Abstract
Retrospectively obvious events are frequently missed when attention is engaged in another task – a phenomenon known as inattentional blindness. Although the task characteristics that predict inattentional blindness rates are relatively well-understood, the observer characteristics that predict inattentional blindness rates are largely unknown. Previously, expert radiologists showed a surprising rate of inattentional blindness to a gorilla photoshopped into a CT scan during lung-cancer screening. However, inattentional blindness rates were higher for a group of naive observers performing the same task, suggesting that perceptual expertise may provide protection against inattentional blindness. Here, we tested whether expertise in radiology predicts inattentional blindness rates for unexpected abnormalities that were clinically-relevant. Fifty radiologists evaluated CT scans for lung-cancer. The final case contained a large (9.1cm) breast mass and lymphadenopathy. 66% of radiologists did not detect breast cancer and 30% did not detect lymphadenopathy when their attention was focused on searching for lung nodules. In contrast, only 3% and 10% of radiologists (N=30), respectively, missed these abnormalities in a follow-up study when searching for a broader range of abnormalities. Neither experience, primary task performance, nor search behavior predicted which radiologists missed the unexpected abnormalities. These findings suggest perceptual expertise does not protect against inattentional blindness, even for unexpected stimuli that are within the domain of expertise.
Introduction
Despite our intuition that visual perception provides a faithful representation of the world, we often miss salient events in our environment when we are focused on something else. This phenomenon is known as inattentional blindness – the failure to notice an unexpected but fully-visible stimulus when attention is engaged in another task (Mack & Rock, 1998; Neisser & Becklen, 1975). The most famous example of inattentional blindness is the “invisible gorilla” (Simons & Chabris, 1999). When observers were asked to watch a ball game and count the number of ball passes between teammates, ~50% of observers missed a person in a gorilla-costume walking through the middle of the game and beating its chest. In homage to this work, Drew et al. (2013) inserted a matchbook-sized image of a gorilla into a chest computed tomography (CT) scan and asked radiologists to look for lung-cancer nodules. Under these conditions, 83% of radiologists did not notice the gorilla, demonstrating that sustained inattentional blindness can occur at high-rates in expert observers performing a task for which they are highly trained.
Although there are several known situational factors that affect inattentional blindness rates, whether some individuals are more susceptible to inattentional blindness than others is a substantial gap in the literature (Simons & Jensen, 2009). Many likely observer characteristics, such as working memory capacity and primary task performance, fail to predict which individuals will experience inattentional blindness (Kreitz et al., 2015; Simons & Jensen, 2009; Bredemeier & Simons, 2012). Consequently, some researchers have proposed that inattentional blindness is largely a stochastic process rather than a stable individual trait (Kreitz et al., 2015; Simons & Jensen, 2009). However, other studies suggest that expertise on the primary task may provide some protection against inattentional blindness. Basketball experience was associated with reduced inattentional blindness rates when viewing a ball-playing game (Memmert, 2006). Similarly, our prior work found that expert radiologists exhibited a lower rate of inattentional blindness than naive undergraduate observers performing the same task (Drew et al., 2013).
Experience may be associated with lower inattentional blindness rates because experts do not need to devote as many cognitive resources to the primary task (Fougnie & Marois, 2007). Specifically, inattentional blindness rates are predicted by relative task difficulty (i.e., effort) rather than an individual’s performance on the primary task per se (Simons & Jensen, 2009). Alternatively, experts may benefit from enhanced perceptual abilities or superior search strategies for tasks within their domain of expertise (Memmert, 2006). For example, enhanced global processing ability in expert radiologists allows them to rapidly detect abnormalities, which might make them more likely to detect the unexpected abnormalities (Carrigan et al., 2018; Evans et al., 2013). In previous research, the beneficial effects of experience have only been demonstrated in groups of experts relative to naive observers (Drew et al., 2013; Memmert, 2006). Thus, it remains unclear whether the observed individual differences are related to domain-specific perceptual abilities acquired through experience (Memmert, 2006) or a higher cognitive load in novices learning a completely new task (Fougnie & Marois, 2007). To address this question, it is necessary to determine whether experience predicts inattentional blindness within a group of observers that are familiar with the primary task. Furthermore, if expertise does protect against inattentional blindness, it is important to differentiate between the various factors that might be driving these effects (e.g., search strategy).
Our prior work suggests that highly unusual abnormalities (e.g., gorillas in a CT scan) may be frequently missed by radiologists if they are unexpected (Drew et al., 2013). However, because radiologists never need to identify images of gorillas in the lung in clinical practice, it may be that clinical experience protects against inattentional blindness to unexpected stimuli that relate to the primary task and to the observer’s experience. Inattentional blindness rates are lower for unexpected objects that are more similar to the observer’s attentional set (Most et al., 2001). This modulation occurs for objects with shared physical features (Most et al., 2001), as well as dissimilar objects from the same semantic category (Koivisto & Revonsuo, 2007; Most, 2013). Therefore, radiologists might be less likely to miss clinically-relevant abnormalities than unexpected stimuli that are unusual within the context of the task. Furthermore, experts are thought to have greater attentional capacity for tasks within their area of expertise, rather than enhanced domain-general abilities (Memmert, Simons, & Grimme, 2009). The perceptual expertise of radiologists may be finely tuned to clinically-relevant features, which might make them less likely to miss unexpected stimuli relevant to their area of expertise. In addition, an important characteristic of inattentional blindness is the unexpected nature of the secondary stimulus (White & Davies, 2008, cf. Ward & Scholl, 2015). However, radiologists are incentivized to identify unexpected abnormalities that are clinically-relevant, such as tumors, which could endanger their patient and lead to costly litigation. This makes inattentional blindness in radiology a unique perspective for studying the classic inattentional blindness phenomenon.
The purpose of this research was to provide a stronger test of the proposal that primary task experience provides protection against inattentional blindness. In Study One, we recruited a large sample of radiologists in order to examine the effect of perceptual expertise on inattentional blindness for stimuli that fit within the experience-set of the participants and the task at hand: detection of a breast mass and lymphadenopathy during lung-cancer screening. In addition, we performed an exploratory analysis to determine if radiologists who detected the unexpected abnormalities engaged in different strategies, such as searching the images more thoroughly, which would provide more opportunities for abnormality detection. In Study Two, we sought to establish baseline detection-rates for the secondary abnormalities when radiologists focused their attention on a broader set of abnormalities.
Study One
Method
Participants and Overview
Fifty-six practicing radiologists and residents in radiology training programs were recruited from the Radiological Society for North America meeting; a hospital in Salt Lake City, UT; and a hospital in Sydney, Australia. Six radiologists were excluded from the study due to equipment failure.
To improve the reliability and reproducibility of the results, the sample size, exclusion criteria, and primary predictions were pre-registered prior to data collection as part of a larger study: https://osf.io/aqkbd (Adibi et al., 2019). The pre-registered sample size was based on previous research on the effects of experience on inattentional blindness in radiology (Drew, et al., 2013). In addition to our pre-registered analyses, we calculated Bayes Factors to assist in the interpretation of null effects (Jeffreys, 1998). The study was approved by the University of Utah Institutional Review Board and the Macquarie University Human Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided informed consent.
Materials
Observers evaluated 7 axial chest CT scans (1 practice and 6 experimental) in a typical lung window/level. Three experimental cases were normal and three contained lung-nodules. The final case contained a large breast mass and lymphadenopathy in addition to lung-cancer (Figure 1). The mass was 9.1cm in maximum diameter and was visible on 17/66 slices. At its maximum diameter, the breast mass was approximately 30x the size of the smallest lung nodules. This case, obtained from clinical practice of author WA, was chosen because the abnormalities were deemed clearly visible using lung window/level settings. Our goal was to use a case with abnormalities that would be easily detected and unambiguous if the radiologist were explicitly looking for them. The remaining images and nodule locations were obtained from the Lung Image Database Consortium (LIDC) (Armato et al., 2011). The practice case had 8 lung nodules, and the abnormal experimental cases contained 14.33 nodules on average (9, 11, & 23 per case). The final, critical case had 11 nodules. For the cases obtained from the LIDC, ground truth was established by four thoracic radiologists that marked nodule locations prior to reviewing the anonymized marks of the other three radiologists and rendering a final decision (Armato et al., 2011). For the final case, author WA marked the nodule locations.
Procedure
Observers were asked to perform a lung nodule screening task and were instructed “to mark all nodules >= 3mm” by clicking on them with the mouse. The radiologists were not given additional instructions regarding any secondary abnormalities that might be present in the image. Observers were able to freely scroll through depth using the scroll-wheel. Responses were considered correct if a click was located within 30 pixels and 2 slices of the nodule’s center of mass. Cases were presented without any additional patient data and the radiologists were not asked to provide any additional diagnosis beyond marking the location of nodules until after the final case. The practice case and the critical case were always presented in the same order (first and last), and the order of the other cases was randomized for each participant. Following the final case, observers answered a series of yes/no questions on the computer to assess for possible inattentional blindness:
Did the final case seem any different than any of the other trials?
Did you notice any other medically-relevant findings on the final case?
Did the final case show signs of breast cancer?
Did the final case show signs of lymphadenopathy?
Questions 1 and 2 were presented in the set order whereas the order of questions 3 and 4 was randomized across participants.
The images subtended ~15.15° of visual angle and were displayed on a 17” laptop (resolution: 1600x900, refresh rate: 60Hz) using Matlab with Psychtoolbox (Brainard, 1997). Participants were situated on a chinrest ~89cm from the monitor. Eye-movements were recorded at 1000Hz using an Eyelink 1000 Plus, and the current slice was co-registered with each sample.
Analysis plan
We divided radiologists into groups based on whether they reported the unexpected abnormalities (i.e., a yes response to either question 3 or 4). We compared groups using a two-tailed, between-participants t-test. This analysis deviated from the pre-registered plan to use linear regression for this dataset because inattentional blindness is a binary measure. For each comparison, we additionally computed Bayes Factors (BF). A BF01 >3 was considered at least moderate evidence for the null relative to the alternative hypothesis, and a BF01 <.33 was considered at least moderate evidence for the alternative relative to the null hypothesis. Bayes Factors between .33 and 3 indicate there is insufficient evidence to merit a strong conclusion (Jeffreys, 1998). Finally, we computed a logistic regression to determine if experience predicted inattentional blindness while controlling for other variables (e.g., age). For those analyses, we reported the Odds Ratio and the p-value from the Wald Test.
Results
The final sample of 50 radiologists included 25 radiology residents in training, 1 fellow in radiology, and 24 attending/practicing radiologists. Of these radiologists, 20 (40%) reported chest imaging as a specialty. The average radiologist in our study completed medical school 12.35 years ago (SD = 12.58, range = [.50, 42]) and reported reading 41 chest CT scans in a typical week (SD = 52, range = [0, 250]). The average age was 41 years (SD=12.88). Trainees (residents and fellows) had 4.09 years of experience (SD=5.88) and reported reading 46.54 scans each week (SD=54.70), whereas attendings had 21.29 years of experience (SD=11.79) and reported reading 34.75 chest CT scans each week (SD=49.33). On average, radiologists detected 58.38% (SD=18.5%, range=[14.01%, 83.25%]) of the lung nodules.
Detection of Incidental Breast Cancer
When searching for lung-cancer, 33/50 (66%) radiologists did not identify a large, clinically-relevant breast mass (Figure 2a)1. These radiologists did not differ from radiologists who successfully detected the breast cancer in years of experience (t(48)=.18, p=.86, BF01=3.35) or lung-nodule detection-rate (t(48)=.35, p=.73, BF01=3.22), and there was weak evidence for no difference in the number of chest CTs read per week (t(47)=1.46, p=.15, BF01=1.45), (Figures 3a–c, Table 1). In a logistic regression, neither years of experience (Odds Ratio=.89, p=.24), age (Odds Ratio=1.12, p=.25), nor the number of chest CTs read per week (Odds Ratio=.99, p=.09) predicted breast cancer detection. Chest imaging experts were no more likely to detect the breast cancer mass than non-chest experts (χ2 = .53, p = .46). In exploratory analyses, we found some evidence that radiologists who detected the mass spent more time looking at the abnormality than those who did not (t(48)=2.23, p=.03, BF01=.48), but this did not reach our threshold for sufficient evidence (BF01<0.33) (Table 1). There does not appear to be a difference between the radiologists who missed the mass and those that detected it in case completion time (t(48)=1.44, p=.16, BF01=1.47) or the percent of the image examined (t(48)=1.12, p=.27, BF01=2.04) (Table 1), but the Bayes Factors indicate more evidence is needed.
Table 1.
Breast Cancer Detection | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Detected Abnormality | Mean | SD | 95% CI Mean Difference | p-value | BF01 | |
Years of experience | Yes | 12.79 | 13.34 | [−8.30, 6.96] | .86 | 3.35 |
No | 12.12 | 12.38 | ||||
Chest CTs per week | Yes | 55.41 | 71.53 | [−53.43, 8.57] | .15 | 1.45 |
No | 32.98 | 36.83 | ||||
% lung nodules found | Yes | 59.67% | 18.25% | [−13.15%, 9.25%] | .73 | 3.22 |
No | 57.71% | 18.86% | ||||
Time spent looking at abnormality | Yes | 2320ms | 4272ms | [−3423ms, −176.6ms] | .03 | .48 |
No | 519.8ms | 1359ms | ||||
Case completion time | Yes | 145.3s | 58.23s | [−57.10s, 9.38s] | .16 | 1.47 |
No | 121.5s | 53.89s | ||||
% of image searched | Yes | 49.36% | 19.05% | [−14.02%, 3.99%] | .27 | 2.04 |
No | 44.34% | 12.49% |
Detection of Incidental Lymphadenopathy
Thirty-percent (15/50) of radiologists reported no signs of lymphadenopathy (Figure 2a)1. The breast mass was missed more often than the lymphadenopathy (Fisher’s Exact test, p=.001). These radiologists did not differ from radiologists who reported the lymphadenopathy in years of experience (t(48)=.36, p=.72, BF01=3.14), and there was insufficient evidence regarding the number of chest CTs read per week (t(47)=1.42, p=.16, BF01=1.47), and lung-nodule detection-rate (t(48)=1.06, p=.29, BF01=2.11) (Figures 3d–f, Table 2). In a logistic regression, neither years of experience (Odds Ratio=.92, p=.34), age (Odds Ratio=1.08, p=.38), nor the number of chest CTs read per week (Odds Ratio=.99, p=.14) predicted breast cancer detection while controlling for the other variables. Chest imaging experts were no more likely to detect the lymphadenopathy than non-chest experts (χ2 = 1.59, p = .21). Observers who detected the abnormality spent less time searching the image than those who did not (t(48)=2.025, p=.0485, BF01=.66), but this effect did not reach our level for substantial evidence (BF01<0.33). We had insufficient evidence to determine whether there were differences in image coverage (t(48)=1.43, p=.16, BF01=1.47) (Table 2).
Table 2.
Lymphadenopathy Detection | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Detected Abnormality | Mean | SD | 95% CI Mean Difference | p-value | BF01 | |
Years of experience | Yes | 12.77 | 12.63 | [−9.28, 6.47] | .72 | 3.14 |
No | 11.37 | 12.86 | ||||
Chest CTs per week | Yes | 47.36 | 58.87 | [−55.77, 9.63] | .16 | 1.47 |
No | 24.29 | 22.09 | ||||
% lung nodules found | Yes | 60.19% | 18.85% | [−17.52%, 5.41%] | .29 | 2.11 |
No | 54.14% | 17.52% | ||||
Case completion time | Yes | 139.8s | 60.03s | [−67.62s, .23s] | .049 | .66 |
No | 105.9s | 36.83s | ||||
% of image searched | Yes | 48.02% | 16.40% | [−15.80%, 2.67%] | .16 | 1.47 |
No | 41.45% | 10.31% |
Study Two
In Study One, the critical case was chosen because the secondary abnormalities seemed clearly visible. However, it is possible these abnormalities would have been missed at high-rates even if the observers were not asked to focus on a narrow clinical task of evaluating only lung-nodules. In order to establish whether these effects are attentional in nature, we performed a second study to determine the baseline detection-rates for the critical abnormalities when attention was focused more broadly.
Method
Participants
Thirty-three radiologists were recruited from hospitals in Salt Lake City, UT and Sydney, Australia, by emailing radiologists who had participated in previous studies, advertising to radiology-oriented groups on social media, and through word-of-mouth. Radiologists that participated in Study One were not recruited to participate in Study Two. Three radiologists were excluded from the study: two did not meet the inclusion criteria (at least a first-year radiology resident) and one did not scroll through the CT scans before providing a response.
The study’s sample size, analysis plan, and exclusion criteria were pre-registered: https://osf.io/73en6. The study was approved by the University of Utah Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided informed consent.
Materials and Procedure
The study was conducted online using a non-mobile device of the radiologist’s choosing. Observers evaluated 2 axial chest CT scans (1 normal case and 1 abnormal case) in a typical lung window/level. The abnormal case was the critical case from Study One, which allowed us to establish a baseline detection-rate for each of the critical abnormalities. The order of the normal and abnormal cases was counterbalanced. For each case, observers were asked: “Which abnormalities are present in this case (check all that apply)?”: lung nodules, a breast mass, lymphadenopathy, pneumonia, pulmonary embolism, or no major findings. The order of the response options was randomized for each participant. Participants could scroll through the image using a scroll-wheel or the arrows on their keyboard.
Results
The final Study Two sample of 30 radiologists included 5 radiology residents in training, 5 radiology fellows, and 20 attending/practicing radiologists. Five (~17%) of these radiologists reported chest imaging as a specialty. On average, the radiologists had 10 (SD=9.90, range=[2, 43] years of experience and reported reading 47.6 (SD=68.17, range=[0, 300]) chest CT scans per week.
When asked about the critical abnormalities directly, only 1/30 (3%) radiologists missed the breast mass and 3/30 (10%) missed the lymphadenopathy (Figure 2b). Fisher’s Exact tests revealed the breast mass was detected more often in Study Two than Study One, p<.001, BF01=1.57e-6. The difference in detection-rate for the lymphadenopathy between the two studies was only marginally significant, p=.053, BF01=.22. However, the Bayes Factor met our criteria for sufficient evidence in favor of a higher detection-rate in Study Two than Study One. Observers also performed well on the lung-cancer detection task: only 1/30 (3%) radiologists missed the lung nodules. 29/30 (97%) radiologists reported there were no signs of pulmonary embolism and 16/30 (53%) radiologists reported there were no signs of pneumonia. No radiologists reported a breast mass or lymphadenopathy in the normal case. McNemar’s test revealed these reports were significantly lower than the abnormal case, both p-values<.001, which demonstrates radiologists only reported the critical abnormalities when they were actually present.
Discussion
Inattentional Blindness in radiology is not limited to missing an image of a gorilla embedded in a CT scan (Drew et al., 2013). Two-thirds of radiologists did not detect a clinically-relevant 9.1cm breast mass when their attention was engaged in an attentionally-demanding lung nodule-detection task. In contrast, only 3% of radiologists missed the mass when directly asked about these abnormalities. These findings demonstrate that inattentional blindness is not limited to completely unexpected stimuli (e.g., a gorilla in a CT scan): the unexpected abnormalities in our study were clinically-relevant and within the observer’s skill set. Critically, these findings suggest perceptual expertise does not provide protection against inattentional blindness: radiologists who missed the unexpected abnormalities did not differ in years of experience or overall task performance. Although task familiarity appears to modulate inattentional blindness rates to an extent (Drew et al., 2013; Memmert, 2006), this finding may be due to the relative difficulty of performing the task rather than domain-specific perceptual expertise (Simons & Jensen, 2009). Our results indicate inattentional blindness may be an inherent limitation of the cognitive system and expertise does not protect against this phenomenon.
Inattentional blindness may provide insight on how to address a growing concern in radiology: missed but clinically-significant incidental findings. Incidental findings are abnormalities in medical images that are unrelated to the patient’s symptoms or present in an asymptomatic patient (O’Sullivan et al., 2018). For example, a medical image taken to determine if a patient has a broken bone might incidentally reveal signs suggestive of cancer. Regardless of the reason the image was obtained, radiologists are expected to report all clinically-significant findings. Missed or incorrect diagnoses impact the quality of patient care and are the leading cause of medical malpractice litigation (Tehrani et al., 2013). A recent meta-analysis found that incidental findings are present on ~1/4 diagnostic images (Lumbreras et al., 2010) but are often undocumented, unreported to patients, and not followed-up for additional testing (Munk et al., 2010). With medical errors estimated to be the third most common cause of death in the United States (Makary & Daniel, 2016), it is vital to better understand the cognitive mechanisms underlying this source of diagnostic error.
Our data suggest the likelihood of experiencing inattentional blindness is strongly influenced by whether the unexpected abnormality occurs in a task-relevant location. The final case was chosen because both abnormalities were clearly visible in the viewing window used for lung-cancer screening. However, detection-rate for the large breast mass was significantly lower than the lymphadenopathy in Study One, which may be because the mass was located outside of the chest wall, whereas the lymphadenopathy was located within the chest wall. Clinicians may adopt the strategy of attending less to areas outside the primary organs of interest when engaged in a difficult task. This pattern of data is consistent with previous findings: unexpected objects are less likely to be noticed when they appear further away from the focus of attention (Most et al., 2000; Wood & Simons, 2019). Similarly, abnormalities outside of the task-relevant location, such as lung-cancer on musculoskeletal radiographs, are often retrospectively visible on images mistakenly classified as normal in clinical practice (Hawkesford & Kalogrianitis, 2015; Kim & Mansfield, 2014). This retrospective identification may be more akin to Study Two, when participants were specifically asked about different abnormalities. Under these conditions, radiologists’ detection-rates for the breast mass increased from 33% to 97%, and there was a similar effect for lymphadenopathy: detection-rates were lower during lung-cancer screening (70%) than a more general task (90%).
This research complements previous work on the common mechanisms of missed targets in visual search. Previous studies show a tendency to miss a second target after the detection of the first target (Berbaum et al., 1990; Tuddenham, 1962). Initially attributed to a premature termination of search, these “subsequent search misses” (Cain et al., 2013) remain a stubborn source of error (Cheng & Rich, 2018). In our study, radiologists who did and did not detect the abnormalities had similar task-completion time and image coverage, which suggests these misses also cannot be attributed to premature case closure. However, there are important differences between our results and subsequent search misses (Wolfe et al., 2017). Subsequent search misses are the failure to detect a known target, typically of the same type of target as the first (e.g., two lung nodules). Here, radiologists missed unspecified abnormalities that were not the focus of the search. This phenomenon meets the criteria for inattentional blindness established by Mack and Rock (1992): 1) The majority of observers failed to notice the abnormalities, 2) The abnormalities were fully visible, 3) Observers were able to identify the abnormalities when asked about them directly (Study Two), and 4) The abnormalities were unexpected, and they were missed more often when attention was focused on another task (Study One) than when it was not (Study Two). More research is needed to determine how the mechanisms behind these two types of misses are related.
There are some limitations to this study. The radiologists viewed these images in a laboratory setting where they knew their decisions would not affect any patients, and our inattentional blindness paradigm differs in important ways from clinical practice. Radiologists were engaged in the same clinical task for multiple consecutive trials before the critical case. Although common in screening tasks, it remains to be seen if the observed effects are influenced by prior task history. Our results are based on a single case that was only viewed in the lung-window, and more work is necessary to determine whether these findings generalize. However, we deliberately chose a case with a large, clearly-visible mass, and we anticipate detection-rate would be even lower for more subtle abnormalities. In future work, it will be beneficial to replicate these results in a clinical setting with a broader set of stimuli. Finally, Study Two was conducted online, which may not match the viewing conditions and observer state of mind of Study One. For example, the monitor specifications (i.e. resolution and brightness) may have differed between studies. Nonetheless, almost every radiologist detected the critical abnormalities, clearly demonstrating these abnormalities are visible and easily detectable when attention is focused more broadly.
Our research demonstrates the powerful influence attention can have on medical image interpretation. Even if an image shows clear signs of an abnormality, the clinician may fail to perceive the finding if it is inconsistent with the reason the exam was obtained. This result is increasingly important in light of the recent push for radiologists to focus their attention on specific tasks in order to reduce the number of non-clinically-relevant incidental findings reported in radiology (Barry, 2014). This research has important implications for considering how to best present relevant information (e.g., patient history) to the clinician and how to evaluate whether ‘standard of care’ has been violated in medical malpractice cases. If most radiologists do not detect a large, clinically-relevant cancer while searching for a different type of abnormality, we should acknowledge that the concept of ‘standard of care’ may need to be expanded to include the exam indication and the instructions provided. However, these results should not be interpreted as evidence that it would be better not to provide patient information. Our results suggest the information given to the radiologist may have a strong influence on which abnormalities they will ultimately detect. Therefore, relevant patient information should be helpful to radiologists when the abnormalities are consistent with the exam indication. In fact, a large body of research demonstrates that clinical history improves sensitivity for relevant abnormalities (Loy & Irwig, 2004). Rather than providing no information, it may be helpful to focus on interventions, such as checklists (Thomassen et al., 2014), that help clinicians focus attention on one task at a time rather than trying to divide attention between multiple competing goals.
Our findings provide a unique perspective on classic inattentional blindness, demonstrating it occurs when the unexpected stimulus is by no means unusual within the context, and that perceptual expertise cannot prevent it. In sum, this is a vivid illustration of Mack and Rock’s assertion that, even with trained experts viewing an image within their area of expertise, “there is no perception without attention” (Mack & Rock, 1998).
Open Practices Statement.
Both studies were pre-registered on the Open Science Framework (Study One: https://osf.io/aqkbd, Study Two: https://osf.io/73en6). The data have not been made available on a permanent third-party archive; requests for the data can be sent via email to the lead author. The experiment script and expertise questionnaire can be found at https://osf.io/je8tm/?view_only=927401fb4b28449fb31bd665fcb4acb5.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank the radiologists that participated in our studies, the National Cancer Institute’s Perception Lab at the annual meeting of the Radiological Society of North America, Daniel J. Williams for creating the online CT viewer in Study Two, and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments during the review process.
Footnotes
Of the 17 radiologists that reported the breast mass, 1 responded ‘no’ to question 1 (‘Did the final case seem any different than any of the other trials?’) only, 1 responded ‘no’ to question 2 (‘Did you notice any other medically-relevant findings on the final case?’) only, and 1 responded ‘no’ to both questions. Of the 35 radiologists that reported lymphadenopathy, 3 responded ‘no’ to question 1 only, 2 responded ‘no’ to question 2 only, and 1 responded ‘no’ to both questions. Although it is impossible to know for certain based on a ‘yes or no’ response to these questions, this pattern of responses may reflect a demand characteristic for a small number of participants when asked directly about specific abnormalities.
References
- Adibi A, Sin D, & Sadatsafavi M (2019). Lowering the P Value Threshold. JAMA, 321(15), 1532–1533. 10.1001/jama.2019.0566 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armato SG, McLennan G, Bidaut L, McNitt-Gray MF, Meyer CR, Reeves AP, Zhao B, Aberle DR, Henschke CI, Hoffman EA, Kazerooni EA, MacMahon H, Van Beeke EJR, Yankelevitz D, Biancardi AM, Bland PH, Brown MS, Engelmann RM, Laderach GE, … Croft BY (2011). The Lung Image Database Consortium (LIDC) and Image Database Resource Initiative (IDRI): A completed reference database of lung nodules on CT scans. Medical Physics, 38(2), 915–931. 10.1118/1.3528204 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barry MJ (2014). Incidentaloma Fatigue. JAMA Internal Medicine, 174(6), 851–852. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2014.672 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berbaum KS, Franken JE, Dorfman DD, Rooholamini SA, Kathol MH, & Barloon TJ (1990). Satisfaction of search in diagnostic radiology. Investigative Radiology, 25. 10.1097/00004424-199002000-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brainard DH (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10(4), 433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bredemeier K, & Simons DJ (2012). Working memory and inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(2), 239–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cain MS, Adamo SH, & Mitroff SR (2013). A taxonomy of errors in multiple-target visual search. Visual Cognition, 21(7), 899–921. [Google Scholar]
- Carrigan AJ, Wardle SG, & Rich AN (2018). Finding cancer in mammograms: If you know it’s there, do you know where? Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng P. (Xin), & Rich AN. (2018). More is better: Relative prevalence of multiple targets affects search accuracy. Journal of Vision, 18(4), 2–2. 10.1167/18.4.2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drew T, Evans K, Võ MLH, Jacobson FL, & Wolfe JM (2013). Informatics in radiology: What can you see in a single glance and how might this guide visual search in medical images? Radiographics, 33. 10.1148/rg.331125023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drew T, Võ MLH, & Wolfe JM (2013). The invisible gorilla strikes again: Sustained inattentional blindness in expert observers. Psychological Science, 24. 10.1177/0956797613479386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans KK, Georgian-Smith D, Tambouret R, Birdwell RL, & Wolfe JM (2013). The gist of the abnormal: Above-chance medical decision making in the blink of an eye. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20. 10.3758/s13423-013-0459-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fougnie D, & Marois R (2007). Executive working memory load induces inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(1), 142–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkesford MPH, & Kalogrianitis S (2015). Delayed diagnosis of lung cancer after missed vertebral metastasis on CT. BJR Case Reports, 1(1). 10.1259/bjrcr.20140001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hitzeman N, Cotton E, & Center SM (2014). Incidentalomas: Initial Management. 90(11), 6. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeffreys H (1998). The theory of probability. OUP Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- Kim YW, & Mansfield LT (2014). Fool Me Twice: Delayed Diagnoses in Radiology With Emphasis on Perpetuated Errors. American Journal of Roentgenology, 202(3), 465–470. 10.2214/AJR.13.11493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koivisto M, & Revonsuo A (2007). How Meaning Shapes Seeing. Psychological Science, 18(10), 845–849. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01989.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreitz C, Furley P, Memmert D, & Simons DJ (2015). Inattentional Blindness and Individual Differences in Cognitive Abilities. PLOS ONE, 10(8), e0134675. 10.1371/journal.pone.0134675 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loy CT, & Irwig L (2004). Accuracy of diagnostic tests read with and without clinical information: A systematic review. Jama, 292(13), 1602–1609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lum TE, Fairbanks RJ, Pennington EC, & Zwemer FL (2005). Profiles in patient safety: Misplaced femoral line guidewire and multiple failures to detect the foreign body on chest radiography. Academic Emergency Medicine, 12(7), 658–662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lumbreras B, Donat L, & Hernández-Aguado I (2010). Incidental findings in imaging diagnostic tests: A systematic review. The British Journal of Radiology, 83(988), 276–289. 10.1259/bjr/98067945 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mack A, & Rock, 1998. (1998). Inattentions! blindness. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Makary MA, & Daniel M (2016). Medical error—The third leading cause of death in the US. BMJ, 353, i2139. 10.1136/bmj.i2139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Memmert D (2006). The effects of eye movements, age, and expertise on inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 620–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Memmert D, Simons DJ, & Grimme T (2009). The relationship between visual attention and expertise in sports. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10(1), 146–151. [Google Scholar]
- Most SB (2013). Setting sights higher: Category-level attentional set modulates sustained inattentional blindness. Psychological Research, 77(2), 139–146. 10.1007/s00426-011-0379-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Most SB, Simons DJ, Scholl BJ, & Chabris CF (2000). Sustained inattentional blindness. Psyche, 6, 14. [Google Scholar]
- Most SB, Simons DJ, Scholl BJ, Jimenez R, Clifford E, & Chabris CF (2001). How not to be Seen: The Contribution of Similarity and Selective Ignoring to Sustained Inattentional Blindness. Psychological Science, 12(1), 9–17. 10.1111/1467-9280.00303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Munk M-D, Peitzman AB, Hostler DP, & Wolfson AB (2010). Frequency and follow-up of incidental findings on trauma computed tomography scans: Experience at a level one trauma center. The Journal of Emergency Medicine, 38(3), 346–350. 10.1016/j.jemermed.2008.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neisser U, & Becklen R (1975). Selective looking: Attending to visually specified events. Cognitive Psychology Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 480–494. /z-wcorg/. [Google Scholar]
- O’Sullivan JW, Muntinga T, Grigg S, & Ioannidis JPA (2018). Prevalence and outcomes of incidental imaging findings: Umbrella review. BMJ, 361, k2387. 10.1136/bmj.k2387 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons DJ, & Chabris CF (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074. 10.1068/p281059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simons Daniel J., & Jensen MS. (2009). The effects of individual differences and task difficulty on inattentional blindness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 398–403. 10.3758/PBR.16.2.398 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tehrani ASS, Lee H, Mathews SC, Shore A, Makary MA, Pronovost PJ, & Newman-Toker DE (2013). 25-Year summary of US malpractice claims for diagnostic errors 1986–2010: An analysis from the National Practitioner Data Bank. BMJ Quality & Safety, 22(8), 672–680. 10.1136/bmjqs-2012-001550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomassen Ø, Storesund A, Søfteland E, & Brattebø G (2014). The effects of safety checklists in medicine: A systematic review. Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica, 58(1), 5–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tuddenham WJ (1962). Visual Search, Image Organization, and Reader Error in Roentgen Diagnosis: Studies of the Psychophysiology of Roentgen Image Perception Memorial Fund Lecture. Radiology, 78(5), 694–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Gijp A, Ravesloot CJ, Jarodzka H, van der Schaaf MF, van der Schaaf IC, van Schaik JPJ, & Ten Cate TJ (2017). How visual search relates to visual diagnostic performance: A narrative systematic review of eye-tracking research in radiology. Advances in Health Sciences Education: Theory and Practice, 22(3), 765–787. 10.1007/s10459-016-9698-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward EJ, & Scholl BJ (2015). Inattentional blindness reflects limitations on perception, not memory: Evidence from repeated failures of awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(3), 722–727. 10.3758/s13423-014-0745-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White RC, & Davies AA (2008). Attention set for number: Expectation and perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe JM, Evans KK, Drew T, Aizenman A, & Josephs E (2016). How do radiologists use the human search engine? Radiation Protection Dosimetry, 169. 10.1093/rpd/ncv501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfe JM, Soce AA, & Schill HM (2017). How did I miss that? Developing mixed hybrid visual search as a ‘model system’for incidental finding errors in radiology. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood K, & Simons DJ (2019). The spatial allocation of attention in an interactive environment. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 4(1), 13. 10.1186/s41235-019-0164-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]