Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2021 Apr 19;11:8442. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-87789-5

Task-related brain functional network reconfigurations relate to motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke

Hsiao-Ju Cheng 1,2, Kwun Kei Ng 1, Xing Qian 1, Fang Ji 1, Zhong Kang Lu 3, Wei Peng Teo 4, Xin Hong 5, Fatima Ali Nasrallah 5,6, Kai Keng Ang 3,7, Kai-Hsiang Chuang 5,6, Cuntai Guan 7, Haoyong Yu 2, Effie Chew 9,10,, Juan Helen Zhou 1,8,11,12,
PMCID: PMC8055891  PMID: 33875691

Abstract

Stroke leads to both regional brain functional disruptions and network reorganization. However, how brain functional networks reconfigure as task demand increases in stroke patients and whether such reorganization at baseline would facilitate post-stroke motor recovery are largely unknown. To address this gap, brain functional connectivity (FC) were examined at rest and motor tasks in eighteen chronic subcortical stroke patients and eleven age-matched healthy controls. Stroke patients underwent a 2-week intervention using a motor imagery-assisted brain computer interface-based (MI-BCI) training with or without transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Motor recovery was determined by calculating the changes of the upper extremity component of the Fugl–Meyer Assessment (FMA) score between pre- and post-intervention divided by the pre-intervention FMA score. The results suggested that as task demand increased (i.e., from resting to passive unaffected hand gripping and to active affected hand gripping), patients showed greater FC disruptions in cognitive networks including the default and dorsal attention networks. Compared to controls, patients had lower task-related spatial similarity in the somatomotor–subcortical, default–somatomotor, salience/ventral attention–subcortical and subcortical–subcortical connections, suggesting greater inefficiency in motor execution. Importantly, higher baseline network-specific FC strength (e.g., dorsal attention and somatomotor) and more efficient brain network reconfigurations (e.g., somatomotor and subcortical) from rest to active affected hand gripping at baseline were related to better future motor recovery. Our findings underscore the importance of studying functional network reorganization during task-free and task conditions for motor recovery prediction in stroke.

Subject terms: Stroke, Neuroscience, Motor control

Introduction

Stroke is caused by interrupted or reduced blood supply to the brain and results in focal lesions1. Changes in blood supply after stroke directly impact the physiological parameters of the blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) signal measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)2, making it a potential tool to investigate brain functional alterations after stroke2. The intrinsic functional connectivity (FC) approach maps the spatially distributed, synchronous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuation and delineates several brain functional networks formed by highly correlated BOLD signals at task-free condition in healthy individuals3.

Task-free fMRI has been advocated for investigating brain functional networks in stroke patients due to its high applicability4,5. FC between brain hemispheres (inter-hemispheric FC) was associated with motor control6, residual motor function7,8, and behavioral impairments in attention, memory, language, and vision in stroke patients7. Increased FC in ipsilesional dorsal attention and default networks in stroke patients compared to healthy controls was also noted8. These findings pointed to network-specific changes at task-free condition after stroke. However, alteration in FC and brain networks corresponding to task demands in stroke patients remains largely unknown. Some studies have found that task-related sensorimotor neural activity was associated with motor recovery in stroke patients911. Another study using the affected hand gripping task at a target pressure of 10% or 30% of the maximum voluntary contraction for each participant showed FC alterations in brain networks including dorsal attention, visual, motor, and default mode networks and found an positive correlation between ipsilesional sensorimotor activity and motor performance12. Previous studies in healthy individuals have demonstrated that FC at rest and task were highly related in terms of activity13,14 and topologies13,15. Applying fMRI with external task demands may provide more insights in uncovering stroke-induced brain network alterations and its relationship with motor recovery.

While the functional network architecture during rest and task might be highly similar15,16, inducing the brain into different states by external cognitive tasks could reveal group differences and individual variations that cannot be readily observed when the brain is in more unconstrainted task-free states17. A study in healthy subjects found that higher efficiency in task-related reconfiguration, i.e., higher similarity between rest and task FC patterns, was related to better performance in language, reasoning, and working memory16. Network-specific high flexibility changing from rest to task was reported in default, control, and salience networks18 and the working memory task-related reconfiguration patterns further revealed the differential roles of the control and default networks in healthy subjects19. Nevertheless, little is known about how brain functional networks reconfigure from rest to task in stroke patients and how these functional network disruptions or reorganization at baseline influence post-stroke motor recovery.

Although previous studies have investigated task-free and task-based FC related to motor recovery in stroke patients, there is a lack of studies examining task-free and task-based FC simultaneously in the same cohort of stroke patients. Furthermore, the similarity between task-free and task-based FC architecture remains largely unknown. The influence of external task demands on both FC and brain functional network reconfiguration in stroke patients is less studied. Importantly, how the changes of FC/brain functional network reconfiguration with different task demands associate with motor recovery in stroke patient? To cover these gaps, we adopted a whole-brain network-based approach as previous findings reported various brain networks associated with post-stroke motor function/recovery such as the default, dorsal attention, and somatomotor networks. We examined the differences in both task-free and task-based FC between chronic subcortical stroke patients and age-matched healthy controls. We hypothesized that stroke patients would have lower FC in somatomotor and subcortical networks than healthy controls at both task-free and task-based conditions; more importantly, we expected more FC disruptions, particularly in the high-order cognitive networks, in stroke patients during more demanding task conditions (active movement) compared to passive/assisted movement. We also predicted that stroke patients would have higher task-based reconfigurations in somatomotor and subcortical networks compared to healthy controls. We sought to test if these network-specific brain FC disruptions and task-related reconfigurations at baseline will relate to motor recovery in stroke patients after undergoing a 2-week motor imagery-assisted brain computer interface-based (MI-BCI) training with or without transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).

