Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2021 Apr 6;10:e66128. doi: 10.7554/eLife.66128

High social status males experience accelerated epigenetic aging in wild baboons

Jordan A Anderson 1,, Rachel A Johnston 1,, Amanda J Lea 2,3,4, Fernando A Campos 2,5, Tawni N Voyles 1, Mercy Y Akinyi 6, Susan C Alberts 1,2, Elizabeth A Archie 7, Jenny Tung 1,2,8,9,
Editors: George H Perry10, George H Perry11
PMCID: PMC8087445  PMID: 33821798

Abstract

Aging, for virtually all life, is inescapable. However, within populations, biological aging rates vary. Understanding sources of variation in this process is central to understanding the biodemography of natural populations. We constructed a DNA methylation-based age predictor for an intensively studied wild baboon population in Kenya. Consistent with findings in humans, the resulting ‘epigenetic clock’ closely tracks chronological age, but individuals are predicted to be somewhat older or younger than their known ages. Surprisingly, these deviations are not explained by the strongest predictors of lifespan in this population, early adversity and social integration. Instead, they are best predicted by male dominance rank: high-ranking males are predicted to be older than their true ages, and epigenetic age tracks changes in rank over time. Our results argue that achieving high rank for male baboons – the best predictor of reproductive success – imposes costs consistent with a ‘live fast, die young’ life-history strategy.

Research organism: P. cynocephalus

eLife digest

For most animals, age is one of the strongest predictors of health and survival, but not all individuals age at the same rate. In fact, animals of the same species can have different 'biological ages' even when they have lived the same number of years. In humans and other mammals this variation in aging shows up in chemical modifications known as DNA methylation marks. Some researchers call these marks 'epigenetic', which literally means 'upon the genes'. And some DNA methylation marks change with age, so their combined pattern of change is often called the ‘epigenetic clock’.

Environmental stressors, such as smoking or lack of physical activity, can make the epigenetic clock ‘tick’ faster, making the DNA of some individuals appear older than expected based on their actual age in years. These ‘biologically older’ individuals may also experience a higher risk of age-related disease. Studies in humans have revealed some of the reasons behind this fast biological aging, but it is unclear whether these results apply in the wild. It is possible that early life events trigger changes in the epigenetic clock, affecting health in adulthood. In primates, for example, adversity in early life has known effects on fertility and survival. Low social status also has a negative effect on health.

To find out whether early experiences and the social environment affect the epigenetic clock, Anderson, Johnston et al. tracked DNA methylation marks in baboons. This revealed that epigenetic clocks are strong predictors of age in wild primates, but neither early adversity nor the strength of social bonds affected the rate at which the clocks ticked. In fact, it was competition for social status that had the most dramatic effect on the clock’s speed. Samples of males taken at different times during their lives showed that their epigenetic clocks sped up or slowed down as they moved up or down the social ladder, reflecting recent social experiences, rather than events early in their lives. On average, epigenetic clock measurements overestimated the age in years of alpha males by almost a year, showing that fighting to be on top comes at a cost.

This study highlights one way in which the social environment can influence aging. The next step is to understand how health is affected by the ways that animals attain social status. This could help researchers who study evolution understand how social interactions and environmental conditions affect survival and reproduction. It could also provide insight into the effects of social status on human health and aging.

Introduction

Aging, the nearly ubiquitous functional decline experienced by organisms over time (López-Otín et al., 2013), is a fundamental component of most animal life histories (Jones et al., 2014). At a physiological level, age affects individual quality, which in turn affects the ability to compete for mates and other resources, invest in reproduction, and maintain somatic integrity. At a demographic level, age is often one of the strongest predictors of survival and mortality risk, which are major determinants of Darwinian fitness. In order for patterns of aging to evolve, individuals must vary in their rates of biological aging. Thus, characterizing variation in biological aging rates and its sources – beyond simply chronological age – is an important goal in evolutionary ecology, with the potential to offer key insight into the trade-offs that shape individual life-history strategies (Monaghan et al., 2008).

Recent work suggests that DNA methylation data can provide exceptionally accurate estimates of chronological age (Horvath and Raj, 2018). These approaches typically use supervised machine learning methods that draw on methylation data from several hundred CpG sites, identified from hundreds of thousands of possible sites, to produce a single chronological age prediction (Hannum et al., 2013; Horvath, 2013; Levine et al., 2018). Intriguingly, some versions of these clocks also predict disease risk and mortality, suggesting that they capture aspects of biological aging that are not captured by chronological age alone (Declerck and Vanden Berghe, 2018). For example, in humans, individuals predicted to be older than their true chronological age are at higher risk of Alzheimer’s disease (Levine et al., 2015), cognitive decline (Levine et al., 2015; Marioni et al., 2015), and obesity (Horvath et al., 2014). Accelerated epigenetic age is in turn predicted by environmental factors with known links to health and lifespan, including childhood social adversity (Jovanovic et al., 2017; Raffington et al., 2020) and cumulative lifetime stress (Zannas et al., 2015). These observations generalize to other animals. Dietary restriction, for instance, decelerates biological aging based on DNA methylation clocks developed for laboratory mice and captive rhesus macaques, and genetic knockout mice with extended lifespans also appear epigenetically young for age (Maegawa et al., 2017; Petkovich et al., 2017; Stubbs et al., 2017). However, while DNA methylation data have been used to estimate the age structure of wild populations (where birthdates are frequently unknown) (De Paoli‐Iseppi, 2018; Polanowski et al., 2014; Thompson et al., 2017; Wright et al., 2018), they have not been applied to investigating sources of variance in biological aging in the wild.

To do so here, we coupled genome-wide data on DNA methylation levels in blood with detailed behavioral and life-history data available for one of the most intensively studied wild mammal populations in the world, the baboons of the Amboseli ecosystem of Kenya (Alberts and Altmann, 2012). First, we calibrated a DNA methylation-based ‘epigenetic clock’ and assessed the clock’s composition. Second, we compared the accuracy of this clock against other age-associated traits and between sexes. Third, we tested whether variance in biological aging was explained by socioenvironmental predictors known to impact fertility or survival in this population. Finally, we investigated an intriguing association between epigenetic age acceleration and male dominance rank. Our results show that predictors of lifespan can be decoupled from rates of epigenetic aging. However, other factors – particularly male dominance rank – are strong predictors of epigenetic clock-based age acceleration. These results are the first to establish a link between social factors and epigenetic aging in any natural animal population. Together, they highlight potential sex-specific trade-offs that may maximize fitness, but also compromise physiological condition and potentially shorten male lifespan.

Results

Epigenetic clock calibration and composition

We used a combination of previously published (Lea et al., 2016) and newly generated reduced-representation bisulfite sequencing (RRBS) data from 245 wild baboons (N = 277 blood samples) living in the Amboseli ecosystem of Kenya (Alberts and Altmann, 2012) to generate a DNA methylation-based age predictor (an ‘epigenetic clock’; Hannum et al., 2013; Horvath, 2013). Starting with a data set of methylation levels for 458,504 CpG sites genome-wide (Figure 1—figure supplement 1; Supplementary file 1), we used elastic net regression to identify a set of 573 CpG sites that together accurately predict baboon age within a median absolute difference (MAD) of 1.1 years ± 1.9 s.d. (Figure 1; Supplementary file 1; Pearson’s r = 0.762, p=9.70×10−54; median adult life expectancy in this population is 10.3 years for females and 7.9 for males; Colchero et al., 2016). The choice of these sites reflects a balance between increasing predictive accuracy within the sample and minimizing generalization error to unobserved samples, using a similar approach as that used to develop epigenetic clocks in humans (Hannum et al., 2013; Horvath, 2013) (see also Materials and methods and Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

Figure 1. Epigenetic clock age predictions in the Amboseli baboons.

Predicted ages are shown relative to true chronological ages for (A) females (Pearson’s r = 0.78, p=6.78×10−30, N = 142 samples) and (B) males (r = 0.86, p=5.49×10−41, N = 135 samples). Solid lines represent the best-fit line; dashed lines show the line for y = x. (C, D) Predictions for individuals with at least two samples in the data set (N = 30; 14 females and 16 males). In 26 of 30 cases (87%), samples collected later were correctly predicted to be from an older animal.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Characteristics of the RRBS data set.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Proportion of the 458,504 evaluated CpG sites that overlapped annotated features of the Panu2 genome. (B) Proportion of annotated features in the Panu2 genome that overlapped at least one of the 458,504 evaluated CpG sites. (C) Distribution of mean DNA methylation levels for CpG sites within annotated features of the Panu2 genome. Each white box represents the interquartile range, with the median value depicted as a black horizontal bar. Whiskers extend to the most extreme values within 1.5× the interquartile range. As expected, CpG sites tended to be highly methylated genome-wide and have lower average methylation in promoters, enhancers, and CpG islands.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Comparison of clock performance across alternative values of alpha.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Alpha was set via grid search across possible values from 0.1 to 1, in steps of 0.1, and chosen based on the highest R2 value between predicted age and known chronological age (red lines). The blue lines show the median absolute difference between predicted and true age (lower is better) and exhibits roughly inverse behavior to R2. (A) For each clock generated with a different alpha value, the total number of CpG sites included in the clock is shown on top, and the number of clock sites that overlap the final clock used in this study (N = 573 sites, alpha = 0.1) is given in parentheses immediately below. (B, C) As in (A), but with results shown specifically for males (B) versus females (C).
Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Enrichment of the epigenetic clock CpG sites by genomic compartment.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

The log2(odds ratio) of CpG sites in the epigenetic clock, relative to all 458,504 CpG sites initially evaluated, in (A) annotated genomic regions and (B) in loci with putative regulatory activity or in or near genes that are responsive to age or immune stimulation. Regions of regulatory activity were identified with the massively parallel reporter assay, mSTARR-Seq (Lea et al., 2018a), following a liftover from the human genome to the baboon genome to identify putatively orthologous coordinates. Age differentially methylated regions (DMR) and genes responsive to lipopolysaccharide (LPS) were previously identified from blood samples from the same baboon population (Lea et al., 2015a; Lea et al., 2018b). Two-sided Fisher’s exact tests were performed separately for epigenetic clock sites that increased (positive clock sites: N = 459) or decreased (negative clock sites: N = 134) in DNA methylation levels with age. See Supplementary file 2 for a complete list of the genomic locations of the 573 epigenetic clock sites. *p<0.05, ***p<0.005.
Figure 1—figure supplement 4. Association between age and DNA methylation level for individual clock CpG sites.

Figure 1—figure supplement 4.

(A) Volcano plot of the effect size (βage) versus the –log10(p-value) of age effects on DNA methylation for males (blue) and females (red), based on estimates from a binomial mixed-effects model designed for bisulfite sequencing data (Lea et al., 2015a). Results for the 534 sites that could be modeled using this approach are shown. Other predictor variables in the model included a fixed effect for sample batch and a random effect that controlled for kinship (estimated via Queller and Goodnight’s r and multilocus microsatellite genotype data in the program coancestry Wang, 2011). Dashed line corresponds to a nominal p-value of 0.01. (B) Age effects on DNA methylation estimated separately in males and females are highly correlated (R2 = 0.83, p=3.35×10−204). The dashed line indicates the y = x line. The solid black line indicates the best-fit line.
Figure 1—figure supplement 5. Comparison of the performance of the epigenetic clock to other predictors of chronological age.

Figure 1—figure supplement 5.

Performance measures of age predictors are presented separately for females (A, C, E) and males (B, D, F) except for differential white blood cell counts (blood smears), where males and females were combined. Predictors are ordered in the same fashion in all panels (epigenetic clock to the left, and then following highest to lowest R2 in females). The breakpoint to define youthful versus aged animal BMI was 10 and 8 years old for females and males, respectively. (A, B) Adjusted R2 between predicted age and true chronological age. (C, D) Absolute difference between the y = x line (slope of one) and the slope of the best-fit line of predicted age as a function of true chronological age. This metric captures bias in age prediction estimates (values that are lower on the reverse-coded y-axis are more biased). (E, F) Median absolute difference (MAD) between each individual’s predicted age and true chronological age (values that are lower on the reverse-coded y-axis have higher MAD).

Consistent with findings in humans (Horvath, 2013), clock sites are enriched in genes, CpG islands, promoter regions, and putative enhancers, compared to the background set of all sites we initially considered (i.e., the 458,504 CpG sites that were candidates for inclusion in the clock; in humans, this background set is the set of analyzable sites on the Illumina 27K methylation array [Horvath, 2013; Figure 1—figure supplement 3]; Fisher’s exact tests, all p<0.05). Clock sites are also more common in age-associated differentially methylated regions in baboons (Figure 1—figure supplement 3; sites that increase with age: log2(odds ratio [OR])=4.189, p=3.64×10−9; sites that decrease with age: log2(OR)=5.344, p=1.54×10−8) (Lea et al., 2015a), such that many, but not all, of the clock sites also exhibit individual associations between DNA methylation levels and age (Figure 1—figure supplement 4 and Figure 2—figure supplement 1; Supplementary file 3). Additionally, clock sites were more likely to be found in regions that exhibit enhancer-like activity in a massively parallel reporter assay (sites that increase with age: log2(OR)=2.685, p=1.22×10−2; sites that decrease with age: log2(OR)=4.789, p=1.78×10−5) (Lea et al., 2018a) and in regions implicated in the gene expression response to bacteria in the Amboseli baboon population (overlap of lipopolysaccharide [LPS] up-regulated genes and sites that increase with age: log2[OR]=0.907, p=7.03×10−4; overlap of LPS down-regulated genes and sites that decrease with age: log2[OR]=1.715, p=1.55×10−3) (Lea et al., 2018b). Our results thus suggest that the Amboseli baboon epigenetic clock not only tracks chronological age, but also captures age-related changes in blood DNA methylation levels that are functionally important for gene regulation, particularly in relation to the immune system.

Comparison with other age-associated traits and differences between sexes

Overall, the clock performed favorably relative to other morphological or biomarker predictors of age in this population. The epigenetic clock generally explained more variance in true chronological age, resulted in lower median error, and exhibited less bias than predictions based on raw body mass index (BMI) or blood cell composition data from flow cytometry or blood smears (traits that change with age in baboons; Altmann et al., 2010; Jayashankar et al., 2003). Its performance was comparable to molar dentine exposure, a classical marker of age (Galbany et al., 2011; Figure 1—figure supplement 5). For a subset of 30 individuals, we had two samples collected at different points in time. The predicted ages from these longitudinally collected samples were older for the later-collected samples, as expected (Figure 1C,D; binomial test p=5.95×10−5). Furthermore, the change in epigenetic clock predictions between successive longitudinal samples positively predicted the actual change in age between sample dates (β = 0.312, p=0.027, controlling for sex; difference between actual change and predicted change: mean 3.11 years ± 3.25 s.d.).

