Abstract
I explore the association between the severity of lockdown policies in the first half of 2020 and mortality rates. Using two indices from the Blavatnik Centre’s COVID-19 policy measures and comparing weekly mortality rates from 24 European countries in the first halves of 2017–2020, addressing policy endogeneity in two different ways, and taking timing into account, I find no clear association between lockdown policies and mortality development.
Keywords: epidemic, COVID-19, policy responses
1. Introduction
The spread of a new corona virus, severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (Sars-CoV-2) that causes coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), came as a genuine surprise to the world. Governments the world over decided to lock down their economies in order to suppress the virus, as early experience from Wuhan in China’s Hubei province brought them to believe that limiting social contacts would contain its spread, protect health care from reaching capacity limits, and limit its death toll. However, whether economic lockdowns work in the sense of suppressing the virus and preventing deaths remains an open question.
Epidemiologists have tried to answer it, indicating that thousands and perhaps millions have been saved. These first answers have nevertheless been exclusively based on forecasts derived from empirically untested models (cf., Flaxman et al. 2020). Conversely, Chaudhry et al. (2020), in an exploratory analysis of data on COVID-19-related deaths across 50 countries, find no association between the degree of lockdown and death rates. Similarly, in the only empirical assessment of its kind to date that takes the endogeneity of policy responses into account, Born et al. (2020) use a synthetic control method to suggest that Sweden’s decision not to lock down society did not contribute substantially to its death toll. They thus question the widely held political belief that lockdowns must have effectively suppressed the spread of the virus. A similar question is raised by, for example, Atkeson et al. (2020) and De Larochelambert et al. (2020), both of which find no difference in mortality development across different mitigation and lockdown strategies.
In this paper, I instead approach the question using a standard approach and standard econometric tools used in economics and political science instead of epidemiological modelling or single-case studies. I compare weekly general mortality rates in the first half of the year in 2017, 2018, 2019, and 2020 in 24 European countries that took very different policy measures against the virus at different points in time. Estimating the effects of these policy measures as captured by the Blavatnik Centre’s COVID-19 policy indices and taking the endogeneity of policy responses into account, the results suggest that stricter lockdown policies have not been associated with lower mortality.
2. Data and Empirical Strategy
The main data consist of weekly mortality numbers from 24 European countries covered by Eurostat (2020), which capture mortality for all causes. In subsequent tests, I use weekly all-cause mortality numbers in four age bands—0–39 years, 40–59 years, 60–79 years, and 80 years and older—from the same source. The reason for using weekly mortality data instead of official data on COVID-19-related deaths is the way these deaths are measured. While weekly mortality counts all actual deaths in the country, COVID-19-related deaths are counted as deaths where the deceased tested positive for the virus but where the death is not necessarily caused by the virus. Country-specific differences in reporting approach and reporting behaviour would therefore bias the results when using official measures of COVID-19-related deaths. As is standard in order to avoid outlier influence, I take the logarithm to these data (cf., Box and Cox 1964; Lütkepohl and Xu 2012).
As a measure of the severity of lockdown policies, I use two indices developed at Oxford University’s Blavatnik Centre by Hale et al. (2020). As documented in Petherick et al. (2020), the full government response index includes 13 elements: school closures, workplace closures, cancellation of public events, restrictions on gatherings, closing public transport, stay-at-home requirements, restrictions on internal movement, international travel controls, income support, debt and contract relief for households, public information campaigns, testing policies, and contact tracing. These components are all scored categorically, based on available public information from each country, between 0 and 4, such that 0 implies no interventions, 1 is mainly recommendations, and 2 to 4 are increasingly restrictive interventions. The Spanish restrictions on public gatherings are, for example, scored a maximum of 4 between 30 March and 1 June when the population was effectively banned from leaving the house for non-essential purposes. By 1 June, small gatherings were allowed, which resulted in a reduction of the rating to 3.
I use the containment and health index, which does not include the two economic elements of income support and debt relief that are irrelevant for virus spread. Alternatively, as a robustness test throughout, I use the stringency measure that in addition excludes the final three measures.1 These indices are the main variables that I use to explore the association between mortality dynamics and policy responses to the new virus.
The specification in which I estimate this association includes three-way fixed effects capturing country-, year-, and week-specific differences, which implies that all effects are identified as week-specific changes in mortality relative to mortality rates in the same week in previous years, and relative to changes in other countries in the same year and week. As such, all approximately time-invariant factors in a four-year perspective such as geography, population density and demography, welfare state and health care type, the regular capacity of the health care system, institutional quality, and political institutions are therefore controlled for (cf., Cepaluni et al. 2020). This effectively reduces the risk of omitted variable bias. The specification is given in (i) where Mt, I is all-cause weekly mortality in week t in country i, αi is a vector of country fixed effects, Pt–x,I is a policy index of country in period t–x (such that x is the lag), Dt is a vector of week fixed effects, At is a vector of annual fixed effects, and εt,i is an error term.
(1) |
A main problem with estimating effects of policy changes on the mortality rate is that effects are far from contemporaneous. The incubation period of Sars-CoV-2—the time from a person is exposed to the virus and until she develops symptoms in case that happens at all—is reported to vary between 2 and 14 days with a typical period around one week. If these symptoms develop further, she will typically be hospitalized after an additional seven days (Garg et al. 2020). Although there is relatively little knowledge of how rapidly COVID-19 results in deaths, Flaxman et al. (2020) implicitly assume a time lag of two to three weeks between infection and death, which appears to be the minimum typical time from infection to death. In the following, I therefore report the results using time lags of lockdown policies varying from one to four weeks.
The second identification problem is the potential endogeneity of the association, which derives from the nature of political reactions to the virus that could rely on the reported number of infections. If an increase in the reported infection rate leads government to introduce lockdown policies, and if a declining reported infection rate subsequently leads them to ease the lockdown, the estimated association between policy stringency and mortality is biased.
A first solution the identification problem relies on the timing of changes in lockdown policies and mortality rates. Although one might think that policy making reacts quickly to changing mortality during an emergency, exploring the determinants of changes in the stringency indices reveals that an increase in the contemporaneous mortality or an increase in the reported number of Sars-CoV-2 cases was not associated with stricter lockdown measures (see also Sebhatu et al. 2020). Lagging these indicators for the severity of the health crisis reveals that mortality changes become significant predictors of stricter measures when lagged three weeks. As such, it is highly unlikely that there is a substantial endogeneity problem in the following as mortality changes only affect policy changes with a three-week lag, and as policy changes cannot affect the mortality rate before another two to three weeks have passed. As such, any bias is likely to be small and practically negligible.
However, endogeneity or simultaneity bias may still be a problem if, for example, omitted time-variant political factors are important. Another standard way of alleviating the endogeneity bias is to find instrumental variables that provide identification of changes in policy stringency while not being associated with mortality dynamics. Although valid and sufficiently strong instrumental variables are often difficult to find in fixed effects settings, it is possible to identify variables that determine the policy indices and that may be plausibly exogenous. In the following, I use the logarithm to the number of days since the first Sars-CoV-2 case was identified in the country and its square, which I interact with the Benefit INEP (Index of Emergency Powers) measure developed by Bjørnskov and Voigt (2018), an index that essentially captures the degree to which the executive gains additional discretionary powers during a state of emergency.2 The theoretical background for this instrumental approach is that, as indicated by previous research, policy responses to emergencies are often informed more by the promise of additional, unchecked power than the severity of the emergency. Bjørnskov and Voigt (2020) for example show that countries hit to the same extent by the Sars-CoV-2 virus were more likely to declare a state of emergency when their constitutional emergency provisions granted them more discretionary power. In other words, policy responses are likely to be stronger and more interventionist in countries in which the constitution effectively enables strong political responses. While the potential effect of constitutional provisions per se is captured by the country fixed effects, as constitutions do not change during the four years explored here, the interaction between the INEP and the time since the first case thus offers exogenous identification of the policy development, defined by the constitutional developments taking place years or decades before the virus could have been known.3
A final challenge is that the timing of the policies, and not just their severity, may affect mortality development. Pei et al. (2020) for example suggest that earlier intervention would have contained the spread of Sars-CoV-2 in the USA, while Maier and Brockmann (2020) make a similar argument for China. Differential effects depending on timing may be a challenge, as Aksoy et al. (2020) indicate that the speed with which the first non-pharmaceutical interventions were implemented varied substantially across countries. While the fixed effects approach prevents a direct assessment of whether early lockdowns were more effective, I approach this problem by separating countries in two groups according to two timing criteria: how quickly a country reached a level of lockdown after the first case was identified, and how quickly it happened after the first death was reported. I apply a cut-off of 40 on the index of containment and health and the policy stringency index, as this is the lowest index value that all countries in the sample reached at any point in time. I then interact these two dummy variables with the lockdown index, which provides a direct test of systematically heterogeneous effectiveness.
All data are summarized in the Appendix Table A1; all countries included are listed in Table A2 along with the number of days between the first registered case and registered death and the country reaching a value of 40 on the stringency index, as well as the mortality rate in the first half of 2020 relative to the three preceding years. However, before turning to the estimates, it may be worthwhile to provide a sense of the dynamics of mortality rates in 2020 and previous years. Figure 1 illustrates the development of weekly mortality (the full lines), relative to the average development in the same weeks in 2017–2019 (the dotted lines). The black line represents the average development in the 12 countries with an average containment and health index in 2020 above the median of all 24 countries, while the grey line represents the same development in countries with below-median indices. As such, the figure shows how mortality changed from week to week for the first half of the years.
Exploring these data, a first interesting detail is that comparing 2020 to the previous years, it is clear that the average European country had negative excess mortality in the first 10 weeks of the year. On average, the 24 countries suffered 200 fewer deaths per million inhabitants (8.7%) in the first 10 weeks of 2020, compared with their averages in 2017–2019. From week 11 to 22, the 24 countries experienced an accumulated excess mortality of 248 deaths per million inhabitants (10.7%). However, as illustrated in Figure 1, the ‘hard lockdown’ group experienced 372 additional deaths per million, while the other group only experienced excess mortality of 123 deaths. This simple pattern may indicate that lockdowns have been directly counterproductive but may also indicate a severe endogeneity problem.
3. Main Results
Table 1 reports the results of estimating the effects of the containment and health index, while Table 2 employs the policy stringency index. In both tables, the goodness-of-fit statistics show that the simple specification does a good job explaining the actual development: year- and week-fixed effects along with a lagged dependent variable explain approximately 70% of the within-country variation. The results in both tables also clearly document the persistence and relatively slow development of mortality, as reflected in the lagged dependent variable.
Table 1.
Lag length | One week | Two weeks | Three weeks | Four weeks |
---|---|---|---|---|
Lagged dependent |
0.648** (0.071) |
0.660** (0.072) |
0.673** (0.073) |
0.681** (0.071) |
Containment and health index |
0.072** (0.018) |
0.047* (0.018) |
0.022 (0.018) |
0.004 (0.015) |
Observations | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 |
R squared | 0.996 | 0.996 | 0.996 | 0.997 |
F statistic | 228.39 | 225.56 | 223.82 | 223.28 |
Only data prior to turning point | ||||
Containment and health index |
0.126** (0.032) |
0.113** (0.035) |
0.094* (0.036) |
0.089* (0.041) |
Only data after turning point | ||||
Containment and health index |
0.058 (0.077) |
0.016 (0.069) |
–0.028 (0.071) |
–0.153* (0.069) |
Instrumental variables estimates | ||||
Containment and health index |
–0.626 (0.325) |
–0.601* (0.302) |
–0.103* (0.049) |
0.071 (0.031) |
Notes: ** (**) denote significance at p < 0.001 (p < 0.05). The dependent variable is the logarithm to weekly all-cause mortality in the first half of 2017, 2018, 2019, and 2020. All estimates are obtained with an OLS estimator with country, year, and week fixed effects. Results denoted as ‘prior to turning point’ refer to effects in weeks prior to the week of 2020 with the highest mortality in a country; results denoted as ‘after turning point’ refer to effects in subsequent weeks.
Table 2.
Lag length | One week | Two weeks | Three weeks | Four weeks |
---|---|---|---|---|
Lagged dependent |
0.643** (0.071) |
0.659** (0.073) |
0.675** (0.074) |
0.685** (0.072) |
Policy stringency index |
0.072** (0.019) |
0.043* (0.018) |
0.015 (0.028) |
–0.006 (0.016) |
Observations | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 |
R squared | 0.996 | 0.996 | 0.886 | 0.996 |
F statistic | 229.10 | 225.49 | 223.55 | 223.31 |
Only data prior to turning point | ||||
Policy stringency index |
0.119** (0.031) |
0.099** (0.032) |
0.068* (0.031) |
0.050 (0.035) |
Only data after turning point | ||||
Policy stringency index |
0.094 (0.072) |
0.062 (0.069) |
0.026 (0.077) |
–0.094 (0.071) |
Instrumental variables estimates | ||||
Policy stringency index |
0.472* (0.208) |
–0.536* (0.238) |
–0.122* (0.053) |
0.002 (0.033) |
Notes: ** (**) denote significance at p < 0.001 (p < 0.05). The dependent variable is the logarithm to weekly all-cause mortality in the first half of 2017, 2018, 2019, and 2020. All estimates are obtained with an OLS estimator with country, year, and week fixed effects. Results denoted as ‘prior to turning point’ refer to effects in weeks prior to the week of 2020 with the highest mortality in a country; results denoted as ‘after turning point’ refer to effects in subsequent weeks.
The main results in the tables show that the estimated effects of lockdown policies are all positive and significant when policy changes are lagged one or two weeks. However, when the lag length extends to three or fourth weeks, that is, the length that is reasonable from the perspective of the virology of Sars-CoV-2, the estimates become very small and insignificant.
The following panels in both tables separate the period in which the mortality rates increased and the subsequent decline. Although these estimates may arguably suffer from bias if lockdown policies significantly move the turning point of mortality development, which would be the case if the association is negative, they provide information about potentially asymmetric effects of the policies. However, the results clearly indicate that lockdown policies are positively associated with mortality development before the mortality rate peaks although the association again tends to break down with lag length. Conversely, there is no clear or significant relation between lockdown stringency and mortality after the virus has peaked. One result, the containment and health index lagged four weeks in the period after the turning point, is negative and significant but the corresponding estimate for the more precise policy stringency index is far from significance.
While the significant association with a one- or two-week lag prior to the turning point may indicate endogeneity—this particular lag structure could indicate that lockdowns follow increases in case numbers that eventually cause increasing mortality—two additional factors speak against this interpretation. First, the alternative approach of applying instrumental variables estimates as reported in the bottom panels is inconsistent with a substantial endogeneity problem.4 These results confirm the overall pattern by being negative and significant when lagged one or two weeks (the period when they cannot have worked) but turning positive and insignificant when lagged four weeks. They therefore do not indicate any substantial endogeneity bias that works against identifying an effect of lockdowns, but quite the opposite. Given the validity of the instruments, these estimates do not provide evidence suggesting that lockdown policies worked as intended. Second, the analysis of the timing of national lockdown decisions in Sebhatu et al. (2020) documents that these decisions were not clearly determined by case numbers or deaths. Additionally, the timing of the lockdowns occurred at the end of the regular virus season, which itself could yield the particular pattern observed in the lag structure.
4. Timing Heterogeneity
However, as indicated by Pei et al. (2020), not only the severity but also the particular timing of the lockdown policies may play a role in reducing the spread. In Tables 3 and 4, I allow for differential effects by interacting the policy indices with one of two dummy variables that capture policy timing relative to the first reported case and the first reported death in a country, respectively. The interaction terms thus indicate whether the effectiveness of lockdown policies is different in countries that implemented them relatively early. Taking into account how the implementation of lockdown policies was timed may arguably be necessary for two reasons. First, earlier implementation of restrictions may have saved lives by protecting the health care system from reaching capacity. Second, early implementation may also have avoided a subsequent situation where the government may perceive that the imposition of harder restrictions is necessary. As such, late implementation may have led countries to impose much tougher measures once the epidemic was seen to get out of control.
Table 3.
Lag length | Three weeks | Four weeks | Three weeks | Four weeks |
---|---|---|---|---|
Lagged dependent |
0.672** (0.073) |
0.681** (0.071) |
0.671** (0.073) |
0.681** (0.072) |
Containment and health index |
0.024 (0.019) |
0.004 (0.016) |
0.026 (0.021) |
0.004 (0.017) |
Early timing (first case) * index |
–0.003 (0.011) |
–0.000 (0.009) |
||
Early timing (first death) * index |
–0.011 (0.012) |
–0.002 (0.011) |
||
Observations | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 |
R squared | 0.996 | 0.996 | 0.996 | 0.997 |
F statistic | 216.28 | 215.76 | 180.35 | 179.25 |
Notes: ** (**) denote significance at p<.001 (p<.05). The dependent variable is the logarithm to weekly all-cause mortality in the first half of 2017, 2018, 2019, and 2020. All estimates are obtained with an OLS estimator with country, year, and week fixed effects. Early timing refers to countries in which lockdown policies were implemented earlier than the average of the sample after the first COVID-related case or death was recorded in the country.
Table 4.
Lag length | Three weeks | Four weeks | Three weeks | Four weeks |
---|---|---|---|---|
Lagged dependent |
0.675** (0.074) |
0.685** (0.072) |
0.674** (0.075) |
0.685** (0.073) |
Policy stringency index |
0.016 (0.019) |
–0.007 (0.016) |
0.017 (0.021) |
–0.007 (0.010) |
Early timing (first case) * index |
–0.003 (0.010) |
–0.000 (0.008) |
||
Early timing (first death) * index |
–0.007 (0.012) |
–0.005 (0.010) |
||
Observations | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 |
R squared | 0.996 | 0.997 | 0.996 | 0.997 |
F statistic | 216.02 | 215.78 | 216.05 | 215.80 |
Notes: ** (**) denote significance at p < 0.001 (p < 0.05). The dependent variable is the logarithm to weekly all-cause mortality in the first half of 2017, 2018, 2019, and 2020. All estimates are obtained with an OLS estimator with country, year, and week fixed effects. Early timing refers to countries in which lockdown policies were implemented earlier than the average of the sample after the first COVID-related case or death was recorded in the country.
The results nevertheless reveal that the interactions are tiny and none of them are near significance at conventional levels.5 Although the two indices are not identical, the results do not support the claim that lockdowns are more effective when implemented early. In addition, exploring the data suggests that early lockdowns did not help governments avoid hard lockdowns, as there is no clear pattern between early timing and subsequent severity.
5. Age Differences
Most deaths in general, as well as most deaths related to Sars-CoV-2, occur among the relatively old. In the present sample, 89% of all deaths occurred among people older than 60 years and 53% occurred among people older than 80. A final issue to address is that it remains possible that the results outlined so far are affected by effect heterogeneity if lockdowns have different effects in different age groups.
However, the results in Table 5 do not substantially differ from those in previous tables. When the estimates are statistically significant, they are positive and as previous estimates, they tend to become smaller with longer lags.6 If anything, the results suggest that lockdowns may have led to significantly higher mortality among the population aged between 60 and 79 years. Across the tables, the overall findings thus indicate that neither differing the lag length, applying instrumental variables, separating countries based on the timing of their interventions, or estimating effects in specific age groups yield support for the effectiveness of such lockdowns.
Table 5.
Lag length | One week | Two weeks | Three weeks | Four weeks |
---|---|---|---|---|
0–39 years | ||||
Lagged dependent |
0.032 (0.029) |
0.032 (0.029) |
0.032 (0.029) |
0.032 (0.030) |
Containment and health index |
–0.004 (0.043) |
–0.044 (0.041) |
–0.033 (0.045) |
–0.026 (0.047) |
Observations | 2611 | 2611 | 2611 | 2611 |
R squared | 0.489 | 0.487 | 0.489 | 0.493 |
F statistic | 2.54 | 2.55 | 2.53 | 2.52 |
40–59 years | ||||
Lagged dependent |
0.156** (0.044) |
0.157** (0.045) |
0.158** (0.045) |
0.159** (0.045) |
Containment and health index |
0.090** (0.029) |
0.075* (0.033) |
0.059 (0.032) |
0.044 (0.032) |
Observations | 2595 | 2595 | 2595 | 2595 |
R squared | 0.949 | 0.949 | 0.951 | 0.952 |
F statistic | 8.26 | 8.14 | 8.02 | 7.92 |
60–79 years | ||||
Lagged dependent |
0.403** (0.101) |
0.411** (0.104) |
0.421** (0.106) |
0.429** (0.107) |
Containment and health index |
0.111** (0.022) |
0.087** (0.021) |
0.059** (0.019) |
0.034* (0.017) |
Observations | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 |
R squared | 0.988 | 0.988 | 0.989 | 0.989 |
F statistic | 64.48 | 63.42 | 62.43 | 61.82 |
80 years and over | ||||
Lagged dependent |
0.621** (0.069) |
0.632** (0.071) |
0.645** (0.072) |
0.653** (0.071) |
Containment and health index |
0.091** (0.019) |
0.062** (0.019) |
0.033 (0.019) |
0.010 (0.017) |
Observations | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 | 2600 |
R squared | .994 | 0.994 | 0.994 | 0.994 |
F statistic | 218.88 | 216.20 | 214.45 | 213.76 |
Notes: ** (**) denote significance at p < 0.001 (p < 0.05). The dependent variable is the logarithm to weekly all-cause mortality in the first half of 2017, 2018, 2019, and 2020 in each of the four age groups in the panel headlines. All estimates are obtained with an OLS estimator with country, year, and week fixed effects.
6. Conclusions
The lockdowns in most Western countries have thrown the world into the most severe recession since World War II and the most rapidly developing recession ever seen in mature market economies. They have also caused an erosion of fundamental rights and the separation of powers in large part of the world as both democratic and autocratic regimes have misused their emergency powers and ignored constitutional limits to policy making (Bjørnskov and Voigt 2020). It is therefore important to evaluate whether and to which extent the lockdowns have worked as officially intended: to suppress the spread of the Sars-CoV-2 virus and prevent deaths associated with it.
Comparing weekly mortality in 24 European countries, the findings in this paper suggest that more severe lockdown policies have not been associated with lower mortality. In other words, the lockdowns have not worked as intended. Further tests also show that early interventions offered no additional benefits or effectiveness and even indicate that the lockdowns of the spring of 2020 were associated with significantly more deaths in the particular age group between 60 and 79 years.
These general findings are consistent with the results of previous papers using a variety of simpler methods to test the effects of lockdowns. Contrary to persistent political claims, as indicated by recent research in Bendavid et al. (2021), the traditional approach of carefully disseminating public information about the epidemic and relying on voluntary measures may have represented the only policy significantly associated with fewer cases than no reaction at all, although almost all other countries went substantially further. Likewise, much has been made of the timing of lockdowns, but the present analysis does not indicate that early lockdowns were more effective. Examples also suggest that lockdown timing may not have been a significant determinant of mortality development: while Belgium and Portugal were among the countries that locked down soon after seeing the first COVID-related death, the former saw an 11% increase in mortality, compared with the preceding three years, while the latter only saw a 2% increase. Among countries that locked down later, the UK experienced an 18% higher mortality than the preceding years, while German mortality in the first half of 2020 was almost exactly average for the time of the year. As such, the data illustrate that strategic examples as are often used in popular media can be highly misleading, which necessitates a more comprehensive evaluation of lockdown efficacy.
The main problem at hand is therefore that the evidence presented here suggests that lockdowns have not significantly affected the development of mortality in Europe. They have nevertheless wreaked economic havoc in most societies and may lead to a substantial number of additional deaths for other reasons. A British government report from April for example assessed that a limited lockdown could cause 185,000 excess deaths over the next years, while UNICEF warns of an increase in child marriages, owing to the economic effects of Western lockdowns in developing countries (DHSC 2020; Philipose and Aika 2021). Evaluated as a whole, at a first glance, the lockdown policies of the Spring of 2020 therefore appear to be substantial long-run government failures.
Acknowledgements
I thank Niclas Berggren, Otto Brøns-Petersen, Jonas Herby, Dan Klein, Panu Poutvaara, Karl Wennberg, and an anonymous reviewer of this journal for comments on earlier versions. I also gratefully acknowledge support from the Jan Wallander and Tom Hedelius Foundation. All remaining errors are naturally mine.
Appendix
Table A1.
Mean | Standard deviation | Observations | |
---|---|---|---|
Log weekly mortality | 7.405 | 1.454 | 2704 |
Log mortality, 0–39 years | 3.549 | 1.296 | 2658 |
Log mortality, 40–59 years | 4.920 | 1.445 | 2701 |
Log mortality, 60–79 years | 6.358 | 1.439 | 2704 |
Log mortality, 80 years and over | 6.771 | 1.489 | 2704 |
Policy stringency index | 9.499 | 23.043 | 3078 |
Containment and health index | 9.335 | 22.630 | 3078 |
Benefit INEP | 0.497 | 0.211 | 3192 |
Table A2.
Country | Rel. mortality | Lockdown timing | Country | Rel. mortality | Lockdown timing |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Austria | 1.01 | 16/0 | Latvia | 0.92 | 11/–19 |
Belgium | 1.11 | 39/3 | Lithuania | 0.965 | –5/–7 |
Bulgaria | 0.94 | 5/2 | Luxembourg | 0.86 | 12/–1 |
Czechia | 0.87 | 10/–11 | Netherlands | 1.02 | 14/6 |
Denmark | 0.97 | 12/1 | Norway | 0.97 | 15/0 |
Estonia | 0.98 | 16/–11 | Poland | 0.84 | 8/0 |
Finland | 0.99 | 43/–9 | Portugal | 1.02 | 10/-4 |
France | 0.99 | 49/28 | Slovakia | 0.82 | 4/-8 |
Germany | 0.98 | 42/0 | Spain | 1.19 | 39/26 |
Hungary | 0.94 | 7/–4 | Sweden | 1.09 | 48/8 |
Iceland | 0.99 | 17/–1 | Switzerland | 1.00 | 17/8 |
Italy | 0.97 | 22/0 | United Kingdom | 1.19 | 51/17 |
Note: the second column numbers are mortality rates in the first half of 2020 relative to the average of 2017–2019; the numbers in the second columns are the number of days from the first registered Sars-Cov-2 case/death and the day the stringency index passed a value of 40.
Footnotes
The exclusion of three components—public information campaigns, testing policies, and contact tracing—is warranted on two different grounds. Conceptually, these three policy elements can be pursued without locking down any economic or social activity in society. Statistically, they are also only weakly correlated with the remaining components. However, with these exceptions, the components entering the stringency index are all highly correlated, indicating that the index is statistically valid. This is confirmed by Cronbach’s Alpha, which is 0.92 (0.84 when excluding all zeros) for the policy stringency index in the present sample.
Bjørnskov and Voigt (2018) develop two INEP measures: one that captures how difficult it is to declare a state of emergency, and another that captures which additional powers the executive gains during an emergency. The Benefit INEP is the second of these indices and is composed of three basic elements: (i) whether the executive can dissolve parliament, how many basic rights can be derogated, and if the executive can expropriate private property and restrict the freedom of expression during emergencies.
Underidentification is not likely to be a problem as the first stage F statistics in the instrumental variables estimates in the following vary between 110 and 178. Likewise, overidentification is not a substantial problem as the time since the first identified case is collinear with the week numbers and therefore is unlikely to bias the estimates.
In principle, poorly identified instrumental variables estimates can degenerate to being equivalent to OLS estimates with additional noise. However, the goodness-of-fit statistics for the first stage of the instrumental variables estimates suggest otherwise. As the first stage F statistics for the estimates in Tables 1 and 2 vary between 100 and 178, they remain far above the regular rule of thumb of 10 and thus indicate that underidentification is unlikely to be a problem (cf., Staiger and Stock 1997; Stock and Yogo 2005).
It remains an option that the timing of policies may be correlated with the timing of and prevalence of voluntary measures such as social distancing. However, Luther (2020) indicates that voluntary measures clearly preceded policy interventions in the USA. The problem is also unlikely, and in all cases minor when noting that quite different countries tended to take similar policy measures at approximately the same time (Sebathu et al. 2020).
I do not report the results of using the policy stringency index as they are practically identical with those in Table 5. Experimenting with even longer lags of five or six weeks reveals no significantly negative results, but the occasional significant positive association between lockdown and mortality. I refrain from reporting these results, as the mass of evidence presented here already may suffer from multiple test bias. In that respect, it should be noted that applying a Bonferoni correction to the standard errors to account for multiple test bias in general renders all negative associations insignificant. An additional robustness test, suggested by a reviewer, is to apply an inverse hyperbolic sine transformation instead of a simple logarithmic transformation. This test yields results that are almost identical to those reported in this paper. I also implemented a final placebo test that may inform about the potential influence of country- and week-specific unobservables on the policy estimate. The test consists of assigning the 2020 policies to the first half of the years 2017–2019 and estimating its ‘effects’ in those three years. The results are statistically insignificant, negative associations of approximately –0.04 with the containment and stringency indices, indicating that if the estimates are subject to bias from unobservables, the bias is most probably negative.
References
- Aksoy C. G., Ganslmeier M., Poutvaara P. (2020), “Public Attention and Policy Responses to COVID-19 Pandemic”, IZA Discussion Paper 13427, IZA Institute of Labor Economics, Bonn.
- Atkeson A., Kopecky K., Zha T. (2020), “Four Stylized Facts about COVID-19”, NBER Working Paper 27719, National Bureau of Economic Research, Cambridge, MA.
- Bendavid E., Oh C., Bhattacharya J., Ioannidis J. P. A. (2021), “ Assessing Mandatory Stay”, ‐at‐Home and Business Closure Effects on the Spread of COVID‐19, forthcoming in European Journal of Clinical Investigation. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bjørnskov C., Voigt S. (2018), “ The Architecture of Emergency Constitutions”, International Journal of Constitutional Law 16, 101–27. [Google Scholar]
- Bjørnskov C., Voigt S. (2020), “This Time is Different? On the Use of Emergency Measures during the Corona Pandemic”, ILE Working Paper No. 36, University of Hamburg.
- Born B., Dietrich A., Müller G. (2020), “The Lockdown Effect: A Counterfactual for Sweden”, CEPR Discussion Paper 14744, Centre for Economic Policy Research, London.
- Box G. E. P., Cox D. R. (1964), “ An Analysis of Transformations”, Journal of the Royal Statistical Society. Series B 26, 211–52. [Google Scholar]
- Cepaluni G., Dorsch M. T., Branyiczki R. (2020), “Political Regimes and Deaths in Early Stages of the COVID-19 Pandemic”, Working Paper, Central European University, Budapest.
- Chaudhry R., Dranitsaris G., Mubashir T., Bartoszko J., Riazia S. (2020), “A Country Level Analysis Measuring the Impact of Government Actions, Country Preparedness and Socioeconomic Factors on COVID-19 Mortality and Related Health Outcomes”, EClinicalMedicine, article 100464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- De Larochelambert Q., Marc A., Antero J., Bourg E. L., Toussaint J.-F. (2020), “ Covid-19 Mortality: A Matter of Vulnerability among Nations Facing Limited Margins of Adaptation”, Frontiers in Public Health 8, 604339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DHSC (2020), Initial Estimates of Excess Deaths from COVID-19, Department of Health and Social Care, Office for National Statistics, Government Actuary’s Department and Home Office, 8 April.
- Eurostat (2020), Weekly Death Statistics, Eurostat, Paris. https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/statistics-explained/index.php?title=Weekly_death_statistics&stable (last accessed December 2020). [Google Scholar]
- Flaxman S., Mishra S., Gandy A., Unwin H. J. T., Mellan T. A., Coupland H., Whittaker C., Zhu H., Berah T., Eaton J. W., Monod M., Ghani A. C., Donnelly C. A., Riley S. M., Vollmer M. A. C., Ferguson N. M., Okell L. C., Bhatt S., Imperial College COVID-19 Response Team (2020), “ Estimating the Effects of Non-Pharmaceutical Interventions on COVID-19 in Europe”, Nature 584, 257–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garg S., Kim L., Whitaker M., O’Halloran A., Cummings C., Holstein R., Prill M., Chai S. J., Kirley P. D., Alden N. B., Kawasaki B., Yousey-Hindes K., Niccolai L., Anderson E. J., Openo K. P., Weigel A., Monroe M. L., Ryan P., Henderson J., Kim S., Como-Sabetti K., Lynfield R., Sosin D., Torres S., Muse A., Bennett N. M., Billing L., Sutton M., West N., Schaffner W., Talbot H. K., Aquino C., George A., Budd A., Brammer L., Langley G., Hall A. J., Fry A. (2020), Hospitalization Rates and Characteristics of Patients Hospitalized with Laboratory-Confirmed Coronavirus Disease 2019 — COVID-NET, 14 States, March 1–30, 2020, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, April 17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Hale T., Webster S., Petherick A., Phillips T., Kira B. (2020), “Oxford COVID-19 Government Response Tracker”, Blavatnik School of Government, Oxford University. https://www.bsg.ox.ac.uk/research/research-projects/coronavirus-government-response-tracker (last accessed July 2020).
- Luther W. J. (2020), “Behavioral and Policy Responses to COVID-19: Evidence from Google Mobility Data on State-Level Stay-at-Home Orders”, working paper, American Institute for Economic Research.
- Lütkepohl H., Xu F. (2012), “ The Role of the Log Transformation in Forecasting Economic Variables”, Empirical Economics 42, 619–38. [Google Scholar]
- Maier B. F., Brockmann D. (2020), “ Effective Containment Explains Subexponential Growth in Recent Confirmed COVID-19 Cases in China”, Science 368, 742–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pei S., Kandula S., Shaman J. (2020), “ Differential Effects of Intervention Timing on COVID-19 Spread in the United States”, Science Advances. 6, eabd6370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petherick A., Kira B., Angrist N., Hale T., Phillips T., Webster S. (2020), “Variation in Government Responses to COVID-19”, BSG Working Paper no. 2020/032, Oxford University.
- Philipose A., Aika M. (2021), Child Marriage in COVID-19 Contexts: Disruptions, Alternative Approaches and Building Programme Resilience, United Nations Children's Fund, Nairobi. [Google Scholar]
- Sebhatu A., Wennberg K., Arora-Jonsson S., Lindberg S. I. (2020), “ Explaining the Homogeneous Diffusion of Covid-19 Policies among Heterogeneous Countries”, PNAS 117, 21201–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staiger D., Stock J. H.. (1997), “ Instrumental Variables Regression with Weak Instruments”, Econometrica 65, 557–86. [Google Scholar]
- Stock J. H., Yogo M..(2005), “Testing for Weak Instruments in Linear IV Regression”, in Andrews D. W. K., ed., Identification and Inference for Econometric Models, Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 80–108. [Google Scholar]