Results

More widespread network disruptions during task than during rest condition post-stroke

To investigate the group (stroke patients vs age-matched healthy controls) and task [task-free vs average of four task conditions (i.e., task-general)] interaction, we performed two-way repeated measure analysis of covariance with mean absolute motion displacement as a nuisance covariate using network-based statistic (NBS) toolbox (see “Methods”). Significant group and task interaction effects were observed in default, control, salience/ventral attention, dorsal attention, and subcortical networks (FDR corrected p < 0.05, partial η2 = 0.024, Fig. 1A,B).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

More widespread network disruptions in task-based condition than task-free condition in stroke patients. (A) (Left panel) Significant interaction effects of group (patients vs controls) and task (rest vs task) were found in intra-network FC within default and DorsAttn networks and in inter-network FC between default and DorsAttn, control and DorsAttn, SalVenAttn and subcortical networks. (Right panel) Compared to age-matched healthy controls, stroke patients showed lower intra-network FC in control, SalVenAttn, and subcortical networks as well as lower inter-network FC between ipsilesional control and bilateral subcortical networks during the task-free condition. During the task-general condition, stroke patients had more widespread network disruptions than resting-state, including lower intra-network FC in default, control, SalVenAttn, SomMot, and subcortical networks. Additionally, lower inter-network FC was found between ipsilesional default and other contralesional networks (except limbic and subcortical), ipsilesional control and other contralesional networks (except limbic, visual, and TempPar), as well as contralesional SalVenAttn and bilateral SomMot and subcortical networks. (B) The average FC of edges showing significant interaction effect for four representative networks impaired based on NBS statistics in panel (A) were shown. Data are presented as mean ± standard error. C contralesional, DorsAttn dorsal attention, FC functional connectivity, I ipsilesional, NBS network-based statistics, SalVenAttn salience/ventral attention, SomMot somatomotor, TempPar temporoparietal.

To compare group differences in FC strength at task-free and task-general conditions, we performed two-sample t tests using NBS (controlled for mean absolute motion displacement). In the task-free condition, compared to age-matched healthy controls, we observed that stroke patients had lower intra-network FC (NBS corrected p = 0.037, Cohen’s d = 0.016) in bilateral control, salience/ventral attention, and subcortical networks as well as lower inter-network FC between ipsilesional control and bilateral subcortical networks (Fig. 1A and Supplementary Figure 2).

In the task-general condition, we observed that stroke patients had more extensive network disruptions in the task condition compared to the rest condition, namely lower intra-network FC in bilateral default, control, salience/ventral attention, somatomotor and subcortical networks. Lower inter-network FC was also found between ipsilesional default and other contralesional networks (except limbic and subcortical), between ipsilesional control and contralesional salience/ventral attention, dorsal attention, somatomotor, and subcortical networks, as well as contralesional salience/ventral attention and bilateral somatomotor and subcortical networks (NBS corrected p = 0.007, Cohen’s d = 0.017, Fig. 1A and Supplementary Figure 2).

After performing control analyses (see “Methods”), similar findings were obtained for both scan length (time) matching (i.e., control analysis I) and number of volumes matching analyses (i.e., control analysis II; Supplementary Figure 3) as well as based on the resting and task fMRI data without global signal regression (control analysis III; Supplementary Figure 4). Controlled for handedness and the affected hand (i.e., control analysis IV), we found similar group and task interaction effect at a slightly lower threshold (height uncorrected p < 0.01). FC disruption patterns at rest and task (Supplementary Figure 5A) as well as anti-correlated FC patterns in task (Supplementary Figure 5C, left panel) in stroke also remained.

More disrupted functional connectivity with higher task demands in stroke patients

For stroke patients, using the affected hand is naturally more difficult than using the unaffected hand. Given the nature of passive and active tasks, it is also reasonable to assume that active condition is more difficult than passive condition. Consequently, the task using the affected hand actively (i.e., AA) was considered the most difficult one, followed by the task using the unaffected actively (i.e., AU) and the task using the affected hand passively (i.e., PA), and the easiest task was the task using the unaffected hand passively (i.e., PU). Using PU as a reference, we performed three separate two-way repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) to examine the effect of group and task (AA/AU/PA versus PU) as well as their interactions using NBS with p < 0.05 FDR corrected (see “Methods”). We noted significant group and task interaction in comparisons between AA/AU/PA and PU (all FDR corrected p < 0.05, AA vs PU partial η2 = 0.093, AU vs PU partial η2 = 0.188, PA vs PU partial η2 = 0.001, Fig. 2A,C).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Disrupted intra- and inter-network functional connectivity at different task demands in stroke patients. (A) Significant group and task interaction effects were found between AA/AU/PA and PU. (B) Compared to age-matched healthy controls, stroke patients showed more disrupted intra- and inter-network FC during AA, AU, and PA tasks. During AA, stroke patients demonstrated lower FC in ipsilesional default as well as bilateral control, SalVenAttn, DorsAttn, SomMot, and subcortical networks. Remarkably, stroke patients presented additionally lower DorsAttn intra-network FC in AA compared to other tasks. Along with the decremental task demands, stroke patients showed less aberrant intra- and inter-network FC disruption. (C) The average FC of edges showing significant interaction effects for four representative networks impaired in stroke patients based on NBS statistics in panel (A) were shown. (D) Especially for the AA task, stroke patients showed more anti-correlation than healthy controls in default, control, and DorsAttn networks. Data are presented as mean ± standard error, AA active-affected, AU active-unaffected, C contralesional, DorsAttn dorsal attention, FC functional connectivity, I ipsilesional, NBS network-based statistics, PA passive-affected, PU passive-unaffected, SalVenAttn salience/ventral attention, SomMot somatomotor, TempPar temporoparietal.

In terms of group comparison examined by two-sample t-tests using NBS (controlled for mean absolute motion displacement), we found significant differences between patients and controls in all but the PU task (AA: p = 0.028, Cohen’s d = 0.009; AU: p = 0.024, Cohen’s d = 0.009; PA: p = 0.020, Cohen’s d = 0.021; Fig. 2B and Supplementary Figure 6). Stroke patients showed more network disruptions when performing a task with higher task demands compared to healthy controls.

