Dement 2018;14:1460–1469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Anonymized data will be made available to any qualified investigator upon request.