Abstract
Simple Summary
Working equids play an essential role in the livelihoods of millions of families around the world. The way people, especially their caretakers, perceive them affects attitudes towards them and consequently their welfare. This study aimed to understand the perceptions and attitudes of soldiers towards the mules they work with. For this, psychological constructs, such as empathy and pain perception, and discourse analysis were used. The results show that soldiers’ empathy towards animals is positively associated with their perception of pain and empathy towards humans. Soldiers prefer to work with mules over donkeys and horses, and perceive mules as intelligent and with the best aptitudes for pack work in the mountains, although they perceive them as aggressive. The text analysis shows that soldiers have a good understanding of mules’ nutritional, environmental and health needs but require a better understanding of their behavioral and emotional needs. Finally, they see mules as strong and noble animals, valuable to work under difficult field conditions and an essential component that supports army logistics in the mountain. Future selection and training strategies for soldiers should include behavior and welfare concepts to facilitate the soldier–mule relationship and improve mules’ welfare.
Abstract
Mules are essential for pack work in mountainous areas, but there is a lack of research on this species. This study intends to assess the perceptions, attitudes, empathy and pain perception of soldiers about mules, to understand the type of human–mule relationship. For this, a survey was applied with closed-ended questions where the empathy and pain perception tools were included and later analyzed through correlations. Open-ended questions were analyzed through text mining. A total of 73 soldiers were surveyed. They had a wide range of ages and years of experience working with equids. Significant positive correlations were found between human empathy, animal empathy and pain perception. Soldiers show a preference for working with mules over donkeys and horses. Text mining analysis shows three clusters associated with the mules’ nutritional, environmental and health needs. In the same line, relevant relations were found for the word “attention” with “load”, “food”, and “harness”. When asked what mules signify for them, two clusters were found, associated with mules’ working capacity and their role in the army. Relevant relations were found between the terms “mountain”, “support”, and “logistics”, and also between “intelligent” and “noble”. To secure mules’ behavioral and emotional needs, future training strategies should include behavior and welfare concepts.
Keywords: mule, animal welfare, empathy, soldiers, human–animal interactions, perceptions
1. Introduction
Equids have participated in the development of practically every aspect of human civilization [1]. There are an estimated 200 million equids worldwide (horses, mules and donkeys) mainly distributed in middle and low-income countries. These animals play a major role in different work types [2], either by load-pulling or pack work. The direct or indirect incomes generated by these activities support the subsistence of approximately 600 million people [3].
Working equids are considered as an essential component of livelihood and can be considered part of the natural, physical, social and financial capitals of people, increasing the resilience capacity of communities [4,5]. The social and economic contribution of equids to livelihoods can be direct (provision of transportation services) or indirect (plowing the land for obtaining agricultural products) [6]; thus, underestimating their impact could negatively affect society [7]. The main welfare problems reported in working equids include lesions due to poor harnessing, dehydration, hoof and shoeing problems, poor body condition scores and behavioral problems, such as aggressiveness [5,8,9]. Although the amount of studies reporting the effectiveness of training strategies, including control groups, is scarce, Stringer et al. [10] showed that a significant change in the knowledge of owners could be achieved by different types of interventions. Similarly, Degefa et al. [11] showed that the training of farriers resulted in improvements in animal-based welfare indicators.
Equids also remain being used as key elements for human military purposes [12], playing a major role as animals of burden, messengers and protectors [13]. They also assume a key role as means of transport during natural disasters, when the army needs to access remote areas to rescue people or provide supplies. Mules, in particular, have been essential in transporting equipment, soldiers and their supplies [14]. This role assumes a redoubled importance in countries, such as Chile, with a long mountainous chain that can be of difficult access for mechanized vehicles and even for horses.
Mules have been considered to be superior to horses for pack work due to their higher endurance capacity, the better quality of hooves, fewer feed requirements, and higher longevity/working life span [15,16]. In mountainous areas, they have a more secure step and a temperament that makes them an excellent pack animal compared to horses [16]. Studies in different countries have shown a predilection for mules over donkeys and horses, but mules are usually less available and thus considered more valuable [17]. On the other hand, they are usually considered more aggressive than horses, making them a difficult animal to work with [18].
The welfare of equids, in this case, mules, can be affected by how they interact with their caregiver, the knowledge the person has on husbandry practices and behavior, and also on how motivated the person is about working with an animal; therefore, positive human–animal interactions can potentially increase productivity [8,19,20,21]. How a person perceives animals can be influenced by knowledge and by the person’s affective states; this is important since perception can influence decision-making in relation to husbandry practices. Thus, recognition of pain and the capacity to be affected by and share the emotional state of another [22] allows taking decisions, such as providing rest, water, pain relief, among others. Perception of animals can also be influenced by previous experiences, culture, religion, and knowledge; hence perception can move from a totally instrumental view of the animal up to a more affective view. Training strategies should be designed considering these perceptions since it increases the likelihood of their success [23].
Text analysis performed using text mining strategies appears as a powerful tool that can allow extracting information from open-ended questions. For example, latent semantic analysis extracts and infers relationships of expected contextual usage of words in passages of discourse [24]. Word and passage meaning representations that result from applying LSA are capable of simulating diverse human cognitive phenomena, including discourse comprehension, analysis of interviews and free text surveys, quantitative literature reviews, and analysis of textual data, among others [24,25]. In spite of its potential, LSA has received little attention; in the field of animal welfare and human–animal relationship, it has been used to analyze how working horses’ owners perceive their animals [26].
The human–animal relationship is based on multiple events, allowing animals to memorize and, more importantly, predict future interactions, thus having important implications for animal welfare [27]. In the case of the army mules, handlers or caregivers constantly change in time, not necessarily allowing the formation of the human–animal relationship, and such fact could have negative effects on the mules’ welfare state. Soldiers working with mules are not responsible for ensuring the acquisition of resources for the mules since the army provides all resources. Moreover, they do not receive specific training in animal welfare aspects, such as mules’ behavioral needs and basic handling of them. The aim of the present study is to assess the perceptions, attitudes, empathy and pain perception of soldiers about the mules they work with. This will allow a better understanding of the type of human–mule relationship that they form while working together.
2. Materials and Methods
This study was approved by the Institutional Committee for the Care and Use of Animals and Bioethics (No.19263-VET-UCH).
A questionnaire was constructed using the Google Forms tool to assess attitudes, perceptions, empathy and pain recognition of a group of soldiers. The questionnaire consisted of six sections, with closed-ended and open-ended questions. The first section included an informed consent of soldiers. After its approval, demographic information was consulted, such as age, sex and level of training within the army. The second section included closed-ended questions on their attitudes towards mules, perceptions about mules and species preference (donkeys, horses or mules) at the moment of working with equids. The third section corresponded to the human–animal empathy scale [28], and the fourth to the brief form of the Davis interpersonal reactivity index instrument (B-IRI) [29,30]. The fifth section was on the perception of the equine pain scale (PEPS). The last section included two open-ended questions (a) “In your opinion, which are the most important resources that mules need to be able to work?” and (b) “What do mules represent or mean for you?” The questionnaire was shared with the soldiers associated with functions with equids, and 73 responses were obtained between March and April of 2020.
2.1. Assessment of Militaries Human–Animal Empathy
A modified version of the human–animal (H–A) empathy scale developed by Paul [28] was used. The instrument was previously modified for its use in Spanish and applied to persons related to equids and with low-level education [21], considering that these instruments need to be designed according to the population in which they are applied [31]. The H–A empathy scale applied consisted of eleven statements concerning animals, all of them suggesting empathic feelings. Response to each statement was in the form of a 9 points Likert scale, where 1 corresponded to “very strongly disagree” and 9 to “very strongly agree”. The final scores of the H–A empathy scale were calculated as the sum of the eleven responses of each respondent. Scores on the empathy scale can range from 11 to 99, with higher scores indicating higher levels of human–animal empathy [21].
2.2. Assessment of Militaries Human–Human Empathy
To determine the level of human–human (H–H) empathy, the brief form of the Davis interpersonal reactivity index (B-IRI) [29,30] was applied. The Spanish version of this instrument has been validated in Chile for university students [32] and already applied to horse owners in Chile by Luna et al. [21]. The B-IRI consists of 16 items that had to be scored on a five-point Likert scale, where 1 was “does not describe me well” and 5 “describes me well”. Scores on the B-IRI can range from 16 to 80, with higher scores indicating higher levels of human–human empathy.
2.3. Assessment of the Perception of the Intensity of Pain in Equids by Soldiers
A previously constructed instrument perception of equine pain scale (PEPS) was applied [21,33]. The scale consisted of 17 color photographs that showed equids suffering from a range of different conditions implying varying intensities of pain, including management procedures, infectious diseases and traumatic injuries. Soldiers were instructed to rate the photographs according to their perception of pain intensity with a five-point Likert scale, where 1 was “no pain” and 5 “maximum possible pain”. The final score for each respondent was calculated as the sum of the 17 responses. Scores for the PEPS can range from 17 to 85, with higher scores indicating a greater pain perception.
2.4. Data Handling for Closed-Ended Questions
Data were analyzed with descriptive statistics and then Spearman’s correlations were calculated due to its nonparametric distribution. The strength of the relationship between variables (very low = 0.00 to 0.19; low = 0.20 to 0.39; modest = 0.40 to 0.69; high = 0.70 to 0.89; very high = 0.90 to 1.00) was defined based on the Spearman rank correlation coefficient, according to Cohen and Holliday [34].
2.5. Text Mining of the Open-Ended Questions
Text mining includes a series of techniques that allow the assessment of nonstructured information, mainly texts. Text mining allows the discovery of previously unknown information within the text and transforms it into data for analysis. In this study, the following techniques were applied: preprocessing of data; term frequency; clustering; semantic correlations and latent semantic analysis. The Kamada–Kawai method [35] and the Calinski method [36] were applied for processing the cluster analysis.
In the determination and implementation of the different semantic algorithms of this study, the open-source software (RStudio 1.2.5, Ide development) was used, and the packages are detailed in Table 1.
Table 1.
Library | Version | Objective |
---|---|---|
Cluster | 2.1.0 | Cluster package |
Corrplot | 0.84 | Correlation matrix package |
Tm | 0.7–6 | Text mining package |
Ggplot2 | 3.2.1 | Graphics package |
Igraph | 1.2.4.1 | Graphics package |
Tcltk | 3.5.1 | Interface package |
Lsa | 0.73.1 | Lsa package |
LSAfun | 0.6 | Lsafun package |
RStudio | 1.2.5 | Ide development |
To predict the answer to the closed-ended question, “Do you like to work with mules?” (yes or no), the answer to each of the open-ended questions was analyzed independently through traditional techniques of machine learning. In this case, the random forest technique was applied. Then the results were analyzed using a confusion matrix (CM).
3. Results
A total of 73 soldiers responded to the questionnaire; from these, 98.6% (n = 72) were men, there was only one woman. Results are presented in sections according to the types of questions: closed-ended and open-ended questions.
3.1. Closed-Ended Questions
The median age of respondents was 36 years (27–54 years), and the experience with equids varied according to the species; most soldiers had experience with horses, followed by mules and then donkeys, but the range of years varied from no experience at all (0 years) to up to 30 years of experience (Table 2).
Table 2.
Mean (SD) | Median | Min | Max | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 36.6 (5.9) | 36 | 27 | 54 |
Years of Experience | ||||
Horses | 13.4 (9.8) | 14 | 0 | 30 |
Mules | 11.5 (8.4) | 11 | 0 | 30 |
Donkeys | 4.6 (8.6) | 0 | 0 | 30 |
Empathy | ||||
H–A (11–99) | 75.8 (13.5) | 77 | 35 | 97 |
H–H (16–80) | 51.8 (9.2) | 52 | 30 | 80 |
Pain Perception | ||||
PEPS (17–85) | 73.8 (7.4) | 74 | 51 | 85 |
H–H = human–human empathy scale; H–A = human–animal empathy scale; PEPS = perception of the equine pain scale. Means, standard deviation (SD), median, minimum and maximum are included.
From the total respondents, 89% (n = 65) said they did like to work with mules, while 11% (n = 8) said they did not like to work with them. Among soldiers, who did not like working with mules, the frequency of interaction ranged from weekly (two individuals) to once a year. In terms of training, 98.6% (n = 72) had received some type of training with equids at the army, and 69.8% (n = 51) had done some type of training with equids as civilians.
When scoring the photos according to the level of pain perceived (Table 3), conditions, such as open fracture of the tibia, articular capsule wound, laminitis with hoof growth and saddle sore, presented the lowest variance and the highest scores of perceived pain. On the other hand, hoof overgrowth, dermatophilosis, poor trimming and shoeing, and pectoral burn wound presented the highest variability, with pain scores ranging between 1 and 5.
Table 3.
Likert-Type Scale | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Painful Conditions | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | Variance | Median | Range |
Open fracture of the tibia | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 73 | 0 | 5 | 5–5 |
Articular capsule wound | 0 | 0 | 0 | 8 | 65 | 0.09 | 5 | 4–5 |
Laminitis with hoof loss | 0 | 0 | 1 | 3 | 69 | 0.16 | 5 | 3–5 |
Saddle sore | 0 | 0 | 2 | 5 | 66 | 0.17 | 5 | 3–5 |
Septic arthritis | 0 | 0 | 3 | 4 | 66 | 0.20 | 5 | 3–5 |
Subsolar abscess | 0 | 0 | 4 | 12 | 57 | 0.31 | 5 | 3–5 |
Mastitis | 0 | 1 | 4 | 16 | 52 | 0.43 | 5 | 2–5 |
Fetlock rope burn | 0 | 0 | 7 | 13 | 53 | 0.43 | 5 | 3–5 |
Evisceration | 1 | 1 | 4 | 6 | 61 | 0.57 | 5 | 1–5 |
Cannon and pastern rope burn wound | 0 | 1 | 14 | 20 | 38 | 0.69 | 5 | 2–5 |
Saddle sore | 1 | 1 | 14 | 27 | 30 | 0.77 | 4 | 1–5 |
Castration | 1 | 1 | 12 | 20 | 39 | 0.79 | 5 | 1–5 |
Hackamore burn wound | 1 | 13 | 31 | 12 | 16 | 1.13 | 3 | 1–5 |
Pectoral burn wound | 1 | 7 | 14 | 18 | 33 | 1.17 | 4 | 1–5 |
Poor trimming and shoeing | 3 | 4 | 13 | 17 | 36 | 1.27 | 4 | 1–5 |
Dermatophilosis | 21 | 12 | 29 | 7 | 4 | 1.36 | 3 | 1–5 |
Hoof overgrowth | 2 | 10 | 13 | 19 | 29 | 1.37 | 4 | 1–5 |
A significant but modest correlation was found between the pain perception of the soldiers and their human–animal empathy (p < 0.001) and also a significant but modest correlation between human–animal and human–human empathy (r = 0.408; p < 0.001) (Table 4).
Table 4.
Experience Horses | PEPS | Age | H–A Empathy | H–H Empathy | Exp. Mules | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pain perception (PEPS) | −0.1751 | |||||
Age | 0.3616 ** | 0.0431 | ||||
H–A empathy | 0.2058 | 0.4054 ** | 0.0782 | |||
H–H empathy | 0.0561 | −0.0294 | 0.1689 | 0.4080 ** | ||
Experience w/mules | 0.4364 ** | 0.0012 | 0.7291 ** | 0.0746 | 0.1134 | |
Experience w/donkeys | 0.3382 ** | 0.0518 | 0.3170 ** | 0.0687 | 0.1672 | 0.2860 * |
* p < 0.05; ** p < 0.001.
The number of training courses in different aspects of equid management varied from 0 training to 6 courses. The amount of training did not correlate to any of the variables, except with experience with mules, where those soldiers with higher experience with mules had a higher number of courses, although this correlation was low (r = 0.29, p = 0.012). The PEPS scores and the empathy scores (both H–H and H–A) were compared between respondents that had veterinary nurse training and those that did not. No significant differences were found for PEPS (p = 0.487), H–H empathy (p = 0.425) nor H–A empathy (p = 0.183).
Mules were the preferred species to work with by the respondents; soldiers also believed that they had a better aptitude for working in the mountains, for pack work and better physical resistance. Although they considered mules the most aggressive equid among the three options, they also considered them the most intelligent ones when compared to horses and donkeys. Donkeys were the least preferred by the soldiers, except aggressiveness, where they obtained the same percentage as horses (Table 5).
Table 5.
From the Following Species, Which: | Horses | Mules | Donkeys | All of Them |
---|---|---|---|---|
You prefer to work with… | 29 (39.73%) | 37 (50.68%) | 0 (0%) | 7 (9.59%) |
Has better aptitude for pack work… | 2 (2.74%) | 71 (97.26%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) |
Has better resistance for work… | 5 (6.85%) | 67 (91.78%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (1.37%) |
Has the best market price… | 72 (98.63%) | 1 (1.37%) | 0 (0%) | 0 (0%) |
Has better aptitudes for work in mountains… | 0 (0%) | 72 (98.63%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (1.37%) |
You consider the most aggressive… | 5 (6.85%) | 58 (79.45%) | 5 (6.85%) | 5 (6.85%) |
You consider the most intelligent… | 27 (39.99%) | 40 (54.79%) | 2 (2.74%) | 4 (5.48%) |
3.2. Open-Ended Questions
For the question, “In your opinion, What are the most important resources that mules need to be able to work?”, the recognition of the mule’s basic needs by the soldiers was analyzed. For the frequency of terms analysis, food, water and supplements were the most frequent terms included in the soldier’s answers. The term food was the most relevant, followed by a relationship between water and forage and a relationship between alfalfa and concentrates. These relationships were visible when constructing the words’ dendrogram (Figure 1). The cluster analysis resulted in 9 clusters, from which the first three are presented in Table 6, with the three main words included in each. The optimal number of clusters was obtained through the Calinski shortest dendrite method.
Table 6.
Clusters | Three Principal Words in the Cluster |
---|---|
Cluster 1 | adequate, attention, enclosure |
Cluster 2 | water, food, adequate |
Cluster 3 | food, care, shoeing |
The correlation analysis for this question shows a high correlation between “attention” and “paddocks” (r = 1), “attention” and “food troughs” (r = 1); “mule” and “personnel” (r = 0.9) “shoeing” and “harness” (r = 0.81), and between “deteriorated” and “harness” (r = 0.81).
The latent semantic analysis provides latent relationships, and not direct ones, within a text based on the construction of particular vectorial spaces. The asymmetry metric measures semantic similarity between words with a proximity in distributional space and identifies hypernyms [37]. The asymmetry metric for words of interest is shown in Table 7; the results show a more relevant relationship between “attention–load” (0.483), “attention–harness” (0.071), “attention–shoeing” (0.071) “food–water” (0.492), “food–supplement” (0.363) and “food–cleanliness” (0.343).
Table 7.
Word of Interest | Objective Word | Asymmetry Metric |
---|---|---|
attention | load | 0.483 |
attention | harness | 0.071 |
attention | shoeing | 0.071 |
attention | hoof trimming | 0.063 |
attention | health | 0.037 |
attention | grooming | 0.022 |
food | water | 0.492 |
food | supplement | 0.363 |
food | cleanliness | 0.343 |
food | quality | 0.326 |
For the question, “What do mules represent or mean for you?” the most frequent words were “animal”, “mountain”, and “support”. The dendrogram analysis (Figure 1) for these questions reveals the relevance of the term animal (in Spanish “animal”) and a relationship between the terms mountain, support and logistics (in Spanish “montaña”, “apoyo”, and “logística”). In a similar way, the terms transport, intelligent and noble (in Spanish, “transporte”, “inteligente”, and “noble”) are closely related. Applying the Kamada–Kawai method [35], the semantic network also showed a close relationship between animal, useful and support (in Spanish, “animal”, “útil” and “apoyo”). Using the Calinski criteria [36] for obtaining clusters, two clusters could be observed (Table 8).
Table 8.
Clusters | Three Principal Words in the Cluster |
---|---|
Cluster 1 | animal, support, capacity |
Cluster 2 | animal, army, support |
When applying the correlation analysis, a very high correlation was observed between “field” and “scenario”, “sick”, “evacuation”, “injured” and “valuable” (r = 1), a high correlation between “transport” and “operations” (r = 0.78), and between “path” and “Andes” (r = 0.72). A modest correlation was found between “unit” and “mule” (r = 0.62) and between “mule” and “Andes” (r = 0.58). When using latent semantic analysis, the asymmetric similarities shown in Table 9 were observed. The most relevant relations were between “animal–noble”, “animal–intelligent” and “animal–load”, while “animal–friend” obtained the lowest closeness and relevance index (0.212). The relevance of each word’s meaning is characterized by its loading on each of the dimensions in the latent semantic space.
Table 9.
Word of Interest | Objective Word | Asymmetry Metric |
---|---|---|
animal | noble | 0.692 |
animal | intelligent | 0.672 |
animal | load | 0.661 |
animal | mountain | 0.620 |
animal | army | 0.601 |
animal | support | 0.535 |
animal | equipment | 0.437 |
animal | loyal | 0.421 |
animal | friend | 0.212 |
The random forest technique, a traditional machine learning technique, was applied to perform a prediction process using the answers to open-questions 1 and 2. In this case questions, 1 and 2 were used to predict the response to the closed-ended question “Do you like to work with mules?”. For question 1, “In your opinion, what are the most important resources that mules need to be able to work?”. The model could predict with an exactitude of 86.7% positive answers but 0% for negative answers. For question 2, “What do mules represent or mean for you?” it was also able to predict the positive answers 80% of the time but could not predict negative answers.
4. Discussion
Mules are the least studied equids [38], but they have gained increasing attention in the scientific literature in the last decade in terms of their physiology and behavior. The aim of adding knowledge about the mule-human interaction is that the present study analyzed attitudes and perceptions of a group of soldiers towards mules.
Pain perception, human–human and human–animal empathy of the participants were assessed since these human psychological attributes can modulate the human–animal interaction and thus affect animal welfare [39]. The human–animal empathy scores obtained in this study were lower than those reported by Luna et al. [21] for working horse owners in Chile. These horses are usually considered as part of the family by their owners [26], and a closer contact can allow owners to better understand their horse’s needs and identify them. Mules have been described as routine makers and forming strong bonds with their caretakers, especially when they are handled from young ages [18,40], something not observed in the army. Temporal and handling routines generate a predictable environment for the animal [41]. Unpredictable environments have been described as a single acute stressor that disrupts normal activities, with negative welfare implications [42]. In the army, mules are randomly assigned to soldiers that do not necessarily use the same signaling systems when handling them or that can change the temporality of husbandry practices, such as feeding, reducing predictability and therefore, hindering the formation of the human–animal bond. This could explain the lower human–animal empathy scores found in the soldiers compared to working horse owners.
In terms of pain perception, the soldiers scored on average higher scores than working horse owners and equine practitioners from Chile [21,33]. Hackamore wounds and dermatophilosis were assigned the lowest scores with a median score of three out of five, conditions, which were also perceived as low pain conditions in horses by equine practitioners [33]. On the other hand, the highest pain scores with the lowest variance were assigned to the photographs of an open fracture of the tibia, scored as maximum possible pain by all soldiers and a wound with exposure of the articular capsule (mean score of five with a range between four and five. Pain assessment is difficult and requires an observer very acquainted with the species [43,44]. The lack of in-depth training in health-related issues, welfare and behavior, could affect the pain scoring provided by the soldiers. Not understanding the physiological processes behind these conditions could signify under or over-estimating welfare issues or not providing rapid treatment when required. Hemsworth and Coleman [45] indicate that stockpersons should be able to quickly identify any changes in the behavior, health or performance of the animals to seek support. In this study, the soldiers tended to overestimate the pain in some of the assessed conditions, thus increasing the probability of veterinary treatment, although this relationship needs to be evaluated in further studies. In spite of the greater pain perception, there is still a great variability for some conditions, similar to what was observed in other studies [21,44,46]. These findings could reflect a lack of experience in diseases of low incidence or insufficient training in health-related issues, welfare and behavior. As in Luna et al. [21] and Ellingsen et al. [44], significant correlations were found for human–animal empathy level and pain perception, indicating that individuals with higher levels of human–animal empathy could be more sensitive to animal pain perception. Moreover, human–animal empathy had a significant, positive, and moderate association with human–human empathy, indicating a relationship between both concepts, similar to previous studies [21,28,47]. Empathy is affected by multiple factors like gender, experience, education and animal species [47,48]; thus, someone’s empathy level cannot be associated with one single factor. For example, it has been indicated that the typical driver of a brick kiln donkey is an adolescent with very little experience in how to fairly handle an equid and with very little motivation in securing animal welfare [8]. By including empathy in the strategies applied to train these drivers, one could improve their motivation and their attitudes towards their equids, thus improving welfare. In studies with urban working horses (cart horses), it was determined that neither the socioeconomic status nor the quality of life of owners could predict the welfare state of the horses [5]. On the other hand, it has been described that owners with higher levels of human–animal empathy and higher perception of pain kept working horses with a better animal welfare index. Moreover, human–animal empathy explained 60% of the animal welfare state [21].
The majority of soldiers perceived mules as more aggressive than horses and donkeys and with the lowest market value. Nevertheless, when it comes to aptitudes related directly to their work, soldiers considered mules better for pack work, with higher physical resistance and the most intelligent ones among the three species, therefore, preferring to work with them. These results align with the literature where mules have been claimed to be the most important animal the army has ever had, with an important role in peacetime as pack animals [49]. The army transport mule has been described as temperamental, unfriendly and difficult to work with [49]. Army training manuals from India and Britain emphasized that mules must be treated gently if an effective human–animal working partnership was the aim and that rough treatment had been proved fatal for successful training [50].
This somehow portraits the special temperament of mules. Historically there has been a cultural bias against mules and in favor of horses provided in different contexts. For example, in the colonial times in America, the hierarchy between the Spanish elite, Mulato laborers and the indigenous workforce was rendered as an analogy of the hierarchy between horses, mules and donkeys [51]. In the same line, recent studies show that handlers frequently describe mules as difficult to handle and aggressive [18,38,40]. Contrary, in the present study, a negative bias was found for donkeys, and horses were only perceived as more expensive and less aggressive than mules.
The discourse analysis applied shows that the soldiers surveyed understand the needs of mules, especially regarding the nutritional, environmental and health-related domains. The cluster analysis identified three clusters from the answers given to the open-ended question associated with, “What are the most important resources that mules need to be able to work?”. Cluster 1 can be associated with the environmental domain, cluster 2 to the nutritional domain and the last one to the health domain according to the five domains model first proposed in 1994 [52] and recently revised in 2020 [52,53]. The same can be appreciated in the latent semantic analysis where the words of interest are “attention” and “food”, with the first one being associated with objective words associated with the environment and health aspects (load, harness, shoeing, health), while the food was associated with objective nutritional words (water and supplement). Coincidently, Pinsky et al. [54] assessed attitudes towards health and husbandry of working ponies in Indonesia, and their results showed that working horses’ drivers identified vitamin injections and appropriate diet as the most important needs, without considering behavioral or emotional needs.
The behavioral and emotional needs of mules were not represented by the soldier’s answers, somehow pointing to the lack of training in applied behavior and welfare aspects during the soldier’s formation process. Mellor et al. [53] highlight the importance of the behavioral interactions of animals, either directed to other animals, including humans or the environment, since when they can achieve their behavioral goals, then positive emotions can be enhanced and thus their welfare. Rodrigues et al. [9] also highlighted the pivotal role of understanding human–animal interactions and human behavior change in enhancing the welfare of working equids.
The answers to the questions “What are the most important resources that mules need to be able to work?” and, “What do mules represent or mean for you?” were able to predict in over 80% of the times a positive answer to the question “Do you like to work with mules?”, but were not able to predict negative answers. This could be due to the low frequency of negative answers in the studied group but should be further studied since it could allow a better selection of soldiers that will then be assigned to work with equids. Working with animals places different demands on people, and there is a need to identify the best attributes to meet the job requirements [55]. The soldiers will not only be exposed to adverse climate and geographical conditions, heavy loads, including uniforms and supplies that need to be transported, but also they need to understand the mules’ needs. Hemsworth and Coleman [45] propose several characteristics for a stockperson that would probably also apply to soldiers, such as general knowledge of the nutritional, climatic, social and health requirements of the animal; practical experience in the care of the animal; ability to quickly identify any changes in the behavior and health of the animals, and the ability to work independently and as a team for the care and maintenance of the animals. If some or all of these attributes could be recognized during the selection process, it would then facilitate the required training process in specific tasks associated with the animals and potentially benefit the mules’ welfare.
Finally, when asking the soldiers, “What do mules represent or mean for you?” the most frequent terms were associated with their work role in the mountains as pack animals that provide support and ease logistics. This can be seen through the very high correlation between words, such as field, scenario, sick, injured evacuation or between transport and operations. The soldiers seem to have a very instrumental view of the mules they work with. Similar results are shown through the cluster analysis, the dendrogram and the latent semantic analysis where the words animal–friend obtained the lowest closeness and relevance index. This is in line with the description of army mules provided through history, where they have been described as “the most important four-legged animal the army had ever had” or as a “splendid” means of transport and “source of general admiration” [49]. The words used by the soldiers to describe the mules they work with differ from those used by urban working horses asked the same question, where horses are viewed as a friend, family member and important income generation tool [26]. One important difference between the present study and previous ones studying perceptions towards working or pleasure/sport equids [21,56] is that, in the case of the army mules, the caretakers are not the owners and do not receive direct income from the mules. Such fact could be the main difference in the perception and attitudes towards them, resulting in a more instrumental and utilitarian view.
The present study is the first approach to understand the perceptions that soldiers have about mules. Nevertheless, it must be acknowledged that it has limitations, such as the sample size. Latent semantic analysis has been mostly used as a methodology for quantitative literature reviews and analysis of textual data. Although analysis of interviews is an area of application, more methodological development work is still necessary for this direction, being this an approximation to its potential use.
5. Conclusions
The present study shows that the soldiers have high levels of human–animal empathy, which is significantly correlated with their capacity to perceive pain in equids. Although they perceive mules as more aggressive than horses and donkeys, they have a higher preference for working with them, considering them intelligent and with better aptitudes for work. This could represent a more instrumental view of mules and explain that they can recognize mules’ basic needs associated with adequate nutrition, health and environment (housing), but there is a lack of understanding about their behavioral and emotional needs. Future selection and training strategies for soldiers should include behavior and welfare concepts since terms associated with these aspects did not appear in their answers. These are essential for allowing better human–animal interaction during work, avoiding accidents for both soldiers and mules and improving the welfare of the mules. This becomes even more important for mules’ case since they have been described as routine makers able to form strong bonds with their caretakers.
Acknowledgments
We would like to acknowledge the Chilean Army for the support given to implement this study and to all the soldiers that took the time to respond to the questionnaire.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, J.L. and T.T.; methodology, J.L., T.T. and M.R.; formal analysis, M.R. and T.T.; writing—original draft preparation, T.T., J.L., J.B.R. and M.R.; writing—review and editing, J.L., T.T., J.B.R. and M.R.; funding acquisition, T.T. and J.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was funded by Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo, FONDECYT Regular grant number 1191068 and Beca de Doctorado de ANID number 21180779.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the Universidad de Chile (protocol code No.19263-VET-UCH) on 22 May 2019.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study prior to responding the questionnaire.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to confidentiality agreement included in the informed consent.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Cunningham P.F. Topics awaiting study: Investigable questions on animal issues. Soc. Anim. 1995;3:89–106. doi: 10.1163/156853095X00071. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.FAO FAOSTAT. [(accessed on 1 February 2021)];2019 Available online: http://www.fao.org/faostat/en/#data/QA.
- 3.McKenna C. Bearing a Heavy Burden. The Brooke; London, UK: 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Pritchard J. What role do working equids play in human livelihoods, and how well is this currently recognised?; Proceedings of the 7th International Colloquium on Working Equids, Royal Holloway; University of London, London, UK. 1–3 July 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lanas R., Luna D., Tadich T. The relationship between working horse welfare and their owners’ socio-economic status. Anim. Welf. 2018;18:27–54. doi: 10.7120/09627286.27.1.047. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 6.The Brooke . Trabajadores Invisibles. Los Aportes Económicos de los Caballos de Trabajo, Mulas y Burros en los Medios de Subsistencia. The Brooke; London, UK: 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Tadich T.A., Stuardo-Escobar L.H. Strategies for improving the welfare of working equids in the Americas: A Chilean example. Rev. Sci. Tech. 2014;33:203–211. doi: 10.20506/rst.33.1.2271. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ali A.B.A., Matoock M.Y., Fouad M.A., Heleski C.R. Are mules or donkeys better adapted for Egyptian brick kiln work? (Until we change the kilns) J. Vet. Behav. 2015;10:158–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.12.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Rodrigues J.B., Sullivan R.J.E., Judge A., Norris S.L., Burden F.A. Quantifying poor working equid welfare in Nepalese brick kilns using a welfare assessment tool. Vet. J. 2020;187:445. doi: 10.1136/vr.106135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Stringer A.P., Bell C.E., Christley R.M., Gebreab F., Tefera G., Reed K., Trawford A., Pinchbeck G.L. A cluster-randomised controlled trial to compare the effectiveness of different knowledge-transfer interventions for rural working equid users in Ethiopia. Prev. Vet. Med. 2011;100:90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2011.02.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Degefa G., Negash T., Bishop S. Measuring impact on equine welfare from the animal and owner perspective; Proceedings of the 6th International Colloquium on Working Equids: Learning from Others, An International Colloquium, India Habitat Centre; New Delhi, India. 29 November–2 December 2010; London, UK: The Brooke; 2010. pp. 14–17. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Lawrence E.A. Animals in war: History and implications for the future. Anthrozoos. 1991;4:145–153. doi: 10.2752/089279391787057189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Nóbrega R.R., Duarte Barboza R.R. Ethnozooloy. Academic Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2018. What about the unusual soldiers? Animals used in war; pp. 323–337. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gorzoni P. Animais nas Guerras: A Força do Exército dos Bichos nas Grandes Batalhas. Matrix Editora; Sao Paulo, Brasil: 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sasimowski E. Animal Energy in Agriculture in Africa and Asia. FAO; Rome, Italy: 1984. Management and utilization of equine animals for work. (FAO Series, Publication No. 42). [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hameed A., Tariq M., Yasin M.A. Assessment of welfare of working donkeys and mules using health and behavior parameters. J. Agric. Sci. Food Technol. 2016;2:69–74. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Galindo F., de Aluja A., Cagigas R., Huerta L.A., Tadich T.A. Application of the hands-on donkey tool for assessing the welfare of working equids at Tuliman, Mexico. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2018;21:93–100. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2017.1351365. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Ali A.B.A., El Sayed M.A., McLean A.K., Heleski C.R. Aggression in working mules and subsequent aggressive treatment by their handlers in Egyptian brick kilns—Cause or effect? J. Vet. Behav. 2019;29:95–101. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2018.05.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hemsworth P.H. Human-animal interactions in livestock production. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2003;81:185–198. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00280-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kielland C., Skjerve E., Østerås O., Zanella A.J. Dairy farmer attitudes and empathy toward animals are associated with animal welfare indicators. J. Dairy Sci. 2010;93:2998–3006. doi: 10.3168/jds.2009-2899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Luna D., Vásquez R.A., Yáñez J.M., Tadich T. The relationship between working horse welfare state and their owners’ empathy level and perception of equine pain. Anim. Welf. 2018;27:115–123. doi: 10.7120/09627286.27.2.115. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.de Waal F.B. Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008;59:279–300. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Upjohn M.M., Pfeiffer D.U., Verheyen K.L.P. Helping working equidae and their owners in developing countries: Monitoring and evaluation of evidence-based interventions. Vet. J. 2014;199:210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2013.09.065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Landauer T.K., Foltz P.W., Laham D. An introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Process. 1998;25:259–284. doi: 10.1080/01638539809545028. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Evangelopoulos N., Zhang X., Prybutok V.R. Latent semantic analysis: Five methodological recommendations. Eur. J. Inf. Syst. 2012;21:70–86. doi: 10.1057/ejis.2010.61. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Luna D., Vásquez R.A., Rojas M., Tadich T.A. Welfare status of working horses and owners’ perceptions of their animals. Animals. 2017;7:56. doi: 10.3390/ani7080056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rault J.L., Waiblinger S., Boivin X., Hemsworth P. The power of a positive human-animal relationship for animal welfare. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020;7:590867. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.590867. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Paul E.S. Empathy with animals and with humans: Are they linked? Anthrozoös. 2000;13:194–202. doi: 10.2752/089279300786999699. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Davis M.H. Multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. Cat. Sel. Doc. Psychol. 1980;10:85–104. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ingoglia S., Lo Coco A., Albiero P. Development of a brief form of the interpersonal reactivity index (B-IRI) J. Personal. Assess. 2016;98:461–471. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2016.1149858. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Spreng R.N., McKinnon M.C., Mar R.A., Levine B. The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire: Scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic solution to multiple empathy measures. J. Personal. Assess. 2009;91:62–71. doi: 10.1080/00223890802484381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Fernández A.M., Dufey M., Kramp U. Testing the psychometric properties of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) in Chile: Empathy in a different cultural context. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2011;27:179–185. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Luna D., Yañez J.M., Tadich T. Level of agreement in the recognition of pain among equine practitioners in Chile. Vet. México OA. 2016;3:1–8. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Cohen L., Holliday M. Practical Statistics for Students: An Introductory Text. Paul Chapman Publishing; London, UK: 1996. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kamada T., Kawai S. An algorithm for drawing general undirected graphs. Inf. Process. Lett. 1989;31:7–15. doi: 10.1016/0020-0190(89)90102-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Calinski T., Harabasz J. A dendrite method for cluster analysis. Commun. Stat. Theory Methods. 1974;3:1–2. doi: 10.1080/03610927408827101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lenci A., Benotto G. Identifying hypernyms in distributional semantic spaces; Proceedings of the First Joint Conference on Lexical and Comptational Semantics; Montreal, QC, Canada. 7–8 June 2012; pp. 75–79. [Google Scholar]
- 38.McLean A., Varnum A., Ali A., Heleski C., Navas González F.J. Comparing and contrasting knowledge on mules and hinnies as a tool to comprehend their behavior and improve their welfare. Animals. 2019;9:488. doi: 10.3390/ani9080488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Luna D., Tadich T.A. Why should human-animal interactions be included in research of working equids’ welfare? Animals. 2019;9:42. doi: 10.3390/ani9020042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.McLean A.K., Navas González F.J., Canisso I.F. Donkey and mule behavior. Vet. Clin. Equine. 2019;35:575–588. doi: 10.1016/j.cveq.2019.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bassett L., Buchanan-Smith M. Effects of predictability on the welfare of captive animals. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007;102:223–245. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Reneerkens J., Piersma T., Ramenofsky M. An experimental test of the relationship between temporal variability of feeding opportunities and baseline levels of corticosterone in a shorebird. J. Exp. Zool. 2002;293:81–88. doi: 10.1002/jez.10113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bateson P. Assessment of pain in animals. Anim. Behav. 1991;42:827–839. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80127-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ellingen K., Zanella A.J., Bjerkas E., Indrebo A. The relationship between empathy, perception of pain and attitudes towards pets among Norwegian dog owners. Anthrozoos. 2010;23:231–243. doi: 10.2752/175303710X12750451258931. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hemsworth P.H., Coleman G.J. Human-Livestock Interactions: The Stockperson and the Productivity and Welfare of Intensively-Farmed Animals. CAB International; Oxon, UK: 1998. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Thomsen P.T., Anneberg I., Herskin M.S. Differences in attitudes of farmers and veterinarians towards pain in dairy cows. Vet. J. 2012;194:94–97. doi: 10.1016/j.tvjl.2012.02.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Calderón-Amor J., Luna D., Tadich T. Study of the levels of human-human and human-animal empathy in veterinary medical students from Chile. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 2017;44:179–186. doi: 10.3138/jvme.0216-038R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hazel S.J., Signal T.D., Taylor N. Can teaching veterinary and animal science students about animal welfare affect their attitude toward animals and human related empathy. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 2011;38:1. doi: 10.3138/jvme.38.1.74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Varnava A. The vagaries and value of the army transport mule in the British army during the First World War. Hist. Res. 2017;90:248. doi: 10.1111/1468-2281.12159. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Webb T., Pearson C., Summerfield P., Riley M. More than human emotional communities: British soldiers and mules in second world war Burma. Cult. Soc. Hist. 2020;17:245–262. doi: 10.1080/14780038.2020.1744879. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Schuerch I. Of horses and men, mules and labourers. Human animal semantics, practices and cultures in the early modern Caribbean. Iberoamericana. 2020;73:13–35. doi: 10.18441/ibam.20.2020.73.13-35. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Mellor D.J., Reid C.S.W. Concepts of animal well-being and predicting the impact of procedures on experimental animals. In: Baker R.M., Jenkin G., Mellor D.J., editors. Improving the Well-Being of Animals in the Research Environment. Australian and New Zealand Council for the Care of Animals in Research and Teaching; Glen Osmond, Australia: 1994. [(accessed on 1 February 2021)]. pp. 3–18. Available online: https://org.uib.no/dyreavd/harmbenefit/Concepts%20of%20animal%20well-being%20and%20predicting.pdf. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Mellor D.J., Beausoleil N.J., Littlewood K.E., McLean A.N., McGreevy P.D., Jones B., Wilkins C. The 2020 five domains model: Including human-animal interactions in assessments of animal welfare. Animals. 2020;10:1870. doi: 10.3390/ani10101870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pinsky T.C., Puja I.K., Aleri J., Hood J., Sasadara M.M., Collins T. A pilot welfare assessment of working ponies on gili trawangan, Indonesia. Animals. 2019;9:433. doi: 10.3390/ani9070433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Coleman G. Personnel management in agricultural systems. In: Benson G.J., Rollin B.E., editors. The Well-Being of Farm Animals. Blackwell Publishing; Ames IA, USA: 2004. pp. 167–181. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Geiger M., Hockenhull J., Buller H., Tefera Engida G., Getachew M., Burden F.A., Whay H.R. Understanding the Attitudes of Communities to the Social, Economic, and Cultural Importance of Working Donkeys in Rural, Peri-urban, and Urban Areas of Ethiopia. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020;7:60. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2020.00060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to confidentiality agreement included in the informed consent.