Specifically, during the AA task, stroke patients presented lower FC than healthy controls (p = 0.035, Cohen’s d = 0.009) in ipsilesional default network as well as bilateral control, salience/ventral attention, dorsal attention, somatomotor, and subcortical networks. Compared to AU, PA, and PU, stroke patients showed additionally lower intra-network FC in the dorsal attention network compared to controls.

During the affected-hand tasks (i.e., AA and PA), stroke patients had lower intra-somatomotor network FC and lower inter-network FC between the default and somatomotor relative to healthy controls. This pattern was missing during the unaffected-hand tasks (i.e., AU and PU). Notably, during the AA task, stroke patients demonstrated more anti-correlation in default, control, and dorsal attention networks compared to healthy controls (Fig. 2D and Supplementary Figure 6). This finding implied that the task demand greatly influenced the brain network organization in stroke patients.

Again, the findings of control analysis III demonstrated task-specific network disruption patterns (Supplementary Figure 7) which is similar to our main findings. The findings of control analysis IV showed a significant group and task interaction effect in comparisons between AA/AU/PA and PU (height threshold uncorrected p < 0.01), which is similar to what we found in the main analysis (Supplementary Figure 5B, top panel). In terms of group comparisons, significant group differences in AA and AU tasks were also found (see Supplementary Figure 5B, lower panel). Anti-correlated FC patterns in AA were observed as well (see Supplementary Figure 5C, right panel).

Inefficient task-related network-specific reconfiguration in stroke patients

Two-sample t tests were used to compare the group difference in task-related reconfigurations for every task (see “Methods”). We did not find significant group difference in task-related whole-brain network reconfiguration, although stroke patients showed numerically higher reconfiguration (i.e., lower correlation coefficient) between task-free and task-based condition than healthy controls. In contrast, stroke patients demonstrated more network-specific reconfiguration at different task demands in comparison with healthy controls (Fig. 3). Specifically, stroke patients showed higher reconfiguration in default and somatomotor inter-network during AU and PU. For AA, PA, and PU, stroke patients had higher reconfiguration in salience/ventral attention and subcortical inter-network as well as subcortical intra-network. This might suggest that subcortical stroke has an impact on not only subcortical network but also other networks such as default, somatomotor, and salience/ventral attention. Comprehensive results of reconfiguration are tabulated in Supplementary Table 4.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Altered network-specific reconfigurations at different task demands in stroke patients. At the network-level, stroke patients presented network-specific brain network reconfiguration compared to healthy controls. Stroke patients had higher task-related reconfiguration (i.e., lower correlation coefficient) between SomMot and subcortical networks across all tasks. Stroke patients also showed higher reconfiguration between default and SomMot networks during AU and PU tasks. Except the AU task, stroke patients presented higher reconfiguration in SalVenAttn and subcortical inter-network as well as subcortical intra-network. Data are presented as mean ± standard error, *p < 0.05. AA active-affected, AU active-unaffected, PA passive-affected, PU passive-unaffected, SalVenAttn salience/ventral attention, SomMot somatomotor.

Control analysis III showed similar findings in somatomotor, salience, and subcortical networks, except that there was no group difference in reconfigurations between the default and somatomotor networks across four tasks (Supplementary Figure 8). Control analysis IV demonstrated task-specific brain network reconfiguration between somatomotor and subcortical networks and within subcortical networks (Supplementary Figure 9).

Baseline network-specific functional connectivity and task-related brain network reconfiguration were associated with motor recovery in stroke patients

To investigate the association between baseline FC and motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke patients, we built one stepwise multiple regression model for each task condition by including FC strengths showing group differences as predictors (Fig. 2B; see “Methods”). Out of all conditions tested, two models examining the association of baseline FC at AA and AU conditions with motor recovery (i.e., normalized FMA change score) were significant. The stepwise multiple regression model of AA explained 41.1% of variance (p = 0.019) with the involvement of ipsilesional dorsal attention intra-network FC (β = 0.433, p = 0.046), and contralesional default–somatomotor inter-network FC (β = 0.426, p = 0.049) (Fig. 4). The stepwise multiple regression model of AU explained 32.1% of variance (p = 0.014) with the involvement of ipsilesional dorsal attention intra-network FC (β = 0.566, p = 0.014). Higher FC at these connections, especially within ipsilesional dorsal attention network, was associated with better motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke patients.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Network-specific functional connectivity of the active-affected task was associated with motor recovery in stroke patients. More FC within ipsilesional DorsAttn network was associated with better motor recovery. More FC between contralesional default and contralesional SomMot was also associated with better motor recovery. C contralesional, DorsAttn dorsal attention, FC functional connectivity, FMA Fugl–Meyer Assessment, I ipsilesional, SomMot somatomotor.

Similarly, we built one stepwise multiple regression model for each task condition by including brain network reconfiguration showing group differences as predictors (Fig. 3) to investigate the association between baseline reconfiguration measures and motor recovery in stroke patients (see “Methods”). Regarding brain network reconfiguration, we found that only the model using brain network reconfiguration at the AA condition at baseline was found significantly predicting motor recovery. It explained 29.3% of variance (p = 0.020) with the involvement of connection reconfiguration between ipsilesional somatomotor network to contralesional subcortical network (β = 0.541, p = 0.020) (Supplementary Figure 10). This suggested that higher brain network spatial similarity (i.e., lower network reconfiguration from rest to AA) at baseline was associated with more motor gains after intervention in chronic subcortical stroke patients.

Discussion

To study how brain functional networks reorganize with motor tasks and how these changes at baseline are associated with motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke, we compared FC strength and network reconfiguration between stroke patients and healthy controls at both task-free and task-based conditions (with different task demands). We found that stroke patients showed more widespread network disruptions during motor task (relative to rest condition) particularly in subcortical, somatomotor, default, and dorsal attention networks compared to healthy controls. We also found more task-specific brain network reconfiguration especially in subcortical and somatomotor networks in stroke patients. Our findings suggested that stroke-induced focal subcortical lesion introduced both local and remote brain functional connectivity changes as well as task-related functional reconfigurations in a network-specific manner, and the brain functional networks involved such as default, attention, somatomotor, and subcortical networks were dependent on the task demands. Importantly, both task-specific FC and brain network reconfiguration during the task using the affected hand actively were associated with motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke patients. Taken together, network-specific brain network reorganizations at rest and task with varying degree of task demands have the potential to predict motor recovery after rehabilitation in chronic subcortical stroke patients.

