Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychiatry logoLink to Frontiers in Psychiatry
. 2021 Apr 14;12:638088. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.638088

Association of Birth Order With Mental Health Problems, Self-Esteem, Resilience, and Happiness Among Children: Results From A-CHILD Study

Yoshifumi Fukuya 1, Takeo Fujiwara 1,*, Aya Isumi 1, Satomi Doi 1, Manami Ochi 2
PMCID: PMC8079750  PMID: 33935832

Abstract

Objective: This study aimed to investigate the association of birth order with mental health problems, self-esteem, resilience, and happiness among children aged 9–10 years.

Methods: This was a cross-sectional study using data from the Adachi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (A-CHILD) study, which was a population-based study of children in fourth grade in public schools in Adachi City, Tokyo, Japan in 2018 (N = 3,744). Parent-rated Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and self-rated resilience, happiness, and self-esteem score were used to assess child mental health. Multiple regression analysis for continuous outcomes and logistic regression for dichotomous outcomes were used to examine the association of birth order with mental health problems, resilience, happiness, and self-esteem. The analysis was controlled for child sex, mother's age, mother's education, caregiver's depressive symptoms, household income, and living with grandparents.

Results: Last-borns showed the lowest total difficulties score in the SDQ, while only children showed the highest (p < 0.001). The highest prosocial behaviors score was found among last-borns (p < 0.001). Resilience score was also highest among last-borns, followed by first-borns, middle-borns, and only children. The lowest happiness score was found among middle-borns. Self-esteem score did not differ by sibling types. These associations were similar in the adjusted model and dichotomized outcomes model.

Conclusions: Differential impacts of birth order on child mental health, for both positive and negative sides, were found. Further research is warranted to elucidate the mechanism of the association between birth order and the development of behavior problems and the positive aspects such as resilience, happiness, and self-esteem among children.

Keywords: birth order, mental health, resilience, happiness, self-esteem

Introduction

There is an increasing number of mental health problems among adolescents, with 10–20% of them estimated to suffer from these problems in the world (1). In addition, half of the cases of lifetime mental health problems begin by the age of 14 (2). Japan is not exception; for example, the number of suicide among children and adolescents has gradually increased over a decade (3), ranked second in Asia, followed by Korea (4). To reveal this situation, it is crucial to understand risk and protective factors for prevention of mental illness in adolescence and adulthood.

Previous research has shown that later-born children aged 7–12 years in the US have been found to have a higher level of depression and anxiety (5). A population-based study in the UK has shown that later-born adolescents are more likely to have suicide attempts and psychiatric problems (6). Similarly, prior register-based studies have reported that later-born adults aged 16 years are at a higher risk of suicidal behavior in Norway and Sweden (7, 8). These findings from Western countries have indicated later-born have an increased risk of mental health problems. There are several considerations underling the mechanisms of the associations. These include later-born children being more likely to have limited interactions with parents, less parental attention, and diluted available home resources (9, 10). These circumstances have adverse effects on mental health throughout sensitive development periods in childhood, which might last across the life course. To date, however, few epidemiological studies have examined whether birth order is associated with mental health problems among children, particularly, pre-adolescent children. Further, most of the previous studies were conducted in Western countries. There is a lack of empirical studies to investigate the associations in non-Western countries, including Japan.

Positive mental health refers to a state of subjective well-being and functioning well (11). Resilience (12) and self-esteem (13) can be considered important aspects of positive mental health (1416). Further, happiness is known to be one of the indicators based on an individual's hedonistic view or emotional well-being, which suggests positive affectivity and satisfaction with life (17). Previous studies in the US have indicated that first-born adolescents are more likely to have higher self-esteem than other siblings and only children (18), whereas middle-born adolescent males are found to have lower self-esteem (19). Moreover, other research has reported that last-born and only children are happier than first-and middle-born among US young children (20). However, it is still unknown whether these findings in the US enable to be generalized to any other population with different sociocultural characteristics, such as Japan. Indeed, the association of birth order with positive aspects in non-Western countries remains unknown due to the lack of research. Furthermore, limited population-based studies focusing on children have been conducted to examine the association.

Japan has unique sociocultural characteristics affecting child-rearing. Japan had the i.e., system prescribed a patriarchal and primogeniture system in place, which was abolished in 1947. Nonetheless, the i.e., system still influences family relationships to some extent (21, 22). In the familial contexts, Japanese parents may assign a perceived ideal role to their children within a family according to the birth order; in particular, the parents require the older siblings to behave more maturely and responsibly, including taking care of younger siblings (23). Second, Japan has a specific concept of parent-child relationships, known as amae (24). Amae refers to the feeling of dependence on others and the presumption on others' acceptance and indulgence (24, 25), which is somewhat similar to the secure base of attachment in terms of needs for closeness and security (22, 26, 27). Amae has influenced Japanese values, which makes a difference in child-rearing practice, compared to other countries. For example, Japanese parents tend to indulge their children and have closer physical proximity to them (23, 28), whereas parents in Western countries are likely to encourage their children to be independent and behave autonomously from an early age (2931). Furthermore, according to the i.e., system and amae, Japanese parents may modify their child-rearing practice according to birth order (32, 33). Indeed, first-borns are more likely to be subjected to stricter upbringing, whereas last-born is to be well-taken care of by their parents and older siblings (3234). Given the differences in parenting practice between siblings, the unique sociocultural characteristics may influence the association of birth order with child development and mental health. A prior study among pre-school children in Japan reported that birth order was associated with developing children's self-reliance (34). To date, however, there is still uncertainty whether birth order has effects on mental health among Japanese pre-adolescent, say, aged 9–10 years old.

Over the years, family structures have changed in developed countries, including Japan. For example, birth rates have decreased (35). That is, more children have fewer siblings or none. Thus, data reflecting current family contexts is needed to assess the association between birth order and mental health among children. However, few empirical studies have investigated the association in recent years. This study aimed to examine the associations of birth order with mental health problems, and happiness, self-esteem, and resilience among children aged 9–10 years in Japan, using a population-based dataset collected in 2018.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We used data from the Adachi Child Health Impact of Living Difficulty (A-CHILD) study, conducted in all 69 public elementary schools in collaboration with city hall and educational committee in Adachi City, Tokyo, Japan (36). This survey was a longitudinal complete-sample survey started in 2015 as a first-wave for all first-grade children in the schools. In 2018 as a third-wave, questionnaires were distributed to all children aged 9–10 years (fourth grade) attending the schools (n = 5,311). Children responded to the self-reported questionnaire at school and took the questionnaire home so that their caregivers answered it. The questionnaire completed by the caregivers was submitted to the school anonymously. A total of 4,290 participants were eligible (response rate = 80.8%). We excluded children who lived with a single parent or not living with parents (n = 546, 12.7% of respondents). Finally, a sample of, 3,744 children and caregivers were used for analysis (Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Flowchart of participants.

Explanatory Variable

Birth Order

The caregivers provided information regarding the children's birth order. Based on previous birth order studies (37, 38), each child was classified into four categories based on individual's birth order: only child (no siblings), first-born (having only younger siblings), middle-born (having both older and younger siblings), and last-born (having only older siblings).

Outcome Variables

Mental Health

Child mental health problems were assessed by the caregivers using the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (39). The reliability and validity of the SDQ in Japanese children were documented (40). The SDQ includes five items, emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactive/inattention, peer relationship problems, and prosocial behaviors. The total difficulties score was calculated by the sum of the four items except for prosocial behavior, which was used as a continuous outcome. Each item has cut-off scores for normal, borderline, and clinical range; 0–12, 13–15, and 16–40 for total difficulties score; 0–3, 4, and 5–10 for emotional symptoms score; 0–3, 4, and 5–10 for conduct problems score; 0–5, 6, and 7–10 for hyperactivity/inattention score; 0–3, 4, and 5–10 for peer problems score; and 6–10, 5, and 0–4 for prosocial behavior score (41). The score of each item was dichotomized to use as a binary variable (normal and borderline range = 0 and clinical range = 1) for statistical analysis.

Self-Esteem

Children were asked about their self-esteem using the subscales from the Japanese version of the Children's Perceived Competence Scale (CPCS) (42), based on the Perceived Competence Scale for Children (43). The subscales consist of 10 items, for example, “are you satisfied with the way you are now?” or “do you think you have few good points?”. The Cronbach's alpha for the scale was 0.84 in this study. Each item was rated from 1 (no), 2 (rather no), 3 (rather yes), and 4 (yes). Each score was summed and used as a continuous outcome. A higher total score indicated a higher level of self-esteem. The community-based survey reported that the percentage of low self-esteem regarding eight questions related to self-esteem was averagely 7.2% among children aged 10–11years and 8.3% among those aged 13–14 years (44). Further, according to a national survey of Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, the percentage of low self-esteem regarding one question related to self-esteem was 6.4% among primary school children and 8.4% among junior high school children on average over 6 years from 2014 to 2019 (45). These results suggest the percentage of children with low self-esteem could be 10% or less. In this study, the percentage of the score < the 10th percentile was 8.4%. Referring to these findings, we defined the score < the 10th percentile as low self-esteem because the 10th percentile cut-off level could incorporate children with low self-esteem.

Resilience

The Children's Resilient Coping Scale (CRCS) was used to assess child resilience (46). This scale consists of eight items, for example, “speaks positively about their future,” “able to get ready for school, study,” and “do his/her chores without directions,” or “able to give up on things they want or do things that they do not like to do for better future outcomes.” The scale has high internal consistency (46). The caregivers rated their child resilience from zero (never) to four (very frequently). The Cronbach's alpha for the scale was 0.84 in this study. Each score of the eight items was summed, ranging from 0 to 32, and used as a continuous outcome. A higher total score indicated higher resilience. The distribution was left-skewed (more children rating higher scores ≥ in the 10th percentile). Then, the total score of resilience was divided into two categories below the 10th percentile as low resilience to capture a very low level of resilience.

Happiness

Children were asked about their happiness by the question, “how happy do you think you are?” which was rated from 0 (not happy) to 10 (very happy) and used as a continuous outcome. Due to the left-skewed distribution, more children rated higher scores ≥ in the 10th percentile. Indeed, the proportion < the 10th percentile was 5.6%. Further, a community-based survey conducted in Tokyo in 2016 showed that 5.1% of adolescents perceived low happiness (44), which was similar to our proportion of the score < the 10th percentile. Referring to the findings, we defined the score < the 10th percentile as low happiness.

Other Variables

We selected potential confounding variables based on previous research investigating birth order and mental health problems (68, 47). The following variables were included in this study: child's sex (boys or girls), household income (<3.0, 3.0 ≤ 6.0, 6.0 ≤ 10.0, 10.0+ million JPY and unknown; 110 JPY≈ 1 USD), mother's age (Seventy-eight missing data were imputed with the mean of the age) and education attainment (high school graduate or less, some college, college or university graduate, others/unknown), parental history of psychiatric disorders (yes or no), respondent's psychological distress, living with grandparents, and children's school absence due to illness and trauma. The respondent's psychological distress was assessed using the Japanese version of the Kessler 6 (K6), which consisted of six items with a 5-point Likert scale (48). The sum of each score was calculated (range: 0–24); a score of 5–12 was defined as moderate psychological distress, and a score of ≥13 was defined as severe. As for living with grandparents, prior studies have indicated that grandparents' support is related to women's fertility decisions (49, 50). Also, grandparents raising are found to affect grandchildren's mental health (5153). Thus, living with grandparents was included. Also, previous research has shown the association of birth order with PTSD (54) and illness such as metabolic problems (55), allergic symptoms (56), and respiratory diseases (57), which are known to relate to children's mental health (5860). According to these findings, we included children's school absence due to illness and trauma in this analysis. Regarding parental age and education, the mother's age and education attainment correlated with the father's ones (r = 0.40 and 0.65, respectively). Further, in Japan, mothers have more child care time than fathers and play the primary role in child development in daily life (61). Accordingly, we did not take father's age and education attainment into consideration.

Statistical Analysis

First, we compared the characteristics among the four groups: only child, first-borns, middle-borns, and last-borns. Second, we treated the outcomes as continuous variables; that is, we calculated the mean of the scores of the SDQ, self-esteem, happiness, and resilience, and compared by analysis of variance (ANOVA) with post-hoc pairwise comparison using Bonferroni's correction. Third, multiple linear regression analyses were used to examine the association between birth order and mental health problems using each SDQ subscales score, the total difficulties score, the self-esteem score, the happiness score, and the resilience score. The only child was treated as a reference group category. In the adjusted model, covariates (children's sex, mother's age, mother's education, parental history of psychiatric disorders, respondent's psychological distress, household income, living with grandparents, and children's school absence due to illness and trauma) were included. Further, to investigate the association between birth order and the clinical or severe range of the outcomes, multiple logistic regression analyses were performed. These covariates were added in the adjusted model, and we confirmed that multicollinearity is unlikely (all variance inflation factor <2.0). Missing data were substituted by dummy variables. A p-value <0.05 was considered as the level of statistical significance. We used STATA version 15.0 (StataCorp., College Station, TX, USA) for all analyses and followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology Statement (STROBE) guidelines.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the National Center for Child Health and Development (Study ID: 1147) and Tokyo Medical and Dental University (Study ID: M2016-284).

Results

Demographics

Table 1 shows the characteristics of the participants. There were 1,278 first-borns (34.1%), 466 middle-borns (12.5%), 1,356 last-borns (36.2%), and 644 only children (17.2%). Almost half of the children were boys (n = 1,908, 51.0%). With regard to mother's age and educational attainment, most of the mothers were 40–44 years old and graduated some college. As for household income, the largest group was between 6 and 10 million JPY. As for caregiver's mental health, the mean scores of K6 showed no difference across all groups. Regardless of the birth order, more than 90% of children lived without grandparents. Approximately 7% of caregivers reported to have psychiatric disease history, and 34% of children had experienced to be absent from school due to illness or trauma.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics.

Overall Only child First-born Middle Last-born
(n = 3,744) (n = 644, 17.2%) (n = 1,278, 34.1%) (n = 466, 12.5%) (n = 1,356, 36.2%)
n % n % n % n % n %
Sex
Boys 1,908 51.0 324 50.3 694 54.3 234 50.2 656 48.4
Girls 1,836 49.0 320 49.7 584 45.7 232 49.8 700 51.6
Mother's educational attainment
High school graduate or less 956 25.5 175 27.2 284 22.2 137 29.4 360 26.6
Some college 1,248 33.3 235 36.5 427 33.4 136 29.2 450 33.2
College or University graduate 610 16.3 94 14.6 265 20.7 58 12.5 193 14.2
Others/unknown 930 24.8 140 21.7 302 23.6 135 29.0 353 26.0
Mother's age (Mean, SD) 41.2 4.9 43.1 4.7 38.9 4.5 39.6 4.5 43.1 4.3
Respondent's mental health (K6)
<5 2,480 66.2 426 66.2 835 65.3 308 66.1 911 67.2
5 ≤ 13 989 26.4 175 27.2 344 26.9 115 24.7 355 26.2
13+ 162 4.3 32 5.0 56 4.4 25 5.4 49 3.6
Missing 113 3.0 11 1.7 43 3.4 18 3.9 41 3.0
Parental history of psychiatric disorder
No 3,472 92.7 574 89.1 1,185 92.7 430 92.3 1,283 94.6
Yes 272 7.3 70 10.9 93 7.3 36 7.7 73 5.4
Household income (million yen)
<3.0 187 5.0 33 5.1 62 4.9 30 6.4 62 4.6
3.0 ≤ 6.0 1,181 31.5 211 32.8 411 32.2 163 35.0 396 29.2
6.0 ≤ 10.0 1,382 36.9 238 37.0 477 37.3 153 32.8 514 37.9
10.0+ 484 12.9 94 14.6 160 12.5 43 9.2 187 13.8
Missing 510 13.6 68 10.6 168 13.2 77 16.5 197 14.5
Living with grandparents
No 3,432 91.7 597 92.7 1,182 92.5 422 90.6 1,231 90.8
Yes 312 8.3 47 7.3 96 7.5 44 9.4 125 9.2
School absences due to illness or trauma
No 2,448 65.4 429 66.6 796 62.3 306 65.7 917 67.6
Yes 1,296 34.7 215 33.4 482 37.7 160 34.3 439 32.4
Child mental health
SDQ
Total difficulties
Normal 2,715 72.5 450 69.9 892 69.8 346 74.3 1,027 75.7
Borderline 411 11.0 79 12.3 151 11.8 45 9.7 136 10.0
Clinical 495 13.2 102 15.8 191 15.0 56 12.0 146 10.8
Missing 123 3.3 13 2.0 44 3.4 19 4.1 47 3.5
Emotional symptoms
Normal 2,954 78.9 499 77.5 996 77.9 383 82.2 1,076 79.4
Borderline 302 8.1 61 9.5 104 8.1 27 5.8 110 8.1
Clinical 365 9.8 71 11.0 134 10.5 37 7.9 123 9.1
Missing 123 3.3 13 2.0 44 3.4 19 4.1 47 3.5
Conduct problems
Normal 2,860 76.4 509 79.0 938 73.4 348 74.7 1,065 78.5
Borderline 335 9.0 60 9.3 130 10.2 39 8.4 106 7.8
Clinical 426 11.4 62 9.6 166 13.0 60 12.9 138 10.2
Missing 123 3.3 13 2.0 44 3.4 19 4.1 47 3.5
Hyperactivity/inattention problems
Normal 2,997 80.1 511 79.4 998 78.1 375 80.5 1,113 82.1
Borderline 237 6.3 43 6.7 95 7.4 20 4.3 79 5.8
Clinical 388 10.4 77 12.0 141 11.0 53 11.4 117 8.6
Missing 122 3.3 13 2.0 44 3.4 18 3.9 47 3.5
Peer relationship problems
Normal 2,996 80.0 464 72.1 1,024 80.1 384 82.4 1,124 82.9
Borderline 285 7.6 80 12.4 90 7.0 25 5.4 90 6.6
Clinical 340 9.1 87 13.5 120 9.4 38 8.2 95 7.0
Missing 123 3.3 13 2.0 44 3.4 19 4.1 47 3.5
Prosocial behavior
Normal 2,596 69.3 452 70.2 850 66.5 320 68.7 974 71.8
Borderline 533 14.2 89 13.8 187 14.6 72 15.5 185 13.6
Clinical 492 13.1 90 14.0 197 15.4 55 11.8 150 11.1
Missing 123 3.3 13 2.0 44 3.4 19 4.1 47 3.5
Self-esteem score
Not low 3,299 88.1 570 88.5 1,118 87.5 415 89.1 1,196 88.2
Low 316 8.4 54 8.4 111 8.7 35 7.5 116 8.6
Missing 129 3.5 20 3.1 49 3.8 16 3.4 44 3.2
Resilience score
Not low 3,457 92.3 583 90.5 1,183 92.6 427 91.6 1,264 93.2
Low 285 7.6 61 9.5 95 7.4 38 8.2 91 6.7
Missing 2 0.1 0 0.0 0 0.0 1 0.2 1 0.1
Happiness score
Not low 3,376 90.2 589 91.5 1,138 89.1 407 87.3 1,242 91.6
Low 208 5.6 30 4.7 78 6.1 38 8.2 62 4.6
Missing 160 4.3 25 3.9 62 4.9 21 4.5 52 3.8

Outcome Variables by Birth Order

Table 2 presents the mean scores of the SDQ, self-esteem, happiness, and resilience for birth order. Last-borns showed lower score on total difficulties, conduct problems and hyperactivity/inattention (p < 0.001, <0.001, and <0.002, respectively) and higher score on prosocial behavior and resilience, compared to other groups (p < 0.001 and <0.001). First-borns showed a higher score of conduct problems than other groups (p < 0.001). The happiness score of middle-borns was the lowest in all groups (p < 0.002). There were no differences in the self-esteem scores among the four groups.

Table 2.

Results of the ANOVA between birth order and mental health problems self-esteem, resilience and happiness.

Overall Only child First-born Middle Last-born F p-value ω2 p-value for post-hoc pairwise comparison
Mean
(SD)
Mean
(SD)
Mean
(SD)
Mean
(SD)
Mean
(SD)
O vs. F O vs. M O vs. L F vs. M F vs. L M vs. L
SDQs
Total difficulties score 9.26
(5.45)
9.98
(5.56)
9.58
(5.59)
8.92
(5.58)
8.74
(5.15)
9.70 <0.001 0.007 0.818 0.010 <0.001 0.162 0.001 1.000
Emotional symptoms score 1.87
(1.89)
2.00
(1.91)
1.93
(1.95)
1.73
(1.86)
1.81
(1.83)
2.77 0.04 0.002 1.000 0.117 0.213 0.297 0.582 1.000
Conduct problems score 2.22
(1.80)
2.16
(1.80)
2.36
(1.83)
2.29
(1.82)
2.08
(1.75)
6.63 <0.001 0.005 0.058 1.000 1.000 1.000 <0.001 0.185
Hyperactivity/inattention score 3.26
(2.33)
3.43
(2.37)
3.38
(2.37)
3.23
(2.40)
3.08
(2.23)
4.82 0.002 0.003 1.000 1.000 0.013 1.000 0.007 1.000
Peer relationship problems score 1.90
(1.77)
2.39
(1.91)
1.88
(1.81)
1.67
(1.67)
1.77
(1.63)
21.44 <0.001 0.017 <0.001 <0.001 <0.001 0.192 0.637 1.000
Prosocial behavior score 6.71
(2.09)
6.54
(2.09)
6.56
(2.12)
6.81
(2.16)
6.89
(2.03)
7.02 <0.001 0.005 1.000 0.203 0.003 0.179 0.001 1.000
Self-esteem score 18.1
(6.07)
18.2
(6.15)
18.3
(6.17)
17.7
(5.85)
17.9
(6.02)
1.54 0.20 0.0004 1.000 0.748 1.000 0.363 0.967 1.000
Resilience score 22.20
(5.23)
21.55
(5.29)
22.23
(5.26)
21.96
(5.33)
22.55
(5.10)
5.73 <0.001 0.004 0.039 1.000 <0.001 1.000 0.724 0.218
Happiness score 8.16
(2.11)
8.29
(2.04)
8.20
(2.12)
7.81
(2.34)
8.18
(2.04)
4.97 0.002 0.003 1.000 0.002 1.000 0.006 1.000 0.010

O, Only child; F, First-born; M, Middle; L, Last-born. Statistical significance: p-value < 0.05.

Further, the results of the post-hoc pairwise comparison showed p-value for the association between categories of birth order (Figures 2, 3). As for the SDQ, a significant difference was observed in the total difficulties score between only children and middle-borns, and only children and last-borns; hyperactivity/inattention between only children and last-borns: peer relationship problems between only children and each categories; prosocial behavior between only children and last-borns. As for the self-esteem score, there were no difference between only children and each categories. Moreover, a significant difference was observed in the resilience score between only children and first-borns, and only children and last-borns; the happiness score between only children and middle-borns.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Association of birth order and the SDQ by ANOVA with the post-hoc pairwise comparison. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Association of birth order and Self-esteem, Resilience, and Happiness by ANOVA with the post-hoc pairwise comparison. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Regression Analyses

Table 3 shows multivariate linear regression and logistic regression analysis examining the association of birth order with scores of the SDQ, self-esteem, happiness, and resilience. As for linear regression analysis, the score of total difficulties and peer relationship problems in last-borns, middle-borns, and first-borns were significantly lower than only children as the reference group in the adjusted model. Table 3 also presents the significant association between last-borns and lower score of hyperactivity/inattention and higher score of prosocial behavior (β = −0.06, p < 0.01; β = 0.07, p < 0.01), middle-borns and lower score of emotional symptoms and happiness (β = −0.06, p < 0.01; β = −0.08, p < 0.001), and first-borns and higher score of conduct problems (β = 0.05, p = 0.03). The resilience score is significantly higher in last-borns (β = 0.08, p < 0.001) and first-borns (β = 0.05, p = 0.03). The self-esteem score was not associated with birth order.

Table 3.

Associations between birth order and mental health problems, self-esteem, resilience, and happiness in linear and logistic regression.

First-born Middle Last-born R2a Powera Fa
Linear regression Logistic regression Linear regression Logistic regression Linear regression Logistic regression
B
(SE)
β t p B
(SE)
OR
(95%CI)
p B
(SE)
β t p B
(SE)
OR
(95%CI)
p B
(SE)
β t p B
(SE)
OR
(95%CI)
p
SDQ
Total difficulties score
Unadjusted −0.40
(0.27)
−0.03 −1.49 0.136 −0.05
(0.13)
0.95
(0.73–1.23)
0.700 −1.06
(0.34)
−0.06 −3.15 0.002 −0.30
(0.18)
0.74
(0.52–1.06)
0.097 −1.24
(0.26)
−0.11 −4.70 <0.001 −0.43
(0.14)
0.65
(0.50–0.86)
0.002 0.008 1.00 9.70***
Adjustedb,d −0.52
(0.26)
−0.05 −1.99 0.047 −0.11
(0.15)
0.90
(0.67–1.20)
0.469 −1.25
(0.32)
−0.08 −3.89 <0.001 −0.42
(0.19)
0.66
(0.45–0.95)
0.027 −1.09
(0.25)
−0.10 −4.38 <0.001 −0.40
(0.15)
0.67
(0.50–0.89)
0.005 0.130 1.00 30.34***
Emotional symptoms score
Unadjusted −0.07
(0.09)
−0.02 −0.74 0.462 −0.04
(0.16)
0.96
(0.71–1.30)
0.797 −0.27
(0.12)
−0.05 −2.34 0.019 −0.34
(0.21)
0.71
(0.47–1.08)
0.110 −0.19
(0.09)
−0.05 −2.10 0.036 −0.20
(0.16)
0.82
(0.60–1.11)
0.202 0.002 0.823 2.77*
Adjustedb,d −0.11
(0.09)
−0.03 −1.21 0.226 −0.16
(0.17)
0.85
(0.61–1.19)
0.349 −0.34
(0.11)
−0.06 −3.01 0.003 −0.52
(0.22)
0.60
(0.38–0.93)
0.021 −0.15
(0.09)
−0.04 −1.76 0.078 −0.17
(0.16)
0.85
(0.61–1.17)
0.305 0.096 1.00 21.22***
Conduct problems score
Unadjusted 0.23
(0.09)
0.06 2.59 0.010 0.36
(0.16)
1.43
(1.05–1.94)
0.024 0.13
(0.11)
0.02 1.21 0.225 0.35
(0.19)
1.42
(0.98–2.08)
0.067 −0.08
(0.09)
−0.02 −0.89 0.372 0.08
(0.16)
1.08
(0.79–1.48)
0.627 0.006 0.994 6.63***
Adjustedb,d 0.20
(0.09)
0.05 2.18 0.030 0.27
(0.17)
1.31
(0.94–1.83)
0.106 0.09
(0.11)
0.02 0.84 0.401 0.27
(0.20)
1.31
(0.88–1.94)
0.181 −0.05
(0.09)
−0.01 −0.58 0.563 0.14
(0.16)
1.14
(0.83–1.58)
0.412 0.058 1.00 12.33***
Hyperactivity/inattention score
Unadjusted −0.05
(0.11)
−0.01 −0.41 0.684 −0.07
(0.15)
0.93
(0.69–1.25)
0.621 −0.19
(0.14)
−0.03 −1.35 0.177 −0.04
(0.19)
0.97
(0.66–1.40)
0.853 −0.35
(0.11)
−0.07 −3.08 0.002 −0.35
(0.16)
0.71
(0.52–0.96)
0.025 0.004 0.968 4.82**
Adjustedb,d −0.11
(0.11)
−0.02 −0.96 0.339 −0.17
(0.16)
0.84
(0.61–1.16)
0.298 −0.27
(0.14)
−0.04 −1.92 0.055 −0.13
(0.20)
0.88
(0.59–1.30)
0.522 −0.29
(0.11)
−0.06 −2.65 0.008 −0.28
(0.16)
0.75
(0.55–1.03)
0.075 0.098 1.00 21.66***
Peer relationship problems score
Unadjusted −0.51
(0.09)
−0.14 −5.94 <0.001 −0.40
(0.15)
0.67
(0.50–0.90)
0.009 −0.72
(0.11)
−0.13 −6.62 <0.001 −0.54
(0.21)
0.58
(0.39–0.87)
0.008 −0.62
(0.08)
−0.17 −7.32 <0.001 −0.71
(0.16)
0.49
(0.36–0.67)
<0.001 0.018 1.00 21.44***
Adjustedb,d −0.50
(0.09)
−0.13 −5.62 <0.001 −0.45
(0.16)
0.64
(0.46–0.88)
0.006 −0.73
(0.11)
−0.14 −6.71 <0.001 −0.63
(0.22)
0.53
(0.35–0.81)
0.004 −0.60
(0.08)
−0.16 −7.15 <0.001 −0.70
(0.16)
0.50
(0.36–0.68)
<0.001 0.063 1.00 13.49***
Prosocial behavior score
Unadjusted 0.02
(0.10)
0.01 0.23 0.817 0.13
(0.14)
1.14
(0.87–1.50)
0.336 0.27
(0.13)
0.04 2.12 0.034 −0.17
(0.18)
0.84
(0.59–1.21)
0.353 0.35
(0.10)
0.08 3.46 0.001 −0.25
(0.14)
0.78
(0.59–1.03)
0.079 0.006 0.996 7.02***
Adjustedb,d −0.10
(0.10)
−0.02 −0.93 0.354 0.18
(0.15)
1.20
(0.90–1.61)
0.214 0.13
(0.13)
0.02 1.04 0.298 −0.10
(0.19)
0.90
(0.62–1.31)
0.585 0.32
(0.10)
0.07 3.21 0.001 −0.21
(0.15)
0.81
(0.61–1.08)
0.147 0.067 1.00 14.37***
Self-esteem score
Unadjusted 0.05
(0.30)
0.004 0.17 0.863 0.05
(0.17)
1.05
(0.75–1.47)
0.787 −0.58
(0.38)
−0.03 −1.54 0.125 −0.12
(0.23)
0.89
(0.57–1.39)
0.608 −0.29
(0.30)
−0.02 −0.97 0.332 0.02
(0.17)
1.02
(0.73–1.44)
0.892 0.001 0.583 1.54
Adjustedc,d 0.12
(0.31)
0.01 0.37 0.710 0.05
(0.19)
1.05
(0.73–1.51)
0.788 −0.36
(0.38)
−0.02 −0.96 0.335 −0.17
(0.23)
0.84
(0.53–1.33)
0.462 −0.35
(0.29)
−0.03 −1.21 0.226 0.04
(0.18)
1.04
(0.74–1.46)
0.826 0.042 1.00 8.78***
Happiness score
Unadjusted −0.09
(0.10)
−0.02 −0.88 0.378 0.30
(0.22)
1.35
(0.87–2.07)
0.179 −0.48
(0.13)
−0.07 −3.64 <0.001 0.61
(0.25)
1.83
(1.12–3.01)
0.016 −0.11
(0.10)
−0.03 −1.10 0.273 −0.02
(0.23)
0.98
(0.63–1.53)
0.930 0.004 0.97 4.97*
Adjustedc,d −0.10
(0.11)
−0.02 −0.91 0.361 0.27
(0.23)
1.31
(0.83–2.07)
0.246 −0.51
(0.13)
−0.08 −3.81 <0.001 0.61
(0.26)
1.84
(1.10–3.06)
0.019 −0.16
(0.10)
−0.04 −1.52 0.129 −0.003
(0.23)
0.997
(0.64–1.56)
0.988 0.033 1.00 6.67***
Resilience score
Unadjusted 0.69
(0.25)
0.06 2.72 0.006 −0.26
(0.17)
0.77
(0.55–1.07)
0.123 0.42
(0.32)
0.03 1.31 0.191 −0.16
(0.22)
0.85
(0.56–1.30)
0.454 1.00
(0.25)
0.09 4.01 <0.001 −0.37
(0.17)
0.69
(0.49–0.97)
0.031 0.005 0.986 5.73***
Adjustedc,d 0.57
(0.25)
0.05 2.24 0.025 −0.26
(0.19)
0.77
(0.53–1.11)
0.158 0.41
(0.31)
0.03 1.32 0.186 −0.24
(0.23)
0.78
(0.50–1.23)
0.285 0.90
(0.24)
0.08 3.74 <0.001 −0.34 (0.18) 0.71 (0.50–1.01) 0.056 0.096 1.00 22.01***

SE, Standard error; OR, Odds ratio; CI, Confidence interval. Reference, Only child group. Significant significance: p-value < 0.05.

*

p-value < 0.05,

**

p-value < 0.01,

***

p-value < 0.001.

a

Values for linear regression.

b

Normal and Borderline range (ref) vs. Clinical range.

c

Not low (ref) vs. Low (the score < the 10th percentile).

d

Adjusted for child's sex, mother's education, mother's age, household income, respondent's K6, parental history of psychiatric disorders, living with grandparents, and school absences due to illness or trauma.

As for multivariate logistic regression analysis, using the clinical range of the SDQ and 10th percentile cut-off score of the self-esteem, happiness, and resilience, last-borns, middle-borns, and first-borns had decreased odds ratio (OR) of peer relation problems score after adjusting for other covariates (OR = 0.64, 95% Cl 0.46–0.88; OR = 0.53, 95% CI 0.35–0.81; OR = 0.50, 95% CI 0.36–0.68, respectively). After the adjustment, last-borns were still associated with lower total difficulties score (OR = 0.67, 95% Cl 0.50–0.86). Further, middle-borns were significantly associated with unhappiness (OR = 1.84, 95% Cl 1.10–3.09). There was no association between the self-esteem score and birth order.

Comparing the results of the two models, the directions of the association between birth order and each mental health variable in the linear regression model were mostly consistent with the logistic regression model (Normal and Borderline range vs. Clinical range in the SDQ; Normal vs. Low self-esteem, Low resilience, Unhappiness). When compared the linear regression with logistic regression models by birth order, first-borns showed a significant increase in the risk of peer relationship problems in both models and total difficulties in only the linear regression model. Middle-borns showed significant results of a decreased risk of total difficulties, emotional symptoms, and peer relationship problems and an increased risk of unhappiness in both models. Further, last-borns showed a significant decrease in the risk of total difficulties in both models. No statistical significance in hyperactivity/inattention, prosocial behavior, and resilience among last-borns was found in the logistic regression model.

Discussion

In this study, we found the association between birth order and mental health among children aged 9–10 years. Our results showed that last-borns were less likely to have mental health problems and more likely to have prosocial behaviors and resilience. Middle-borns were found to show the lowest level of happiness, and first-borns were associated with conduct problems. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to examine the association of birth order with mental health among pre-adolescent children on both positive and negative sides.

Our findings of last-borns having a lower risk of mental health problems are partially in line with previous research in the UK (47). They identified that later-born children were less likely to have mental health problems than children with no older siblings. Our study focused on the definite birth order position, whereas the UK study assessed the number of siblings, regardless of the order. These findings suggest that the presence of older siblings may play a role in mitigating the risk of mental health problems among children; in turn, last-borns may be in the most advantageous position to receive the benefits by nature. There are several possible explanations for the association. First, interactions with older siblings provide contexts to develop social and emotional competencies, which are known as protective factors for mental health and peer relationship problems (6264). In daily life, children with siblings usually spend more time with siblings than parents (65). Interactions with siblings promote understanding of others' emotions, thoughts, and intentions (66), which foster their development of social competence (67). Besides, plays and conflicts with siblings develop emotional regulation (68, 69) and problem-solving skills (70, 71). Interactions through teaching, sharing, and cooperation facilitate prosocial behavior (7274). Given the interactive opportunities, last-borns may have more chances to develop social and emotional competencies from early childhood than other siblings and only one child.

Second, older siblings may play a complementary role in caregiving. When parents are unavailable, siblings can become candidates for attachment relationships and also take on the role of a secure base for younger siblings (75). These relationships provide a sense of security and comfort to younger siblings in insecure family situations (76, 77). Furthermore, last-borns may be more likely to receive more emotional support from older siblings (78). Thus, older siblings can be a source of security for last-borns, which may contribute to the prevention of mental health problems.

Third, parental differential treatment may affect the association of birth order with mental health problems. Parental differential treatment, such as favoritism and unfavorable comparison, can cause not only sibling jealousy (79) but also behavior problems (80), emotional issues (81), and lower self-esteem (82). In Japan, the i.e., system and the practice of amae may strengthen parental differential treatment. It may affect mental health more negatively among older siblings. On the contrary, last-borns may be less likely to be affected, which may lead to a decreased risk of their mental health problems. However, the association between these unique characteristics and mental health problems among Japanese children is still unclear. Hence, further research is needed to confirm the association.

However, previous studies have reported that last-born adolescents and adults have an increased risk of mental illness in the US, the UK, Norway, and Sweden (58), which is inconsistent with our findings. There are two possible explanations for the inconsistency. The one is that the positive effects of the last position on mental health in childhood may vary in adolescence and adulthood. In adolescence, children spend more time with peers and less with family and have more opportunities to face stressful events and peer relationship problems (8385). The benefits of the last position within a family may be attenuated in different life stages. The other one is that sociocultural characteristics in each country may contribute to the inconsistency. Japanese sociocultural characteristics, that is amae and i.e., system, might account for the inconsistent findings between Japan and other countries. Further longitudinal studies are needed to reveal the mechanism of the inconsistency, comparing with other countries.

Our study also demonstrated that last-borns showed a higher resilience score. Previous studies have reported that the development of resilience is associated with social competence (86), problem-solving skills (87) and self-regulation skills (88), and familial factors such as support availabilities (89, 90). Given the presence of older siblings, last-borns may be more likely to develop these skills through interactions with the siblings and receive emotional support from the siblings, which may contribute to promoting the development of resilience from early childhood.

Furthermore, our findings showed that first-borns were associated with conduct problems, which is partially consistent with a prior study in the UK reporting older siblings having an increased risk of the problems (47). This association may be related to several possible explanations. First, first-borns are required to adopt to siblinghood after the birth of siblings (91). After subsequent sibling birth, first-borns may experience less parental attention and interactions, described as “dethronement” by Adler's theory (66). The behavior problems of first-borns may be considered as a stress response to the change of the home environment (47). Second, parents might change their child-rearing style between the first-born and the subsequent children, which may induce jealousy toward younger siblings (92). That is, parental differential treatment may cause first-borns to suffer from emotional dysregulation, impulsiveness, and vulnerability to frustration (82, 93). In addition, as discussed previously, Japanese sociocultural characteristics may promote the differential treatment. Third, parents may require the first-borns to be a role model for younger siblings. Notably, in Japan, parents tend to expect first-borns to be the heir (32). Consequently, the first-borns may feel more responsibilities and emotional pressure (94). Hence, first-borns may be more likely to feel more emotional distress than younger siblings and only one child and, in turn, have an increased risk of conduct problems.

Moreover, we found that middle-borns showed the lowest level of happiness, compared to only children. Prior research demonstrated that parents rated that last-borns and only children aged 3–9 years were happier than first-borns and middle-borns in the US (20). Our study is the first to report that middle-born children rated themselves as the most unhappy. Kidwell suggested that the middle-borns have a lack of uniqueness in the family and no inherent reasons to receive parental attention and recognition, compared to first-and last-borns (19). In addition, a prior study has demonstrated that middle-born youth tend to perceive less closeness to parents (95) and express less positive attitudes toward family than first-borns and last-borns and more positive views toward friends (96). These findings suggest that the presence of both older and younger siblings may induce perceived unhappiness among middle-borns, and the state of unhappiness in childhood may affect their values toward family and peers in adolescence and adulthood. However, the effects of unhappiness on later life among middle-borns remained unclear; further longitudinal research is needed to examine the trajectories.

Our study found no differential association of birth order with self-esteem among pre-adolescent children. Previous research has shown that first-borns and only children tend to have higher self-esteem than later-borns (5, 97). Further, another study has reported that middle-borns have significantly lower self-esteem than first-borns and last-borns (19). However, it is noteworthy that these studies were conducted more than three decades ago and focused on adolescents and young adults, which may lead to inconsistency with our findings. Self-esteem is found to decline across adolescence through the experience of stressful life events (98), for example, school transitions (84, 85). In Japan, school transitions generally occur between the ages of 12 and 15 years. For this reason, the participants in our study typically have no experience of school transitions. Thus, the experience of other stressful life events may contribute to the inconsistency. Moreover, Baldwin and Hoffmann indicated that perceived family support has preventive effects on the decline of self-esteem in adolescence (99). Given the negative views toward family among middle-borns (95, 96), they may have lower stability of self-esteem when facing stressful events; in turn, they may have an increased risk of a decline in self-esteem in adolescence, compared to other siblings and only children. Further longitudinal research is required to reveal the association between birth order and self-esteem from childhood to adulthood.

This study has several limitations. First, this study did not evaluate the quality of sibling relationships. Poor sibling relationships are found to affect children's mental health negatively (100). Second, we did not differentiate the sibling compositions, such as boy-boy, boy-girl, girl-boy, girl-girl (four types by sex of two siblings), boy-boy-boy, boy-boy-girl, boy-girl-boy, boy-girl-girl, girl-boy-boy, girl-boy-girl, girl-girl-boy, girl-girl-girl (eight types by sex of three siblings), or age difference, say, 1 year difference or 5 years difference, due to limited sample size. Sibling relationships may vary depending on the compositions (92). Sibling compositions might change the magnitude of the effects of birth order on mental health. Third, we could not assess the influence of peer relationships. Peer relationships, such as bullying and victimization, are important factors that affect mental health among school children, and such exposure might vary by sibling positions (101). Thus, the quality of peer relationships might modify the effects of birth order. Fourth, we did not assess families who had experienced perinatal loss through miscarriage, stillbirth, and neonatal death. Prior research reports that the experience influences subsequent pregnancy (102, 103) and parenting behavior (103, 104). Thus, the experience may affect the number of children and parenting styles, which suggests that the effects of birth order among families having the experience may differ from those who have not. Fifth, children's medical issues, such as specific psychiatric disorders and environmental stress, and trauma experience, were not assessed. Also, in this study, children's mental health conditions were not evaluated by more objective measurement tools, such as DSM-5 criteria and the teacher-rated SDQ. Sixth, we did not assess parental personality and parenting styles. These may affect the association between birth order and children's mental health. Finally, this study used cross-sectional data and could not consider past mental health conditions. Further studies are needed to elucidate the effects of birth order on child mental health trajectories, adjusting for parenting style and sibling relationships in a longitudinal design.

Despite these limitations, our findings can help parents identify children at risk for mental health problems in pre-adolescence. Information and guidance about the association of birth order with mental health may allow parents to notice initial symptoms of child mental health problems and manage these problems before adolescence at an earlier stage. In addition, supporting parental rearing practice may also help minimize the adverse effects of birth order on mental health among children. Further, based on birth-order, intervention focusing on both parent-child and sibling relationships may be beneficial to improving mental health among children.

In conclusion, our study found that birth order had differential associations with mental health in both positive and negative aspects among Japanese children aged 9–10. These findings may be helpful to prevent mental health problems depending on birth order in adolescence. Future longitudinal studies are needed to elucidate the mechanisms of the effects of birth order on mental health and the trajectories across the life stages.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Tokyo Medical and Dental University Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

YF contributed to conception, design, analysis, interpretation, drafted, and critically revised the manuscript. SD and AI contributed to conception, design, data acquisition, analysis, interpretation, and critically revised the manuscript. MO contributed to conception, design, data acquisition, and critically revised the manuscript. TF contributed to conception, design, data acquisition, analysis, interpretation, drafted, and critically revised the manuscript. All authors gave final approval and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are particularly grateful to the staff members and central office of Adachi City Hall for conducting the survey. We would like to thank everyone who participated in the surveys. In particular, we would also like to thank Mayor Yayoi Kondo, Mr. Syuichiro Akiu, and Ms. Yuko Baba of Adachi City Hall, all of whom contributed significantly to completion of this study.

Footnotes

Funding. This study was supported by a Health Labor Sciences Research Grant, Comprehensive Research on Lifestyle Disease from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare (H27-Jyunkankito-ippan-002), Research of Policy Planning and Evaluation from the Japanese Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare (H29-Seisaku-Shitei-004), Innovative Research Program on Suicide Countermeasures (IRPSC), and Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers 16H03276 and 16K21669), St. Luke's Life Science Institute Grants, and the Japan Health Foundation Grants.

References

  • 1.Merikangas KR, He J-P, Burstein M, Swanson SA, Avenevoli S, Cui L, et al. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:980–9. 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.05.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. JAMA Psychiatry. (2005) 62:593–602. 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ministory of Health Labour and Welfare . White Paper on Suicide Prevention in Japan. (2020). Available online at: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/content/r2h-1-3.pdf (accessed January 29, 2021).
  • 4.OECD . Health at a Glance 2015: OECD Indicators (2015). Available from: 10.1787/health_glance-2015-en (accessed February 20, 2020). [DOI]
  • 5.Gates L, Lineberger MR, Crockett J, Hubbard J. Birth order and its relationship to depression, anxiety, and self-concept test scores in children. J Genet Psychol. (1988) 149:29–34. 10.1080/00221325.1988.10532136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Easey KE, Mars B, Pearson R, Heron J, Gunnell D. Association of birth order with adolescent mental health and suicide attempts: a population-based longitudinal study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2019) 28:1079–86. 10.1007/s00787-018-1266-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bjørngaard JH, Bjerkeset O, Vatten L, Janszky I, Gunnell D, Romundstad P. Maternal age at child birth, birth order, and suicide at a young age: a sibling comparison. Am J Epidemiol. (2013) 177:638–44. 10.1093/aje/kwt014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Rostila M, Saarela J, Kawachi I. Birth order and suicide in adulthood: evidence from Swedish population data. Am J Epidemiol. (2014) 179:1450–7. 10.1093/aje/kwu090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Downey DB. Number of siblings and intellectual development: the resource dilution explanation. Am Psychol. (2001) 56:497–504. 10.1037/0003-066X.56.6-7.497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Price J. Parent-child quality time: does birth order matter? J Hum Resour. (2008) 43:240–65. 10.3368/jhr.43.1.240 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Keyes CLM. Subjective well-being in mental health and human development research worldwide: an introduction. Soc Indic Res. (2006) 77:1–10. 10.1007/s11205-005-5550-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Rutter M. Resilience concepts and findings: implications for family therapy. J Fam Ther. (1999) 21:119–44. 10.1111/1467-6427.00108 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Mann M, Hosman CMH, Schaalma HP, de Vries NK. Self-esteem in a broad-spectrum approach for mental health promotion. Health Educ Res. (2004) 19:357–72. 10.1093/her/cyg041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Das JK, Salam RA, Lassi ZS, Khan MN, Mahmood W, Patel V, et al. Interventions for adolescent mental health: an overview of systematic reviews. J Adolesc Health. (2016) 59:S49–60. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.06.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Dray J, Bowman J, Campbell E, Freund M, Wolfenden L, Hodder RK, et al. Systematic review of universal resilience-focused interventions targeting child and adolescent mental health in the school setting. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 56:813–24. 10.1016/j.jaac.2017.07.780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Orpana H, Vachon J, Dykxhoorn J, McRae L, Jayaraman G. Monitoring positive mental health and its determinants in Canada: the development of the positive mental health surveillance indicator framework. Health Promot Chronic Dis Prev Can. (2016) 36:1–10. 10.24095/hpcdp.36.1.01 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 9:1–11. 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Falbo T. Relationships between birth category, achievement, and interpersonal orientation. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1981) 41:121–31. 10.1037/0022-3514.41.1.121 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Kidwell JS. The neglected birth order: middleborns. J Marriage Fam. (1982) 44:225–35. 10.2307/351276 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Park N, Peterson C. Character strengths and happiness among young children: content analysis of parental descriptions. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 7:323–41. 10.1007/s10902-005-3648-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Sugimoto Y. An Introduction to Japanese Society. 4th ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; (2014). p. 64–90. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Umemura T, Traphagan JW. Reviewing Japanese concepts of Amae and Ie to deeper understand the relevance of secure-base behavior in the context of Japanese caregiver-child interactions. Integr Psychol Behav Sci. (2015) 49:714–36. 10.1007/s12124-015-9316-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Holloway SD, Behrens KY. Parenting self-efficacy among Japanese mothers: qualitative and quantitative perspectives on its association with childhood memories of family relations. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (2002) 96:27–43. 10.1002/cd.42 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Doi T. The Anatomy of Dependence. JP: Kodansha International; (1973). [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Shaffer DR. Developmental Psychology: Childhood and Adolescence. 4th ed. Bermont: Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co. (1995). p. 689. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rothbaum F, Kakinuma M, Nagaoka R, Azuma H. Attachment and Amae. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2007) 38:465–86. 10.1177/0022022107302315 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rothbaum F, Weisz J, Pott M, Miyake K, Morelli G. Attachment and culture: security in the United States and Japan. Am Psychol. (2000) 55:1093–104. 10.1037/0003-066X.55.10.1093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Behrens KY. A multifaceted view of the concept of Amae: reconsidering the indigenous Japanese concept of relatedness. Hum Dev. (2004) 47:1–27. 10.1159/000075366 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Niiya Y, Ellsworth PC, Yamaguchi S. Amae in Japan and the United States: an exploration of a “culturally unique” emotion. Emotion. (2006) 6:279–95. 10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.279 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Power TG, Kobayashi-Winata H, Kelley ML. Childrearing patterns in Japan and the United States: a cluster analytic study. Int J Behav Dev. (1992) 15:185–205. 10.1177/016502549201500202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Markus HR, Kitayama S. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol Rev. (1991) 98:224–53. 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Shirasa T. Kyoudai Kankei to Sono Kankei Ryouiki no Bunkenshuusei 2 (Japanese). JP: Kawashima shoten; (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Someya T, Uehara T, Kadowaki M, Tang SW, Takahashi S. Effects of gender difference and birth order on perceived parenting styles, measured by the EMBU scale, in Japanese two-sibling subjects. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2000) 54:77–81. 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2000.00640.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Kojima Y. Effects of birth order on self-reliance of Japanese preschoolers during daily routines. Psychology. (2019) 10:1262–8. 10.4236/psych.2019.109081 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ministory of Health Labour and Welfare . Vital Statistics of Japan. (2017). Available online at: https://www.mhlw.go.jp/toukei/saikin/hw/jinkou/suikei18/dl/2018gaiyou.pdf (accessed November 1, 2019).
  • 36.Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T, Doi S, Fujiwara T. Adachi child health impact of living difficulty (A-CHILD) study: research protocol and profiles of participants. J Epidemiol. (2020) 31:77–89. 10.2188/jea.JE20190177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Carballo JJ, Garcia-Nieto R, Alvarez-Garcia R, Caro-Canizares I, Lopez-Castroman J, Munoz-Lorenzo L, et al. Sibship size, birth order, family structure and childhood mental disorders. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2013) 48:1327–33. 10.1007/s00127-013-0661-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Damian RI, Roberts BW. The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U.S. high school students. J Res Pers. (2015) 58:96–105. 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.05.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (1997) 38:581–6. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Moriwaki A, Kamio Y. Normative data and psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire among Japanese school-aged children. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2014) 8:1. 10.1186/1753-2000-8-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Matsuishi T, Nagano M, Araki Y, Tanaka Y, Iwasaki M, Yamashita Y, et al. Scale properties of the Japanese version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): a study of infant and school children in community samples. Brain Dev. (2008) 30:410–5. 10.1016/j.braindev.2007.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sakurai S. The investigation of self-consciousness in the 5th-and 6th-grade children. Jap J Exp Soc Psyhol. (1992) 32:85–94. 10.2130/jjesp.32.85 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Harter S. The perceived competence scale for children. Child Dev. (1982) 53:87–97. 10.2307/1129640 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bureau of Social Welfare and Public Health . Survey of Living Conditions on Chidren in Tokyo. (2017). Available online at: https://www.fukushihoken.metro.tokyo.lg.jp/smph/joho/soshiki/syoushi/syoushi/oshirase/kodomoseikatsujittaityousakekka.html (accessed January 29, 2021).
  • 45.Ministry of Education Culture Sports Science and Technology . Zenkokutekina gakuryoku chousa (Zenkoku gakuryoku gakushuu joukyou chousatou) (2019). Available online at: https://www.nier.go.jp/19chousakekkahoukoku/19summary.pdf (accessed January 30, 2021).
  • 46.Doi S, Fujiwara T, Ochi M, Isumi A, Kato T. Association of sleep habits with behavior problems and resilience of 6- to 7-year-old children: results from the A-CHILD study. Sleep Med. (2018) 45:62–8. 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.12.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lawson DW, Mace R. Siblings and childhood mental health: evidence for a later-born advantage. Soc Sci Med. (2010) 70:2061–9. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.03.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Furukawa TA, Kawakami N, Saitoh M, Ono Y, Nakane Y, Nakamura Y, et al. The performance of the Japanese version of the K6 and K10 in the World Mental Health Survey Japan. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2008) 17:152–8. 10.1002/mpr.257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hayslip B, Jr., Blumenthal H, Garner A. Social support and grandparent caregiver health: one-year longitudinal findings for grandparents raising their grandchildren. J Gerontol Series B. (2014) 70:804–12. 10.1093/geronb/gbu165 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tanskanen AO, Jokela M, Danielsbacka M, Rotkirch A. Grandparental effects on fertility vary by lineage in the United Kingdom. Hum Nat (Hawthorne, NY). (2014) 25:269–84. 10.1007/s12110-014-9200-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Dunifon R. The influence of grandparents on the lives of children and adolescents. Child Dev Perspect. (2013) 7:55–60. 10.1111/cdep.12016 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Pearson RM, Culpin I, Loret de Mola C, Matijasevich A, Santos IS, Horta BL. Grandmothers' mental health is associated with grandchildren's emotional and behavioral development: a three-generation prospective study in Brazil. BMC Psychiatry. (2019) 19:184. 10.1186/s12888-019-2166-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sadruddin AFA, Ponguta LA, Zonderman AL, Wiley KS, Grimshaw A, Panter-Brick C. How do grandparents influence child health and development? A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. (2019) 239:112476. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2019.112476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Green B, Griffiths EC. Birth order and post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Health Med. (2014) 19:24–32. 10.1080/13548506.2013.774432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ayyavoo A, Savage T, Derraik JGB, Hofman PL, Cutfield WS. First-born children have reduced insulin sensitivity and higher daytime blood pressure compared to later-born children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. (2013) 98:1248–53. 10.1210/jc.2012-3531 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Perzanowski MS, Canfield SM, Chew GL, Mellins RB, Hoepner LA, Jacobson JS, et al. Birth order, atopy, and symptoms of allergy and asthma among inner-city children attending Head Start in New York City. Clin Exp Allergy. (2008) 38:968–76. 10.1111/j.1365-2222.2008.02967.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Rudan I, Boschi-Pinto C, Biloglav Z, Mulholland K, Campbell H. Epidemiology and etiology of childhood pneumonia. Bull World Health Organ. (2008) 86:408–16. 10.2471/BLT.07.048769 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Craske MG, Poulton R, Tsao JC, Plotkin D. Paths to panic disorder/agoraphobia: an exploratory analysis from age 3 to 21 in an unselected birth cohort. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2001) 40:556–63. 10.1097/00004583-200105000-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Goodwin RD, Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ. Asthma and depressive and anxiety disorders among young persons in the community. Psychol Med. (2004) 34:1465–74. 10.1017/S0033291704002739 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Teyhan A, Galobardes B, Henderson J. Child allergic symptoms and well-being at school: findings from ALSPAC, a UK cohort study. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0135271. 10.1371/journal.pone.0135271 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Statistics Bureau of Japan . Japan Statistical Yearbook 2018. (2018). Available online at: https://www.stat.go.jp/english/data/nenkan/67nenkan/index.html (accessed January 29, 2021).
  • 62.Slaughter V, Imuta K, Peterson CC, Henry JD. Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Dev. (2015) 86:1159–74. 10.1111/cdev.12372 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.McGee R, Stanton WR. Sources of distress among New Zealand adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1992) 33:999–1010. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00921.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Stormshak EA, Bellanti CJ, Bierman KL. The quality of sibling relationships and the development of social competence and behavioral control in aggressive children. Dev Psychol. (1996) 32:79–89. 10.1037/0012-1649.32.1.79 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Tucker CJ, McHale SM, Crouter AC. Links between older and younger adolescent siblings' adjustment: the moderating role of shared activities. Int J Behav Dev. (2008) 32:152–60. 10.1177/0165025407087214 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.McHale SM, Updegraff KA, Whiteman SD. Sibling relationships and influences in childhood and adolescence. J Marriage Fam. (2012) 74:913–30. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.01011.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bosacki S, Wilde Astington J. Theory of mind in preadolescence: relations between social understanding and social competence. Soc Dev. (1999) 8:237–55. 10.1111/1467-9507.00093 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Youngblade LM, Dunn J. Individual differences in young children's pretend play with mother and sibling: links to relationships and understanding of other people's feelings and beliefs. Child Dev. (1995) 66:1472–92. 10.2307/1131658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Kramer L. Learning emotional understanding and emotion regulation through sibling interaction. Early Educ Dev. (2014) 25:160–84. 10.1080/10409289.2014.838824 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Brown JR, Donelan-McCall N, Dunn J. Why talk about mental states? The significance of children's conversations with friends, siblings, and mothers. Child Dev. (1996) 67:836–49. 10.2307/1131864 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Howe N, Rinaldi CM, Jennings M, Petrakos H. “No! the lambs can stay out because they got cozies”: constructive and destructive sibling conflict, pretend play, and social understanding. Child Dev. (2002) 73:1460–73. 10.1111/1467-8624.00483 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Dunn J, Munn P. Siblings and the development of prosocial behaviour. Int J Behav Dev. (1986) 9:265–84. 10.1177/016502548600900301 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Howe N, Recchia H. Playmates and teachers: reciprocal and complementary interactions between siblings. J Fam Psychol. (2005) 19:497–502. 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Howe N, Recchia H. Individual differences in sibling teaching in early and middle childhood. Early Educ Dev. (2009) 20:174–97. 10.1080/10409280802206627 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Samuels HR. The effect of an older sibling on infant locomotor exploration of a new environment. Child Dev. (1980) 51:607–9. 10.2307/1129305 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Jenkins J. Sibling Relationships in Disharmonious Homes: Potential Difficulties and Protective Effects. Children's Sibling Relationships: Developmental and Clinical Issues. Hillsdale, MI: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. (1992). p. 125–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Teti DM, Ablard KE. Security of attachment and infant-sibling relationships: a laboratory study. Child Dev. (1989) 60:1519–28. 10.2307/1130940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Gass K, Jenkins J, Dunn J. Are sibling relationships protective? A longitudinal study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2007) 48:167–75. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01699.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Loeser MK, Whiteman SD, McHale SM. Siblings' perceptions of differential treatment, fairness, and jealousy and adolescent adjustment: a moderated indirect effects model. J Child Fam Stud. (2016) 25:2405–14. 10.1007/s10826-016-0429-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Meunier JC, Roskam I, Stievenart M, De Moortele GV, Browne DT, Wade M. Parental differential treatment, child's externalizing behavior and sibling relationships: bridging links with child's perception of favoritism and personality, and parents' self-efficacy. J Soc Pers Relat. (2012) 29:612–38. 10.1177/0265407512443419 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Shanahan L, McHale SM, Crouter AC, Osgood DW. Linkages between parents' differential treatment, youth depressive symptoms, and sibling relationships. J Marriage Fam. (2008) 70:480–94. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00495.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Feinberg ME, Neiderhiser JM, Simmens S, Reiss D, Hetherington EM. Sibling comparison of differential parental treatment in adolescence: gender, self-esteem, and emotionality as mediators of the parenting-adjustment association. Child Dev. (2000) 71:1611–28. 10.1111/1467-8624.00252 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Brown BB, Larson J. Peer relationships in adolescence. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L. editors. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology: Contextual Influences on Adolescent Development, Vol 2, 3rd edn. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc. (2009). p. 74–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cole DA, Maxwell SE, Martin JM, Peeke LG, Seroczynski AD, Tram JM, et al. The development of multiple domains of child and adolescent self-concept: a cohort sequential longitudinal design. Child Dev. (2001) 72:1723–46. 10.1111/1467-8624.00375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Wigfield A, Eccles JS, Mac Iver D, Reuman DA, Midgley C. Transitions during early adolescence: changes in children's domain-specific self-perceptions and general self-esteem across the transition to junior high school. Dev Psychol. (1991) 27:552–65. 10.1037/0012-1649.27.4.552 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Personality, adrenal steroid hormones, and resilience in maltreated children: a multilevel perspective. Dev Psychopathol. (2007) 19:787–809. 10.1017/S0954579407000399 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Masten AS, Barnes AJ. Resilience in children: developmental perspectives. Children. (2018) 5:98. 10.3390/children5070098 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zolkoski SM, Bullock LM. Resilience in children and youth: a review. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2012) 34:2295–303. 10.1016/j.childyouth.2012.08.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Dirks MA, Persram R, Recchia HE, Howe N. Sibling relationships as sources of risk and resilience in the development and maintenance of internalizing and externalizing problems during childhood and adolescence. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 42:145–55. 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Benzies K, Mychasiuk R. Fostering family resiliency: a review of the key protective factors. Child Fam Soc Work. (2009) 14:103–14. 10.1111/j.1365-2206.2008.00586.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Volling BL. The transition to siblinghood: a developmental ecological systems perspective and directions for future research. J Fam Psychol. (2005) 19:542–9. 10.1037/0893-3200.19.4.542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Whiteman SD, McHale SM, Soli A. Theoretical perspectives on sibling relationships. J Fam Theory Rev. (2011) 3:124–39. 10.1111/j.1756-2589.2011.00087.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Volling BL. Family transitions following the birth of a sibling: an empirical review of changes in the firstborn's adjustment. Psychol Bull. (2012) 138:497–528. 10.1037/a0026921 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Baskett LM. Sibling status effects: adult expectations. Dev Psychol. (1985) 21:441–5. 10.1037/0012-1649.21.3.441 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Salmon CA, Daly M. Birth order and familial sentiment: middleborns are different. Evol Hum Behav. (1998) 19:299–312. 10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00022-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Salmon C. Birth order and relationships. Hum Nat. (2003) 14:73–88. 10.1007/s12110-003-1017-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Schwab MR, Lundgren DC. Birth order, perceived appraisals by significant others, and self-esteem. Psychol Rep. (1978) 43:443–54. 10.2466/pr0.1978.43.2.443 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Robins RW, Trzesniewski KH. Self-esteem development across the lifespan. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. (2005) 14:158–62. 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00353.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Baldwin SA, Hoffmann JP. The dynamics of self-esteem: a growth-curve analysis. J Youth Adolesc. (2002) 31:101–13. 10.1023/A:101406582559820495855 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Buist KL, Vermande M. Sibling relationship patterns and their associations with child competence and problem behavior. J Fam Psychol. (2014) 28:529–37. 10.1037/a0036990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Menesini E, Camodeca M, Nocentini A. Bullying among siblings: the role of personality and relational variables. Br J Dev Psychol. (2010) 28:921–39. 10.1348/026151009X479402 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Geller PA, Kerns D, Klier CM. Anxiety following miscarriage and the subsequent pregnancy: a review of the literature and future directions. J Psychosom Res. (2004) 56:35–45. 10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00042-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Lamb EH. The impact of previous perinatal loss on subsequent pregnancy and parenting. J Perinat Educ. (2002) 11:33–40. 10.1891/1058-1243.11.2.33 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Coleman PK, Reardon DC, Cougle J. The quality of the caregiving environment and child developmental outcomes associated with maternal history of abortion using the NLSY data. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2002) 43:743–57. 10.1111/1469-7610.00095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.


Articles from Frontiers in Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES