Abstract
Even after conventional patching treatment, individuals with a history of amblyopia typically lack good stereo vision. This is often attributed to atypical suppression between the eyes, yet the specific mechanism is still unclear. Guided by computational models of binocular vision, we tested explicit predictions about how neural responses to contrast might differ in individuals with impaired binocular vision. Participants with a history of amblyopia (N = 25), and control participants with typical visual development (N = 19) took part in the study. Neural responses to different combinations of contrast in the left and right eyes, were measured using both electroencephalography (EEG) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Stimuli were sinusoidal gratings with a spatial frequency of 3c/deg, flickering at 4 Hz. In the fMRI experiment, we also ran population receptive field and retinotopic mapping sequences, and a phase-encoded localiser stimulus, to identify voxels in primary visual cortex (V1) sensitive to the main stimulus. Neural responses in both modalities increased monotonically with stimulus contrast. When measured with EEG, responses were attenuated in the weaker eye, consistent with a fixed tonic suppression of that eye. When measured with fMRI, a low contrast stimulus in the weaker eye substantially reduced the response to a high contrast stimulus in the stronger eye. This effect was stronger than when the stimulus-eye pairings were reversed, consistent with unbalanced dynamic suppression between the eyes. Measuring neural responses using different methods leads to different conclusions about visual differences in individuals with impaired binocular vision. Both of the atypical suppression effects may relate to binocular perceptual deficits, e.g. in stereopsis, and we anticipate that these measures could be informative for monitoring the progress of treatments aimed at recovering binocular vision.
Keywords: Dichoptic, fMRI, Interocular suppression, SSVEP, V1
1. Introduction
The binocular visual system is exquisitely sensitive, and has the ability to detect differences (disparities) between the eyes of well below one minute of arc (Coutant and Westheimer, 1993). This results in a vivid perception of depth from stereopsis (Julesz, 1986) that benefits everyday tasks such as fine motor control (e.g. threading a needle) and the judgement of relative object distance (e.g. during driving). But in a substantial minority of individuals (around 1.4%; Fu et al., 2020), an optical (e.g. anisometropia) or muscular (e.g. strabismus) asymmetry between the eyes during childhood disrupts the development of binocular vision. This can lead to amblyopia, in which vision through the affected eye is significantly impaired (Hess, 1979). Such problems can be treated to some extent by orthoptic or surgical interventions, which recover sensitivity in the weaker eye in a proportion of cases (Levi, 2020). But even if treatment is successful in improving vision in the amblyopic eye, binocular vision may not be restored, with stereopsis rarely reaching normal levels (Levi et al., 2015), and physiological measures of binocular function also being impaired (Baitch et al., 1991; Baitch and Levi, 1988). An enduring mystery is the identity of the neural mechanism that disrupts binocular vision, even in situations where the eyes have similar acuity and sensitivity.
In clinical practice, binocular visual disturbances in amblyopia are typically attributed to a process of suppression, whereby the fellow eye suppresses signals from the amblyopic eye (Jampolsky, 1955; Pratt-Johnson and Tillson, 1984; Travers, 1938). This suppression could take several different forms. For example, ‘tonic’ suppression should persist even when there is no input to the fellow eye (e.g. if it is closed, patched or pressure blinded, or simply shown a blank display). This amounts to a fixed attenuation of the signal in the amblyopic eye that is invariant to signals from the fellow eye (Baker et al., 2008). Alternatively, a more ‘dynamic’ form of suppression would depend on the current stimulation of the two eyes, such that higher contrasts in one eye produce greater suppression of the other eye. Interocular suppression has been widely studied in intact binocular vision, and has several perceptual consequences, such as ocularity invariance (the observation that our general perception of the world is unchanged whether one or both eyes are open (Baker et al., 2007a)) and binocular rivalry (the alternation in perception between conflicting images shown to the two eyes (Levelt, 1966)). Impaired binocular vision might result from an imbalance of these existing processes of interocular suppression.
Distinguishing between these, and other, explanations for binocular impairments has proved challenging. In some psychophysical paradigms, such as dichoptic contrast discrimination, similar performance can result even over a wide range of relative amounts of suppression between the eyes (Baker et al., 2008). In other paradigms, tonic and dynamic suppression are equally able to account for the results (Ding and Levi, 2014). Isolating a direct neural measure of suppression would allow us to distinguish between different models, and potentially provide an objective index of binocular impairment that could be used to track improvements during treatment. In the present study we measured visual responses to stimuli of different contrasts directly with two methods: functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electroencephalography (EEG). These methods have complementary strengths and weaknesses: fMRI has excellent spatial precision, but poor temporal resolution, whereas EEG has poor spatial precision, but good temporal resolution. Previous work measuring visual responses in amblyopia with one or other of these methods has reported generally weaker responses to stimuli in the amblyopic eye (Baker et al., 2015; Barnes et al., 2001). However, the two techniques have not previously been directly compared using common stimuli.
Here we use both methods to measure contrast-response functions (Fig. 1a) for factorial combinations of contrast shown to the left and right eyes (Fig. 2g; see also (Baker et al., 2007a)). We plot the results as a series of functions, where stimuli of increasing contrast are shown to one eye (the ‘target’ stimuli) in the presence of a fixed-contrast (but otherwise identical) stimulus in the other eye (the ‘mask’ stimulus). The general character of these functions can be predicted by contemporary models of binocular signal combination (Meese et al., 2006; Moradi and Heeger, 2009), and correspond well to previous measurements in intact binocular visual systems using both fMRI (Moradi and Heeger, 2009) and EEG (Baker and Wade, 2017). When the mask is absent (0% contrast), the model produces a monotonically increasing monocular contrast response function (left-most curve in Fig. 1a). As mask contrast increases, the overall response becomes larger because it combines the target and mask signals together. However, interocular suppression causes a surprising reduction in the response to a high contrast mask when a target of intermediate contrast is added (Baker et al., 2007a, 2013). This produces the u-shaped function shown in the final curve of Fig. 1a - the response increase caused by excitation is outweighed by the response reduction caused by suppression. Responses therefore go down before they go up, giving a direct measure of interocular suppression.
We can disrupt the model shown in Fig. 1a in two key ways. First, we can implement tonic suppression by attenuating the signal in one eye by a constant factor (Baker et al., 2008). This reduces the response in the affected eye, and also weakens its impact on the fellow eye (Fig. 1b). Second, we can implement dynamic suppression by increasing the weight of suppression from one eye onto the other (Fig. 1c). This has no effect on monocular presentations (as there is no signal in the opposite eye to cause suppression), but with high-contrast masks there is a much greater reduction in response for intermediate signal contrasts (the u-shaped functions in the right-most plot become deeper). This experimental paradigm therefore has the potential to distinguish between these two types of suppression.
Our aim in this study was to empirically test specific predictions of these competing models by measuring neural responses with fMRI and EEG to a common set of visual stimuli. In addition to testing control participants with typical binocular vision, we also recruited individuals with a history of binocular disturbance. Although these participants do not all currently meet the diagnostic criteria for amblyopia (owing to successful treatment), they would very likely have done so in childhood and/or had they not been treated. Given the widespread incidence of treatment in countries with developed healthcare systems, understanding the residual binocular deficits in treated amblyopes is of substantial clinical importance. To summarise our results, we find attenuated responses to stimuli in the amblyopic eye when measured using EEG, and increased and asymmetrical interocular suppression in individuals with impaired binocular vision when measured using fMRI. Surprisingly this takes the form of stronger suppression of the dominant eye by the weaker eye.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
A total of 44 participants completed the EEG experiment, 19 of whom were control participants with no history of binocular visual abnormalities, and clinically normal vision. The remaining 25 participants had been diagnosed with amblyopia, or treated for strabismus during childhood (see Table 1 for further details). Approximately half of these participants (12/25, highlighted in bold) still met the diagnostic criteria for amblyopia at the time of testing, based on a corrected visual acuity difference of two lines or more between the eyes. The MRI experiments were completed by 10 of the control participants, and 12 of the patients (A1 - A12 in Table 1). Participants gave written informed consent (consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki), and received financial compensation for their time (£20 per experiment). The study protocols were approved by the research governance committee of the York Neuroimaging Centre.
Table 1.
Participant | Age/sex | Amblyopic Eye | R acuity | L acuity | Stereo acuity | Detected | Patching | Surgery | Correction |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
A1 | 20/F | Right | 20/32 | 20/25 | None | 18 mo | 1 - 6 y | <18mo | None |
A2 | 22/M | Right | 20/50 | 20/20 | 80s | 9 y | 9 y | None | None |
A3 | 26/M | Left | 20/20 | 20/50 | 140s | 4 y | 4 y | None | None |
A4 | 21/M | Left | 20/20 | 20/32 | 140s | 18 mo | 5 y | 18 mo | None |
A5 | 19/F | Left | 20/20 | 20/40 | 40s | 5 y | 5 - 6 y | None | LE: −2.5; RE: + 0.75 |
A6 | 21/M | Left | 20/20 | 20/40 | 80s | 4 y | 4 y | None | None |
A7 | 35/F | Right | 20/40 | 20/20 | 60s | ~10 y | None | None | None |
A8 | 22/F | Left | 20/40 | 20/80 | None | 11 y | 11 - 12 y | None | None |
A9 | 20/F | Left | 20/16 | 20/16 | 400s | 6 y | None | None | None |
A10 | 21/F | Left | 20/16 | 20/20 | None | 18 mo | 4 y | None | LE: +4; RE:+3.75 |
A11 | 22/M | Right | 20/20 | 20/20 | 140s | 5 y | 5 y | None | None |
A12 | 22/F | Right | 20/20 | 20/20 | 400s | 3 y | None | 3 & 4 y | None |
A13 | 40/F | Left | 20/20 | 20/50 | None | 5 y | 5 - 6 y | None | None |
A14 | 17/F | Left | 20/25 | 20/32 | 40s | 4 y | 4 - 6 y | None | None |
A15 | 49/M | Left | 20/16 | 20/32 | None | 20 mo | 4 y | 1 & 7 y | Not known |
A16 | 35/M | Right | 20/120 | 20/25 | 400s | 9 y | None | None | None |
A17 | 22/M | Right | 20/100 | 20/16 | None | 4 y | 4 y | None | None |
A18 | 19/F | Left | 20/16 | 20/16 | None | 7 y | 7 - 10 y | None | None |
A19 | 19/F | Left | 20/20 | 20/200 | None | 18 mo | 18 mo | None | None |
A20 | 19/M | Left | 20/50 | 20/50 | 200s | <6 y | 6 y | None | None |
A21 | 21/F | Right | 20/20 | 20/16 | 40s | 5 y | 5 y | None | LE: −1.25; RE: −1.75 |
A22 | 21/F | Left | 20/20 | 20/32 | None | ~6 y | 6 - 7 y | ~6 y | LE: +10.5; RE: +9.5 |
A23 | 20/M | Right | 20/32 | 20/20 | 400s | 5 y | 5 y | 16 y | None |
A24 | 20/F | Left | 20/20 | 20/40 | 40s | 4 y | 4 - 5 y | None | None |
A25 | 23/M | Left | 20/16 | 20/20 | 140s | 2 y | 2 - 3 y | None | None |
2.2. Apparatus and stimuli
In the MRI scanner, stimuli were displayed using a ProPixx DLP projector (VPixx Ltd., Quebec, Canada) with a refresh rate of 120 Hz, and a resolution of 1920 ✕ 1080 pixels. Viewed from a distance of 57 cm, there were 36 pixels per degree of visual angle. The projector was driven by a high performance PC. A circular polariser interleaved images intended for the left and right eyes (effective refresh rate of 60 Hz per eye). Images were projected onto a custom acrylic display panel that maintained the polarisation, and viewed through a front-silvered mirror and passive stereo polarizer glasses. The maximum luminance was 356 cd/m2 when viewed through the glasses. In the EEG lab, stimuli were displayed using a gamma-corrected ViewPixx 3D LCD display (VPixx Ltd.) with a refresh rate of 120 Hz, and a resolution of 1920 ✕ 1080 pixels. Viewed from a distance of 57 cm, there were 36 pixels per degree of visual angle. The display was driven by a Mac Pro computer. Active stereo shutter goggles (NVidia 3D Vision), synchronised by an infrared signal, allowed segregation of images to the left and right eyes. Through the goggles, the maximum luminance was 26 cd/m2. Both display systems had low levels of crosstalk, as measured using a photometer (Baker et al., 2016). All experiments were programmed in Matlab, using the Psychophysics Toolbox extensions (Brainard, 1997; Kleiner et al., 2007; Pelli, 1997).
Retinotopic mapping involved binocularly presented ring and wedge stimuli constructed from a radial square wave plaid, as illustrated in Fig. 2a and b. The plaid had an angular wavelength of 45° (i.e. 8 complete cycles in 360°), a radial frequency of 0.8 cycles per degree, and flickered in counterphase at 4 Hz. Expanding rings had a width of 1 plaid cycle, and a period of 12 s per sequence (4 ring positions). Rotating wedges were 45° (1 cycle) wide, with a period of 24 s per rotation (8 positions in 45° clockwise steps). Population receptive field (pRF) mapping used a drifting bar (0.5 ✕ 10°) of dynamic 1/f noise, with an RMS contrast of 0.2 (see Fig. 2c). The bar drifted at a speed of 0.4 deg/sec, and followed the sequence illustrated in Fig. 2e. The pRF stimulus was presented to either the left or right eye in different blocks, with the other eye viewing mean luminance. We used a phase-encoded localiser stimulus, constructed from a radial plaid with a width of 4° (see Fig. 2d), presented binocularly. The localiser stimulus counterphase flickered at 4 Hz, and changed position every 6 s, according to the sequence illustrated in Fig. 2d. Each stimulus location had an x-y offset of ±2.34° from fixation.
Stimuli for the main experiments were four horizontal sine-wave gratings with a spatial frequency of 3c/deg, a cosine-blurred spatial window, and a width of 4° (see Fig. 2f). The choice of spatial frequency was motivated by previous work (Hess, 1979), and was balanced between low spatial frequencies where amblyopic deficits may not be apparent, and high spatial frequency regimes where responses may not be measurable in the amblyopic eye. Five different Michelson contrast values (defined as 100*(Lmax-Lmin)/(Lmax+Lmin), where L is luminance) were presented in different combinations (see Fig. 2g). The stimuli flickered sinusoidally between 0 and their nominal contrast (on/off flicker) at a frequency of 4 Hz (see Fig. 2h). The grating stimuli had x-y offsets of ±2.34° from fixation. In all experiments, a static binocular texture was presented to aid fusion. This was constructed from low spatial frequency bandpass filtered noise, and filled the display beyond the central 12° stimulus aperture (see Fig. 2 for examples). The combination of the binocular fusion texture and central fixation marker helped ensure participants could maintain good binocular alignment, and we confirmed this verbally before starting the experiments.
2.3. MRI acquisition
All MRI data were acquired using a GE 3T HDx Excite MRI scanner. We collected two high resolution T1-weighted structural scans (TR 7.8 ms; TE 3 ms; voxel size 1 ✕ 1 ✕ 1 mm; 12° flip angle; matrix size 256 ✕ 256; FOV 256 mm), and two T2*-weighted fast gradient recalled echo scans (TR 400 ms; TE 4.2 ms; voxel size 1 ✕ 1 ✕ 2 mm; 25° flip angle; matrix size 128 ✕ 128; FOV 260 ✕ 260 mm), using an 8-channel surface coil (Nova Medical, Wilmington, MA, USA). We acquired functional images using an EPI sequence with a 16-channel posterior surface coil (Nova Medical, Wilmington, MA, USA), to optimize signal-to-noise ratio at the occipital pole. The slice prescription covered the region containing the calcarine sulcus and occipital pole with 39 axial slices (TR 3000 ms; TE 30 ms; voxel size 2 ✕ 2 ✕ 2 mm; 90° flip angle; matrix size 96 ✕ 96; FOV 192 ✕ 192 mm). We also acquired an in-plane proton density scan using the same slice prescription to aid alignment with the structural scans.
Participants completed the MRI experiments in two sessions. In the first session, we collected the structural scans, and the retinotopic mapping and pRF data. Each pRF sequence lasted 396 s (132 TRs), and either the left or right eye was stimulated. Two repetitions for each eye were completed. The retinotopic mapping (ring and wedge) sequences were collected as a single scan lasting 204 s (68 TRs). In the second session, the phase-encoded localiser and contrast response function data were collected. The localiser scan lasted 156 s (52 TRs), and consisted of a blank period (12 s), followed by 6 repetitions of the localiser sequence. The contrast response function sequence lasted 612 s (204 TRs), and tested each of the 25 conditions (see Fig. 2g) once, with 12-second blank periods between each 12-second trial. This was repeated four times for each participant. During all functional scans, participants performed a fixation task, in which they monitored a grid of 9 squares (3 × 3, each 0.14° wide) with random luminances in the centre of the screen. They were instructed to press a button whenever the fixation marker was changed by re-randomising the luminances. This occurred at randomly determined times, on average once every 48 s (i.e. once every two trials). The task was intended to maintain attention and fixation, and we did not record the responses.
2.4. MRI analysis
Primary MRI analysis was conducted in Matlab using the mrVista toolbox (https://github.com/vistalab/vistasoft). Functional data were motion-corrected within and between scans, and aligned to the in-plane (proton density) scan, and subsequently to the participant's anatomical space. The first 12 s (4 TRs) of each functional scan were discarded to account for magnetic saturation effects. Structural scans were processed using Freesurfer (Dale et al., 1999; Fischl, 2012) to generate a 3D model of the cortex. We created flat patches of unfolded cortex for each hemisphere (120 mm in diameter), centred on the occipital pole (see Fig. 3) to facilitate data visualisation and the creation of Regions of Interest (ROIs). The ring and wedge retinotopy and localiser scans were summarised by a coherence (travelling wave) analysis (Engel et al., 1994) to calculate the phase of the BOLD response at the repetition frequency of the stimulus for each voxel. The pRF data were fit by estimating (at each voxel) the parameters of a 2D Gaussian function that best predicted the BOLD timecourse, given the position of the bar stimulus (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008). This was done independently for the left and right eye scans. The contrast response function data were combined across repetition and analysed using a general linear model (GLM), with regressors (β weights) for each of the 25 conditions. We used a combination of retinotopy and pRF results to define a V1 ROI on the flattened cortex using the location of the calcarine sulcus and reversals of phase angle. ROIs were further restricted using the localiser data, by retaining voxels with a coherence exceeding 0.3. Results were saved as Matlab files, and imported into R for statistical analysis and visualisation. We also converted GLM β weights to MNI space using tools from FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012), and averaged them across participants for visualisation on an inflated cortex in the Connectome Workbench software (Marcus et al., 2011).
2.5. EEG acquisition
All EEG data were acquired using a 64-channel ANT Neuroscan system, with electrodes positioned in a Waveguard cap according to the 10–20 system. Signals were recorded at 1000 Hz, and referenced to the whole-head average. Low-latency digital triggers were sent from the stimulus computer to the EEG amplifier using a parallel cable, and recorded stimulus onset and condition codes to the EEG trace. Participants completed 8 repetitions of the contrast response function experiment. On each repetition, stimuli were presented for trials of 12 s, with an intertrial interval of 3 s. All 25 conditions (see Fig. 2g) were presented once per repetition in a random order, taking 375 s per block. Participants were given breaks between blocks. The same fixation task as described for the MRI experiments was performed throughout the experiment to maintain attention.
2.6. EEG analysis
Raw data were converted to a compressed csv format using functions from EEGlab (Delorme and Makeig, 2004), and were then imported into R for analysis. We took the Fourier transform of the EEG waveform at each electrode, for a ten-second window beginning one second after stimulus onset (to avoid onset transients). Fourier spectra were averaged across four occipital electrodes (Oz, POz, O1 and O2), and across repetition, using coherent averaging (i.e. retaining the phase information). We then calculated signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs) by dividing the absolute amplitude in the signal bin (4 Hz) by the mean of the ten adjacent bins (±0.5 Hz in steps of 0.1 Hz). These SNRs were averaged across participants, and standard errors were calculated using bootstrapping. To plot the timecourse of SSVEP activity, we repeated the Fourier transform using a sliding one-second window (in steps of 10 ms), and scaled by the two adjacent bins (±1 Hz) to calculate the SNR.
3. Results
We used the results from retinotopic mapping scans with ring and wedge stimuli (Fig. 2a and b), and population receptive field (pRF) sequences (Fig. 2c and e), to identify primary visual cortex (V1) on flattened discs of occipital cortex for each hemisphere. Example flat maps are shown in Fig. 3 for one control participant (see the project repository for equivalent plots for all participants: https://osf.io/x9zr8/). The phase angle of the BOLD response to the ring stimuli, and the pRF eccentricity values, showed a typical central-to-peripheral gradient (Dumoulin and Wandell, 2008), and were highly consistent (e.g. across left and right eye pRF sequences). The phase angle across the wedge and pRF scans showed strong hemisphere/hemi-field segregation (i.e. the right hemisphere responded to stimuli in the left hemi-field and vice versa), as well as the expected phase reversals (Sereno et al., 1995) that were used to determine boundaries between V1, V2 and V3 (shown by the white triangles in each map - the middle triangle is V1). The V1 region-of-interest (ROI) was further restricted using the responses to a phase-encoded localiser stimulus (see Fig. 2d). These responses also showed strong hemisphere/hemi-field and dorsal/ventral segregation, and we retained voxels in the V1 ROI that produced responses with a coherence of 0.3 or higher (right-most flat maps in Fig. 3). This resulted in a mean total V1 ROI size (combined across hemispheres) of 601 voxels, and no significant difference in ROI size between patients and controls (t(20)=0.54, p = 0.60).
The localiser-restricted V1 ROIs for each participant were then used to estimate neural responses to stimuli of different contrasts presented to the two eyes (Fig. 2f). The BOLD response in this ROI had a typical timecourse (Boynton et al., 1996), which was modulated by stimulus contrast (see Fig. 4a). We fitted a general linear model (GLM) to the full timecourse to estimate a β coefficient for each stimulus condition at each voxel. The β weights were strongly modulated by stimulus contrast at the occipital pole (red shading in Fig. 4b). In a separate experiment using identical stimuli, we also recorded steady-state visual evoked potentials (SSVEPs) using EEG. These showed clear modulation of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) with stimulus contrast at the flicker frequency of the stimuli (4 Hz, Fig. 2g), but with a less sluggish timecourse than the BOLD response (see Fig. 4c). The responses were well-isolated in the Fourier amplitude spectrum (see Fig. 4d), and localised to occipital electrodes (Fig. 4d inset). We therefore used the fMRI β weights averaged across the ROI, and the SSVEP SNRs averaged across four occipital electrodes (black points in the Fig. 4d inset) to calculate contrast response functions for each experiment. Exploratory analyses in V2 and V3, and using different localiser thresholds to define the V1 ROI, produced very similar functions (not shown), as did using raw amplitude values for the EEG experiment (rather than SNR values).
Fig. 5 shows contrast response functions from both experiments, split by participant group (control participants in Fig. 5a and c, patients in Fig. 5b and d). The first contrast response function in each row (circle symbols) is for monocular stimulus presentation, and shows the expected monotonic increase for each data set. As predicted by our computational models (see Fig. 1), as mask contrast increased, the functions rose from baseline across the plot. Statistically, there were significant main effects of both target and mask contrast, and significant interactions, for all data sets (see Table 2). For the patients there was a very slight reduction of response in the amblyopic eye (orange circles) compared with the fellow eye (blue circles) at the highest target contrast in the fMRI data (Fig. 5b), though this was not significant (t(11)=1.20, p = 0.26, d = 0.35). The difference was more pronounced in the SSVEP data (Fig. 5d), and was significant at 24% target contrast (t(24)=2.91, p<0.01, d = 0.58), though not at 96% contrast (t(24)=1.96, p = 0.06, d = 0.39).
Table 2.
Data set | Effect | F-ratio (df) | p-value | Effect size (ω2) |
---|---|---|---|---|
Control fMRI | Left eye contrast | 18.82 (4,36) | <0.001 | 0.27 |
Control fMRI | Right eye contrast | 19.56 (4,36) | <0.001 | 0.22 |
Control fMRI | Interaction | 2.91 (16,144) | <0.001 | 0.18 |
Patient fMRI | Fellow eye contrast | 12.80 (4,44) | <0.001 | 0.14 |
Patient fMRI | Amblyopic eye contrast | 8.47 (4,44) | <0.001 | 0.14 |
Patient fMRI | Interaction | 2.92 (16,176) | <0.001 | 0.15 |
Control SSVEP | Left eye contrast | 29.93 (4,72) | <0.001 | 0.31 |
Control SSVEP | Right eye contrast | 27.21 (4,72) | <0.001 | 0.28 |
Control SSVEP | Interaction | 6.43 (16,288) | <0.001 | 0.14 |
Patient SSVEP | Fellow eye contrast | 34.88 (4,96) | <0.001 | 0.38 |
Patient SSVEP | Amblyopic eye contrast | 19.26 (4,96) | <0.001 | 0.17 |
Patient SSVEP | Interaction | 2.96 (16,384) | <0.001 | 0.04 |
For the control participants, there was evidence of interocular suppression (a u-shaped function) for the highest mask contrast when measured using fMRI (final function in Fig. 5a), though this was not statistically significant (paired samples t-test comparing 0% and 6% target contrast conditions when a 96% contrast mask was present, t(9)=1.8 p = 0.10, d = 0.57, mean difference of 0.13 β units). For the control EEG data interocular suppression occurred at a higher target contrast (96%, see final function in Fig. 5c), but was also not significant (t(18)=0.96, p = 0.35, d = 0.22, mean difference of 0.44 SNR units). The u-shaped function was more pronounced in the patients (Fig. 5b and d). For the fMRI data, suppression was more substantial when the mask was shown to the fellow eye (orange inverted triangles comparing 0% and 6% target levels with a 96% mask; t(11)=3.25, p = 0.008, d = 0.94, mean difference of 0.46 β units) than the amblyopic eye (blue inverted triangles; t(11)=2.51, p = 0.029, d = 0.72, mean difference of 0.22 β units). A very subtle suppression effect was qualitatively apparent for the SSVEP data (Fig. 5d), but this did not reach statistical significance for either eye (both p>0.05). SSVEP responses at the second harmonic frequency (8 Hz) were broadly similar to those at the fundamental (see Supplementary Fig. S1).
To further investigate the interocular suppression effect, we plotted full BOLD timecourses for the condition where the mask only was shown (96% contrast mask to one eye, 0% contrast target to the other), and the condition where the same mask was paired with a 6% contrast target in the other eye (see Fig. 6). Interocular suppression is clear in each data set, as the white points appear below the black points over much of the function. Our computational model (see right-most functions in Fig. 1a and c) predicts that this happens because the excitatory impact of the 6% contrast stimulus is outweighed by its suppression of the response to the 96% contrast stimulus in the other eye: overall activity goes down instead of up. However this effect is much more substantial for the fellow eye of the patients (Fig. 6b) than for the amblyopic eye (Fig. 6c), consistent with our finding from the β weights (Fig. 5b). The cortical meshes along the upper row of Fig. 6 show the difference in β weights between conditions, with blue shading indicating stronger suppression. Suppression is apparent at the occipital pole, and is again strongest for the fellow eye of the patients (Fig. 6b).
The lower two panels of Fig. 6 show polar plots comparing monocular SSVEP responses to stimuli of 24% contrast (Fig 6d), and 96% contrast (Fig 6e). There is a phase lag between the fellow and amblyopic eye in both panels. For the 24% contrast target, this is approximately 112°, which corresponds to a lag of around 78 ms at the 4 Hz flicker frequency used here. For the 96% contrast, the lag is around 28° (20 ms). This latter estimate corresponds well with previously reported phase lags using a similar paradigm in magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Chadnova et al., 2017).
4. Discussion
We measured neural responses to different combinations of contrast in the left and right eyes, using both EEG and fMRI. In participants with atypical binocular vision, we found reduced responses in the amblyopic eye using EEG, and increased suppression between the eyes (compared with controls) in V1 using fMRI. These different effects are consistent with greater tonic and dynamic suppression (respectively) in individuals with impaired binocular vision, and may be responsible for the deficits in stereopsis experienced by the majority of these participants (see Table 1). We now discuss why the results differ across measurement methods, what these findings tell us about amblyopic suppression, and how treatments might be targeted towards the development of functioning binocular vision.
4.1. Comparison of EEG and fMRI measures
This is the first study to use both EEG and fMRI to investigate contrast processing in impaired binocular vision. Previous studies using either EEG or MEG (Baker et al., 2015; Chadnova et al., 2017) have typically found larger amblyopic deficits in the monocular response than those using fMRI (Conner et al., 2007; Li et al., 2007) (though some work has shown substantial fMRI deficits (Hess et al., 2010a)), mirroring our results here (left-most functions of Fig. 5b and d). Considering only this previous work, heterogeneity of stimuli and participants across studies might well have explained the differences. However in the present experiments we used identical stimuli, and the same participants completed both experiments (Supplementary Fig. S2 shows the EEG data for only the participants who completed the MRI experiments). The reduction in response amplitude to stimuli in the weaker eye is clearer using SSVEP than using fMRI. It is conceivable that the absence of attenuation effects measured with fMRI might be a consequence of the smaller sample size (and hence lower power) for this experiment. Consistent with this possibility, the attenuation effect was also not significant for the SSVEP experiment when calculated only using the participants who completed the fMRI experiments (see Supplementary Fig. S2 caption). However, differences in dynamic interocular suppression were only apparent when measured with fMRI (right-most functions in Figs. 5a,b and 6a–c).
Analogous differences between these two methods have recently been reported in the study of both schizophrenia and attention. Calderone et al. (2013) also found differences in the steady-state signal-to-noise ratio between control participants and patients with schizophrenia, measured using EEG. But these differences were largely absent when the experiment was repeated using fMRI. Itthipuripat et al. (2019) found that spatial attention produces a change in baseline response when measured using fMRI, but not when measured using EEG (including evoked potentials and SSVEPs). Instead fMRI, which measures global neural activity indirectly via oxygen consumption, appears to be more closely related to late ERP components and alpha band oscillations recorded using EEG (Itthipuripat et al., 2019), rather than the stimulus onset transients detected by SSVEP and early ERP components. Although our contrast response functions do not involve baseline shifts, the other differences between our fMRI and EEG results are consistent with different features of neural activity being probed by these two methods. Further studies have found that attentional modulation measured using SSVEPs is weaker in amblyopes than in controls (Hou et al., 2016), which suggests a potential mechanism for signal attenuation in amblyopia. Indeed, superficially the pattern of attended and unattended contrast response functions closely resembles that for amblyopic vs. fellow eyes, reported both here (Fig. 5) and in previous work (Baker et al., 2015). The differences between imaging modalities across these studies (and in the present work) is consistent with the idea that SSVEPs are more sensitive than fMRI to gain modulations from subsequent stages of processing (i.e. top-down feedback).
One alternative explanation for the differences in monocular response between methods is that the firing of visual neurons responsive to the amblyopic eye might be desynchronised (Bankó et al., 2014; Roelfsema et al., 1994). This would have a greater effect on phase-locked SSVEP responses – which depend upon synchronised firing – than on fMRI BOLD responses, which are a proxy for overall neural activity. Instead, asynchronous activity in higher frequency bands (50 – 200 Hz) shows a closer correspondence with the BOLD response (Hermes et al., 2017), and these signals can be detected with extracranial techniques such as MEG (Kupers et al., 2018). In terms of the differences in suppression, this could reflect processing in different layers of cortex. Evoked responses measured using EEG correspond most closely to activity in the more superficial (supragranular) layers of cortex, whereas much inhibitory processing involves deeper (granular) layers (Bruyns-Haylett et al., 2017). Future work using laminar fMRI at higher field strength (7T or above; de Hollander et al., 2021), or multi-unit electrophysiology (Hirsch et al., 2003; Martinez et al., 2005), could allow dissociation of excitatory and inhibitory responses in amblyopia. In addition, these methods can resolve ocular dominance columns in V1, allowing eye-specific inputs to be measured directly.
4.2. What is amblyopic suppression?
Suppression in amblyopia is measured very differently in clinical practice compared with the methods used in lab-based neuroscience research. The most common clinical measures are the Worth 4-dot test, and Bagolini striated lenses, both of which give a qualitative indication of whether one eye's image is substantively suppressed by the other eye during binocular viewing. Lab-based measures involve a variety of paradigms, including psychophysical approaches such as dichoptic masking (Baker et al., 2008; Harrad and Hess, 1992; Huang et al., 2012; Zhou et al., 2018), or assessing binocular fusion of edges (Spiegel et al., 2016) or gratings (Ding et al., 2013), and more direct neurophysiological estimates of suppression in animal models (Sengpiel and Blakemore, 1994; Shooner et al., 2017). However, because both eyes are being stimulated during testing, clinical suppression could in principle be explained by either tonic or dynamic suppression, and most psychophysical work also cannot distinguish between these possibilities. Many of our patients (around half) no longer met the clinical criterion for amblyopia, yet as a group they still exhibited greater dynamic interocular suppression than our control participants, as well as tonic suppression of one eye. The finding that both types of suppression are apparent, even in individuals who have received patching or surgical interventions, strongly suggests that both will also be present in untreated amblyopes. Amblyopic suppression might therefore involve a combination of these two processes, and perhaps also be mediated by other factors such as attention (Hou et al., 2016).
One feature that remains to be determined is whether one type of suppression is a primary cause of amblyopia, and the other a later consequence. Cortical suppression, characterised as a process of gain control (Carandini and Heeger, 2012), is a dynamic, adaptive process that acts to optimise the sensory response (Westrick et al., 2016). It is clear that binocular vision is particularly plastic, as the relative weighting of the two eyes can be altered following a brief period of occlusion (Lunghi et al., 2011), and this may be the mechanism by which clinical patching treatment improves vision. Our finding of increased suppression of the fellow eye by the amblyopic eye could be an attempt to rebalance an asymmetrical system. If so, this might influence the development of novel treatments geared towards modulating interocular suppression.
4.3. Perceptual consequences of suppression, and potential for targeted treatment
The attenuator model outlined in Fig. 1b was developed to explain contrast detection and discrimination data in individuals with amblyopia (Baker et al., 2008). The finding that it also gives a good account of SSVEP data in the present study implies a mapping between neural and perceptual measures. Other work has shown direct correspondences between psychophysical discrimination performance and fMRI responses, linked using models of the same general design (Boynton et al., 1996; Zenger-Landolt and Heeger, 2003). Our fMRI data are less consistent with the fine details of the attenuator model. However, as demonstrated in Fig. 7 of Baker et al. (2008), psychophysical dichoptic pedestal masking data are relatively unaffected by variations in the balance of interocular suppression. The asymmetrical suppression we report here (see Figs. 5 and 6) is therefore not inconsistent with the levels of dichoptic masking data reported by Baker et al. (2008). We did not measure psychophysical performance in the present study, so are unable to make direct comparisons between perceptual performance and neural responses in our participants.
It is generally assumed that clinical suppression results in viewing the world ‘through’ the fellow eye, with signals from the amblyopic eye being completely suppressed. However, other work has shown that when signals in the amblyopic eye are boosted by an appropriate amount, information is still summed binocularly (Baker et al., 2007b). Indeed, the principle of ocularity invariance (that the world does not change when one eye is closed) makes it very difficult to distinguish between suppression and fusion outside of the laboratory or clinic. However, even if the amblyopic eye still contributes to perception, stereopsis is extremely sensitive to imbalances between the eyes, and breaks down when one eye receives a stronger input (Legge and Gu, 1989). The two types of suppression we identify here would likely unbalance signals in exactly this way, which may contribute to the poorer stereopsis for most of our patients (see Table 1).
Consistent with this idea, several treatments have recently been proposed that aim to reduce suppression between the eyes. For example, antisuppression therapy (Hess et al., 2010b) involves presenting dot motion stimuli, in which signals are shown to one eye, and noise distractors to the other. To perform the task, patients must favour information from the signal (amblyopic) eye. Performance improves over time, implying that suppression is reduced. This approach also improves acuity and stereopsis, even in amblyopic adults far beyond the critical period (in childhood) for traditional treatment. Related treatments that involve playing dichoptic video games (Backus et al., 2018; Li et al., 2013; Vedamurthy et al., 2015), or watching dichoptic movies (Bossi et al., 2017), may work in a similar way. Measuring the two types of suppression identified here throughout treatment would reveal causal relationships between suppression and visual function, potentially allowing clinicians to optimise treatment schedules and monitor progress.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Freya A. Lygo: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - original draft, Visualization, Project administration. Bruno Richard: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - review & editing, Visualization, Project administration. Alex R. Wade: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing - review & editing. Antony B. Morland: Conceptualization, Resources, Writing - review & editing. Daniel H. Baker: Conceptualization, Methodology, Software, Formal analysis, Data curation, Writing - original draft, Visualization, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare no competing interests.
Acknowledgments
Data availability statement
Data, experiment code (in Matlab) and analysis code (in Matlab and R) is available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/X9ZR8.
Funding
This work was supported in part by a Wellcome Trust (ref: 105624) grant, through the Centre for Chronic Diseases and Disorders (C2D2) at the University of York. FL was funded by a PhD studentship from the University of York. The sponsor or funding organization had no role in the design or conduct of this research.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to all of our participants for their involvement in this work, and to Robert Hess for helpful discussions on suppression in amblyopia.
Footnotes
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117780.
Appendix A – details of computational models
The two stage model of Meese et al. (2006) comprises an initial stage of monocular gain control, followed by binocular summation, defined as:
where m = 1.3, S = 1, and ωR = ωL = 1, CL and CR are the input contrasts to the left and right eyes. The output of the model follows a further nonlinearity:
where p = 8, q = 6.5, and Z = 0.1. The predictions shown in Fig. 1a are the output of the model for different combinations of CL and CR. To predict the effects of attenuating one eye's input (Fig. 1b), an attenuator parameter (Baker et al., 2008) is added to one eye's input:
where a = 0.5. Finally, to assess the impact of unbalanced interocular suppression (Fig. 1c), we set ωR = 2, but left ωL = 1 (and set a = 1). Precise parameter values, e.g. of exponents, are not essential to produce the overall model behaviour.
Appendix B. Supplementary materials
References
- Backus B.T., Dornbos B.D., Tran T.A., Blaha J.B., Gupta M.Z. Use of virtual reality to assess and treat weakness in human stereoscopic vision. Electron. Imaging. 2018;2018 doi: 10.2352/ISSN.2470-1173.2018.04.SDA-109. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Baitch L.W., Levi D.M. Evidence for nonlinear binocular interactions in human visual cortex. Vis. Res. 1988;28:1139–1143. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(88)90140-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baitch L.W., Ridder W.H., Harwerth R.S., Smith E.L. Binocular beat VEPs: losses of cortical binocularity in monkeys reared with abnormal visual experience. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1991;32:3096–3103. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Kaestner M., Gouws A.D. Measurement of crosstalk in stereoscopic display systems used for vision research. J. Vis. 2016;16(15) doi: 10.1167/16.15.14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Meese T.S., Georgeson M.A. Paradoxical psychometric functions (“swan functions”) are explained by dilution masking in four stimulus dimensions. I-Perception. 2013;4:17–35. doi: 10.1068/i0552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Meese T.S., Georgeson M.A. Binocular interaction: contrast matching and contrast discrimination are predicted by the same model. Spat. Vis. 2007;20:397–413. doi: 10.1163/156856807781503622. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Meese T.S., Hess R.F. Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vis. Res. 2008;48:1625–1640. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Meese T.S., Mansouri B., Hess R.F. Binocular summation of contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2007;48:5332–5338. doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Simard M., Saint-Amour D., Hess R.F. Steady-state contrast response functions provide a sensitive and objective index of amblyopic deficits. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2015;56:1208–1216. doi: 10.1167/iovs.14-15611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker D.H., Wade A.R. Evidence for an optimal algorithm underlying signal combination in human visual cortex. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:254–264. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bankó É.M., Körtvélyes J., Németh J., Vidnyánszky Z. Amblyopic deficit beyond the fovea: delayed and variable single-trial erp response latencies, but unaltered amplitudes. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2014;55(1109):1109–1117. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-13507. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barnes G.R., Hess R.F., Dumoulin S.O., Achtman R.L., Pike G.B. The cortical deficit in humans with strabismic amblyopia. J. Physiol. Lond. 2001;533:281–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0281b.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bossi M., Tailor V.K., Anderson E.J., Bex P.J., Greenwood J.A., Dahlmann-Noor A., Dakin S.C. Binocular therapy for childhood amblyopia improves vision without breaking interocular suppression. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2017;58:3031–3043. doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-20913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boynton G.M., Engel S.A., Glover G.H., Heeger D.J. Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:4207–4221. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-13-04207.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard D.H. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 1997;10:433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruyns-Haylett M., Luo J., Kennerley A.J., Harris S., Boorman L., Milne E., Vautrelle N., Hayashi Y., Whalley B.J., Jones M., Berwick J., Riera J., Zheng Y. The neurogenesis of P1 and N1: a concurrent EEG/LFP study. Neuroimage. 2017;146:575–588. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calderone D.J., Martinez A., Zemon V., Hoptman M.J., Hu G., Watkins J.E., Javitt D.C., Butler P.D. Comparison of psychophysical, electrophysiological, and fMRI assessment of visual contrast responses in patients with schizophrenia. Neuroimage. 2013;67:153–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carandini M., Heeger D.J. Normalization as a canonical neural computation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:51–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chadnova E., Reynaud A., Clavagnier S., Hess R.F. Latent binocular function in amblyopia. Vis. Res. 2017;140:73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2017.07.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conner I.P., Odom J.V., Schwartz T.L., Mendola J.D. Monocular activation of V1 and V2 in amblyopic adults measured with functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. AAPOS. 2007;11:341–350. doi: 10.1016/j.jaapos.2007.01.119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coutant B.E., Westheimer G. Population distribution of stereoscopic ability. Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 1993;13:3–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-1313.1993.tb00419.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dale A.M., Fischl B., Sereno M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Hollander G., van der Zwaag W., Qian C., Zhang P., Knapen T. Ultra-high field fMRI reveals origins of feedforward and feedback activity within laminae of human ocular dominance columns. Neuroimage. 2021;228 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117683. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delorme A., Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding J., Klein S.A., Levi D.M. Binocular combination in abnormal binocular vision. J. Vis. 2013;13(2) doi: 10.1167/13.2.14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ding J., Levi D.M. Rebalancing binocular vision in amblyopia. Ophthal. Physiol. Opt. 2014;34:199–213. doi: 10.1111/opo.12115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dumoulin S.O., Wandell B.A. Population receptive field estimates in human visual cortex. Neuroimage. 2008;39:647–660. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Engel S.A., Rumelhart D.E., Wandell B.A., Lee A.T., Glover G.H., Chichilnisky E.J., Shadlen M.N. fMRI of human visual cortex. Nature. 1994;369(525):525. doi: 10.1038/369525a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fu Z., Hong H., Su Z., Lou B., Pan C.-.W., Liu H. Global prevalence of amblyopia and disease burden projections through 2040: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 2020;104:1164–1170. doi: 10.1136/bjophthalmol-2019-314759. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrad R.A., Hess R.F. Binocular integration of contrast information in amblyopia. Vis. Res. 1992;32:2135–2150. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90075-t. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hermes D., Nguyen M., Winawer J. Neuronal synchrony and the relation between the blood-oxygen-level dependent response and the local field potential. PLoS Biol. 2017;15 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2001461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess R.F. Contrast sensitivity assessment of functional amblyopia in humans. Trans. Ophthalmol. Soc. U.K. 1979;99:391–397. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess R.F., Li X., Lu G., Thompson B., Hansen B.C. The contrast dependence of the cortical fMRI deficit in amblyopia; a selective loss at higher contrasts. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2010;31:1233–1248. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess R.F., Mansouri B., Thompson B. A binocular approach to treating amblyopia: antisuppression therapy. Optom. Vis. Sci. 2010;87:697–704. doi: 10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181ea18e9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hirsch J.A., Martinez L.M., Pillai C., Alonso J.-.M., Wang Q., Sommer F.T. Functionally distinct inhibitory neurons at the first stage of visual cortical processing. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:1300–1308. doi: 10.1038/nn1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hou C., Kim Y.-.J., Lai X.J., Verghese P. Degraded attentional modulation of cortical neural populations in strabismic amblyopia. J. Vis. 2016;16(3) doi: 10.1167/16.3.16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang P.-.C., Baker D.H., Hess R.F. Interocular suppression in normal and amblyopic vision: spatio-temporal properties. J. Vis. 2012;12(11) doi: 10.1167/12.11.29. 29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itthipuripat S., Sprague T.C., Serences J.T. Functional MRI and EEG index complementary attentional modulations. J. Neurosci. 2019;39:6162–6179. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2519-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jampolsky A. Characteristics of suppression in strabismus. Arch. Ophthalmol. 1955;54:683–696. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1955.00930020689010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkinson M., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E.J., Woolrich M.W., Smith S.M. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62:782–790. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Julesz B. Stereoscopic vision. Vis. Res. 1986;26:1601–1612. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90178-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiner M., Brainard D.H., Pelli D.G. What's new in Psychtoolbox-3? Perception. 2007;36(S):14. [Google Scholar]
- Kupers E.R., Wang H.X., Amano K., Kay K.N., Heeger D.J., Winawer J. A non-invasive, quantitative study of broadband spectral responses in human visual cortex. PLoS ONE. 2018;13 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Legge G.E., Gu Y.C. Stereopsis and contrast. Vis. Res. 1989;29:989–1004. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90114-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levelt W.J.M. The alternation process in binocular rivalry. Br. J. Psychol. 1966;57:225–238. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1966.tb01023.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Levi D.M. Rethinking amblyopia 2020. Vis. Res. 2020;176:118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2020.07.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi D.M., Knill D.C., Bavelier D. Stereopsis and amblyopia: a mini-review. Vis. Res. 2015;114:17–30. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2015.01.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li J., Thompson B., Deng D., Chan L.Y.L., Yu M., Hess R.F. Dichoptic training enables the adult amblyopic brain to learn. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:R308–R309. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.059. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X., Dumoulin S.O., Mansouri B., Hess R.F. Cortical deficits in human amblyopia: their regional distribution and their relationship to the contrast detection deficit. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2007;48:1575–1591. doi: 10.1167/iovs.06-1021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lunghi C., Burr D.C., Morrone C. Brief periods of monocular deprivation disrupt ocular balance in human adult visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 2011;21:R538–R539. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marcus D.S., Harwell J., Olsen T., Hodge M., Glasser M.F., Prior F., Jenkinson M., Laumann T., Curtiss S.W., Van Essen D.C. Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for the human connectome project. Front. Neuroinform. 2011;5(4) doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martinez L.M., Wang Q., Reid R.C., Pillai C., Alonso J.-.M., Sommer F.T., Hirsch J.A. Receptive field structure varies with layer in the primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8:372–379. doi: 10.1038/nn1404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meese T.S., Georgeson M.A., Baker D.H. Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold. J. Vis. 2006;6:1224–1243. doi: 10.1167/6.11.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moradi F., Heeger D.J. Inter-ocular contrast normalization in human visual cortex. J. Vis. 2009;9(3) doi: 10.1167/9.3.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pelli D.G. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat. Vis. 1997;10:437–442. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pratt-Johnson J.A., Tillson G. Suppression in strabismus–an update. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 1984;68:174–178. doi: 10.1136/bjo.68.3.174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roelfsema P.R., König P., Engel A.K., Sireteanu R., Singer W. Reduced synchronization in the visual cortex of cats with strabismic amblyopia. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1994;6:1645–1655. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1994.tb00556.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sengpiel F., Blakemore C. Interocular control of neuronal responsiveness in cat visual cortex. Nature. 1994;368:847–850. doi: 10.1038/368847a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sereno M.I., Dale A.M., Reppas J.B., Kwong K.K., Belliveau J.W., Brady T.J., Rosen B.R., Tootell R.B. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science. 1995;268(5212):889–893. doi: 10.1126/science.7754376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shooner C., Hallum L.E., Kumbhani R.D., García-Marín V., Kelly J.G., Majaj N.J., Movshon J.A., Kiorpes L. Asymmetric dichoptic masking in visual cortex of amblyopic macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:8734–8741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1760-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spiegel D.P., Baldwin A.S., Hess R.F. The relationship between fusion, suppression, and diplopia in normal and amblyopic vision. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2016;57:5810–5817. doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-20438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Travers T.aB. Suppression of vision in squint and its association with retinal correspondence and amblyopia. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 1938;22:577–604. doi: 10.1136/bjo.22.10.577. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vedamurthy I., Nahum M., Bavelier D., Levi D.M. Mechanisms of recovery of visual function in adult amblyopia through a tailored action video game. Sci. Rep. 2015;5 doi: 10.1038/srep08482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Westrick Z.M., Heeger D.J., Landy M.S. Pattern adaptation and normalization reweighting. J. Neurosci. 2016;36:9805–9816. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1067-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zenger-Landolt B., Heeger D.J. Response suppression in V1 agrees with psychophysics of surround masking. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:6884–6893. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-17-06884.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou J., Reynaud A., Yao Z., Liu R., Feng L., Zhou Y., Hess R.F. Amblyopic suppression: passive attenuation, enhanced dichoptic masking by the fellow eye or reduced dichoptic masking by the amblyopic eye? Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2018;59:4190–4197. doi: 10.1167/iovs.18-24206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data, experiment code (in Matlab) and analysis code (in Matlab and R) is available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/X9ZR8.