Brain functional architecture in healthy individuals are organized into highly specialized networks associated with various functions. FC can reveal specialization (i.e., high correlations within networks), segregation (i.e., low correlations in BOLD signals between networks), and integration (i.e., high correlations between networks)20. In terms of group differences, we found lower task-free and task-based FC in stroke patients compared to healthy controls in subcortical, somatomotor, default, control, salience/ventral attention and dorsal attention networks, which was consistent with previous findings7,12,21. Furthermore, stroke patients had more widespread FC disruptions during task compared to rest conditions, especially in higher-order cognitive (the default, dorsal attention, control and salience/ventral attention) and subcortical networks. Such phenomenon is even more pronounced during the high demanding tasks (e.g., active affected hand task) in the higher-order cognitive, subcortical, and somatomotor networks. Overall, our findings suggested stroke patients had a loss of network specialization (e.g., intra-network FC disruption in the default network during task), degraded network segregation (e.g., increased FC between salience/ventral attention during task), and less network integration (e.g., decreased FC between control and dorsal attention during task). Our findings are robust as all validation analyses including unequal scan length between rest and task fMRI data, no global signal regression, and the affected hand showed similar findings.

Focal subcortical lesions might influence local and remotely associated cortical regions through extensive white matter tracts and/or damage of the core integrative and associative input to other cortical regions residing at the lesion sites22. Stroke-induced reduction of metabolic diaschisis and disruption of inter-regional BOLD signal incoherence are also associated with abnormalities in multiple regions and networks22. Indeed, subcortical stroke impairs the integrity of corticospinal system and then influences the recruitment of bilateral secondary motor networks23. Additionally, subcortical regions play key roles in the functioning of the salience network and control network2426. Consequently, subcortical stroke leads to widespread network disruptions observed in task-based fMRI12.

Dorsal attention network is associated with cognitive function regarding attentional selection and detection of behavioral relevant stimuli after stroke27. Altered FC between default, control, and attention networks after subcortical stroke was also reported, suggesting the reorganization of the attention system28. In order to perform the most difficult motor task (i.e., AA) at a satisfactory level, patients may have required more attention and action selection resources. Hence, they had to engage not only the subcortical and somatomotor network but also default, control, salience/ventral attention, dorsal attention networks to better execute the task with a planned motor control strategy. Such task-induced increase in brain FC may not be always readily observed using task-free MRI, supporting the complementary value of studying task-based fMRI FC17.

Interestingly, task-related reconfiguration patterns demonstrated similar network-specific disruptions observed in task-related FC. During motor task execution, the subcortical network transmits integrated sensorimotor information for motor planning to somatomotor network29, and therefore somatomotor–subcortical pathway was the most damaged across tasks. Brain lesions in subcortical regions also impacted brain network reconfiguration within subcortical network and cognitive networks such as salience/ventral attention network, consequently leading to reconfiguration inefficiency across tasks except the AU task. Of the supportive role of the default network in motor function12, the default–somatomotor pathway did not show inefficient brain network reconfiguration in affected-hand tasks in chronic subcortical stroke patients. This may imply the adaptive reorganization of the functional networks after stroke30. In line with previous findings, network-specific task-related reconfiguration patterns may underline the essential roles of subcortical, somatomotor, and cognitive networks in motor control.

Importantly, less FC disruptions and more efficient task-related reconfiguration at the most demanding task (AA) at baseline predicted motor recovery better than resting state and other conditions with lower demands in chronic subcortical stroke patients. This underscores the importance of evaluating neural networks underpinning using the affected hand actively (AA task). Previous studies suggested that reduced FC in the dorsal attention network may imply limited attention load in stroke patients for dual task demands31,32. Moreover, the corticospinal system is activity- and use-dependent33. The AA task required high level of attention to visual stimuli and movement and integration of sensory and motor information, many of which subserved by the dorsal attention network34. It hence suggested that more preserved FC within ipsilesional dorsal attention plays a facilitatory role in motor recovery.

We noted a difference in FC disruptions between affected-hand tasks (AA and PA), implying differential reorganization pattern mediated by task. The contribution of the contralesional hemisphere to affected-hand function may be resulted from several possible mechanisms of compensation of motor deficits35,36. For instance, diminished inhibition from ipsilesional primary motor cortex to contralesional primary motor cortex is a possible mechanism of motor recovery in stroke35,36. More activation of the contralesional cortical network was related to better motor recovery37 with additional neural resources recruited38. As stroke causes widespread brain functional network disruptions with varying degrees, involvement of the contralesional somatomotor network may represent adaptive motor learning strategies39. Moreover, newly formed transcallosal connections projecting from the contralesional hemisphere to the ipsilesional hemisphere may enable the contralesional hemisphere to control movements of the affected side40. Indeed, our results of diffusion tensor imaging in these patients showed increased fractional anisotropy in the corpus callosum41. Involvement of contralesional default network also support residual motor function with extra information integration within its main hubs12. Thus, contralesional hemisphere could be potentially involved as a compensatory mechanism for motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke patients.

In addition, we observed that brain network reconfiguration between ipsilesional somatomotor and contralesional subcortical networks of the AA task predicted motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke patients. Previous studies suggested that residual corticospinal and cortico-subcortical connections may facilitate motor recovery of the affected hand42. More efficient brain network reconfiguration between ipsilesional cortical somatomotor and contralesional subcortical regions via cortico-subcortical connections might hence promote motor recovery. This again supported that contralesional hemisphere may assist ipsilesional hemisphere in integration of somatosensory input and motor control in chronic subcortical stroke patients, indicating that contralesional hemisphere plays an essential role in motor recovery.

A few limitations should be noted. First, our sample size is small and caution needs to be taken when interpreting the current results, which is a pilot study of stroke rehabilitation involving multimodal complex design of neuroimaging, tDCS and MI-BCI. Based on our findings here, we provided effect size and power estimates to associate task-specific FC with motor recovery, which will help design of future task-free and task-fMRI studies with larger sample in stroke rehabilitation (see Supplementary Results). Second, the relative short duration and unequal length between rest and task fMRI data may potentially confound the FC reorganization findings, but a 5 min scan can generate moderate reliability43. Additional validation analyses to match the scan time and number of volumes between rest and task showed highly similar findings. Given that individual differences are preserved in FC on top of task modulations44, our findings on the rest and task FC changes in stroke patients were less likely to be influenced by short or unequal scan length in terms of time or number of volumes. Third, due to the limited sample size, we were not able to control for stroke severity or lesion size and location. However, we have provided spatial maps of lesion distributions in a network-specific manner (Supplementary Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 3) for interpretations. Future research should incorporate these potential important factors to comprehensively understand brain network reorganization after stroke and their relationship with motor recovery. For example, investigations on the degree of overlap between the lesion mask and the connectivity findings and derivation of seed-based functional connectivity using the cavity as a seed to predict the patterns of brain network reorganization would be worth exploring. Finally, the generalizability of our findings may be limited as only chronic subcortical stroke patients were examined in this study. Future work is needed to further examine and corroborate our findings in stroke patients with different characteristics.

Conclusions

In conclusion, compared to age-matched healthy controls, chronic subcortical stroke patients had more widespread brain functional network disruptions at task condition relative to rest condition, especially in cognitive, somatomotor, and subcortical networks. Chronic subcortical stroke patients also had less efficient task-related brain network reconfiguration than healthy controls at different task demands. Importantly, baseline network-specific FC and reconfiguration could predict motor recovery. Our findings highlight the necessity of simultaneously examining large-scale brain functional networks at task-free and task-based conditions in stroke patients. Future studies are needed to examine the intervention effect on brain structural and functional network reorganization in association with motor recovery in stroke patients.

Methods

Participants

Nineteen chronic subcortical stroke patients and eleven age-matched healthy subjects were recruited in this study. The inclusion criteria for stroke patients were (1) first ever subcortical stroke > 9 months before the study enrolment; and (2) unilateral moderate to severe upper extremity motor impairment measured by the upper extremity component of the Fugl–Meyer Assessment (FMA) (score range 11–45). The exclusion criteria for stroke patients were (1) epilepsy, neglect, cognitive impairment, and other neurological or psychiatric diseases; (2) severe arm pain; (3) severe spasticity measured by the Modified Ashworth Scale in the shoulder or elbow (score > 2); (4) contraindications to tDCS; (5) weak grip strength measured by a dynamometer (weight < 10 kg); and (6) participation in other interventions or trials that target post-stroke motor recovery. All participants performed one baseline MRI scan. All stroke patients underwent an MI-BCI training with or without tDCS for 2 weeks. The combination of MI-BCI and tDCS may promote brain reorganization and functional recovery in stroke patients. Within each session, 20 min of real or sham tDCS were administered followed by 40 min of MI-BCI training. The tDCS was applied at 1 mA with the anode and cathode placed over the ipsilesional and contralesional primary cortex, respectively. A motor-imagined reaching task was used to trigger the InMotion MIT-Manus robot (Interactive Motion Technologies, MA, USA) for upper limb rehabilitation. Stroke patients had their motor function of the affected arm evaluated by the FMA before and two weeks after the intervention (for details see Supplementary Methods41,45,46). Here, we refer to the before intervention timepoint as the baseline timepoint. The FMA change scores between baseline and two weeks after intervention did not have significant group difference (mean 5.00 (SD 4.40) in MI-BCI + real tDCS group (n = 10) and mean 5.75 (SD 5.99) in MI-BCI + sham tDCS group (n = 9), p = 0.763). One patient was excluded because of excessive motion during the scan. The demographic and clinical information is tabulated in Table 1. More detailed demographic and clinical information in stroke patients is tabulated in Supplementary Table 1.

Table 1.

Subject demographic and behavioral characteristics.

Healthy controls (n = 11) Stroke patients (n = 18) p-value
(2-tailed)
Age (years), mean (SD) 56.73 (4.47) 52.89 (10.32) 0.180
Sex (male/female) 6/5 14/4 0.425
Handedness (left/right) 0/11 2/16 0.520
Time post-stroke (months), mean (SD) 33.61 (19.80)
Affected hemisphere (left/right) 8/10
Baseline Fugl–Meyer Assessment score, mean (SD) 34.33 (7.50)
Fugl–Meyer Assessment change score, mean (SD) 5.33 (5.02)
Baseline grip strength (kg), mean (SD) 6.12 (3.94)
Baseline box and block test, mean (SD) 3.67 (4.50)

Independent t test was performed to compare the group difference in age. Fisher’s exact test was performed to compare the group difference in sex and handedness separately. There was no significant group difference in age, sex, and handedness (p > 0.05). Fugl–Meyer Assessment change score refers to the change after motor imagery-assisted brain computer interface-based training with and without transcranial direct current stimulation intervention (i.e., post-intervention minus pre-intervention).

The stroke lesion was manually drawn by a research fellow (X.H.) based on T2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery images and non-linearly registered to Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) 152 space. The stroke lesion overlapping map is illustrated in Supplementary Figure 1. This study was conducted in accordance with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association and approved by the Domain-Specific Review Board of the National Healthcare Group (NHG) in Singapore. NHG oversees National University Health System including National University Hospital and Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore. This study was also registered on ClinicalTrials.gov (Clinical Trial Registration Unique Identifier: NCT01897025 (11/07/2013); https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT01897025). Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Task-free and motor task-based fMRI

The MRI session included one run of task-free resting state fMRI (7 min) followed by four runs of a block-design hand-gripping task (4.45 min per run). In the task-free run, participants were instructed to close their eyes but remain awake without thinking of anything in particular. In each motor task run, participants were cued to grip a pressure sensor using either their right or left hand actively or passively. The order of the four runs was counter-balanced across participants. Each block began with a cue signaling rest for 33 s, followed by a cue signaling hand gripping/rest for 21 s repeated nine times, and finally ended by another cued rest for 45 s. In each motor task run, participants were cued to grip a pressure sensor at a target pressure of 30% of their maximum voluntary contraction using either their right or left hand actively or passively. The gripping pressure was fixed for four motor tasks and was recorded to evaluate the task performance. The order of the four runs was counter-balanced across participants. A research assistant assisted participants in the passive hand gripping task according to the visual cue whereas participants performed active hand gripping task without assistance. Two tasks required participants to perform hand gripping actively without external assistance were categorized into active task, and two tasks assisted by a research assistant were categorized into passive task. By visual inspecting of the task performance, the peak of each movement onset with attenuation of hand gripping pressure could be clearly identified, indicating that all stroke patients and healthy controls were able to perform the active tasks using either affected or unaffected hand. We additionally investigated whether patients and controls showed any differences in terms of achieving the target pressure per visual cue in the active motor tasks by dividing the number of trials meeting the target pressure by the total number of trials per participant. After performing two-sample t-tests, we did not find any group differences between healthy controls and stroke patients in both active tasks using affected or unaffected hand. For patients with stroke lesions in the right hemisphere, we flipped their MR images (lesion mask, task-free and task-based fMRI) to ensure that lesions were on the left side of the hemisphere. Thereafter, the left hemisphere was regarded as the ipsilesional hemisphere and right hemisphere was regarded as the contralesional hemisphere in stroke patients. For healthy controls, their MR images were not flipped. Accordingly, the right hand was treated as the affected hand and the left hand as the unaffected hand. This resulted in four motor task conditions ordered in decreasing demands: active-affected (AA), active-unaffected (AU), passive-affected (PA), and passive-unaffected (PU).

Image acquisition

Participants were scanned in a 3 T Siemens Magnetom Trim Trio scanner with a 32-channel head array coil at the Clinical Imaging Research Center, Singapore41. The scan protocol included a T1-weighted magnetization prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence (inversion time = 900 ms, repetition time = 1900 ms, echo time = 2.5 ms, flip angle = 9°, field of view = 256 × 256 mm2, 176 sagittal slices, matrix size = 256 × 256, and voxel size = 1 mm3 isotropic), a task-free T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (repetition time = 1750 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 74°, field of view = 220 × 220 mm2, voxel size = 3.4 mm3 isotropic, slice thickness = 3.4 mm, 33 axial slices), and four task-based T2*-weighted echo planar imaging (repetition time = 3000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view = 220 × 220 mm2, voxel size = 3.4 mm3 isotropic, slice thickness = 3.4 mm, 42 axial slices).

Image preprocessing

Both task-free and task-based fMRI images were preprocessed using the Analysis of Functional NeuroImages software (AFNI)47 and FMRIB (Oxford Centre for Functional MRI of the Brain) Software Library (FSL)48, The preprocessing steps for task-free data followed our previous protocol4951, consisting of: (i) removal of first six volumes for magnetic field stabilization; (ii) slice-time correction; (iii) motion correction, (iv) despiking; (v) spatial smoothing (6-mm FWHM Gaussian kernel); (vi) grand mean scaling; (vii) band-pass temporal filtering (0.009–0.1 Hz); (viii) detrending (removal of first and second order); (ix) use of Boundary-Based Registration (BBR) for co-registration of T1 image52 and nonlinear registration tool (FNIRT) for subsequent registration to the MNI 152 space, and (x) regression of nuisance signals (white matter, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF), whole-brain global signals, and six motion parameters).

For task-based data, we performed preprocessing steps (i) (with first five volumes removed), (ii), (iii), (v), and (ix) above. We did not perform despiking, grant mean scaling and band-pass filtering and detrending to avoid removing task-relevant signals following previous literature15,4951,53. Furthermore, we used the Statistical Parametric Mapping toolbox (SPM12) (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) to regress out the global signal, white matter, CSF, and motion parameters as well as the task-related mean activation signals15,53 by modeling the onset of each task block as a boxcar regressor convolved with a generic hemodynamic response function in the same general linear model in (x). Subsequently, the residual task-free and task-based signals were used for FC analysis. Voxels within stroke lesion masks were not excluded during preprocessing but the visual inspection results did not reveal influences of stroke lesions.

To ensure adequate confounds control, we visually inspected the quality of co-registration and normalization. Motion scrubbing was performed on task-free fMRI by censoring frames with framewise displacement (FD) > 0.5 mm and DVARS > 0.00554 and the number of remaining volumes did not differ between patients and controls (patients: mean 225.22 (SD 22.25), controls: mean 231.00 (SD 4.47), p = 0.405). No significant group and task interaction effect on motion parameters was found. The motion parameter characteristics are tabulated in Supplementary Table 2. We further controlled for mean absolute motion displacement in the FC analyses.

Task-free and task-based functional connectivity derivation and analysis

For both task-free and task-based fMRI data, we parceled the brain into 144 regions of interest (ROIs)55 and extracted the BOLD time series of each ROI (averaged across all voxels in the ROI) for every participant. ROIs falling within a lesion or intersecting with the lesion were not excluded (see Supplementary Table 3 for the average frequency of lesion occurrence in the ipsilesional hemisphere). We then computed the Pearson’s correlation between the mean time series of every pair of ROIs. These correlation coefficients were Fisher’s r-to-z transformed to generate a 144 × 144 FC matrix for every participant for every condition (resting-state and four task conditions). FC of default, control, limbic, salience/ventral attention, dorsal attention, somatomotor, visual, temporoparietal, and subcortical networks were consequently derived by averaging the corresponding cells in the FC matrix. We also created a task-general FC matrix by concatenating the fMRI time-series data from all four task conditions. Therefore, every participant had six FC matrices representing task-free, task-general, AA, AU, PA, and PU conditions.

To investigate the group (patients vs controls) and task (rest vs task-general) interaction, we performed two-way repeated measure analysis of covariance with mean absolute motion displacement as a nuisance covariate using NBS toolbox with p < 0.05 FDR corrected. We chose PU motor task, the easiest condition among the four, as our reference condition and performed three separate two-way repeated measures ANCOVA to examine the effect of group and task (AA/AU/PA versus PU) as well as their interactions using NBS with p < 0.05 FDR corrected.

To compare group differences in FC strength at task-free and task-based conditions, we performed two-sample t tests on task-free and each of five task-based FC matrices between healthy controls and stroke patients using NBS, a nonparametric massive univariate method, controlling for mean absolute motion displacement, with a height threshold of p < 0.001 and a cluster-extent threshold of p < 0.05 (familywise-error corrected)56.

Four control analyses were performed. As the scan length differed between task-free and motor task-based fMRI, in control analysis I, we repeated the analysis by trimming each participant’s motor task fMRI images to maintain equal scan lengths as resting-state fMRI. In control analysis II, we matched the volumes of scans between task and resting-state fMRI given different TRs and repeated the analysis. As existing literature suggested global signal may contain information related to cognition and behavior57, we repeated our analysis based on the resting and task fMRI data without global signal regression in control analysis III. Lastly, as there were two left-handed stroke patients, we repeated the FC analyses controlling for handedness and the affected hand in control analysis IV.

Task-related brain network reconfiguration analysis

Task-related brain network reconfiguration (i.e., spatial similarity between task-free and task-based FC matrices) was computed following a prior work16. Higher spatial similarity represents less task-related brain network reconfigurations compared to resting-state. Two-sample t-tests were used to compare the group difference in four network-specific task-related reconfigurations (i.e., AA, AU, PA, and PU) at the significance level of p < 0.05 (two-tailed) for every task.

Association between baseline task-specific FC/reconfiguration and motor recovery

To investigate the association between baseline task-specific brain measures (i.e., FC and brain network reconfiguration) and motor recovery in chronic subcortical stroke patients, we built one stepwise multiple regression model for each task condition by including FC strengths or reconfiguration measures showing group differences as predictors (Figs. 2B and 3). Each intra- and inter-network FC measure (i.e., strength and reconfiguration) was further categorized into ipsilesional, contralesional, or inter-hemispheric. In terms of motor recovery, we calculated a normalized FMA change score representing motor gains after intervention: FMAchange = (FMApost-intervention – FMApre-intervention)/FMApre-intervention. We performed stepwise multiple regression with normalized FMA change score as the dependent variable and FC or reconfiguration measures derived from networks showing group differences of each task (i.e., AA, AU, PA, and PU) as independent variables of interest. Age, sex, handedness, and time post-stroke were included as nuisance covariates. Therefore, there were a total of eight models for FC and reconfiguration of four tasks.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all participants for their participation in this study. This research was supported by the National Medical Research Council NMRC0088/2015 and the Duke-NUS Medical School Signature Research Program funded by Ministry of Health, Singapore (J.H.Z.) and National Medical Research Council NMRC/NIG/1013/2010 (E.C.). This work was also partially supported by the RIE2020 AME Programmatic Fund, Singapore (No. A20G8b0102).

Author contributions

H.-J.C. analyzed data, interpreted results, and prepared the manuscript. K.K.N., X.Q., and F.J. contributed to data analysis, result interpretation, and manuscript preparation. Z.K.L., W.P.T., X.H., F.A.N., K.K.A., K.-H.C., C.G., and E.C. collected and provided data and contributed to manuscript preparation. H.Y. contributed to manuscript preparation. J.H.Z. conceptualized the study and contributed to result interpretation and manuscript preparation. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Effie Chew, Email: effie_chew@nuhs.edu.sg.

Juan Helen Zhou, Email: helen.zhou@nus.edu.sg.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-87789-5.

References

  • 1.Rodgers H. In: Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Barnes MP, Good DC, editors. Elsevier; 2013. pp. 427–433. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Lake EM, Bazzigaluppi P, Stefanovic B. Functional magnetic resonance imaging in chronic ischaemic stroke. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2016 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Biswal B, Zerrin Yetkin F, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 1995;34:537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Siegel JS, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Measuring functional connectivity in stroke: Approaches and considerations. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2017;37:2665–2678. doi: 10.1177/0271678X17709198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lv H, et al. Resting-state functional MRI: Everything that nonexperts have always wanted to know. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2018;39:1390. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A5527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Urbin MA, Hong X, Lang CE, Carter AR. Resting-state functional connectivity and its association with multiple domains of upper-extremity function in chronic stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2014;28:761–769. doi: 10.1177/1545968314522349. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Siegel JS, et al. Disruptions of network connectivity predict impairment in multiple behavioral domains after stroke. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2016;113:E4367–E4376. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1521083113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lee J, et al. Alteration and role of interhemispheric and intrahemispheric connectivity in motor network after stroke. Brain Topogr. 2018;31:708–719. doi: 10.1007/s10548-018-0644-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Richards LG, Stewart KC, Woodbury ML, Senesac C, Cauraugh JH. Movement-dependent stroke recovery: A systematic review and meta-analysis of TMS and fMR1 evidence. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Favre I, et al. Upper limb recovery after stroke Is associated with ipsilesional primary motor cortical activity a meta-analysis. Stroke. 2014;45:1077–1083. doi: 10.1161/strokeaha.113.003168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Tang Q, et al. Modulation of interhemispheric activation balance in motor-related areas of stroke patients with motor recovery: Systematic review and meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2015;57:392–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Lariviere S, Ward NS, Boudrias MH. Disrupted functional network integrity and flexibility after stroke: Relation to motor impairments. NeuroImage Clin. 2018;19:883–891. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Di X, Gohel S, Kim EH, Biswal BB. Task vs rest-different network configurations between the coactivation and the resting-state brain networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013;7:493. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Smith SM, et al. Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2009;106:13040–13045. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Cole MW, Bassett DS, Power JD, Braver TS, Petersen SE. Intrinsic and task-evoked network architectures of the human brain. Neuron. 2014;83:238–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Schultz DH, Cole MW. Higher intelligence is associated with less task-related brain network reconfiguration. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:8551–8561. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0358-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gratton C, et al. Functional brain networks are dominated by stable group and individual factors, not cognitive or daily variation. Neuron. 2018;98:439–452.e435. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Zuo N, Yang Z, Liu Y, Li J, Jiang T. Both activated and less-activated regions identified by functional MRI reconfigure to support task executions. Brain Behav. 2018;8:e00893. doi: 10.1002/brb3.893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Zuo N, Yang Z, Liu Y, Li J, Jiang T. Core networks and their reconfiguration patterns across cognitive loads. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2018;39:3546–3557. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Petersen SE, Sporns O. Brain networks and cognitive architectures. Neuron. 2015;88:207–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Baldassarre A, Ramsey LE, Siegel JS, Shulman GL, Corbetta M. Brain connectivity and neurological disorders after stroke. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2016;29:706–713. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000396. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Corbetta M, et al. Common behavioral clusters and subcortical anatomy in stroke. Neuron. 2015;85:927–941. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Thickbroom GW, Byrnes ML, Archer SA, Mastaglia FL. Motor outcome after subcortical stroke correlates with the degree of cortical reorganization. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115:2144–2150. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Menon V. In: Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference. Toga AW, editor. Academic Press; 2015. pp. 597–611. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 2010;214:655–667. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Marek S, Dosenbach NUF. The frontoparietal network: Function, electrophysiology, and importance of individual precision mapping. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2018;20:133–140. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2018.20.2/smarek. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Vossel S, Geng JJ, Fink GR. Dorsal and ventral attention systems: Distinct neural circuits but collaborative roles. Neuroscientist. 2014;20:150–159. doi: 10.1177/1073858413494269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wang C, et al. Altered functional organization within and between resting-state networks in chronic subcortical infarction. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2014;34:597–605. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Doyon J, Benali H. Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005;15:161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wang L, et al. Dynamic functional reorganization of the motor execution network after stroke. Brain. 2010;133:1224–1238. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Marshall SC, Grinnell D, Heisel B, Newall A, Hunt L. Attentional deficits in stroke patients: A visual dual task experiment. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1997;78:7–12. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(97)90002-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Yang YR, Wang RY, Chen YC, Kao MJ. Dual-task exercise improves walking ability in chronic stroke: A randomized controlled trial. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2007;88:1236–1240. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.06.762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Martin JH. The corticospinal system: From development to motor control. Neuroscientist. 2005;11:161–173. doi: 10.1177/1073858404270843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ptak R, Schnider A. The dorsal attention network mediates orienting toward behaviorally relevant stimuli in spatial neglect. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:12557–12565. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2722-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Dodd KC, Nair VA, Prabhakaran V. Role of the contralesional vs. ipsilesional hemisphere in stroke recovery. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2017;11:9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Jones TA. Motor compensation and its effects on neural reorganization after stroke. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017;18:267–280. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Schaechter JD, Perdue KL. Enhanced cortical activation in the contralesional hemisphere of chronic stroke patients in response to motor skill challenge. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:638–647. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Riecker A, et al. The role of the unaffected hemisphere in motor recovery after stroke. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2010;31:1017–1029. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.van Dokkum LEH, et al. Modified brain activations of the nondamaged hemisphere during ipsilesional upper-limb movement in persons with initial severe motor deficits poststroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2018;32:34–45. doi: 10.1177/1545968317746783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Jankowska E, Edgley SA. How can corticospinal tract neurons contribute to ipsilateral movements? A question with implications for recovery of motor functions. Neuroscientist. 2006;12:67–79. doi: 10.1177/1073858405283392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Hong X, et al. Brain plasticity following MI-BCI training combined with tDCS in a randomized trial in chronic subcortical stroke subjects: A preliminary study. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:9222. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08928-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ejaz N, et al. Evidence for a subcortical origin of mirror movements after stroke: A longitudinal study. Brain. 2018;141:837–847. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx384. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Van Dijk KRA, et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: Theory, properties, and optimization. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;103:297–321. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Shah LM, Cramer JA, Ferguson MA, Birn RM, Anderson JS. Reliability and reproducibility of individual differences in functional connectivity acquired during task and resting state. Brain Behav. 2016;6:e00456. doi: 10.1002/brb3.456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ang KK, et al. Facilitating effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor imagery brain-computer interface with robotic feedback for stroke rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2015;96:S79–S87. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2014.08.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Chew E, et al. Using transcranial direct current stimulation to augment the effect of motor imagery-assisted brain–computer interface training in chronic stroke patients-cortical reorganization considerations. Front. Neurol. 2020;11:948. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cox RW. AFNI: Software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 1996;29:162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Chong JSX, et al. Influence of cerebrovascular disease on brain networks in prodromal and clinical Alzheimer's disease. Brain. 2017;140:3012–3022. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ng KK, Lo JC, Lim JKW, Chee MWL, Zhou J. Reduced functional segregation between the default mode network and the executive control network in healthy older adults: A longitudinal study. Neuroimage. 2016;133:321–330. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.03.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wang C, et al. Disrupted salience network functional connectivity and white-matter microstructure in persons at risk for psychosis: Findings from the LYRIKS study. Psychol. Med. 2016;46:2771–2783. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Greve DN, Fischl B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage. 2009;48:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Lou W, et al. Decreased activity with increased background network efficiency in amnestic MCI during a visuospatial working memory task. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;36:3387–3403. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Power JD, et al. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. 2014;84:320–341. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Zalesky A, Fornito A, Bullmore ET. Network-based statistic: Identifying differences in brain networks. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1197–1207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Li J, et al. Topography and behavioral relevance of the global signal in the human brain. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:14286. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-50750-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES