Abstract
Aims
Mental disorders are associated with lower subjective social status (SSS), but a more nuanced understanding of this relationship is needed. We examined the influence of disorder age of onset and recency on SSS and studied whether mental disorders are also associated with the discrepancy between actual and desired SSS.
Method
Data are from the baseline and second wave of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2 (NEMESIS-2). Mental disorders were assessed with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI 3.0), while both actual and desired SSS were assessed with a ten-rung ladder. Linear regression was used to examine the association between mental disorders and SSS.
Results
Of 5303 participants, 2237 had a lifetime mental disorder at baseline. These participants reported significantly lower actual SSS (6.28) at follow-up than healthy participants (6.66, B = −0.38 [95% CI −0.48 to −0.27], p < 0.001) and a significantly greater actual-desired SSS discrepancy (1.14 v. 1.05 after controlling for actual SSS, B = 0.09 [0.01–0.17], p = 0.024). Lower age of onset of the first mental disorder was marginally significantly associated with lower actual SSS (B = 0.006 [0.000–0.012], p = 0.046). More recent disorders were also associated with lower actual SSS (B = 0.015 [0.005–0.026], p = 0.005), such that participants whose disorder remitted ⩾6 years before baseline were statistically indistinguishable from healthy participants.
Conclusions
Lifetime mental disorders are associated with lower actual SSS and a slightly greater discrepancy between actual and desired SSS. However, people with mental disorders in (long-term) remission have a similar social status as healthy participants.
Key words: Mental disorders, remission, social status, subjective social status
Introduction
Mental disorders are associated with lower socioeconomic status (SES) (Lorant et al., 2003; Hudson, 2005). They are, for instance, associated with premature termination of education (Breslau et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2009) and reduced earnings (Kessler et al., 2008; Levinson et al., 2010). Causality appears to run in both directions: low SES increases the risk of mental disorders, while the presence of mental disorders also increases the risk of low SES (Johnson et al., 1999; Elovainio et al., 2012; Pino et al., 2018). While SES has traditionally been indicated by objective measures such as education, occupational status and income, more recently interest has shifted to examining subjective social status (SSS), a person's subjective judgement of their social position (Adler and Epel, 2000). It is thought that SSS may represent a kind of ‘cognitive averaging’ of various SES indicators (Singh-Manoux et al., 2003) and hence might be a more comprehensive measure than traditional SES indicators. SSS has generally been found to be associated with (mental) health outcomes even after controlling for objective SES (Adler and Epel, 2000; Singh-Manoux et al., 2003, 2005; Operario et al., 2004; Hu et al., 2005; Franzini and Fernandez-Esquer, 2006; Adler et al., 2008; Collins and Goldman, 2008; Demakakos et al., 2008; Hamad et al., 2008; Leu et al., 2008; Wong et al., 2008; Sakurai et al., 2010; Wolff et al., 2010; Karvonen and Rahkonen, 2011; McLaughlin et al., 2012; Miyakawa et al., 2012; Subramanyam et al., 2012; Euteneuer, 2014; Honjo et al., 2014; Quon and McGrath, 2014; Scott et al., 2014; Präg et al., 2016; Hoebel et al., 2017; Chen et al., 2019), which suggests that SSS is indeed a more comprehensive measure of SES or that a person's subjective sense of social status matters over and above objective SES.
However, research to date is limited in a number of respects. First, previous studies have relied upon symptom questionnaires rather than examining diagnosable mental disorders, with only a few exceptions (McLaughlin et al., 2012; Honjo et al., 2014; Scott et al., 2014; Chen et al., 2019). While symptom questionnaires are useful as screening tools, they are ‘context free’ and hence cannot distinguish between mental disorders and normal distress, and tend to result in large numbers of false positives (Henkel et al., 2003; Vilagut et al., 2016). Second, it is important to better understand other variables that affect this relationship to provide starting points for ameliorating the SSS of people with mental disorders, for instance by focusing on particular high-risk groups. In this study, we focus on disorder age of onset and remission. Given the effects of early-onset mental disorders on educational attainment (Breslau et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2009), early-onset disorders might have particularly large associations with SSS as well. It also seems plausible that remission of mental disorders is associated with improvement in SSS, but to date, it is unknown whether participants in long-term remission from mental disorders still have a lower SSS than participants who never suffered from a mental disorder. Third, to our knowledge, no study has examined the association of mental disorders with the discrepancy between actual and desired SSS. Previous research has, however, examined the effect of a counterfactual SSS by asking single mothers and unemployed persons what their social status would have been if they had not become single parents or unemployed (Euteneuer et al., 2019). This study found that the discrepancy between a person's actual and their counterfactual SSS significantly predicted symptoms of stress and depression, even after controlling for actual SSS. This suggests that desired SSS might also be related to mental health, over and above the associations with actual SSS.
In the current study, we aimed to shed more light on the relationship between mental disorders and SSS by examining the role of disorder age of onset and remission, and by also considering the role of the discrepancy between actual and desired SSS.
Methods
Participants
We used data from the first two waves of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2 (NEMESIS-2). NEMESIS-2 is a psychiatric epidemiological cohort study in a representative sample of the adult population of the Netherlands. Participants were selected by means of a multistage, stratified sampling procedure, with one respondent (aged 18–64) being randomly sampled from randomly selected households from randomly selected municipalities. Face-to-face interviews were performed with each respondent. In the first wave (T0), which took place between November 2007 and July 2009, 6.646 individuals participated (65.1% response rate). The sample was nationally representative, with the exception that younger individuals were somewhat under-represented (de Graaf et al., 2010). All T0 respondents were approached for participation in a second wave (T1) 3 years later, from November 2010 to June 2012. A total of 5.303 participants were interviewed again (80.4% response rate among non-deceased participants). T1 non-respondents were younger, lower educated and more frequently unemployed than T1 respondents, but there was no significant association between 12-month mental disorders at T0 and attrition (de Graaf et al., 2013).
All procedures involving human subjects were approved by the Medical Ethics Review Committee for Institutions on Mental Health Care. Written informed consent was obtained from all respondents. Further details about the study design are provided elsewhere (de Graaf et al., 2010).
Measures
Lifetime DSM-IV diagnoses for mental disorders were assessed at T0 by means of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) version 3.0, a fully-structured diagnostic interview administered by trained lay interviewers (Kessler and Üstün, 2004). The disorders assessed included mood and anxiety disorders (major depressive disorder, dysthymia, bipolar disorder, generalised anxiety disorder, panic disorder with or without agoraphobia, agoraphobia without panic disorder, specific phobia and social phobia), impulse control disorders (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD], conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder) and substance use disorders (alcohol or drug abuse and dependence). Due to concerns about recall bias, impulse control disorders were only assessed in respondents aged 45 and below. CIDI diagnoses generally have good validity compared to clinical reappraisal interviews (Haro et al., 2006). The CIDI was also used to assess the age of onset, using a series of recall probes that have been shown to yield more plausible distributions of age of onset than conventional recall questions (Knäuper et al., 1999). In our analyses, we used age of onset as a continuous variable to test its association with SSS and also categorised it into four categories (4–12, 13–19, 20–29, 30–64) to further examine the association between early- or late-onset mental disorders and SSS. Because very few participants had an onset of substance use disorder before the age of 13, we categorised age of onset into three categories (4–19, 20–29, 30–64) for substance use disorders. Recency of each mental disorder was assessed by asking respondents whether they experienced symptoms in the past 12 months and, if not, at what age they last experienced symptoms. Like age of onset, we used recency as a continuous variable to test its association with SSS and also categorised it into four categories (<1 year before T0, 1–5, 6–10, >10 years) to further examine the association between recent or long-remitted mental disorders and SSS.
SSS was assessed at T1 using the MacArthur subjective social status scale, the most widely used scale for SSS (Adler and Epel, 2000). Respondents were presented with a picture of a ten-rung ladder, described as: ‘Think of this ladder as representing where people stand in the Netherlands. At the top of the ladder are the people who are the best off – those who have the most money, the most education and the most respected jobs. At the bottom are the people who are the worst off – who have the least money, least education, and the least respected jobs or no job. The higher up you are on the ladder, the closer you are to the people at the very top; the lower you are, the closer you are to the people at the very bottom.’ They were then asked to place an X on the rung where they thought they stood at this time in their life (actual SSS). In another picture of a ten-rung ladder, they were asked to place an X on the rung where they would like to stand (desired SSS). We calculated a difference by subtracting the actual SSS from the desired SSS (actual–desired SSS discrepancy). We use SSS as an umbrella term for these concepts.
As objective SES indicators, we used education (primary education or lower secondary education, higher secondary education, higher professional education or university), paid employment situation (employed v. not employed), household income category (low, middle or high) and living situation (with partner v. not with partner). All objective SES indicators were assessed at T0.
Missingness was very limited (<1%) for all variables except income (9.65% unweighted missingness). To retain participants with missing data for income in our analyses, we included a ‘missing answer’ category in our categorical income variable.
Analyses
We used linear regression to assess the association of specific lifetime mental disorders at baseline with actual SSS and the discrepancy between actual and desired SSS at follow-up. For subsequent analyses examining the relationship of age of onset and recency to actual SSS and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy, we examined any disorder and the following disorder categories: mood or anxiety disorders, impulse control disorders and substance use disorders. For age of onset, we used dummy variables to compare respondents with a disorder onset in a given age category to respondents without a disorder (group). Hence, this analysis tests whether each age of onset group is significantly associated with SSS compared to participants without a disorder (group). Within the group of participants with a disorder, we also tested the association of age of onset (as a continuous variable) with actual SSS and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy. In contrast to the analysis with dummy variables, this analysis tests whether certain ages of onset are more strongly associated with SSS than other ages of onset, given the presence of a disorder. Because impulse control disorders had an early age of onset (<20 years of age) by definition, we did not include tests for age of onset for this disorder category. We performed analogous analyses to examine mental disorder recency.
All analyses were performed twice: the first model only controlled for age and gender; a second model also controlled for objective SES. In models for the actual–desired SSS discrepancy, we additionally controlled for actual SSS in both models, as actual SSS and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy are related (i.e. lower actual SSS would result in a larger discrepancy, all other things being equal). All analyses were performed in Stata, using survey commands to account for the clustering and weighting due to the complex sampling design.
Results
Baseline demographics
Demographic characteristics of the sample by the presence or absence of lifetime disorders and by age of onset of lifetime mental disorder are presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the groups with regard to gender and educational achievement. However, participants with the first onset of a disorder prior to age 20 were significantly younger at the time of the interview (37.2 years) than healthy participants (42.5 years) and participants with the first onset of a disorder at age 20 or later (46.4 years). Participants with both early- and late-onset disorders were significantly more likely to be unemployed or on disability leave (9.7 and 11.2%, respectively) than healthy participants (3.8%). While participants with late-onset disorders and healthy participants were about as likely to have a low income (20.5 and 21.7%, respectively), participants with an early-onset disorder were significantly more likely to have a low income (35.2%). Participants with an early-onset disorder were also less likely to live with a partner (56.9%) than participants with a late-onset disorder (69.3%) or healthy participants (71.9%).
Table 1.
First onset of a disorder | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
No disorder (N = 3066) | <20 years of age (N = 1290) | ⩾20 years of age (N = 946) | p-value | |
Age, mean | 42.5 | 37.2 | 46.4 | <0.001 |
Sex (% female) | 49.6 | 48.5 | 50.9 | 0.69 |
Education (%) | ||||
Primary/lower secondary | 29.0 | 31.4 | 27.8 | |
Higher secondary | 40.7 | 43.2 | 42.5 | |
Higher professional/university | 30.3 | 25.3 | 29.7 | 0.11 |
Employment (%) | ||||
Employed | 72.7 | 67.9 | 68.9 | |
Homemaker | 11.4 | 9.9 | 11.0 | |
Student | 6.5 | 10.1 | 2.0 | |
Unemployed/disability | 3.8 | 9.7 | 11.2 | |
Retired/other | 5.6 | 2.3 | 6.9 | <0.001 |
Living situation (%) | ||||
With partner | 71.9 | 56.9 | 69.3 | |
Single parent | 4.4 | 5.4 | 7.8 | |
Single (without children) | 12.4 | 19.1 | 18.1 | |
With others | 11.4 | 18.5 | 4.8 | <0.001 |
Income (%) | ||||
Low | 20.5 | 35.2 | 21.7 | |
Middle | 40.9 | 35.9 | 44.5 | |
High | 25.8 | 18.8 | 23.5 | |
No answer | 12.8 | 10.1 | 10.3 | <0.001 |
Lifetime mental disorders and SSS
Most disorders were associated with a statistically significantly lower actual SSS (Table 2, model 1), with the exception (p = 0.052–0.127) of agoraphobia (without panic), panic disorder, ADHD and drug abuse. Among the mood disorders, dysthymia and bipolar disorder were associated with a much lower actual SSS (−0.96 and −0.99, respectively) than major depression (−0.40), while among the substance use disorders, alcohol or drug dependence was associated with a much lower actual SSS (−0.67 and −1.04) than abuse (−0.28 and −0.30). Participants with any lifetime mental disorder had a mean actual SSS of 6.28, which was 0.38 (95% CI 0.27–0.48, p < 0.001) lower than the mean actual SSS of participants without a lifetime mental disorder. Controlling for objective SES attenuated the magnitude of associations (e.g. from −0.38 to −0.26 for any disorder, Table 2, model 2). All disorder groups remained significantly associated with lower actual SSS, although statistical significance was lost for some individual disorders.
Table 2.
Actual SSS | Discrepancy | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | ||||
Disorder | n | Mean | B [95% CI] | B [95% CI] | Mean | B [95% CI] | B [95% CI] |
Agoraphobia without panic | 48 | 5.96 | −0.54 [−1.08 to 0.00] | −0.35 [−0.76 to 0.06] | 1.15 | 0.06 [−0.26 to 0.38] | 0.06 [−0.24 to 0.36] |
Generalised anxiety disorder | 252 | 6.10 | −0.42 [−0.79 to −0.04]* | −0.24 [−0.57 to 0.09] | 1.17 | 0.08 [−0.07 to 0.23] | 0.07 [−0.08 to 0.23] |
Panic disorder | 198 | 6.25 | −0.25 [−0.53 to 0.03] | −0.19 [−0.44 to 0.05] | 1.13 | 0.04 [−0.21 to 0.29] | 0.04 [−0.21 to 0.30] |
Social phobia | 516 | 6.13 | −0.40 [−0.60 to −0.21]*** | −0.28 [−0.46 to −0.09]** | 1.19 | 0.11 [−0.06 to 0.27] | 0.10 [−0.06 to 0.26] |
Specific phobia | 439 | 6.19 | −0.33 [−0.56 to −0.09]** | −0.17 [−0.39 to 0.06] | 1.09 | 0.00 [−0.16 to 0.16] | −0.00 [−0.16 to 0.15] |
Any anxiety disorder | 1080 | 6.21 | −0.36 [−0.50 to −0.22]*** | −0.21 [−0.34 to −0.08]** | 1.13 | 0.05 [−0.07 to 0.17] | 0.04 [−0.07 to 0.15] |
Major depression | 1014 | 6.17 | −0.40 [−0.56 to −0.24]*** | −0.30 [−0.45 to −0.14]*** | 1.19 | 0.13 [0.02 to 0.24]* | 0.12 [0.01 to 0.24]* |
Dysthymia | 80 | 5.55 | −0.96 [−1.51 to −0.41]*** | −0.66 [−1.16 to −0.17]** | 0.95 | −0.14 [−0.70 to 0.43] | −0.21 [−0.78 to 0.35] |
Bipolar disorder | 69 | 5.52 | −0.99 [−1.55 to −0.44]*** | −0.54 [−1.08 to 0.00] | 1.56 | 0.48 [−0.16 to 1.11] | 0.45 [−0.16 to 1.06] |
Any mood disorder | 1091 | 6.14 | −0.45 [−0.61 to −0.30]*** | −0.32 [−0.47 to −0.17]*** | 1.22 | 0.16 [0.05 to 0.27]** | 0.15 [0.04 to 0.26]** |
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder | 71 | 5.94 | −0.56 [−1.29 to 0.16] | −0.12 [−0.82 to 0.58] | 1.10 | 0.01 [−0.43 to 0.45] | −0.03 [−0.45 to 0.40] |
Conduct disorder | 167 | 5.95 | −0.56 [−1.04 to −0.09]* | −0.33 [−0.76 to 0.09] | 1.55 | 0.48 [0.08 to 0.88]* | 0.46 [0.08 to 0.83]* |
Oppositional-defiant disorder | 77 | 5.82 | −0.69 [−1.26 to −0.11]* | −0.57 [−1.10 to −0.03]* | 1.18 | 0.09 [−0.20 to 0.38] | 0.08 [−0.21 to 0.38] |
Any impulse control disorder | 266 | 5.88 | −0.65 [−0.99 to −0.30]*** | −0.41 [−0.72 to −0.10]* | 1.33 | 0.25 [−0.03 to 0.53] | 0.22 [−0.05 to 0.49] |
Alcohol abuse | 765 | 6.25 | −0.28 [−0.45 to −0.11]** | −0.24 [−0.40 to −0.09]** | 1.14 | 0.06 [−0.09 to 0.21] | 0.05 [−0.09 to 0.20] |
Alcohol dependence | 114 | 5.84 | −0.67 [−1.13 to −0.21]** | −0.18 [−0.64 to 0.29] | 1.41 | 0.32 [−0.03 to 0.68] | 0.27 [−0.06 to 0.59] |
Drug abuse | 202 | 6.20 | −0.30 [−0.67 to 0.06] | −0.07 [−0.38 to 0.23] | 1.17 | 0.08 [−0.15 to 0.31] | 0.06 [−0.16 to 0.28] |
Drug dependence | 111 | 5.48 | −1.04 [−1.62 to −0.45]*** | −0.71 [−1.25 to −0.17]* | 1.65 | 0.57 [0.04 to 1.10]* | 0.53 [0.02 to 1.04]* |
Any substance use disorder | 1017 | 6.19 | −0.38 [−0.52 to −0.24]*** | −0.25 [−0.37 to −0.13]*** | 1.17 | 0.10 [−0.04 to 0.25] | 0.09 [−0.05 to 0.22] |
Any disorder | 2302 | 6.28 | −0.38 [−0.48 to −0.27]*** | −0.26 [−0.36 to −0.17]*** | 1.14 | 0.09 [0.01 to 0.17]* | 0.08 [0.00 to 0.16]* |
Notes: Model 1 only controls for age and gender. Model 2 additionally controls for education, income, job status and living situation. For the discrepancy, both models additionally control for actual SSS. The reference group consists of participants without that particular disorder (group).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
There were few associations between specific mental disorders and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy (controlling for actual SSS, Table 2). However, participants with any lifetime mental disorder had a mean actual–desired SSS discrepancy of 1.14, which was 0.09 (95% CI 0.01–0.17) larger than that of participants without a lifetime mental disorder. Major depression and any mood disorder were also associated with small increases in the discrepancy (B = 0.13 and 0.16), while conduct disorder and drug dependence were associated with relatively large increases in the actual–desired SSS discrepancy (B = 0.48 and 0.57). Associations were essentially unchanged after controlling for objective SES.
Age of onset, recency and SSS
Tables 3 and 4 show the association between mental disorders and SSS, separated out by disorder category and by age of onset (Table 3) or recency (Table 4) category. Having a lifetime mental disorder was significantly associated with actual SSS for each age of onset category and each disorder category (regression coefficients ranging from −1.01 to −0.25, p < 0.001–0.015). Associations were somewhat attenuated when controlling for objective SES, but most remained significant (Table 3, model 2). There were few associations between any age of onset and disorder category and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy. Only having any lifetime mental disorder with late onset (between age 30 and 64) was associated with the actual–desired SSS discrepancy (B = 0.13, p = 0.021); this association remained unchanged after controlling for objective SES.
Table 3.
Age at onset | Actual SSS | Discrepancy | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | |
Any disorder | ||||||||
4–12 | −0.52 [−0.69 to −0.35]*** | <0.001 | −0.36 [−0.51 to −0.21]*** | <0.001 | 0.06 [−0.07 to 0.19] | 0.376 | 0.05 [−0.08 to 0.17] | 0.478 |
13–19 | −0.37 [−0.58 to −0.16]*** | <0.001 | −0.25 [−0.45 to −0.04]* | 0.019 | 0.12 [−0.03 to 0.27] | 0.122 | 0.11 [−0.04 to 0.27] | 0.159 |
20–29 | −0.27 [−0.43 to −0.11]*** | <0.001 | −0.30 [−0.44 to −0.16]*** | <0.001 | 0.06 [−0.07 to 0.19] | 0.389 | 0.06 [−0.08 to 0.19] | 0.410 |
30–64 | −0.25 [−0.41 to −0.09]** | 0.002 | −0.09 [−0.23 to 0.06] | 0.231 | 0.13 [0.02 to 0.24]* | 0.021 | 0.13 [0.02 to 0.24]* | 0.024 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |||||
Mood or anxiety | ||||||||
4–12 | −0.53 [−0.72 to −0.34]*** | <0.001 | −0.39 [−0.56 to −0.21]*** | <0.001 | 0.10 [−0.04 to 0.25] | 0.168 | 0.09 [−0.05 to 0.23] | 0.201 |
13–19 | −0.47 [−0.77 to −0.18]** | 0.002 | −0.28 [−0.57 to 0.00] | 0.051 | 0.08 [−0.15 to 0.32] | 0.486 | 0.07 [−0.17 to 0.31] | 0.564 |
20–29 | −0.40 [−0.72 to −0.08]* | 0.015 | −0.41 [−0.70 to −0.13]** | 0.005 | 0.23 [−0.01 to 0.47] | 0.062 | 0.23 [−0.01 to 0.47] | 0.056 |
30–64 | −0.24 [−0.40 to −0.08]** | 0.004 | −0.11 [−0.26 to 0.04] | 0.152 | 0.09 [−0.02 to 0.21] | 0.113 | 0.10 [−0.01 to 0.22] | 0.086 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |||||
Impulse control | ||||||||
4–12 | −0.50 [−0.86 to −0.15]** | 0.005 | −0.24 [−0.56 to 0.08] | 0.140 | 0.13 [−0.16 to 0.42] | 0.378 | 0.10 [−0.17 to 0.38] | 0.462 |
13–19 | −1.01 [−1.82 to −0.20]* | 0.015 | −0.83 [−1.55 to −0.12]* | 0.022 | 0.55 [−0.11 to 1.22] | 0.099 | 0.52 [−0.10 to 1.15] | 0.100 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | |||||
Substance use | ||||||||
4–19 | −0.33 [−0.57 to −0.10]** | 0.005 | −0.18 [−0.38 to 0.02] | 0.070 | 0.19 [−0.01 to 0.38] | 0.058 | 0.18 [−0.01 to 0.36] | 0.066 |
20–29 | −0.32 [−0.52 to −0.13]** | 0.001 | −0.32 [−0.48 to −0.17]*** | <0.001 | −0.04 [−0.22 to 0.13] | 0.627 | −0.05 [−0.23 to 0.12] | 0.553 |
30–64 | −0.64 [−0.88 to −0.40]*** | <0.001 | −0.32 [−0.53 to −0.11]** | 0.003 | 0.10 [−0.17 to 0.37] | 0.471 | 0.05 [−0.21 to 0.31] | 0.700 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Notes: Model 1 only controls for age and gender. Model 2 additionally controls for education, income, job status and living situation. For the discrepancy, both models additionally control for actual SSS. The reference group consists of participants without that particular disorder (group).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Table 4.
Recency | Actual SSS | Discrepancy | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | |
Any disorder | ||||||||
Past year | −0.62 [−0.77 to −0.47]*** | <0.001 | −0.36 [−0.50 to −0.22]*** | <0.001 | 0.11 [−0.02 to 0.23] | 0.103 | 0.08 [−0.04 to 0.20] | 0.183 |
1–5 years | −0.40 [−0.60 to −0.20]*** | <0.001 | −0.35 [−0.53 to −0.17]*** | <0.001 | 0.13 [−0.02 to 0.28] | 0.091 | 0.13 [−0.02 to 0.28] | 0.089 |
6–10 years | −0.09 [−0.26 to 0.07] | 0.255 | −0.14 [−0.26 to −0.01]* | 0.037 | 0.03 [−0.10 to 0.16] | 0.649 | 0.04 [−0.09 to 0.16] | 0.552 |
>10 years | −0.12 [−0.31 to 0.06] | 0.198 | −0.14 [−0.32 to 0.04] | 0.12 | 0.06 [−0.09 to 0.21] | 0.445 | 0.07 [−0.08 to 0.22] | 0.368 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | ||||||
Mood or anxiety | ||||||||
Past year | −0.63 [−0.80 to −0.46]*** | <0.001 | −0.40 [−0.56 to −0.25]*** | <0.001 | 0.17** [0.04 to 0.29] | 0.008 | 0.15* [0.03 to 0.26] | 0.011 |
1–5 years | −0.43 [−0.67 to −0.19]*** | <0.001 | −0.36 [−0.56 to −0.16]*** | <0.001 | 0.06 [−0.12 to 0.24] | 0.525 | 0.06 [−0.12 to 0.24] | 0.482 |
6–10 years | −0.15 [−0.31 to 0.01] | 0.071 | −0.16 [−0.30 to −0.01]* | 0.035 | 0.02 [−0.14 to 0.18] | 0.799 | 0.03 [−0.12 to 0.18] | 0.712 |
>10 years | −0.2 [−0.52 to 0.12] | 0.225 | −0.2 [−0.49 to 0.09] | 0.184 | 0.18 [−0.06 to 0.43] | 0.144 | 0.19 [−0.06 to 0.44] | 0.133 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | ||||||
Impulse control | ||||||||
Past year | −0.65 [−1.29 to −0.00]* | 0.049 | −0.07 [−0.66 to 0.53] | 0.827 | 0.18 [−0.26 to 0.62] | 0.423 | 0.11 [−0.32 to 0.53] | 0.615 |
1–5 years | −1.48 [−2.53 to −0.43]** | 0.006 | −1.07 [−2.02 to −0.12]* | 0.028 | 1.26* [0.18 to 2.34] | 0.022 | 1.22* [0.20 to 2.23] | 0.019 |
6–10 years | −0.73 [−1.38 to −0.09]* | 0.025 | −0.68 [−1.22 to −0.13]* | 0.015 | 0.08 [−0.33 to 0.48] | 0.711 | 0.07 [−0.31 to 0.44] | 0.723 |
>10 years | −0.17 [−0.62 to 0.29] | 0.473 | −0.27 [−0.62 to 0.07] | 0.114 | −0.14 [−0.39 to 0.12] | 0.294 | −0.12 [−0.37 to 0.14] | 0.361 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 | ||||||
Substance use | ||||||||
Past year | −0.74 [−1.01 to −0.46]*** | <0.001 | −0.42 [−0.68 to −0.15]** | 0.002 | 0.10 [−0.18 to 0.38] | 0.478 | 0.05 [−0.21 to 0.32] | 0.698 |
1–5 years | −0.39 [−0.73 to −0.05]* | 0.023 | −0.25 [−0.56 to 0.06] | 0.111 | 0.24 [−0.01 to 0.50] | 0.061 | 0.22 [−0.02 to 0.47] | 0.075 |
6–10 years | −0.14 [−0.45 to 0.16] | 0.349 | −0.19 [−0.41 to 0.02] | 0.081 | 0.09 [−0.13 to 0.31] | 0.420 | 0.10 [−0.12 to 0.32] | 0.362 |
>10 years | −0.18 [−0.42 to 0.06] | 0.146 | −0.14 [−0.35 to 0.08] | 0.21 | 0.02 [−0.19 to 0.24] | 0.842 | 0.02 [−0.19 to 0.23] | 0.850 |
No disorder | 0.00 | 0.00 |
Notes: Model 1 only controls for age and gender. Model 2 additionally controls for education, income, job status and living situation. For the discrepancy, both models additionally control for actual SSS. The reference group consists of participants without that particular disorder (group).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Age of onset as a continuous variable was marginally significantly associated with actual SSS among those with any disorder (B = 0.006, p = 0.046) and among those with substance use disorder specifically (B = −0.016, p = 0.041), but not with the discrepancy between actual and desired SSS (B = −0.007 to 0.002, p = 0.300–563) (see Table 5, model 1). Younger age of onset tended to be associated with a lower actual SSS than later age of onset among those with any disorder, while the pattern was remarkably reversed for substance use disorder. After controlling for objective SES indicators, age of onset was no longer significantly related to actual SSS (Table 5, model 2).
Table 5.
Actual SSS | Discrepancy | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Model 1 | Model 2 | Model 1 | Model 2 | |||||
B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | B [95% CI] | p-value | |
Age of onset | ||||||||
Any disorder | 0.006 [0.000 to 0.012]* | 0.046 | 0.006 [−0.000 to 0.012] | 0.052 | 0.002 [−0.003 to 0.007] | 0.380 | 0.003 [−0.002 to 0.008] | 0.279 |
Mood or anxiety | 0.004 [−0.003 to 0.011] | 0.240 | 0.004 [−0.004 to 0.011] | 0.345 | 0.002 [−0.004 to 0.007] | 0.563 | 0.003 [−0.003 to 0.008] | 0.368 |
Substance use | −0.016 [−0.032 to −0.001]* | 0.041 | −0.006 [−0.020 to 0.008] | 0.379 | −0.007 [−0.019 to 0.006] | 0.300 | −0.008 [−0.019 to 0.003] | 0.143 |
Recency | ||||||||
Any disorder | 0.015 [0.005 to 0.026]** | 0.005 | 0.005 [−0.005 to 0.014] | 0.358 | −0.003 [−0.009 to 0.004] | 0.452 | −0.001 [−0.007 to 0.006] | 0.827 |
Mood or anxiety | 0.016 [0.000 to 0.032]* | 0.046 | 0.004 [−0.011 to 0.019] | 0.572 | 0.002 [−0.009 to 0.013] | 0.729 | 0.003 [−0.007 to 0.013] | 0.598 |
Impulse control | 0.035 [−0.009 to 0.080] | 0.121 | 0.009 [−0.028 to 0.047] | 0.628 | −0.012 [−0.036 to 0.012] | 0.336 | 0.001 [−0.025 to 0.026] | 0.952 |
Substance use | 0.011 [−0.008 to 0.029] | 0.275 | 0.002 [−0.013 to 0.018] | 0.752 | 0.002 [−0.013 to 0.017] | 0.771 | 0.004 [−0.009 to 0.016] | 0.566 |
Notes: Model 1 only controls for age and gender. Model 2 additionally controls for education, income, job status and living situation. For the discrepancy, both models additionally control for actual SSS. Age of onset and recency are entered into the model as continuous variables.
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
With regard to recency, mental disorders in the year before baseline and in the 1–5 years before baseline were negatively associated with actual SSS for each disorder category (B = −1.48 to −0.39, p < 0.001–0.049). However, mental disorders that remitted 6 or more years before baseline were no longer significantly associated with actual SSS (B = −0.20 to −0.09, p = 0.071–0.473), with the exception of impulse control disorders in the 6–10 years before baseline (B = −0.73, p = 0.025) (Table 4, model 1). Controlling for objective SES generally attenuated the magnitude of associations (Table 4, model 2). There were few associations between recency categories and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy, with only past-year mood or anxiety disorders (B = 0.17, p = 0.008) and impulse control disorders in the 1–5 years before baseline (B = 1.26, p = 0.022) being statistically significantly associated with the discrepancy. These associations were unchanged after controlling for objective SES.
Recency was significantly associated with actual SSS among those with any disorder (B = 0.015, p = 0.005) and marginally significantly so among those with mood or anxiety disorders specifically (B = 0.016, p = 0.046). After controlling for objective SES, these associations became non-significant (Table 5, model 2). Recency was not significantly associated with the actual–desired SSS discrepancy (p = 0.336–0.771, controlling for actual SSS) (Table 5, model 1).
Discussion
Principal findings
In this study, we showed that lifetime mental disorders are associated with lower actual SSS and, to a lesser extent, with a slightly larger discrepancy between desired and actual SSS. Thus, people with mental disorders do not come as close to achieving their desired social position as people without mental disorders. Associations for actual SSS were attenuated, but largely persisted after controlling for objective SES, while associations with the actual–desired SSS discrepancy were unchanged after controlling for objective SES. Our study therefore confirms and extends previous work showing that mental health problems are associated with SSS (McLaughlin et al., 2012; Honjo et al., 2014; Scott et al., 2014).
Our analyses using categorical ages of onset showed that participants with mental disorders generally had a lower SSS than healthy participants regardless of age of onset of the disorder. However, our analyses using continuous age of onset within the group of participants with a disorder provided inconclusive evidence that earlier age of onset is associated with lower SSS than later age of onset, given the presence of a disorder. Our categorical analyses also showed that disorders in long-term remission were not associated with significantly lower SSS. The lower SSS experienced by people with recent mental disorders compared to those with long-remitted mental disorders appeared to be at least partly related to lower objective SES, as controlling for objective SES attenuated the association between (continuous) recency and SSS.
To our knowledge, no previous work has examined the discrepancy between actual and desired SSS. Our finding that people with any lifetime mental disorder have a larger discrepancy than healthy participants suggests that people with lifetime mental disorders are particularly dissatisfied with their position in life, which could potentially contribute to mental health problems. However, associations of mental disorders with the actual–desired SSS discrepancy (after controlling for actual SSS) were generally small in magnitude and only significant for a few specific mental disorders. While participants with mental disorders do have a larger actual–desired SSS discrepancy than healthy participants (results not shown), this is largely explained by differences in actual SSS between participants with and without mental disorders.
Hence, the association between mental disorders and the actual–desired SSS discrepancy may generally be of relatively limited clinical importance compared to the association between mental disorders and actual SSS, although a few specific disorders (conduct disorder and drug dependence) did show quite large associations with the actual–desired SSS discrepancy even after controlling for actual SSS. This contrasts with previous research that found that the discrepancy between actual SSS and a counterfactual SSS (if participants had not become unemployed or had not become single parents) was as strongly associated with depressive symptoms as actual SSS (Euteneuer et al., 2019). It is possible that the counterfactual SSS in that study was more salient to participants, given that it represents a plausible alternative reality that was ‘lost’ (upon becoming unemployed or becoming a single parent). The salience of ‘lost’ alternative selves (i.e. clarity of the mental image and frequency of thinking about it) has been related to reduced well-being (King and Smith, 2004; King and Hicks, 2007). The concept of desired SSS used in this study, on the other hand, could be a more nebulous ideal that participants do not have a very clear picture of and that they may or may not have ever realistically expected to achieve.
The finding that disorders in long-term remission were not associated with statistically significantly lower actual SSS is encouraging and suggests that people with mental disorders can fully recover in this regard. This finding concurs with other research showing the desirability of full remission as a treatment outcome to maximise functioning and well-being (Zajecka, 2003). However, we cannot exclude the possibility that the association between recency and SSS is confounded by disorder severity. In general, mild disorders are more likely to remit, while severe disorders are more likely to persist (Spijker et al., 2004; Hendriks et al., 2013), so the lack of association between long-remitted disorders and actual SSS could also reflect the fact that remitting disorders tend to be milder. Longitudinal research is necessary to disentangle course and severity of the disorder and definitively establish whether the SSS of people with mental disorders tends to normalise after remission.
Although we investigated the association between age of onset and SSS and found a marginally significant positive association with age of onset among those with any mental disorder, the evidence was not sufficiently strong to confidently either rule in or rule out larger associations between early-onset disorders and SSS than between late-onset disorders and SSS. In contrast to previous research (Breslau et al., 2008; Lee et al., 2009), we found no association between early-onset mental disorders and educational achievement, although early-onset mental disorders were associated with low income. Since education is a plausible mediating variable between early-onset disorders and later SSS, this might explain our inconclusive findings regarding age of onset and SSS. We also found suggestive evidence that the association between age of onset and SSS may be reversed for substance use disorders, with late-onset substance use disorders being more strongly associated with SSS than early-onset disorders. We speculate that this might reflect the fact that early-onset substance use problems are relatively normative and often developmentally limited to adolescence and young adulthood (Maggs and Schulenberg, 2005). However, further research on this topic is necessary.
Strengths and limitations
One of the strengths of this study is that the NEMESIS-2 cohort is a large sample that is representative of the general population. Furthermore, in contrast to most previous research, we used a validated structured interview (CIDI) to assess diagnosable disorders, rather than using symptom questionnaires. Finally, by examining both actual and desired SSS, we shed some light on whether people with mental disorders adjust their expectations for their life.
A limitation of this study is that we did not investigate the longitudinal and possibly bidirectional relationships between SSS and mental disorders. The observational nature of our study also precludes clear causal inferences. While NEMESIS-2 is a longitudinal cohort, SSS was not assessed at baseline. Consequently, we cannot entirely exclude the possibility that the relationship between baseline mental disorders and follow-up SSS is actually explained by baseline SSS. A limited body of experimental research suggests that an experimental manipulation of SSS resulted in changes in depressive cognitions and stress-reactive ruminations (though no difference in self-reported depressive symptoms) (Schubert et al., 2016), implying that lower SSS could have a causal effect on mental health. On the other hand, an experimental manipulation of mood did not result in changes in self-reported SSS (Kraus et al., 2013). However, this area of research is still in its infancy, and its relevance to the long-term relationship between SSS and mental health is unclear. Furthermore, we examined a general population cohort and some findings, such as the lack of association between disorders in long-term remission and social status, may not generalise to a more severely affected clinical population. Finally, age of onset and recency were estimated retrospectively. While the CIDI was designed using special probe questions that have been shown to generate more plausible distributions of age of onset (Knäuper et al., 1999), some recall bias likely persists.
Conclusions
In this large, population-representative cohort, we found that lifetime mental disorders were associated with lower SSS and, to a lesser extent, larger discrepancies between actual and desired SSS. The association with actual SSS was somewhat attenuated but persisted after controlling for objective indicators of social status, such as income. Encouragingly, mental disorders that had been in remission for several years were not associated with lower SSS. This suggests that SSS might normalise after remission, and future longitudinal research should investigate this possibility.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Availability of data and materials
The data on which this manuscript is based are not publicly available. However, data from NEMESIS-2 are available upon request. The Dutch ministry of health financed the data and the agreement is that these data can be used freely under certain restrictions and always under the supervision of the principal investigator (PI) of the study. Thus, some access restrictions do apply to the data.
At any time, researchers can contact the PI of NEMESIS-2 (Margreet ten Have, mhave@trimbos.nl) and submit a research plan, describing its background, research questions, variables to be used in the analyses and an outline of the analyses. If a request for data sharing is approved, a written agreement will be signed stating that the data will only be used for addressing the agreed research questions described and not for other purposes.
Financial support
NEMESIS-2 is conducted by the Netherlands Institute of Mental Health and Addiction (Trimbos Institute) in Utrecht. Financial support has been received from the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport, with supplementary support from the Netherlands Organization for Health Research and Development (ZonMw) and the Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) investigators.
Ethical standards
The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.
Conflict of interest
None.
References
- Adler NE and Epel ES (2000) Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychology 19, 586–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Adler N, Singh-Manoux A, Schwartz J, Stewart J, Matthews K and Marmot MG (2008) Social status and health: a comparison of British civil servants in Whitehall-II with European- and African-Americans in CARDIA. Social Science and Medicine 66, 1034–1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau J, Lane M, Sampson N and Kessler RC (2008) Mental disorders and subsequent educational attainment in a US national sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research 42, 708–716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen R, Kessler RC, Sadikova E, Nemoyer A, Sampson NA, Alvarez K, Vilsaint CL, Greif J, Mclaughlin KA, Jackson JS, Alegría M and Williams DR (2019) Racial and ethnic differences in individual-level and area-based socioeconomic status and 12-month DSM-IV mental disorders. Journal of Psychiatric Research 119, 48–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins AL and Goldman N (2008) Perceived social position and health in older adults in Taiwan. Social Science and Medicine 66, 536–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Graaf R, ten Have M and van Dorsselaer S (2010) The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2 (NEMESIS-2): design and methods. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 19, 125–141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Graaf R, van Dorsselaer S, Tuithof M and ten Have M (2013) Sociodemographic and psychiatric predictors of attrition in a prospective psychiatric epidemiological study among the general population. Result of the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study-2. Comprehensive Psychiatry 54, 1131–1139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demakakos P, Nazroo J, Breeze E and Marmot M (2008) Socioeconomic status and health: the role of subjective social status. Social Science and Medicine 67, 330–340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elovainio M, Pulkki-Råback L, Jokela M, Kivimäki M, Hintsanen M, Hintsa T, Viikari J, Raitakari OT and Keltikangas-Järvinen L (2012) Socioeconomic status and the development of depressive symptoms from childhood to adulthood: a longitudinal analysis across 27 years of follow-up in the Young Finns study. Social Science and Medicine 74, 923–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Euteneuer F (2014) Subjective social status and health. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 27, 337–343. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Euteneuer F, Schaefer SJ, Neubert M, Rief W and Süssenbach P (2019) What if I had not fallen from grace? Psychological distress and the gap between factual and counterfactual subjective social status. Stress and Health, smi.2892, 1–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Franzini L and Fernandez-Esquer ME (2006) The association of subjective social status and health in low-income Mexican-origin individuals in Texas. Social Science and Medicine 63, 788–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamad R, Fernald LCH, Karlan DS and Zinman J (2008) Social and economic correlates of depressive symptoms and perceived stress in South African adults. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 62, 538–544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haro JM, Arbabzadeh-Bouchez S, Brugha TS, De Girolamo G, Guyer ME, Jin R, Lepine JP, Mazzi F, Reneses B, Vilagut G, Sampson NA and Kessler RC (2006) Concordance of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview Version 3.0 (CIDI 3.0) with standardized clinical assessments in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 15, 167–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hendriks SM, Spijker J, Licht CMM, Beekman ATF and Penninx BWJH (2013) Two-year course of anxiety disorders: different across disorders or dimensions? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 128, 212–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henkel V, Mergl R, Kohnen R, Maier W, Möller H-J and Hegerl U (2003) Identifying depression in primary care: a comparison of different methods in a prospective cohort study. BMJ 326, 200–201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoebel J, Maske UE, Zeeb H and Lampert T (2017) Social inequalities and depressive symptoms in adults: the role of objective and subjective socioeconomic status. PLoS ONE 12, e0169764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Honjo K, Kawakami N, Tsuchiya M, Sakurai K and WMH-J 2002–2006 Survey Group (2014) Association of subjective and objective socioeconomic status with subjective mental health and mental disorders among Japanese men and women. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 21, 421–429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hu P, Adler NE, Goldman N, Weinstein M and Seeman TE (2005) Relationship between subjective social status and measures of health in older Taiwanese persons. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 53, 483–488. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudson CG (2005) Socioeconomic status and mental illness: tests of the social causation and selection hypotheses. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry 75, 3–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson JG, Cohen P, Dohrenwend BP, Link BG and Brook JS (1999) A longitudinal investigation of social causation and social selection processes involved in the association between socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 108, 490–499. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karvonen S and Rahkonen O (2011) Subjective social status and health in young people. Sociology of Health & Illness 33, 372–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC and Üstün BB (2004) The World Mental Health (WMH) survey initiative version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 13, 93–117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler RC, Heeringa S, Lakoma MD, Petukhova M, Rupp AE, Schoenbaum M, Wang PS and Zaslavsky AM (2008) Individual and societal effects of mental disorders on earnings in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 703–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King LA and Smith NG (2004) Gay and straight possible selves: goals, identity, subjective well-being, and personality development. Journal of Personality 72, 967–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King LA and Hicks JA (2007) Lost and found possible selves: goals, development, and well-being. New Directions for Adult and Continuing Education 114, 27–37. [Google Scholar]
- Knäuper B, Cannell C, Schwarz N, Bruce M and Kessler RC (1999) Improving the accuracy of major depression age of onset reports in the US National Comorbidity Survey. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 8, 39–48. [Google Scholar]
- Kraus MW, Adler N and Chen TWD (2013) Is the association of subjective SES and self-rated health confounded by negative mood? An experimental approach. Health Psychology 32, 138–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S, Tsang A, Breslau J, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Angermeyer M, Borges G, Bromet E, Bruffaerts R, de Girolamo G, Fayyad J, Gureje O, Haro JM, Kawakami N, Levinson D, Oakley Browne MA, Ormel J, Posada-Villa J, Williams DR and Kessler RC (2009) Mental disorders and termination of education in high-income and low- and middle-income countries: epidemiological study. British Journal of Psychiatry 194, 411–417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leu J, Yen IH, Gansky SA, Walton E, Adler NE and Takeuchi DT (2008) The association between subjective social status and mental health among Asian immigrants: investigating the influence of age at immigration. Social Science & Medicine 66, 1152–1164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levinson D, Lakoma MD, Petukhova M, Schoenbaum M, Zaslavsky AM, Angermeyer M, Borges G, Bruffaerts R, de Girolamo G, de Graaf R, Gureje O, Haro JM, Hu C, Karam AN, Kawakami N, Lee S, Lepine J, Browne MO, Okoliyski M, Posada-Villa J, Sagar R, Viana MC, Williams DR and Kessler RC (2010) Associations of serious mental illness with earnings: results from the WHO World Mental Health surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 114–121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorant V, Deliège D, Eaton W, Robert A, Philippot P and Ansseau M (2003) Socioeconomic inequalities in depression: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Epidemiology 157, 98–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maggs JL and Schulenberg JE (2005) Initiation and course of alcohol consumption among adolescents and young adults. In Galanter M, Lowman C, Boyd GM, Faden VB, Witt E and Lagressa D (eds), Recent Developments in Alcoholism (Alcohol Problems in Adolescents and Young Adults), vol 17. Boston, MA: Springer. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McLaughlin KA, Costello EJ, Leblanc W, Sampson NA and Kessler RC (2012) Socioeconomic status and adolescent mental disorders. American Journal of Public Health 102, 1742–1750. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyakawa M, Magnusson Hanson LL, Theorell T and Westerlund H (2012) Subjective social status: its determinants and association with health in the Swedish working population (the SLOSH study). European Journal of Public Health 22, 593–597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Operario D, Adler NE and Williams DR (2004) Subjective social status: reliability and predictive utility for global health. Psychology and Health 19, 237–246. [Google Scholar]
- Pino EC, Damus K, Jack B, Henderson D, Milanovic S and Kalesan B (2018) Adolescent socioeconomic status and depressive symptoms in later life: evidence from structural equation models. Journal of Affective Disorders 225, 702–708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Präg P, Mills MC and Wittek R (2016) Subjective socioeconomic status and health in cross-national comparison. Social Science & Medicine 149, 84–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quon EC and McGrath JJ (2014) Subjective socioeconomic status and adolescent health: a meta-analysis. Health Psychology 33, 433–447. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sakurai K, Kawakami N, Yamaoka K, Ishikawa H and Hashimoto H (2010) The impact of subjective and objective social status on psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Social Science and Medicine 70, 1832–1839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schubert T, Süssenbach P, Schäfer SJ and Euteneuer F (2016) The effect of subjective social status on depressive thinking: an experimental examination. Psychiatry Research 241, 22–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott KM, Al-Hamzawi AO, Andrade LH, Borges G, Caldas-de-Almeida JM, Fiestas F, Gureje O, Hu C, Karam EG, Kawakami N, Lee S, Levinson D, Lim CCW, Navarro-Mateu F, Okoliyski M, Posada-Villa J, Torres Y, Williams DR, Zakhozha V and Kessler RC (2014) Associations between subjective social status and DSM-IV mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 1400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh-Manoux A, Adler NE and Marmot MG (2003) Subjective social status: its determinants and its association with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study. Social Science & Medicine 56, 1321–1333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh-Manoux A, Marmot MG and Adler NE (2005) Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status? Psychosomatic Medicine 67, 855–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spijker J, de Graaf R, Bijl RV, Beekman ATF, Ormel J and Nolen WA (2004) Determinants of persistence of major depressive episodes in the general population. Results from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). Journal of Affective Disorders 81, 231–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subramanyam MA, Diez-Roux AV, Hickson DMA, Sarpong DF, Sims M, Taylor HA, Williams DR and Wyatt SB (2012) Subjective social status and psychosocial and metabolic risk factors for cardiovascular disease among African Americans in the Jackson Heart Study. Social Science and Medicine 74, 1146–1154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vilagut G, Forero CG, Barbaglia G and Alonso J (2016) Screening for depression in the general population with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D): a systematic review with meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 11, 1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolff LS, Subramanian SV, Acevedo-Garcia D, Weber D and Kawachi I (2010) Compared to whom? Subjective social status, self-rated health, and referent group sensitivity in a diverse US sample. Social Science and Medicine 70, 2019–2028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong SY, Mercer SW, Woo J and Leung J (2008) The influence of multi-morbidity and self-reported socio-economic standing on the prevalence of depression in an elderly Hong Kong population. BMC Public Health 8, 1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zajecka JM (2003) Treating depression to remission. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 64, 7–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data on which this manuscript is based are not publicly available. However, data from NEMESIS-2 are available upon request. The Dutch ministry of health financed the data and the agreement is that these data can be used freely under certain restrictions and always under the supervision of the principal investigator (PI) of the study. Thus, some access restrictions do apply to the data.
At any time, researchers can contact the PI of NEMESIS-2 (Margreet ten Have, mhave@trimbos.nl) and submit a research plan, describing its background, research questions, variables to be used in the analyses and an outline of the analyses. If a request for data sharing is approved, a written agreement will be signed stating that the data will only be used for addressing the agreed research questions described and not for other purposes.