However, clock performance was not equivalent in males and females. Specifically, we observed that the clock was significantly more accurate in males (Figure 1; males: N = 135; MAD = 0.85 years±1.0 s.d.; Pearson’s r = 0.86, p=5.49×10−41; females: N = 142; MAD = 1.6 years±2.4 s.d.; r = 0.78, p=6.78×10−30; two-sided Wilcoxon test for differences in absolute error by sex: p=4.37×10−9). Sex differences were also apparent in the slope of the relationship between predicted age and chronological age. Males show a 2.2-fold higher rate of change in predicted age, as a function of chronological age, compared to females (Figure 1A,B; chronological age by sex interaction in a linear model for predicted age: β = 0.448, p=9.66×10−19, N = 277). Interestingly, sex differences are not apparent in animals <8 years, which roughly corresponds to the age at which the majority of males have achieved adult dominance rank and dispersed from their natal group (Alberts and Altmann, 1995a; Alberts and Altmann, 1995b; Alberts et al., 2003) (N = 158, chronological age by sex interaction β = −0.038, p=0.808). Rather, sex differences become apparent after baboons have reached full physiological and social adulthood (N = 119, chronological age by sex interaction β = 0.459, p=9.74×10−7 in animals ≥ 8 years), when divergence between male and female life-history strategies is most marked (Alberts and Altmann, 1995a; Alberts and Altmann, 1995b; Alberts et al., 2003) and when aging rates between the sexes are predicted to diverge (Clutton-Brock and Isvaran, 2007; Kirkwood and Rose, 1991; Williams, 1957).

Because of these differences, we separated males and females for all subsequent analyses. However, we note that the effects of age on DNA methylation levels at individual clock sites are highly correlated between the sexes (Pearson’s r = 0.91, p=3.35×10−204), with effect sizes that are, on average, more precisely estimated in males (paired t-test p=4.53×10−74 for standard errors of βage; Figure 1—figure supplement 4). In other words, the sex differences in clock performance reflect changes in methylation that occur at the same CpG sites, but with higher variance in females. Lower accuracy in females compared to males therefore appears to result from the greater variability in DNA methylation change in older females (Figure 1).

Socioenvironmental predictors of variance in biological aging

Although the baboon epigenetic clock is a good predictor of age overall, individuals were often predicted to be somewhat older or younger than their known chronological age. In humans and some model systems, the sign and magnitude of this deviation captures information about physiological decline and/or mortality risk beyond that contained in chronological age alone (Maegawa et al., 2017; Petkovich et al., 2017; Stubbs et al., 2017; Ryan et al., 2020).

To test whether this observation extends to wild baboons, we focused on four factors of known importance to fitness in the Amboseli population. First, we considered cumulative early adversity, which is a strong predictor of shortened lifespan and offspring survival for female baboons (Tung et al., 2016; Zipple et al., 2019). We measured cumulative adversity as a count of major adverse experiences suffered in early life, including low maternal social status, early-life drought, a competing younger sibling, maternal loss, and high experienced population density (i.e., social group size). Second, we considered social bond strength in adulthood, which positively predicts longer adult lifespan in baboons, humans, and other wild social mammals (Archie et al., 2014a; Campos et al., 2020; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2020). Third, we considered dominance rank, which is a major determinant of access to mates, social partners, and other resources in the Amboseli baboons (Archie et al., 2014a; Alberts et al., 2006; Gesquiere et al., 2018; Lea et al., 2015b). Finally, we considered BMI, a measure of body condition that, in the Amboseli baboons, primarily reflects lean muscle mass (mean body fat percentages have been estimated at <2% in adult females and <9% in adult males) (Altmann et al., 1993). Because raw BMI (i.e., BMI not correcting for age) also tracks growth and development (increasing as baboons reach their prime and then declining thereafter [Altmann et al., 2010; Figure 2—figure supplement 2]; Pearson’s r in males between rank and raw BMI = −0.56, p=6.38×10−9), we calculated BMI relative to the expected value for each animal’s age using piecewise regression, which also eliminates correlations between BMI and male rank (Pearson’s r = −0.070, p=0.504). We refer to this adjusted measure of BMI as age-adjusted BMI.

Because high cumulative early adversity and low social bond strength are associated with increased mortality risk in the Amboseli baboons, we predicted that they would also be linked to increased epigenetic age. For rank and age-adjusted BMI, our predictions were less clear: improved resource access could conceivably slow biological aging, but increased investment in growth and reproduction (either through higher fertility in females or physical competition for rank in males) could also be energetically costly. To investigate these possibilities, we modeled the deviation between predicted age and known chronological age (Δage) as a function of cumulative early adversity, ordinal dominance rank, age-adjusted BMI, and for females, social bond strength to other females. Social bond strength was not included in the model for males, as this measure was not available for a large proportion of males in this data set (53.8%). We also included chronological age as a predictor in the model, as epigenetic age tends to be systematically overpredicted for young individuals and underpredicted for old individuals (Figure 1A,B; this bias has been observed in both foundational work on epigenetic clocks [Hannum et al., 2013] and recent epigenetic clocks calibrated for rhesus macaques [Horvath, 2020], as well as for elastic net regression analyses more generally [Engebretsen and Bohlin, 2019]). Including chronological age in the model, as previous studies have done (Hannum et al., 2013; Levine et al., 2018), controls for this compression effect. None of the predictor variables were strongly linearly correlated (all Pearson’s r < 0.35; Supplementary file 4).

Surprisingly, despite being two of the strongest known predictors of lifespan in wild baboons, neither cumulative early-life adversity nor social bond strength explain variation in Δage (Table 1). In contrast, high male dominance rank is strongly and significantly associated with larger values of Δage (β = −0.078, p=7.39×10−4; Figure 2; Table 1; Figure 2—figure supplement 3). Alpha males are predicted to be an average of 10.95 months older than their true chronological age – a difference that translates to 11.5% of a male baboon’s expected adult lifespan in Amboseli (Colchero et al., 2016). In contrast, dominance rank did not predict Δage in females (p=0.228; Table 1). Finally, age-adjusted BMI also predicted Δage in males (p=6.33×10−3), but not in females (p=0.682; Table 1). These results are robust to inclusion of read depth and bisulfite conversion rate as covariates in the model (Supplementary file 5; in males, read depth is correlated with chronological age [R2 = −0.409, p=0.038], but is not correlated with Δage [R2 = 0.003, p=0.561]).

Table 1. Predictors of Δage*.

Covariate β
(female)
p-value (female) β
(male)
p-value
(male)
Intercept 5.400 1.33 × 10−15 3.294 1.19 × 10−8
Cumulative early adversity −0.050 0.807 −0.005 0.973
Social bond strength 0.382 0.164
Dominance rank 0.025 0.228 −0.078 7.39 × 10−4
Age-adjusted BMI 0.026 0.682 0.111 6.33 × 10−3
Chronological age −0.699 1.62 × 10−28 −0.277 8.36 × 10−8

*Separate linear models for Δage were fit for females (N = 66) and for males (N = 93) for whom no data values were missing; social bond strength was not included in the model for males. Significant results are shown in bold.

Figure 2. Dominance rank predicts relative epigenetic age in male baboons.

High rank is associated with elevated values of Δage (β = −0.0785, p=7.39×10−4, N = 105). The y-axis shows relative epigenetic age, a measure of epigenetic aging similar to Δage that is based on the sample-specific residuals from the relationship between predicted age and true chronological age. Positive (negative) values correspond to predicted ages that are older (younger) than expected for that chronological age. Dominance rank is measured using ordinal values, such that smaller values indicate higher rank. Dots and error bars represent the means and standard errors, respectively. Gray values above the x-axis indicate sample sizes for each rank.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Methylation levels of clock CpG sites across different genomic compartments.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

Each circle represents a sample, with chronological age of the animal at time of sampling shown on the x-axis. The y-axis represents the average methylation level for that sample across CpG clock sites that overlap the annotated genomic region shown in the panel label, stratified by sites that increased (denoted ‘hyper’) or decreased (denoted ‘hypo’) methylation levels with age. Number of clock sites overlapping each annotated region is given in each panel title; a clock site can overlap multiple annotated regions and can therefore be represented in more than one plot. Red and blue lines represent best-fit lines for female and male samples, respectively. All best-fit lines are significant (p<1×10−4).
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. The relationship between age and body mass index in the Amboseli baboons.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Chronological age in years at the time of sampling versus body mass index (kilograms/meters2) for males and females in our sample. Two distinct patterns are observable for both sexes: a stage when animals are still growing (prior to ~7–8 years old) and a stage in which animals vary in BMI as adults. BMI in baboons is measured using the distance between the crown of the head and the rump as the ‘height’ measure and so differs in scale from humans, where BMI is calculated using standing height. Dashed gray line at BMI = 41 shows the cut-off for the analysis in which only males with BMI > 41 were retained for modeling Δage.
Figure 2—figure supplement 3. Relative epigenetic age versus chronological age.

Figure 2—figure supplement 3.

Each circle represents a baboon, colored by the animal’s dominance rank at the time of sampling. The y-axis shows relative epigenetic age, a measure of epigenetic aging similar to Δage that is based on the sample-specific residuals from the relationship between predicted age and true chronological age. Positive (negative) values correspond to predicted ages that are older (younger) than expected for that chronological age. Dominance rank is measured using ordinal values, such that smaller values indicate higher rank.
Figure 2—figure supplement 4. Male dominance rank versus chronological age.

Figure 2—figure supplement 4.

Each circle represents a male baboon at the time of sampling. Nearly all males in the top four rank positions are between ages 7 and 12 years (but not all 7–12 year olds are also high ranking: range of rank positions = 1–20), whereas both young and old males tend to be lower ranking. The quadratic curve represents the model with dominance rank as the response variable and age and age2 as the predictor variables. Rank-for-age was defined as the residuals of this model.

Despite the tendency for high-ranking males to have higher raw BMI due to increased muscle mass, the effects of rank and age-adjusted BMI in males are distinct. Specifically, modeling dominance rank after adjusting for raw BMI also produces a significant effect of rank on Δage in the same direction (p=9.93×10−4; Supplementary file 5), as does substituting the age-adjusted BMI measure for either raw BMI or the residuals of raw BMI after adjusting for dominance rank (rank p=1.52×10−2 and p=1.88×10−4, respectively; Supplementary file 5). In contrast, BMI is only a significant predictor of male Δage when corrected for age (i.e., age-adjusted) and when rank is included in the same model (Table 1; Supplementary file 5). Furthermore, we obtain the same qualitative results if all low BMI males are removed from the sample (BMI < 41; this cut-off was used because it drops all young males who have clearly not reached full adult size; p=7.14×10−3; Supplementary file 5). Dropping these males also eliminates the age-raw BMI correlation (Pearson’s r = −0.16, p=0.21).

Male dominance rank predicts epigenetic age

In baboon males, achieving high rank depends on physical condition and fighting ability (Alberts et al., 2003). Consequently, rank in males is dynamic across the life course: males tend to attain their highest rank between 7 and 12 years of age and fall in rank thereafter (Figure 2—figure supplement 4). Thus, nearly all males in the top four rank positions in our data set were between 7 and 12 years of age at the time they were sampled (however, because not all 7–12 year olds are high ranking, low-rank positions include males across the age range; Supplementary file 1, Figure 2—figure supplement 4). We therefore asked whether the association between high rank in males and accelerated epigenetic aging is a function of absolute rank values, regardless of age, or deviations from the expected mean rank given a male’s age (i.e., ‘rank-for-age’; Figure 2—figure supplement 4). We found that including rank-for-age as an additional covariate in the Δage model recapitulates the significant effect of ordinal male rank (p=0.045), but finds no effect of rank-for-age (p=0.819; Supplementary file 5). Our results therefore imply that males incur the costs of high rank primarily in early- to mid-adulthood, and only if they succeed in attaining high rank.

If attainment of high rank is linked to changes in epigenetic age within individuals, this pattern should be reflected in longitudinal samples. Specifically, males who improved in rank between samples should look older for age in their second sample relative to their first and vice versa. To assess this possibility, we calculated ‘relative epigenetic age’ (the residuals of the best-fit line relating chronological age and predicted age) for 14 males for whom we had repeated samples over time, 13 of whom changed ranks across sample dates (N = 28 samples, two per male). Samples collected when males were higher status predicted higher values of relative epigenetic age compared to samples collected when they were lower status (Figure 3; paired t-test, t = −2.99, p=0.011). For example, in the case of a male whom we first sampled at low status (ordinal rank = 18) and then after he had attained the alpha position (ordinal rank 1), the actual time that elapsed between samples was 0.79 years, but he exhibited an increase in predicted age of 2.6 years. Moreover, the two males that showed a decrease in predicted age, despite increasing in chronological age (Figure 1D), were among those that experienced the greatest drop in social status between samples. Thus, change in rank between samples for the same male predicts change in Δage, controlling for chronological age (R2 = 0.37, p=0.021). Consistent with our cross-sectional results, we found a suggestive relationship between change in Δage and BMI (R2 = 0.31, p=0.08). Here, too, the effect of dominance rank does not seem to be driven by BMI: while the association between change in Δage and change in rank is no longer significant when modeling rank after adjusting for raw BMI, the correlation remains consistent (R2 = 0.20, p=0.167). In contrast, raw BMI adjusted for rank explains almost none of the variance in change in Δage (R2 = 0.01, p=0.779).

Figure 3. Male baboons exhibit higher relative epigenetic age when they occupy higher ranks.

Figure 3.

Relative epigenetic age for males in which multiple samples were collected when they occupied different ordinal rank values. Arrow indicates the temporal direction of rank changes: left-facing arrows represent cases in which the later sample was collected when males were higher ranking, and right-facing arrows represent cases in which the later sample was collected when males were lower ranking.

Discussion

Together, our findings indicate that major environmental predictors of lifespan and mortality risk – particularly social bond strength and early-life adversity in this population – do not necessarily predict epigenetic measures of biological age. Although this assumption is widespread in the literature, including for epigenetic clock analyses (Liu et al., 2019; Shalev and Belsky, 2016), our results are broadly consistent with empirical results in humans. Specifically, while studies of early-life adversity, which also predicts lifespan in human populations, find relatively consistent support for a relationship between early adversity and accelerated epigenetic aging in children and adolescents (Jovanovic et al., 2017; Raffington et al., 2020; Brody et al., 2016a; Brody et al., 2016b; Davis et al., 2017; Marini, 2018; Sumner et al., 2019), there is little evidence for the long-term effects of early adversity on epigenetic age in adulthood (Zannas et al., 2015; Austin et al., 2018; Boks et al., 2015; Lawn et al., 2018; Simons et al., 2016; Wolf et al., 2018). Thus, while DNA methylation may make an important contribution to the biological embedding of early adversity into adulthood (Aristizabal et al., 2020; Hertzman, 2012), it does not seem to do so through affecting the epigenetic clock itself. Social and environmental effects on the clock instead seem to be most influenced by concurrent conditions, lending support to ‘recency’ models for environmental effects on aging that posit that health is more affected by the current environment than past experience (Ben-Shlomo and Kuh, 2002; Shanahan et al., 2011; Shanahan and Hofer, 2011). Additional longitudinal sampling will be necessary to evaluate whether current conditions alone can explain accelerated epigenetic aging or whether it also requires integrating the effects of exposures across the life course (the ‘accumulation’ model; Ben-Shlomo and Kuh, 2002; Shanahan and Hofer, 2011). Alternatively, the effects of early-life adversity and social bond strength may act through entirely distinct pathways than those captured by our epigenetic clock (including targeting tissues or cell types that we were unable to assess here). Indeed, the proliferation of alternative epigenetic clocks in humans has revealed that the clocks that best predict chronological age are not necessarily the clocks that most closely track environmental exposures, and the same is likely to be true in other species (Levine et al., 2018; Belsky et al., 2020). Notably, the functional significance of the clock – that is, whether it reflects the mechanisms that causally drive aging, or instead serves as a passive biomarker – also remains unclear.

We found that the most robust socioenvironmental predictor of epigenetic age in the Amboseli baboons is male dominance rank, with a secondary effect of age-adjusted BMI observable when rank is included in the same model. Although high BMI also predicts accelerated epigenetic age in some human populations (Ryan et al., 2020), high BMI in these human populations is related to being overweight or obese. In contrast, because wild-feeding baboons in Amboseli are extremely lean (Altmann et al., 1993), the range of BMI in most human populations is distinct from the range exhibited by our study subjects (importantly, BMI in humans is calculated differently than BMI in baboons [see Materials and methods], and therefore the BMI scales are species specific). Instead, the higher BMI values in our dataset represent baboons in better body condition (more muscle mass). Given that rank in male baboons is determined by physical fighting ability (Alberts et al., 2003), these results suggest that investment in body condition incurs physiological costs that accelerate biological age. If so, the rank effect we observe may be better interpreted as a marker of competitiveness, not as a consequence of being in a ‘high rank’ environment. In support of this idea, work on dominance rank and gene expression levels in the Amboseli baboons suggests that gene expression differences associated with male dominance rank tend to precede attainment of high rank, rather than being a consequence of behaviors exhibited after high rank is achieved (Lea et al., 2018b). Consistent with potential costs of attaining or maintaining high status, alpha males in Amboseli also exhibit elevated glucocorticoid levels (Gesquiere et al., 2011), increased expression of genes involved in innate immunity and inflammation (Lea et al., 2018b), and a trend toward elevated mortality risk (Campos et al., 2020). Males who can tolerate these costs and maintain high rank are nevertheless likely to enjoy higher lifetime reproductive success, since high rank is the single best predictor of mating and paternity success in baboon males (Alberts et al., 2003).

This interpretation may also explain major sex differences in the effects of rank on epigenetic age, where dominance rank shows no detectable effect in females. Dominance rank in female baboons is determined by nepotism, not physical competition: females typically insert into rank hierarchies directly below their mothers, and hierarchies therefore tend to remain stable over time (and even intergenerationally) (Hausfater et al., 1982). Our results contribute to an emerging picture in which dominance rank effects on both physiological and demographic outcomes are asymmetrical across sexes, and larger in males. Specifically, in addition to Δage, male rank is a better predictor of immune cell gene expression and glucocorticoid levels than female rank (Lea et al., 2018b; Gesquiere et al., 2011; Levy et al., 2020). Recent findings suggest that high rank may also predict increased mortality risk in male Amboseli baboons, whereas neither high rank nor low rank predicts increased mortality risk in females (Campos et al., 2020). Together, these results argue that social status/dominance rank effects should not be interpreted as a universal phenomenon. Instead, the manner through which social status is achieved and maintained is likely to be key to understanding its consequences for physiology, health, and fitness (Simons and Tung, 2019). Specifically, we predict that high status will be most likely to accelerate the aging process, including epigenetic age, in species-sex combinations where high status increases reproductive success or fecundity, and achieving status is energetically costly (e.g., male red deer, mandrills, and geladas; female meerkats Clutton-Brock et al., 2006; Clutton-Brock and Huchard, 2013; Emery Thompson and Georgiev, 2014). Expanding studies of biological aging to a broader set of natural populations, especially those for which behavioral and demographic data are also available, will be key to testing these predictions.

Materials and methods

Study population and biological sample collection

This study focused on a longitudinally monitored population of wild baboons (Papio cynocephalus, the yellow baboon, with some admixture from the closely related anubis baboon P. anubis Alberts and Altmann, 2001; Tung et al., 2008) in the Amboseli ecosystem of Kenya. This population has been continuously monitored by the Amboseli Baboon Research Project (ABRP) since 1971 (Alberts and Altmann, 2012). For the majority of study subjects (N = 242 of 245 individuals), birth dates were therefore known to within a few days’ error; for the remaining three individuals, birth dates were known within 3 months’ error (Supplementary file 1).

All DNA methylation data were generated from blood-derived DNA obtained during periodic darting efforts, as detailed in Lea et al., 2018b; Altmann et al., 1996; Tung et al., 2015. Samples were obtained under approval from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Duke University (currently #A044-21-02) and adhered to all the laws and regulations of Kenya. In brief, individually recognized study subjects were temporarily anesthetized using a Telazol-loaded dart delivered through a blow gun. Baboons were then safely moved to a new location where blood samples and morphometric data, including body mass and crown-rump length, were collected. Baboons were then allowed to recover from anesthesia in a covered holding cage and released to their group within 2–4 hr. Blood samples were stored at −20° C in Kenya until export to the United States.

DNA methylation data

DNA methylation data were generated from blood-extracted DNA collected from known individuals in the Amboseli study population (N = 277 samples from 245 animals; 13 females and 15 males were each sampled twice, and 1 female and 1 male were each sampled three times). Here, we analyzed a combined data set that included previously published RRBS (Meissner et al., 2005) data from the same population (N = 36 samples) (Lea et al., 2016) and new RRBS data from 241 additional samples.

RRBS libraries were constructed following Boyle et al., 2012, using ~200 ng baboon DNA plus 0.2 ng unmethylated lambda phage DNA per sample as input. Samples were sequenced to a mean depth of 17.8 (±10.5 s.d.) million reads on either the Illumina HiSeq 2000 or HiSeq 4000 platform (Supplementary file 1), with an estimated mean bisulfite conversion efficiency (based on the conversion rate of lambda phage DNA) of 99.8% (minimum = 98.1%). Sequence reads were trimmed with Trim Galore! (Krueger, 2012) to remove adapters and low quality sequence (Phred score < 20). Trimmed reads were mapped with BSMAP (Xi and Li, 2009) to the baboon genome (Panu2.0), allowing a 10% mismatch rate to account for the degenerate composition of bisulfite-converted DNA. We used autosomally mapped reads to count the number of methylated and total reads per CpG site, per sample (Xi and Li, 2009). To control for possible local genetic variation, we used BSMAP’s rescaled ‘effective total counts’ measures, which adjusts for the presence of possible CpG site disrupting genetic variants. Importantly, although our population consists of hybrids, previous work on DNA methylation variation across baboon species shows that species differences have a negligible effect on quantifying DNA methylation (i.e., the rate of incorrect calls differs by <0.4% between anubis and yellow baboons, the two species that contribute to ancestry in Amboseli; Vilgalys et al., 2019).

Following Lea et al., 2016; Lea et al., 2015a, CpG sites were filtered to retain sites with a mean methylation level between 0.1 and 0.9 (i.e., to exclude constitutively hyper- or hypo-methylated sites) and mean coverage of ≥5×. We also excluded any CpG sites with missing data for ≥5% of individuals in the sample. After filtering, we retained N = 458,504 CpG sites for downstream analysis. For the remaining missing data (mean number of missing sites per sample = 1.4 ± 3.5% s.d., equivalent to 6409 ± 16,024 s.d. sites), we imputed methylation levels using a k-nearest neighbors approach in the R package impute, using default parameters (Hastie et al., 2001).

Building the epigenetic clock

We used the R package glmnet (Friedman et al., 2009) version 2.0.10 to build a DNA methylation clock for baboons. Specifically, we fit a linear model in which the predictor variables were normalized levels of DNA methylation at 458,504 candidate clock CpG sites across the genome and the response variable was chronological age. To account for the excess of CpG sites relative to samples, glmnet uses an elastic net penalty to shrink predictor coefficients toward 0 (Friedman et al., 2010). Optimal alpha parameters were identified by grid searching across a range of alphas from 0 (equivalent to ridge regression) to 1 (equivalent to Lasso) by increments of 0.1, which impacts the number of clock CpG sites by varying the degree of shrinkage of the predictor coefficients toward 0 (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). We defined the optimal alpha as the value that maximized R2 between predicted and true chronological age across all samples. We set the regularization parameter lambda to the value that minimized mean-squared error during n-fold internal cross-validation.

To generate predicted age estimates for a given sample, we used a leave-one-out cross-validation approach in which all samples but the ‘test’ sample were included for model training, and the resulting model was used to predict age for the left-out test sample. To avoid leaking information from the training set into the test set, and to maximize the generalizability of the clock, we did not remove batch effects from the quantile normalized methylation ratio estimates. However, we confirmed that our results in the main model, for both males and for females, were robust if we added batch effect (previously generated samples [n = 36] versus newly generated samples [n = 241]) as a covariate. Training samples were scaled independently of the test sample in each leave-one-out model to avoid bleed-through of information from the test data into the training data. To do so, we first quantile normalized methylation ratios (the proportion of methylated counts to total counts for each CpG site) within each sample to a standard normal distribution. Training samples were then separated from the test sample and the methylation levels for each CpG site in the training set were quantile normalized across samples to a standard normal distribution. To predict age in the test sample, we compared the methylation value for each site in the test sample to the empirical cumulative distribution function for the training samples (at the same site) to estimate the quantile in which the training sample methylation ratio fell. The training sample was then assigned the same quantile value from the standard normal distribution using the function qnorm in R.

Epigenetic clock enrichment analyses

To evaluate whether CpG sites included in the epigenetic clock, relative to the 458,504 CpG background sites, were enriched in functionally important regions of the baboon genome (Lea et al., 2015a; Vilgalys et al., 2019), we used two-sided Fisher’s exact tests to investigate enrichment/depletion of the 573 epigenetic clock sites in (1) gene bodies and exons, based on the Ensembl annotation Panu2.0.90; (2) CpG islands annotated in the UCSC Genome Browser; (3) CpG shores, defined as the 2000 basepairs flanking CpG islands (following Lea et al., 2015a; Vilgalys et al., 2019; Irizarry et al., 2009); and (4) promoter regions, defined as the 2000 basepairs upstream of the 5′-most annotated transcription start site for each gene (following Lea et al., 2015a; Vilgalys et al., 2019). We also considered (5) putative enhancer regions, which have not been annotated for the Panu2.0 assembly. We therefore used ENCODE H3K4me1 ChIP-seq data from human peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) (ENCODE Project Consortium, 2012) and the liftOver tool to define likely enhancer coordinates in Panu2.0.

We also tested for enrichment of clock sites in regions of the genome that have been identified by previous empirical studies to be of special interest. First, we considered regions that likely have regulatory activity in blood cells, defined as all 200 base-pair windows that showed evidence of enhancer activity in a recently performed massively parallel reporter assay (Lea et al., 2018a). We used liftOver to identify the inferred homologous Panu2.0 coordinates for these windows, which were originally defined in the human genome. Second, we defined age-related differentially methylated regions in the Amboseli baboons based on genomic intervals found, in previous analyses, to contain at least three closely spaced age-associated CpG sites (inter-CpG distance ≤1 kb), as described in Lea et al., 2015a. Third, because inflammatory processes involved in innate immunity are strongly implicated in the aging process, we defined LPS up-regulated and LPS down-regulated genes as those genes that were significantly differentially expressed (1% false discovery rate) between unstimulated Amboseli baboon white blood cells and LPS-stimulated cells from the same individual, following 10 hr of culture in parallel (Lea et al., 2018b).

Comparisons to alternative predictors of aging

To assess the utility of the DNA methylation clock relative to other data types, we compared its predictive accuracy to clocks based on three other age-related phenotypes: tooth wear (percent molar dentine exposure; Galbany et al., 2011), body condition (BMI; Altmann et al., 2010), and blood cell type composition (blood smear counts and lymphocyte/monocyte proportions from flow cytometry performed on peripheral blood mononuclear cells, as in Lea et al., 2018b; Snyder-Mackler et al., 2016). Leave-one-out model training and prediction were performed for each data type using linear modeling. To compare the relative predictive accuracy of each data type, we calculated the R2 between predicted and chronological age, the MAD between predicted and chronological age, and the bias in age predictions (the absolute value of 1 − slope of the best-fit line between predicted and chronological age) (Figure 1—figure supplement 5).

Tooth wear

Molar enamel in baboons wears away with age to expose the underlying dentine layer. Percent dentine exposure (PDE) on the molar occlusal surface has been shown to be strongly age-correlated in previous work (Galbany et al., 2011). To assess its predictive power, we obtained PDE data from tooth casts reported by Galbany et al., 2011 for the left upper molars (tooth positions M1, M2, M3) and left lower molars (tooth positions M1, M2, M3) for 39 males and 34 females in our data set. For each molar position (M1, M2, M3) within each individual, we calculated PDE as the mean for the upper and lower molars. Because dentine exposure scales quadratically with respect to age (Galbany et al., 2011), we fit age as a function of PDE using the following model: 

age ~PDEM1+PDEM2 +PDEM3

Body mass index

For both male and female baboons in Amboseli, body mass increases with age until individuals reach peak size and then tends to decrease with age as animals lose fat and/or muscle mass (Altmann et al., 2010). To quantify body condition using body mass, we calculated BMI values for 139 males and 154 females for whom body mass and crown-rump length data were available from periodic darting efforts. We retained only measures taken from animals born into and sampled in wild-feeding study groups, when sex-skin swellings (in females only) that could affect crown-rump length measures were absent. BMI was calculated as mass (kilograms) divided by crown-rump length (meters squared), following Altmann et al., 1993. To assess the predictive power of age-adjusted BMI, we built sex-specific piecewise-regression models using the package segmented in R (Muggeo and Muggeo, 2017). Breakpoints for the piecewise-regression models (to separate ‘youthful’ versus ‘aged’ animals) were initialized at 8 years old for males and 10 years old for females, following findings from previous work on body mass in the Amboseli population (Altmann et al., 2010).

Blood cell type composition

The proportions of different cell types in blood change across the life course, including in baboons (Jayashankar et al., 2003). We assessed the predictive power of blood cell composition for age using two data sets. First, we used data collected from blood smear counts (N = 134) for five major white blood cell types: basophils, eosinophils, monocytes, lymphocytes, and neutrophils. Second, we used data on the proportional representation of five PBMC subsets: cytotoxic T cells, helper T cells, B cells, monocytes, and natural killer cells, measured using flow cytometry as reported by Lea et al., 2018b (N = 53). Cell types were included as individual covariates for leave-one-out model training.

Sources of variance in predicted age

We asked whether factors known to be associated with inter-individual variation in fertility or survival also predict inter-individual variation in Δage (predicted age from the epigenetic clock minus known chronological age). To do so, we fit linear models separately for males and females, with Δage as the dependent variable and dominance rank at the time of sampling, cumulative early adversity, age-adjusted BMI, and chronological age as predictor variables (Tung et al., 2016). For females, we also included a measure of social bond strength to other females as a predictor variable, based on findings that show that socially isolated females experience higher mortality rates in adulthood (Archie et al., 2014a; Silk et al., 2010). Samples with missing values for any of the predictor variables were excluded in the model, resulting in a final analysis set of 66 female samples (from 59 females) and 93 male samples (from 84 males). The chronological ages of samples with complete data relative to samples with missing data were equivalent for females (t-test, t = 1.95, p=0.053) but were slightly lower for males (t-test, t = −3.04, p=0.003; mean chronological ages are 7.98 and 9.65 years for complete and missing samples, respectively). Predictor variables were measured as follows.

Dominance rank

Sex-specific dominance hierarchies were constructed monthly for every social group in the study population based on the outcomes of dyadic agonistic encounters. An animal was considered to win a dyadic agonistic encounter if it gave aggressive or neutral, but not submissive, gestures, and the other animal gave submissive gestures only (Hausfater, 1975). These wins and losses were entered into a sex-specific data matrix, such that animals were ordered to minimize the number of entries falling below the matrix diagonal (which would indicate that the lower ranked individual won a dyadic encounter). Ordinal dominance ranks were assigned on a monthly basis to every adult based on these matrices, such that low numbers represent high rank/social status and high numbers represent low rank/social status (Alberts et al., 2003; Hausfater et al., 1982). Although most analyses of data from the Amboseli baboons have used ordinal ranks as the primary measure of social status, in some analyses proportional rank (i.e., the proportion of same-sex members of an individual’s social group that he or she dominates) has proven to be a stronger predictor of other trait outcomes (Archie et al., 2014b; Levy, 2020). In this study, we chose to use ordinal ranks, but proportional and ordinal dominance rank were highly correlated in this particular dataset (R2 = 0.70, p=1.13×10−58). Using ordinal rank rather than proportional rank therefore did not qualitatively affect our analyses. Additionally, to investigate whether the patterns we observed are due to a consistent effect of rank across all ages, or instead an effect of being high or low rank relative to the expected (mean) value for a male’s age, we also calculated a ‘rank-for-age’ value. Rank-for-age is defined as the residuals of a model with dominance rank as the response variable and age and age2 as the predictor variables (Figure 2—figure supplement 4).

Cumulative early adversity

Previous work in Amboseli defined a cumulative early adversity score as the sum of six different adverse conditions that a baboon could experience during early life (Tung et al., 2016). This index strongly predicts adult lifespan in female baboons, and a modified version of this index also predicts offspring survival (Zipple et al., 2019). To maximize the sample size available for analysis, we excluded maternal social connectedness, the source of adversity with the highest frequency of missing data, leaving us with a cumulative early adversity score generated from five different binary-coded adverse experiences. These experiences were as follows: (1) early-life drought (defined as ≤200 mm of rainfall in the first year of life), which is linked to reduced fertility in females (Lea et al., 2015b; Beehner et al., 2006); (2) having a low ranking mother (defined as falling within the lowest quartile of ranks for individuals in the data set), which predicts age at maturation (Altmann and Alberts, 2003a; Altmann et al., 1988; Charpentier et al., 2008); (3) having a close-in-age younger sibling (<1.5 years), which may redirect maternal investment to the sibling (Altmann et al., 1978), (4) being born into a large social group, which may increase within-group competition for shared resources (Lea et al., 2015b; Charpentier et al., 2008; Altmann and Alberts, 2003b), and (5) maternal death before the age of 4, which results in a loss of both social and nutritional resources (Charpentier et al., 2008; Lea et al., 2014).

Body mass index

Age-adjusted BMI was modeled as the residuals from sex-specific piecewise-regression models relating raw BMI to age. By taking this approach, we asked whether having relatively high BMI for one’s age and sex predicted higher (or lower) Δage. To calculate rank-adjusted BMI values, we modeled raw BMI as a function of rank in a linear model and calculated the residuals from the model. To calculate dominance rank adjusted for raw BMI, we took the inverse approach. We note that BMI for baboons is not directly comparable to BMI for humans because baboon BMI is measured as body mass divided by the square of crown-rump length (because baboons are quadrupedal), whereas human BMI is calculated as body mass divided by the square of standing height.

Social bond strength

For this analysis, we measured female social bond strength to other females using the dyadic sociality index (DSIF) (Campos et al., 2020). We did not include this parameter (male’s social bond strength to females) for the male model because this measure is unavailable for many males in this data set. DSIF was calculated as an individual’s average bond strength with her top three female social partners, in the 365 days prior to the day of sampling, controlling for observer effort. This approach is based on representative interaction sampling of grooming interactions between females, in which observers record all grooming interactions in their line of sight while moving through the group conducting random-ordered, 10 min long focal animal samples of pre-selected individuals. Because smaller groups receive more observer effort per individual and per dyad (and thus record more grooming interactions per individual or dyad), we estimated observer effort for dyad d in year y as:

Ed,y=cd(sd)fd 

where cd is the number of days the two females in a dyad were coresident in the same social group, sd is the number of focal samples taken during the dyad’s coresidence, and fd is the average number of females in the group during the dyad’s coresidence.

DSIF for each adult female dyad in each year is the z-scored residual, ε, from the model:

log(Rd,y)=β(log(Ed,y))+ε

where Rd,y is the number of grooming interactions for dyad d in year y divided by the number of days that the two individuals were coresident, and Ed,y is observer effort.

Analysis of longitudinal samples

To test whether changes in rank predict changes in relative epigenetic age within individuals, we used data from 11 males from the original data set and generated additional RRBS data for nine samples, resulting in a final set of 14 males who each were sampled at least twice in the data set, 13 of whom occupied different ordinal ranks at different sampling dates (mean years elapsed between samples = 3.7 ± 1.65 s.d.; mean absolute difference in dominance ranks = 1.29 ± 8.34 s.d.). This effort increased our total sample size to N = 286 samples from 248 unique individuals. To incorporate the new samples into our analysis, we reperformed leave-one-out age prediction with N-fold internal cross-validation at the optimal alpha selected for the original N = 277 samples (alpha = 0.1). For the 277 samples carried over from the original analysis, we verified that age predictions were nearly identical between the previous analysis and the expanded data set (R2 = 0.98, p=2.21×10−239; Supplementary file 1).

Based on the new age predictions for males in the data set (N = 140), we again calculated relative epigenetic age as the residual of the best-fit line relating predicted age to chronological age. We then used the 14 males with repeated DNA methylation profiles and rank measures in this data set to test whether, within individuals, changes in dominance rank or rank-for-age explained changes in relative epigenetic age between samples. In total, five males were sampled three times. For four of these five, we only included the two samples that were sampled the farthest apart in time (i.e., excluded the temporal middle sample) to maximize the age change between sample dates. For the fifth male, BMI information was missing for the third sample, so we included the first two samples collected in time.

Code availability

All R code used to analyze data in this study is available at https://github.com/janderson94/BaboonEpigeneticAging; Anderson, 2021; with a copy archived at swh:1:rev:58ca836d3416c2a447cbd055aee66c11140aec86.

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the support provided by the National Science Foundation and the National Institutes of Health for the majority of the data represented here, currently through NSF IOS 1456832, NIH R01AG053308, R01AG053330, R01HD088558, and P01AG031719. RAJ is supported by NIH F32HD095616 and JAA by NSF #2018264636. We also acknowledge support from the Canadian Institute of Advanced Research (Child and Brain Development Program); support for high-performance computing resources from the North Carolina Biotechnology Center (Grant Number 2016-IDG-1013); and a seed grant from the Center for Population Health and Aging (P30AG034424 to A O’Rand). We thank the members of the Amboseli Baboon Research Project for collecting the data presented here, especially J Altmann for her foundational role in establishing the study population and these data sets; J Gordon, N Learn, and K Pinc for managing the database; RS Mututua, S Sayialel, and JK Warutere for data collection in the field; and T Wango and V Oudu for their assistance in Nairobi. We also thank the Kenya Wildlife Service, University of Nairobi, the Institute of Primate Research, the National Museums of Kenya, the National Council for Science, Technology, and Innovation, members of the Amboseli-Longido pastoralist communities, the Enduimet Wildlife Management Area, Ker and Downey Safaris, Air Kenya, and Safarilink for their assistance in Kenya. Finally, we thank J Galbany for assistance with the molar dentine data set; current and past members of the Tung, Alberts, Archie, and Altmann labs for their helpful feedback; and J Higham, C Kuzawa, and three anonymous reviewers for constructive critiques of a previous version of this manuscript. This research was approved by IACUCs at Duke University, University of Notre Dame, and Princeton University and adhered to all the laws and regulations of Kenya. For a complete set of acknowledgments of funding sources, logistical assistance, and data collection and management, please visit http://amboselibaboons.nd.edu/acknowledgements/.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Jenny Tung, Email: jenny.tung@duke.edu.

George H Perry, Pennsylvania State University, United States.

George H Perry, Pennsylvania State University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Science Foundation IOS 1456832 to Susan C Alberts.

  • National Institutes of Health R01AG053308 to Susan C Alberts.

  • National Institutes of Health R01AG053330 to Elizabeth A Archie.

  • National Institutes of Health R01HD088558 to Jenny Tung.

  • National Institutes of Health P01AG031719 to Susan C Alberts.

  • National Institutes of Health F32HD095616 to Rachel A Johnston.

  • National Science Foundation 2018264636 to Jordan A Anderson.

  • Foerster-Bernstein Foundation Postdoctoral Fellowship to Rachel A Johnston.

  • North Carolina Biotechnology Center 2016-IDG-1013 to Jenny Tung.

  • Center for Population Health and Aging P30AG034424 to Jenny Tung.

  • Canadian Institute for Advanced Research to Jenny Tung.

Additional information

Competing interests

Reviewing editor, eLife.

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: Samples were obtained under approval from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) of Duke University (#A273-17-12) and adhered to all the laws and regulations of Kenya.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Samples used for generating the RRBS dataset.
elife-66128-supp1.xlsx (45KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 2. Genomic coordinates, average annual methylation level change (averaged across the 277 leave-one-out models), and genomic context for the 573 CpG sites in the epigenetic clock.
elife-66128-supp2.xlsx (51.3KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 3. Results from site-by-site models (for each of 534 testable clock sites) predicting chronological age, controlling for relatedness.
elife-66128-supp3.xlsx (43.1KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 4. Pearson correlations among covariates for females (above diagonal) and males (below diagonal), with p-values in parentheses.
elife-66128-supp4.xlsx (9.3KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 5. Linear models for Δage, fit for males, with significant results shown in bold.
elife-66128-supp5.xlsx (10.6KB, xlsx)
Transparent reporting form

Data availability

All sequencing data generated during this study are available in the NCBI Sequence Read Archive (project accession PRJNA648767), and processed counts data are available in the Dryad digital repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.qjq2bvqf0).

The following datasets were generated:

Anderson JA, Johnston RA, Lea AJ, Campos FA, Voyles TN, Akinyi MY, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. 2021. Baboon Epigenetic Aging. NCBI BioProject. PRJNA648767

Anderson JA, Johnston RA, Lea AJ, Campos FA, Voyles TN, Akinyi MY, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. 2021. High social status males experience accelerated epigenetic aging in wild baboons. Dryad Digital Repository.

The following previously published dataset was used:

Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Tung J. 2015. Papio cynocephalus Epigenomics. NCBI BioProject. SRP058411

References

  1. Alberts SC, Watts HE, Altmann J. Queuing and queue-jumping: long-term patterns of reproductive skew in male savannah baboons, Papio cynocephalus. Animal Behaviour. 2003;65:821–840. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2106. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Alberts SC, Buchan JC, Altmann J. Sexual selection in wild baboons: from mating opportunities to paternity success. Animal Behaviour. 2006;72:1177–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.05.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Alberts SC, Altmann J. Balancing costs and opportunities: dispersal in male baboons. The American Naturalist. 1995a;145:279–306. doi: 10.1086/285740. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Alberts SC, Altmann J. Preparation and activation: determinants of age at reproductive maturity in male baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1995b;36:397–406. doi: 10.1007/BF00177335. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  5. Alberts SC, Altmann J. Immigration and hybridization patterns of yellow and anubis baboons in and around Amboseli, Kenya. American Journal of Primatology. 2001;53:139–154. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Alberts SC, Altmann J. Long-Term Field Studies of Primates. Springer; 2012. [Google Scholar]
  7. Altmann J, Altmann SA, Hausfater G. Primate infant's effects on mother's future reproduction. Science. 1978;201:1028–1030. doi: 10.1126/science.98844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Altmann J, Hausfater G, Altmann SA. Reproductive Success: Studies of Individual Variation in Contrasting Breeding Systems. The University of Chicago Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  9. Altmann J, Schoeller D, Altmann SA, Muruthi P, Sapolsky RM. Body size and fatness of free-living baboons reflect food availability and activity levels. American Journal of Primatology. 1993;30:149–161. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350300207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Altmann J, Alberts SC, Haines SA, Dubach J, Muruthi P, Coote T, Geffen E, Cheesman DJ, Mututua RS, Saiyalel SN, Wayne RK, Lacy RC, Bruford MW. Behavior predicts genes structure in a wild primate group. PNAS. 1996;93:5797–5801. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.12.5797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Altmann J, Gesquiere L, Galbany J, Onyango PO, Alberts SC. Life history context of reproductive aging in a wild primate model. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2010;1204:127–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05531.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Altmann J, Alberts SC. Offspring: The Biodemography of Fertility and Family Behavior. The National Academies Press; 2003a. [Google Scholar]
  13. Altmann J, Alberts SC. Variability in reproductive success viewed from a life-history perspective in baboons. American Journal of Human Biology. 2003b;15:401–409. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.10157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Anderson JA. Baboon Epigenetic Aging (Anderson and Johnston et al. 2021, eLife) swh:1:rev:58ca836d3416c2a447cbd055aee66c11140aec86Software Hertage. 2021 https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:dir:e0f99c04d36a427d57ff7fcc92173fce62e8117d;origin=https://github.com/janderson94/BaboonEpigeneticAging;visit=swh:1:snp:0e2467265f144587628488053f736caadb7af6e5;anchor=swh:1:rev:58ca836d3416c2a447cbd055aee66c11140aec86
  15. Archie EA, Tung J, Clark M, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Social affiliation matters: both same-sex and opposite-sex relationships predict survival in wild female baboons. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2014a;281:20141261. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Archie EA, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Costs of reproduction in a long-lived female primate: injury risk and wound healing. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2014b;68:1183–1193. doi: 10.1007/s00265-014-1729-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Aristizabal MJ, Anreiter I, Halldorsdottir T, Odgers CL, McDade TW, Goldenberg A, Mostafavi S, Kobor MS, Binder EB, Sokolowski MB, O'Donnell KJ. Biological embedding of experience: a primer on epigenetics. PNAS. 2020;117:23261–23269. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1820838116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Austin MK, Chen E, Ross KM, McEwen LM, Maclsaac JL, Kobor MS, Miller GE. Early-life socioeconomic disadvantage, not current, predicts accelerated epigenetic aging of monocytes. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;97:131–134. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Beehner JC, Onderdonk DA, Alberts SC, Altmann J. The ecology of conception and pregnancy failure in wild baboons. Behavioral Ecology. 2006;17:741–750. doi: 10.1093/beheco/arl006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Belsky DW, Caspi A, Arseneault L, Baccarelli A, Corcoran DL, Gao X, Hannon E, Harrington HL, Rasmussen LJ, Houts R, Huffman K, Kraus WE, Kwon D, Mill J, Pieper CF, Prinz JA, Poulton R, Schwartz J, Sugden K, Vokonas P, Williams BS, Moffitt TE. Quantification of the pace of biological aging in humans through a blood test, the DunedinPoAm DNA methylation algorithm. eLife. 2020;9:e54870. doi: 10.7554/eLife.54870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Ben-Shlomo Y, Kuh D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2002;31:285–293. doi: 10.1093/ije/31.2.285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Boks MP, van Mierlo HC, Rutten BP, Radstake TR, De Witte L, Geuze E, Horvath S, Schalkwyk LC, Vinkers CH, Broen JC, Vermetten E. Longitudinal changes of telomere length and epigenetic age related to traumatic stress and post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;51:506–512. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Boyle P, Clement K, Gu H, Smith ZD, Ziller M, Fostel JL, Holmes L, Meldrim J, Kelley F, Gnirke A, Meissner A. Gel-free multiplexed reduced representation bisulfite sequencing for large-scale DNA methylation profiling. Genome Biology. 2012;13:R92. doi: 10.1186/gb-2012-13-10-r92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Brody GH, Miller GE, Yu T, Beach SR, Chen E. Supportive family environments ameliorate the Link Between Racial Discrimination and Epigenetic Aging: A Replication Across Two Longitudinal Cohorts. Psychological Science. 2016a;27:530–541. doi: 10.1177/0956797615626703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Brody GH, Yu T, Chen E, Beach SR, Miller GE. Family-centered prevention ameliorates the longitudinal association between risky family processes and epigenetic aging. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2016b;57:566–574. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Campos FA, Villavicencio F, Archie EA, Colchero F, Alberts SC. Social bonds, social status and survival in wild baboons: a tale of two sexes. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2020;375:20190621. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Charpentier MJ, Tung J, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Age at maturity in wild baboons: genetic, environmental and demographic influences. Molecular Ecology. 2008;17:2026–2040. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.03724.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Clutton-Brock TH, Hodge SJ, Spong G, Russell AF, Jordan NR, Bennett NC, Sharpe LL, Manser MB. Intrasexual competition and sexual selection in cooperative mammals. Nature. 2006;444:1065–1068. doi: 10.1038/nature05386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Clutton-Brock TH, Huchard E. Social competition and selection in males and females. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2013;368:20130074. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Clutton-Brock TH, Isvaran K. Sex differences in ageing in natural populations of vertebrates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2007;274:3097–3104. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2007.1138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Colchero F, Rau R, Jones OR, Barthold JA, Conde DA, Lenart A, Nemeth L, Scheuerlein A, Schoeley J, Torres C, Zarulli V, Altmann J, Brockman DK, Bronikowski AM, Fedigan LM, Pusey AE, Stoinski TS, Strier KB, Baudisch A, Alberts SC, Vaupel JW. The emergence of longevous populations. PNAS. 2016;113:E7681–E7690. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612191113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Davis EG, Humphreys KL, McEwen LM, Sacchet MD, Camacho MC, MacIsaac JL, Lin DTS, Kobor MS, Gotlib IH. Accelerated DNA methylation age in adolescent girls: associations with elevated diurnal cortisol and reduced hippocampal volume. Translational Psychiatry. 2017;7:e1223. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. De Paoli‐Iseppi R. Age estimation in a long‐lived seabird (Ardenna tenuirostris) using DNA methylation‐based biomarkers. Molecular Ecology Resources. 2018;19:12981. doi: 10.1111/1755-0998.12981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Declerck K, Vanden Berghe W. Back to the future: epigenetic clock plasticity towards healthy aging. Mechanisms of Ageing and Development. 2018;174:18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2018.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Emery Thompson M, Georgiev AV. The high price of success: costs of mating effort in male primates. International Journal of Primatology. 2014;35:609–627. doi: 10.1007/s10764-014-9790-4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. ENCODE Project Consortium An integrated encyclopedia of DNA elements in the human genome. Nature. 2012;489:57–74. doi: 10.1038/nature11247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Engebretsen S, Bohlin J. Statistical predictions with glmnet. Clinical Epigenetics. 2019;11:1–3. doi: 10.1186/s13148-019-0730-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. R package version; 2009. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/glmnet/index.html [Google Scholar]
  39. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. Journal of Statistical Software. 2010;33:1. doi: 10.18637/jss.v033.i01. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Galbany J, Altmann J, Pérez-Pérez A, Alberts SC. Age and individual foraging behavior predict tooth wear in amboseli baboons. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2011;144:51–59. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.21368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Gesquiere LR, Learn NH, Simao MC, Onyango PO, Alberts SC, Altmann J. Life at the top: rank and stress in wild male baboons. Science. 2011;333:357–360. doi: 10.1126/science.1207120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Gesquiere LR, Altmann J, Archie EA, Alberts SC. Interbirth intervals in wild baboons: environmental predictors and hormonal correlates. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 2018;166:107–126. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.23407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hannum G, Guinney J, Zhao L, Zhang L, Hughes G, Sadda S, Klotzle B, Bibikova M, Fan JB, Gao Y, Deconde R, Chen M, Rajapakse I, Friend S, Ideker T, Zhang K. Genome-wide methylation profiles reveal quantitative views of human aging rates. Molecular Cell. 2013;49:359–367. doi: 10.1016/j.molcel.2012.10.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Hastie T, Tibshirani R, Narasimhan B, Chu G. Impute: imputation for microarray data. Bioinformatics. 2001;17:520–525. [Google Scholar]
  45. Hausfater G. Dominance and reproduction in baboons (Papio cynocephalus) Contributions to Primatology. 1975;7:1–150. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Hausfater G, Altmann J, Altmann S. Long-Term consistency of dominance relations among female baboons (Papio cynocephalus) Science. 1982;217:752–755. doi: 10.1126/science.217.4561.752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hertzman C. Putting the concept of biological embedding in historical perspective. PNAS. 2012;109:17160–17167. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1202203109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLOS Medicine. 2010;7:e1000316. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Horvath S. DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biology. 2013;14:R115. doi: 10.1186/gb-2013-14-10-r115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Horvath S, Erhart W, Brosch M, Ammerpohl O, von Schönfels W, Ahrens M, Heits N, Bell JT, Tsai PC, Spector TD, Deloukas P, Siebert R, Sipos B, Becker T, Röcken C, Schafmayer C, Hampe J. Obesity accelerates epigenetic aging of human liver. PNAS. 2014;111:15538–15543. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1412759111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Horvath S. Epigenetic clock and methylation studies in the rhesus macaque. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.09.21.307108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  52. Horvath S, Raj K. DNA methylation-based biomarkers and the epigenetic clock theory of ageing. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2018;19:371–384. doi: 10.1038/s41576-018-0004-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Irizarry RA, Ladd-Acosta C, Wen B, Wu Z, Montano C, Onyango P, Cui H, Gabo K, Rongione M, Webster M, Ji H, Potash J, Sabunciyan S, Feinberg AP. The human Colon cancer methylome shows similar hypo- and hypermethylation at conserved tissue-specific CpG island shores. Nature Genetics. 2009;41:178–186. doi: 10.1038/ng.298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Jayashankar L, Brasky KM, Ward JA, Attanasio R. Lymphocyte modulation in a baboon model of immunosenescence. Clinical Diagnostic Laboratory Immunology. 2003;10:870–875. doi: 10.1128/CDLI.10.5.870-875.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Jones OR, Scheuerlein A, Salguero-Gómez R, Camarda CG, Schaible R, Casper BB, Dahlgren JP, Ehrlén J, García MB, Menges ES, Quintana-Ascencio PF, Caswell H, Baudisch A, Vaupel JW. Diversity of ageing across the tree of life. Nature. 2014;505:169–173. doi: 10.1038/nature12789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Jovanovic T, Vance LA, Cross D, Knight AK, Kilaru V, Michopoulos V, Klengel T, Smith AK. Exposure to violence accelerates epigenetic aging in children. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:8962. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-09235-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kirkwood TB, Rose MR. Evolution of senescence: late survival sacrificed for reproduction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 1991;332:15–24. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1991.0028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Krueger F. Trim Galore: a wrapper tool around Cutadapt and FastQC to consistently apply quality and adapter trimming to FastQ files, with some extra functionality for MspI-digested RRBS-type (Reduced Representation Bisufite-Seq) libraries. v3Babraham Bioinformatics. 2012 http://www.bioinformatics.babraham.ac.uk/projects/trim_galore/
  59. Lawn RB, Anderson EL, Suderman M, Simpkin AJ, Gaunt TR, Teschendorff AE, Widschwendter M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Relton CL, Howe LD. Psychosocial adversity and socioeconomic position during childhood and epigenetic age: analysis of two prospective cohort studies. Human Molecular Genetics. 2018;27:1301–1308. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddy036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lea AJ, Learn NH, Theus MJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Complex sources of variance in female dominance rank in a nepotistic society. Animal Behaviour. 2014;94:87–99. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lea AJ, Tung J, Zhou X. A flexible, efficient binomial mixed model for identifying differential DNA methylation in bisulfite sequencing data. PLOS Genetics. 2015a;11:e1005650. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1005650. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Tung J. Developmental constraints in a wild primate. The American Naturalist. 2015b;185:809–821. doi: 10.1086/681016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Tung J. Resource base influences genome-wide DNA methylation levels in wild baboons (Papio cynocephalus) Molecular Ecology. 2016;25:1681–1696. doi: 10.1111/mec.13436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lea AJ, Vockley CM, Johnston RA, Del Carpio CA, Barreiro LB, Reddy TE, Tung J. Genome-wide quantification of the effects of DNA methylation on human gene regulation. eLife. 2018a;7:e37513. doi: 10.7554/eLife.37513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lea AJ, Akinyi MY, Nyakundi R, Mareri P, Nyundo F, Kariuki T, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. Dominance rank-associated gene expression is widespread, sex-specific, and a precursor to high social status in wild male baboons. PNAS. 2018b;115:E12163–E12171. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1811967115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Levine ME, Lu AT, Bennett DA, Horvath S. Epigenetic age of the pre-frontal cortex is associated with neuritic plaques, amyloid load, and Alzheimer's disease related cognitive functioning. Aging. 2015;7:1198–1211. doi: 10.18632/aging.100864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Levine ME, Lu AT, Quach A, Chen BH, Assimes TL, Bandinelli S, Hou L, Baccarelli AA, Stewart JD, Li Y, Whitsel EA, Wilson JG, Reiner AP, Aviv A, Lohman K, Liu Y, Ferrucci L, Horvath S. An epigenetic biomarker of aging for lifespan and healthspan. Aging. 2018;10:573–591. doi: 10.18632/aging.101414. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Levy EJ, Gesquiere LR, McLean E, Franz M, Warutere JK, Sayialel SN, Mututua RS, Wango TL, Oudu VK, Altmann J, Archie EA, Alberts SC. Higher dominance rank is associated with lower glucocorticoids in wild female baboons: a rank metric comparison. Hormones and Behavior. 2020;125:104826. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Levy EJ. Comparing proportional and ordinal dominance ranks reveals multiple competitive landscapes in an animal society. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.04.30.065805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  70. Liu Z, Chen BH, Assimes TL, Ferrucci L, Horvath S, Levine ME. The role of epigenetic aging in education and racial/ethnic mortality disparities among older U.S. women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019;104:18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. López-Otín C, Blasco MA, Partridge L, Serrano M, Kroemer G. The hallmarks of aging. Cell. 2013;153:1194–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Maegawa S, Lu Y, Tahara T, Lee JT, Madzo J, Liang S, Jelinek J, Colman RJ, Issa JJ. Caloric restriction delays age-related methylation drift. Nature Communications. 2017;8:539. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-00607-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Marini S. Predicting cellular aging following exposure to adversity: does accumulation, recency, or developmental timing of exposure matter? bioRxiv. 2018 doi: 10.1101/355743. [DOI]
  74. Marioni RE, Shah S, McRae AF, Ritchie SJ, Muniz-Terrera G, Harris SE, Gibson J, Redmond P, Cox SR, Pattie A, Corley J, Taylor A, Murphy L, Starr JM, Horvath S, Visscher PM, Wray NR, Deary IJ. The epigenetic clock is correlated with physical and cognitive fitness in the lothian birth cohort 1936. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2015;44:1388–1396. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyu277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Meissner A, Gnirke A, Bell GW, Ramsahoye B, Lander ES, Jaenisch R. Reduced representation bisulfite sequencing for comparative high-resolution DNA methylation analysis. Nucleic Acids Research. 2005;33:5868–5877. doi: 10.1093/nar/gki901. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Monaghan P, Charmantier A, Nussey DH, Ricklefs RE. The evolutionary ecology of senescence. Functional Ecology. 2008;22:371–378. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2435.2008.01418.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  77. Muggeo VM, Muggeo MVM. Package ‘segmented’. 1.3-3Biometrika. 2017 https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/segmented/segmented.pdf
  78. Petkovich DA, Podolskiy DI, Lobanov AV, Lee SG, Miller RA, Gladyshev VN. Using DNA methylation profiling to evaluate biological age and longevity interventions. Cell Metabolism. 2017;25:954–960. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2017.03.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Polanowski AM, Robbins J, Chandler D, Jarman SN. Epigenetic estimation of age in humpback whales. Molecular Ecology Resources. 2014;14:976–987. doi: 10.1111/1755-0998.12247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Raffington LAS, Belsky DW, Malanchini M, Tucker-Drob EM, Harden KP. Analysis of socioeconomic disadvantage and pace of aging measured in saliva DNA methylation of children and adolescents. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.06.04.134502. [DOI]
  81. Ryan J, Wrigglesworth J, Loong J, Fransquet PD, Woods RL. A systematic review and Meta-analysis of environmental, lifestyle, and health factors associated with DNA methylation age. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A. 2020;75:481–494. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glz099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Shalev I, Belsky J. Early-life stress and reproductive cost: a two-hit developmental model of accelerated aging? Medical Hypotheses. 2016;90:41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2016.03.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Shanahan L, Copeland WE, Costello EJ, Angold A. Child-, adolescent- and young adult-onset depressions: differential risk factors in development? Psychological Medicine. 2011;41:2265–2274. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Shanahan MJ, Hofer SM. Handbook of Aging and the Social Sciences. Elsevier; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  85. Silk JB, Beehner JC, Bergman TJ, Crockford C, Engh AL, Moscovice LR, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current Biology. 2010;20:1359–1361. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.05.067. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Simons RL, Lei MK, Beach SR, Philibert RA, Cutrona CE, Gibbons FX, Barr A. Economic hardship and biological weathering: the epigenetics of aging in a U.S. sample of black women. Social Science & Medicine. 2016;150:192–200. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.12.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Simons ND, Tung J. Social status and gene regulation: conservation and context dependence in primates. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2019;23:722–725. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Snyder-Mackler N, Sanz J, Kohn JN, Brinkworth JF, Morrow S, Shaver AO, Grenier JC, Pique-Regi R, Johnson ZP, Wilson ME, Barreiro LB, Tung J. Social status alters immune regulation and response to infection in macaques. Science. 2016;354:1041–1045. doi: 10.1126/science.aah3580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Snyder-Mackler N, Burger JR, Gaydosh L, Belsky DW, Noppert GA, Campos FA, Bartolomucci A, Yang YC, Aiello AE, O'Rand A, Harris KM, Shively CA, Alberts SC, Tung J. Social determinants of health and survival in humans and other animals. Science. 2020;368:eaax9553. doi: 10.1126/science.aax9553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Stubbs TM, Bonder MJ, Stark AK, Krueger F, von Meyenn F, Stegle O, Reik W, BI Ageing Clock Team Multi-tissue DNA methylation age predictor in mouse. Genome Biology. 2017;18:68. doi: 10.1186/s13059-017-1203-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Sumner JA, Colich NL, Uddin M, Armstrong D, McLaughlin KA. Early experiences of threat, but not deprivation, are associated with accelerated biological aging in children and adolescents. Biological Psychiatry. 2019;85:268–278. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.09.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Thompson MJ, vonHoldt B, Horvath S, Pellegrini M. An epigenetic aging clock for dogs and wolves. Aging. 2017;9:1055–1068. doi: 10.18632/aging.101211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Tung J, Charpentier MJ, Garfield DA, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Genetic evidence reveals temporal change in hybridization patterns in a wild baboon population. Molecular Ecology. 2008;17:1998–2011. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.03723.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Tung J, Zhou X, Alberts SC, Stephens M, Gilad Y. The genetic architecture of gene expression levels in wild baboons. eLife. 2015;4:e04729. doi: 10.7554/eLife.04729. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Tung J, Archie EA, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Cumulative early life adversity predicts longevity in wild baboons. Nature Communications. 2016;7:11181. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Vilgalys TP, Rogers J, Jolly CJ, Mukherjee S, Tung J. Evolution of DNA methylation in Papio baboons. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 2019;36:527–540. doi: 10.1093/molbev/msy227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Wang J. COANCESTRY: a program for simulating, estimating and analysing relatedness and inbreeding coefficients. Molecular Ecology Resources. 2011;11:141–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1755-0998.2010.02885.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Williams GC. Pleiotropy, natural selection, and the evolution of senescence. Evolution. 1957;11:398–411. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1957.tb02911.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  99. Wolf EJ, Maniates H, Nugent N, Maihofer AX, Armstrong D, Ratanatharathorn A, Ashley-Koch AE, Garrett M, Kimbrel NA, Lori A, Aiello AE, Baker DG, Beckham JC, Boks MP, Galea S, Geuze E, Hauser MA, Kessler RC, Koenen KC, Miller MW, Ressler KJ, Risbrough V, Rutten BPF, Stein MB, Ursano RJ, Vermetten E, Vinkers CH, Uddin M, Smith AK, Nievergelt CM, Logue MW, Va Mid-Atlantic Mirecc Workgroup Traumatic stress and accelerated DNA methylation age: a meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;92:123–134. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.12.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Wright PGR, Mathews F, Schofield H, Morris C, Burrage J, Smith A, Dempster EL, Hamilton PB. Application of a novel molecular method to age free-living wild Bechstein's bats. Molecular Ecology Resources. 2018;18:1374–1380. doi: 10.1111/1755-0998.12925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Xi Y, Li W. BSMAP: whole genome bisulfite sequence MAPping program. BMC Bioinformatics. 2009;10:232. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-10-232. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Zannas AS, Arloth J, Carrillo-Roa T, Iurato S, Röh S, Ressler KJ, Nemeroff CB, Smith AK, Bradley B, Heim C, Menke A, Lange JF, Brückl T, Ising M, Wray NR, Erhardt A, Binder EB, Mehta D. Lifetime stress accelerates epigenetic aging in an urban, african american cohort: relevance of glucocorticoid signaling. Genome Biology. 2015;16:266. doi: 10.1186/s13059-015-0828-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Zipple MN, Archie EA, Tung J, Altmann J, Alberts SC. Intergenerational effects of early adversity on survival in wild baboons. eLife. 2019;8:e47433. doi: 10.7554/eLife.47433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: George H Perry1
Reviewed by: James Higham2, Chris Kuzawa3

Our editorial process produces two outputs: i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Acceptance summary:

In this paper, the authors collect epigenomic data from a well-studied wild baboon community, which they use to construct an epigenetic clock, a method of measuring "biological age" that is increasingly used as a tool in human aging research. The authors find that deviations between biological and chronological age can in part be explained by social phenomena. In particular, for male baboons, maintaining social dominance may play an important role in accelerating the dimension of aging indexed by this measure. This is a foundational study for social-biological-health research.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "High social status males experience accelerated epigenetic aging in wild baboons" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 2 peer reviewers, and the evaluation has been overseen by a Reviewing Editor and George Perry as the Senior Editor. The following individuals involved in review of your submission have agreed to reveal their identity: James Higham (Reviewer #1); Chris Kuzawa (Reviewer #2).

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential Revisions:

1. Address comments from reviewer #4 from the previous journal.

2. Revise in response to the two minor comments/suggestions from Reviewer #2.

3. Consider whether residuals should be analyzed in the models or if it is better to set up the model differently, from a statistical best practices standpoint.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

It has already been through multiple rounds of review at another journal, which have improved it substantively. While there are some outstanding comments to address from another reviewer, in my view the manuscript does not need much further work before it could be published in eLife.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

I have been asked to review this manuscript, which has already gone through 3 rounds of review at another journal. I have had access to that set of reviews and the authors' responses, and read them, and the new manuscript draft, carefully. Of the three reviewers, two provided suggestions and were satisfied with the authors' revisions in addressing their points and recommended publication. One reviewer seems to have held up publication in that journal. That reviewer raised three main issues: around the possible confounding role of BMI, about whether to adjust for chronological age, and about the need to provide evidence of replication. As I will discuss in my brief comments here, I feel that the authors addressed all of these points effectively and thoroughly, and I feel that the paper should have been accepted for publication at the prior journal. I support publication at eLife, assuming that new methods/statistical points noted in a new review (by reviewer 4) can be addressed, once the authors have seen them and had a chance to respond to them.

Prior point #1: Does the BMI confound associations between rank and epigenetic age acceleration in males? Based upon findings in human studies, reviewer 1 argues that the higher BMIs of high ranking individuals could be the true cause of their apparent acceleration in biological age, rather than this being due to rank per se. The authors argue compellingly that the BMI in humans may tell us about adiposity (which can lead to metabolic derangement, inflammation etc – thereby potentially accelerating biological age measures), whereas in wild baboons (especially male) it is more informative about lean mass. I think the authors very thoroughly and effectively addressed these points, and I had the same reaction to the reviewer's critique when I read it.

Prior point #2: Should models control for chronological age? I found this critique hard to understand. In analyses of epigenetic clocks, it is acceleration – defined as deviations from expected, chronological age – that are generally of interest. This requires adjustment for chronological age by definition, so that what is being predicted are the residuals. In addition, some of the measures (BMI) change with age, and thus, not adjusting for age would convert those measures into age proxies. In my view, the authors carefully and thoroughly addressed the reviewer's concern in their responses to prior rounds of review.

Prior point #3: Validation in another species or population is necessary. The reviewer argued that papers published in high impact journals require replication. While that is the norm in certain fields (e.g. GWAS studies of the predictors of polygenic traits in humans), that standard is less applicable in the context of a study of a wild primate population. As the authors note, many (most, or almost all) high impact analyses of longitudinal studies of this sort do not include replication. In some instances, populations (like Amboslei) provide unique opportunities given their unusual design (in the case of Amboseli, more than half century duration). The analyses here require empirically measured ages tracked longitudinally. The idea of replicating in another species, as the reviewer suggests, misses the point made in a compelling way by the authors – the nature of hierarchy, related stressors etc. vary across species and demanding that similar relationships should replicate across species is to miss a big part of the story.

In sum, I felt that the authors effectively addressed the main critiques of the first round of review and that publication would have been warranted at that stage. They provided even more thorough and compelling responses in rounds 2 and 3. I am aware of a new set of comments, primarily from a statistical/methods angle, which the authors have not yet seen or addressed, and that are outside my area of expertise. Assuming that they can address those suggestions in a satisfactory way, I support publication of this analysis.

I think the manuscript in its current form is tight and compelling and is basically ready for publication. Because I anticipate that the authors may be revising in response to the new statistical review, I provide a few minor suggestions on how they might tighten and hone several of the points made in the manuscript:

I would suggest rereading the manuscript to make sure that it is clear for the reader that the clock they are reporting is measured specifically in blood – that is, immune cells, and should be interpreted in that light. Some past work has generated clocks that work across multiple tissue types, but that is not known here. I do not see this as at all problematic, and this is obviously the only ethical option when studying wild primates. But I do think the authors could be clearer about this when describing their findings and their functional significance: These findings speak specifically to markers of aging in immunity, and it is not clear whether they are informative about changes in other tissues that may not be harvested in wild primate populations.

Another, related point is the fact that the functional significance of epigenetic clocks (why they "work" at all) remains uncertain. The extent to which they are in the causal pathway to health outcomes (as suggested by at least one recent study that likely postdates this draft: Lu, Yuancheng, et al. "Reprogramming to recover youthful epigenetic information and restore vision." Nature 588.7836 (2020): 124-129), or merely a marker, is unclear. In the end, these are useful measures if they predict relevant outcomes, but I think noting this current ambiguity around their functional basis might warrant a sentence or two in the discussion.

eLife. 2021 Apr 6;10:e66128. doi: 10.7554/eLife.66128.sa2

Author response


Essential Revisions:

1. Address comments from reviewer #4 from the previous journal.

We have addressed all comments from Reviewer #4 (see below for details).

2. Revise in response to the two minor comments/suggestions from Reviewer #2 (see below).

We have made revisions in response to both of Reviewer #2’s suggestions. Specifically, we now clarify that our analyses pertain specifically to DNA methylation in the peripheral blood (lines 64; 109-110) and highlight the importance of resolving the mechanistic implications of the epigenetic clock (if any) in future work (lines 302-304).

3. Consider whether residuals should be analyzed in the models or if it is better to set up the model differently, from a statistical best practices standpoint.

We incorporated residuals in our models in three places, which we have chosen to retain in the revised manuscript for the following reasons. First, for the purposes of our longitudinal analysis of male rank and epigenetic age, we defined “relative epigenetic age” as the residuals of the best fit line relating chronological age and predicted age (lines 257-259). We followed this approach because we were interested in testing whether samples looked “old-for-age” in males who improved their rank between samples (and vice-versa for males who fell in rank). In other words, our question was specifically about the sign and magnitude of residual error in the epigenetic age prediction. Importantly, the results of this analysis are concordant with our cross-sectional analysis of ∆age, which did not use residuals.

In the second instance, we defined a measure of “rank-for-age”—i.e., whether males were higher or lower rank than expected, given the typical age-related pattern for male rank—based on the residuals of rank after regressing out age (lines 243-246). In this case, we were again testing an explicit hypothesis about the sign and magnitude of residual error.

Finally, we considered the possibility that body mass index might drive the relationship between rank and epigenetic age. We found this explanation unlikely, but as it was a major concern of a previous reviewer, we evaluated whether our main model of ∆age was perturbed by (i) replacing rank with the residuals of rank after regressing out raw BMI; (ii) replacing age-adjusted BMI with raw BMI; or (iii) replacing age-adjusted BMI with the residuals of raw BMI controlling for rank (lines 273-277). None of these alternative predictors changed our main findings, suggesting that the choice to model residuals does not actually matter to our results. However, the inclusion of these analyses may clarify the nature of the rank effect to readers who have the same concern as the reviewer.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

[…]

I think the manuscript in its current form is tight and compelling and is basically ready for publication. Because I anticipate that the authors may be revising in response to the new statistical review, I provide a few minor suggestions on how they might tighten and hone several of the points made in the manuscript:

I would suggest rereading the manuscript to make sure that it is clear for the reader that the clock they are reporting is measured specifically in blood – that is, immune cells, and should be interpreted in that light. Some past work has generated clocks that work across multiple tissue types, but that is not known here. I do not see this as at all problematic, and this is obviously the only ethical option when studying wild primates. But I do think the authors could be clearer about this when describing their findings and their functional significance: These findings speak specifically to markers of aging in immunity, and it is not clear whether they are informative about changes in other tissues that may not be harvested in wild primate populations.

We have revised the text to emphasize that our analyses pertain specifically to methylation in peripheral blood, so may be more relevant to immune function than if we had conducted them in a different tissue (lines 64; 109-112).

Another, related point is the fact that the functional significance of epigenetic clocks (why they "work" at all) remains uncertain. The extent to which they are in the causal pathway to health outcomes (as suggested by at least one recent study that likely postdates this draft: Lu, Yuancheng, et al. "Reprogramming to recover youthful epigenetic information and restore vision." Nature 588.7836 (2020): 124-129), or merely a marker, is unclear. In the end, these are useful measures if they predict relevant outcomes, but I think noting this current ambiguity around their functional basis might warrant a sentence or two in the discussion.

We have revised the text to highlight that the functional significance of DNA methylation-based epigenetic clocks—i.e., whether they are passive biomarkers or part of the actual mechanisms of aging—remains unclear (lines 302-304).

Previous Reviewer #4

Anderson et al. have produced a manuscript investigating DNA methylation (DNAm) assessed epigenetic age in a wild baboon population in Kenya. They have constructed a clock from RRBS DNA methylome data from whole blood from 245 baboons (277 samples). This clock comprised 573 CpGs and could predict age within a mean absolute difference of 1.1 years ± 1.9 SD (r = 0.762). The accuracy of this clock was subsequently compared against other age- associated traits and between sexes. It tracked well with age-related phenotypes, such as molar dentine exposure, but was less accurate in females. This divergence occurred after 8 years when the majority of males achieve adult dominance rank and disperse from their birth group. Clock-assessed age acceleration was not associated with the strongest predictors of lifespan in this population, early adversity and social integration. Instead, male dominance rank was most strongly associated with positively accelerated age.

This manuscript has already been reviewed, so the responses to this first round of reviews are available, and as well, I have assessed the methodological approach. The authors’ interpretation of these results is a ‘life fast die young’ effect. My concerns for the authors to consider are listed below.

Major

Thanks for making this point. We have revised this text to emphasize that it is the first clock to assess the effects of natural variation in the social environment in wild primates (lines 74-75). This change emphasizes the most important aspect of our study rather than the construction of the clock itself.

2. Regarding the composition of the clock (line 104), it is incorrect regarding human clocks being specifically enriched in genes, CpG islands, promoter regions, and putative enhancers, compared to the background. These clocks have been devised with data from human DNAm arrays, which predominately only probe these regions – the earlier 27k array used for the construction of the Horvath clock is almost exclusively promoter based. It is stated that the baboon clock is functionally important for gene regulation, which is actually not specifically the case for human clocks [3].

We have clarified that the enrichment observed in humans was relative to the background set of sites tested (i.e., measurable CpG sites on the array) rather than the background of all CpG sites in the genome (lines 95-97). Similarly, in our analyses, we performed enrichment analyses relative to all sites that could have been possibly included in the clock (i.e., those profiled by RRBS; lines 429-430), not all CpG sites in the genome (RRBS analysis also enriches for a subset of functional compartments, especially promoters and CpG islands, compared to all sites in the genome).

3. What was the explanation of the outlying result for the Dominance rank 10 in Figure 2? – How robust are these ~20 ranks – as seems a large number?

We do not have a definite explanation for the apparent increase in relative epigenetic age in rank 10 animals relative to the fit linear relationship. We note that this outlier is based on only 5 individuals, so it may simply reflect stochastic sampling. Indeed, 2 of the 5 males have relative epigenetic age values below 0, so the overall pattern is driven by only 2 other males with large positive values. None of these males are unusual in terms of read depth or age at the time of sampling, and none experienced major shifts in rank close to the time of sampling.

With respect to our rank assignments, most groups have smaller numbers of post-reproductive maturation males, but some groups in Amboseli range up to 120 animals and have more males (and hence, more positions in the hierarchy). Our methods for assigning rank are based on the same near-daily observations used for this population to assess male rank effects on mating success, paternity success, glucocorticoid levels, lifespan, and other outcomes (see lines 519-523). Thus, we view them as generally highly reliable, although in periods where there is a high degree of rank competition, there can be rapid change.

4. The authors state that Amboseli α males also exhibit elevated glucocorticoid levels, increased expression of genes involved in innate immunity and inflammation. Is there any support in the DNAm data for this in the high-ranking males – i.e. showing epigenetic changes also found in human blood consistent with this? [4, 5].

Thanks, this is an interesting question that is the subject of ongoing analysis in our group. Specifically, we are investigating the socioecological predictors of DNA methylation at individual CpG sites, as well as the potential for this variation to explain earlier published results on dominance rank and gene expression (Lea et al. 2018, PNAS). We view these analyses as beyond the scope of the current manuscript. However, we do show that the clock sites that contribute to male rank effects on relative epigenetic age are enriched in or near genes important in the gene expression response to the pro-inflammatory agent lipopolysaccharide (lines 102-109; Figure 1 —figure supplement 3).

5. There is significant complexity in the baboon genome [6]. What is the potential impact of the use of the mixture of different Papio species – cynocephalus and anubis? Were the CpGs in the clock assessed for any evidence of genetic confounding in the two separate species – by methods such as Gap Hunting? [7]

Thanks, we have also worried about possible mapping (and hence DNA methylation quantification) errors introduced by the hybrid nature of our population, especially because the reference genome is for the anubis baboon (we note that we did not use a mixture of individuals from different species here; rather, all of the animals in Amboseli are hybrids between these two ancestries: see Wall et al. 2016, Molecular Ecology). We therefore addressed this concern in a previous paper (Vilgalys et al. 2019, Molecular Biology and Evolution; see Supplementary Information 1.5 and Figure S7). Specifically, we simulated bisulfite sequencing data from both the anubis genome and the yellow baboon genome and quantified mapping and quantification biases depending on the ancestry of the original sample. In brief, we found that the percent of incorrect calls differed by less than 0.4% between yellow and anubis baboons, making it highly unlikely that they affect our results. We reference these previous results in the revised text (lines 382-387). Notably, if CpG methylation measures were strongly affected by ancestry in our data set, those sites would also become poor predictors of age and would be unlikely to be incorporated into the epigenetic clock.

6. Was any evidence or adjustment made for potential batch effects as the RRBS was a combination of two analytical batches? (n=36 and n=277).

We did not perform batch correction prior to creating the clock (although we do so routinely when analyzing DNA methylation variation at individual CpG sites: e.g., Lea et al. 2015 PLoS Genetics). This choice was made because we were interested in developing a generalizable clock that could predict well across batches; additionally, correcting for batch effects in the full sample could “leak” information about the full data set into the test samples. However, we confirmed that our results in the main model (Table 1) were robust if we added batch effect (previously generated samples [n=36] versus newly generated samples [n=241]) as a covariate. We have clarified these points in the revised paper (lines 411-416).

7. With all the caveats and issues with BMI, sex-specific effects etc the authors need to confirm that data quality issues have not impacted on the conclusion drawn

a. Specifically, there is a big range of sequencing depth 17.8 (± 10.5 s.d.) million reads. 20 Million reads normal for good (www.diagenode.com/en/p/rrbs-service).

Thanks, we agree that deeper read coverage can provide better estimation of individual CpG site methylation levels. However, we observe no effect of read depth on ∆age in our data, in either males or females. Specifically, we checked whether including an additional covariate of read depth in the models reported in Table 1 explained additional variation in ∆age. It did not (p = 0.11 for females and p = 0.23 for males), and also did not change any of the other parameter estimates. We have therefore elected to keep the original model results in the main text, but revised the text to note that our results are robust to variation in read depth (lines 213-216; Supplementary Table 5).

b. There is a negative correlation between age and reads that is stronger in males

Thanks for pointing this out. We now highlight in the revised text that, although read depth is correlated with chronological age in our sample, read depth is not correlated with ∆age, the value of interest in our analysis (lines 213-216; see also our response above). Importantly, we also control for chronological age in our analysis of ∆age.

c. A threshold of 35x is low, 10x more standard (ENCODE) – “at least 10-fold coverage of a CpG is required for accurate measurement of percent methylation” https://www.encodeproject.org/documents/e82fdfdf-f387-47d8- af03-df67bbea0e72/@@download/attachment/2010-05-30_mod- ENCODE_TF_Chrom_Data_Standards.pdf
30x used for Roadmap standard – and even issues at this level [8]

We agree that 5x coverage will translate to some noise in DNA methylation level estimation for any given CpG site. However, unlike the Roadmap Epigenomics data sets, we do not intend for any particular sample to serve as a reference data set for the field. Rather, we are focused on understanding the sources of population-level variation across hundreds of samples, with a primary interest in composite prediction accuracy across the methylome, rather than at individual CpG sites. Thus, our data reflect a classical trade-off between analyzing more individuals versus sequencing each individual at deeper coverage. In population-based studies, favoring more individuals is a common solution.

d. Coverage issues are the clear disadvantage of sequencing-based clocks – with much more stochastic coverage of the CpGs included. This further reduces its use in any replication compared with the human data using robust and consistent CpG arrays [3]. How well were the 573 CpGs actually chosen in the clock covered in the cohort? Age changes are ubiquitous so multiple clocks should be able to be constructed from these data. If only build the clock (excluding these 573) from a smaller number of higher confidence CpGs from the ~458k with higher CpG coverage are results consistent?

We agree that multiple clocks can be constructed from the data. Indeed, our clock incorporates all 573 unique sites that were used in at least one epigenetic clock in our training-test sets. Median average coverage for sites included in the clock was 23.0x. Alternative clocks can be built with similar prediction accuracy and smaller numbers of sites, e.g., by increasing the strength of regularization (α parameter; Figure 1—figure supplement 2).

e. There is a small distinct number of outliers for BS conversion (lower ~98%) – which could also impact particularly as the wild-card aligner BSMAP is more susceptible to false positives related to inefficient conversion [9].

We agree that small differences in conversion can lead to artifacts (although no bisulfite conversion rates were < 98%). To address this concern, we checked whether the ∆age models in Table 1 were robust to exclusion of the 6 samples with bisulfite conversion rates < 0.99. All results are qualitatively the same, and all significant results remain significant. We obtain the same result if bisulfite conversion rate is run as a model covariate. We have revised the text to note that our models are robust to the small variation in bisulfite conversion rates (lines 213-216; Supplementary Table 5).

Author response image 1.

Author response image 1.

There was a higher variance in females – were sex chromosome CpGs excluded from clock? Were the enrichments compared against the background set of the possible ~458k CpGs these clock CpG came from?

Yes, we constructed the clock based on autosomes only, and all enrichments were relative to the background set of 458k sites that we originally used in calibrating the clock. Both points are now clarified in the text (lines 381 and 93-97, 429-430).

State the tissue type used for the ENCODE H3K4me1 ChIP-seq data from humans that was lifted over.

The tissue type was peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs), now noted in line 438.

References:

1. Horvath, S., DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biol, 2013. 14(10): p. R115.

2. De Paoli-Iseppi, R., et al., Measuring Animal Age with DNA Methylation: From Humans to Wild Animals. Front Genet, 2017. 8: p. 106.

3. Bell, C.G., et al., DNA methylation aging clocks: challenges and recommendations. Genome Biology, 2019. 20(1): p. 249.Ligthart, S., et al., DNA methylation signatures of chronic low-grade inflammation are associated with complex diseases. Genome Biology, 2016. 17(1): p. 255.

4. Tang, R., et al., Adverse childhood experiences, DNA methylation age acceleration, and cortisol in UK children: a prospective population-based cohort study. Clin Epigenetics, 2020. 12(1): p. 55.

5. Rogers, J., et al., The comparative genomics and complex population history of Papio baboons. Sci Adv, 2019. 5(1): p. eaau6947.

6. Andrews, S.V., et al., "Gap hunting" to characterize clustered probe signals in Illumina methylation array data. Epigenetics Chromatin, 2016. 9: p. 56.

7. Libertini, E., et al., Saturation analysis for whole-genome bisulfite sequencing data. Nat Biotechnol, 2016: p. doi:10.1038/nbt.3524.

8. Rauluseviciute, I., F. Drablos, and M.B. Rye, DNA methylation data by sequencing: experimental approaches and recommendations for tools and pipelines for data analysis. Clin Epigenetics, 2019. 11(1): p. 193.

[Editors' note: we include below the reviews that the authors received from another journal, along with the authors’ responses.]

Reviewer 1

In this paper, Anderson et al. have claimed that high social status have been found to accelerate the epigenetic aging rates in wild baboons. However, in the sample set most of the highly ranked animals had higher BMIs. Higher BMI has previously been reported to increase epigenetic age. Therefore, it is unclear to me how novel the authors’ claims are. Overall, this study is not conclusive, and additional studies should be considered for this study to be relevant and accurate.

Thanks for this clear summary of your major concern. In response, we have conducted a series of additional analyses, which increase our confidence that BMI does not drive the results we report for male rank.

Specifically, we now show that if we force rank and BMI to be independent by modeling Δage as a function of the residuals of male dominance rank controlling for BMI (instead of dominance rank itself), residual dominance rank continues to significantly predict Δage in the same direction as our original model (prank(residual for BMI) = 9.95 x 10-4; lines 198-200; Supplementary Table 5). In contrast, if we model the residuals of BMI controlling for dominance rank, BMI does not significantly predict Δage (pBMI(residual for rank) = 0.139), but dominance rank remains highly significant (prank = 1.88 x 10-4; lines 198-204, Supplementary Table 5). These results are consistent with our original finding that male rank significantly predicts Δage when BMI is included as a covariate (lines 191-192). We note that in our main analysis we modeled BMI controlling for chronological age (now denoted as age-adjusted BMI for additional clarity, lines 172-173), resulting in a measure of BMI that is uncorrelated with dominance rank (Pearson’s r = -0.068, p = 0.516; see lines 167-172 and Supplementary Table 4). However, this decision does not affect our results: in an alternative multiple regression that uses raw BMI, dominance rank is still a significant predictor of Δage but raw BMI is not (likely because variation in raw BMI primarily captures growth and development in baboons; lines 198-204).

These results, now reported in lines 196-204 and in the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Table 5), dovetail with our finding that age-adjusted BMI also does not predict Δage in female baboons (Table 1). Below, we also discuss the difference between the interpretation of variation in BMI in humans and variation in BMI in wild baboons, which—along with the new analyses described above—make it highly unlikely that our results simply recapitulate previous findings about BMI and epigenetic aging in humans.

Major comments:

1. It is well known that a higher BMI is associated with accelerating epigenetic aging and this has been shown in previous reports from multiple research groups (PMID: 30785999, PMID: 28289477, PMID: 29159506, PMID: 28089957, PMID: 31001624, PMID: 31480455, PMID: 28198702). In this study, there seems to be a trend for higher ranked animals having a higher BMI. With a higher social hierarchy, these animals would have a higher priority to accessing foods and consequently would have higher BMIs. It cannot be denied that the detected phenomenon potentially is only reflecting the BMI differences in the epigenetic age, not necessarily their social ranking. Therefore, it is not surprising at all that there is a correlation between acceleration of epigenetic aging and social rank because the higher ranked animals have higher BMIs.

I am not sure whether the authors’ finding has any novelty. I believe the actual novelty of this study is in fact that higher social ranked baboons had higher BMIs. Rather than the effects of ranking on epigenetic age, it would be interesting if the authors focused on the differences between sexes found in the relationships of rank and BMI. In males, higher ranked animals have higher BMI. In contrast, lower ranked female animals have relatively higher-BMI, although higher ranked (1-5) animals have high-BMI.

Thanks, these comments have led us to clarify some important distinctions between the variation observed for BMI in humans (the subject of all the studies cited above) versus the variation in BMI observed in wild baboons. Specifically, in the human-centered studies above, high BMI translates to classification as overweight or obese. In contrast, wild baboons in Amboseli are never overweight or obese: females average 1.9% body fat, based on subscapular skinfold estimates, and males average <9% body fat, based on stable isotope measures (skinfold estimates are unreliable in males) (Altmann et al. 1993, American Journal of Primatology 30: 149-161). These values, which are several times lower than typical body fat percentages for obese men and women, reflect the fact that BMI variation in male baboons is a function of lean muscle mass, a distinction we now clarify in the revised manuscript (lines 166-167; 196-197; 286-290).

Our findings are thus distinct from the epigenetic age-BMI associations in the papers cited above. Additionally, based on the new analyses described above (lines 196-204), we are confident that our findings are driven by rank, not BMI, although age-adjusted BMI has some additional predictive value for Δage in males after rank is taken into account (Table 1). We now clarify this point in the revised manuscript (lines 286-292).

With respect to novelty, we also clarify that our results report the first epigenetic clock to be calibrated for a wild primate (as noted by Reviewer 2) and are the first to establish a link between social factors and epigenetic aging in any natural animal population (lines 72-76). Finally, with respect to sex differences in the rank-BMI relationship, there is no relationship between age-adjusted BMI and rank in the females in our sample (Pearson’s r = 0.058, p = 0.646). This is not surprising because rank in females is nepotistically determined, but rank in males is determined by physical competition (see Hausfater et al. 1982, Science 217: 752-755 and Alberts et al. 2003, Animal Behaviour 65: 821-840). We have thus chosen not to focus on sex differences in the BMI-rank relationship because this difference in rank dynamics is well-established in the literature. However, your comments stress the importance of providing this context to readers, and we now do so in lines 304-307.

2. The authors should make clear the universal effects of social ranking in other sample sets to show whether the detected phenomena is specific to this sample set/cohort/species. For example, there is methylome data derived from age matched rhesus monkey samples with social rank information in a previous publication (PMID: 22493251). The authors could analyze the epigenetic age differences between high and low ranked rhesus monkeys to compare (and contrast?) with their gathered data from their sample set.

Thanks for the suggestion. Our results strongly suggest, which we now clarify, that rank effects are in fact not universal (lines 191-192; 313-314). Specifically, we argue that the observed sex-specific rank-Δage association stems from the nature of dominance rank determination in wild male baboons, where rank is dynamic and linked to physical competition. In contrast, rank is highly stable and nepotistic in both female baboons and female rhesus macaques (who are the subject of PMID: 22493251). Indeed, one hypothesis generated by our findings is that high dominance rank specifically accelerates epigenetic aging when achieving and maintaining rank is energetically costly. The degree to which this condition holds differs across species, and, within species like baboons, also between males and females (the energetic costs of rank competition are much more relevant in males). We have highlighted this point more effectively—including the predictions it makes about when and where our findings should generalize—in the revised manuscript (lines 313-321).

Notably, the study cited above (PMID: 2249325, which was led by the senior author of the present study) used experimental manipulations to randomize dominance rank in captive female macaques; it also generated quite limited data on DNA methylation (N = 6). Thus, it does not capture natural rank dynamics for baboons (or rhesus macaques) of either sex. Because it is not an appropriate comparison case, and no other research group has generated population-level DNA methylation data for wild primates, we have now revised the Discussion to include promising test cases for future work (lines 316319).

3. To eliminate the effects of the differences in BMI and chronological age, the authors should only assess animal samples from same age ranges and BMI, and further analyze their agerelated methylation status by dividing into high and low social rank. However, it seems like the number of samples matching these conditions are short of analyzing with enough statistic power. As the authors pointed out in the Discussion section, additional longitudinal sampling will be necessary to make the conclusion the authors have made.

Please see our responses above and lines 196-204, where we report a series of analyses to address the concern about confounding between rank and BMI.

4. The authors tried to detect the effects of promotion/demotion in social ranking (figure 3). For their claims that social ranking has effects on epigenetic aging speed, the authors failed to test using BMI, which is another significant covariate. Without doing so, it is not possible to eliminate the possibility that BMI status affects epigenetic aging speed. I wonder whether the BMI changes (samples collected at higher/lower BMI) is correlated with epigenetic aging (relative epigenetic age).

Thanks for this suggestion. We have now performed an analysis of whether longitudinal change in age-adjusted BMI predicts longitudinal change in Δage, which parallels our analysis of longitudinal change in male rank. We find that both change in age-adjusted BMI and change in dominance rank significantly predict change in epigenetic age, although the effect of rank is larger (lines 254-255). Consistent with this observation, longitudinal change in the residuals of rank after regressing out raw BMI still explains an estimated 20% of the variance in longitudinal change in Δage, although this effect is no longer significant. In contrast, BMI adjusted for rank explains almost none of the variance in change in Δage (R2 = 0.01) (lines 255-259). These results again suggest that our findings are a consequence of rank, not BMI.

Minor comments:

1. Use same scale both in females and males in Figure 1 to provide better comprehendible visualization. Add dashed lines for y=x in Figure 1C and 1D.

We have made these changes (see revised Figures 1C-D).

2. Need to change the title of Figure 2 as appropriate. It is too strongly worded and does not seem to reflect the contents shown in the figure.

We have revised to “Dominance rank predicts relative epigenetic age in male baboons,” which directly corresponds to the y-variable we plot in Figure 2.

3. Add unit (years) for “Age” in the Supplementary Table 1.

Done.

4. Plot and check the association between age (x axis) and methylation percentage (y axis; for example, average of each individual in each genetic regions such as promoter, CpG island, shore etc) with color codes based on ranking (for example, by grouping; 1-5, 6-10, 11-15, etc) using CpG sites showed age-related increase/decrease in DNA methylation.

We have included this information in new Supplementary Figure 5, based on CpG sites included in the epigenetic clock. Change with age is visually apparent for clock sites in all genomic contexts.

5. Include epigenetic age (biological age) in the Supplementary Table 1.

Done.

6. Include genomic region of the CpG site with gene name in the Supplementary Table 2.

Done.

7. Correct the reference figure (Figure 3 to 4) in line 199, p7.

Done.

8. Perform further analyses about age-related methylation changes in wild baboon such as volcano plot between young and old, then pathway assay using significant genes etc. in each sex.

Thanks for the suggestion. We performed site-by-site age-associated differential methylation analysis, identified age-related differentially methylated regions, and performed pathway enrichment analyses in a previous paper (Lea et al. 2015, PLoS Genetics 11: e1005650). Consequently, we have not repeated these analyses because they are somewhat tangential to our main point, given limited space. However, we now report site-level analyses for the 573 sites in the calibrated clock (Supplementary Figure 4; lines 94-99). Additionally, we have made the complete DNA methylation data set and age/sex information publicly available to support further re-analysis (newly generated data are available via NCBI Sequence Read Archive project accession number PRJNA648767).

Reviewer 2:

This is an interesting manuscript which does a lot – both building and validating an epigenetic clock in the Amboseli baboons, and then looking to see which factors predict deviations in epigenetic age relative to chronological age. There’s lots to like about this study, and it’s perhaps (?) the first published epigenetic clock from a free-ranging primate population. I think there’s some components of the manuscript that could be improved.

We appreciate the positive feedback, as well as the constructive suggestions for improvement.

My major comments are:

1. The manuscript needs structure. There is not a single subheading in the long “Results and Discussion” section, which keeps going and bringing up new issues and new analyses. The lack of a macrostructure makes it much harder to read and follow than it needs to be. I think the most useful thing would be to set out a list of (numbered?) aims/objectives in the last paragraph of the Introduction, so it’s clear what the specific set of aims is. For example, this could be something like: 1) Epigenetic clock validation; 2) Sex differences; 3) Predictors of epigenetic age. The Aims should then be used to structure the manuscript as subsections of the Results/Discussion. It will make it easier to follow.

Thanks. We have followed your suggestion to lay out the study aims in the introduction, and have added subheadings to delineate natural breaks in the results. We also separated the Results and Discussion sections to improve readability.

2. One of the major contributions of the manuscript is the development and validation of the epigenetic clock. I think it’s likely that this will be used in many further Amboseli papers with the current manuscript referenced for the validation, so I think it’s important to make sure that this description is as useful as possible. In that regard, I’d like to see more information on how well the 593 chosen CpG sites predict chronological age specifically compared to other potential combinations and numbers of sites.

Thanks for this suggestion. We now include additional information in the text (lines 8891; 371-375) and a new supplementary figure (Supplementary Figure 2) that describes how we identified the 573-site clock that we applied in the main text. In brief, we used a standard approach for the elastic net to identify the optimal trade-off (α) between L1 (Lasso) and L2 (ridge regression) regularization. The resulting set of sites includes a core set of sites that are always chosen as part of the clock, regardless of α, as well as a smaller number that are more variable. In new Supplementary Figure 2, we also show how all 573 sites perform, in terms of median absolute difference and R2 between predicted and true chronological age, compared to more restrictive sets of CpG sites.

Line 82 – also please give a measure of variance around the difference between the chronological age and that predicted from the epigenetic clock.

We now report the standard deviation of this difference in line 86.

In addition, the authors state: “The predicted ages from these 95 longitudinally collected samples were older for the later-collected samples, as expected (Figure 1C-D; binomial test p = 5.95 x 10-5 96 ). Furthermore, the change in epigenetic clock predictions between successive longitudinal samples positively predicted the actual change in age between sample dates (β = 0.312, p = 0.027).” Is it possible to provide more information here? What is the mean (with a measure of variance) difference between the amount of intra-individual chronological aging and that predicted by the two measures in the epigenetic clock?

We now report the mean and standard deviation of the difference between change in chronological age and change in epigenetic age predictions in line 121.

3. I don’t have a good sense of what the Authors think the mechanism is by which male dominance rank impacts the pace of aging, nor other elements of genomic regulation and downstream physiology. In part I think I’m unclear how they see dominance rank itself. Do they see it purely as a behavioral construct that is made up of different behavioral measures, and which simply reflects patterns of e.g. aggression given and received, and the associated behaviors intended to mediate that aggression (displacement, avoidance, etc.). Or, do they think that dominance rank reflects something separate and independent, which we are able to *measure* behaviorally, but which exists separately to those behaviors. If the former, then perhaps the behavioral measures themselves are the best thing to model rather than dominance rank itself? In this regard a specific question: does dominance rank predict epigenetic age better than, say, aggression given to other adult males, aggression received from other adult males, or other behaviors that primarily determine an individual’s assigned dominance rank? I would like to see such analyses. To be clear: I’m not sure if the Authors think that it is the behaviors themselves, such as giving and receiving aggression, that lead to changes in the genome, or something else intrinsic about “high social status” that they are capturing via their dominance rank measure. If what ages high ranking males faster is the constant aggression involved in being challenged and putting other individuals in their place, then in my view it is better just to focus on the behaviors involved as the mechanisms linking behavior to the body’s biology and condition are much clearer.

Thanks for this very interesting and important set of questions. We feel that we are limited in being able to address them here, primarily because our ability to assign male dominance rank is substantially better than our estimates of the rates of agonistic interactions that underly these ranks. Specifically, our rank assignments depend upon sampling enough agonisms to establish consistency in the patterns of wins and losses among males. In contrast, to test whether dominance rank is a better predictor of outcomes than rates of agonistic interactions would require highly accurate measures of rates. We don’t currently sample male agonistic behavior at a sufficient level of intensity to make us confident in such a comparison; male-male interactions can be subtle and occur very quickly (e.g., displacements, threat faces), and male agonism rates can vary substantially over short periods of time (e.g., males can be relatively pacific until there is active competition for rank—and we can miss those episodes if the group is not observed on the corresponding days). Thus, we believe the rank data are likely to be systematically more accurate than estimates of agonism rates themselves.

Indeed, when we asked whether substituting male dominance rank for either agonisms given or agonisms received is a better predictor of Δage than rank itself, we found (i) that only agonisms given was a significant predictor of Δage; but that (ii) a model including rank is a better fit to the data than a model substituting rank for agonisms given (agonisms directed to any group member: ΔAIC = 7.11; agonisms involving only adult males: ΔAIC = 3.13). We also found that agonisms given is not a significant mediator of the rank-Δage relationship (p = 0.053); further, although the trend is suggestive, the mediation effect only attenuates the original rank effect by a small percentage (19%). These results are consistent with two, non-mutually exclusive explanations: first, that rank captures something beyond the effects of agonism rates alone; or, second, that measures of agonism rates are simply more noisy than measures of rank. Because we are not able to differentiate these explanations here, we have chosen to leave these analyses out of the revised manuscript.

However, we agree on the importance of your question about the “meaning” of rank. While fully addressing it is beyond the scope of this study, we do now discuss our results in the context of previous work in the Amboseli baboons, which was able to deploy a Mendelian randomization (MR) analysis to investigate the directionality of rank-gene expression associations in the Amboseli baboons (Lea et al. 2018, PNAS). In that case, MR analysis suggested that, instead of being a consequence of rank, gene expression differences preceded attainment of high rank. Those findings suggest that the male rank epigenetic age relationship we describe in this paper may capture more about the characteristics of males who compete successfully for high rank than about the behaviors they exhibit upon attaining it (now discussed in lines 292-299).

Lastly, I also think that given the centrality of male dominance rank to the manuscript’s conclusions, more details on how this is measured and calculated would be useful, including more details on the specific behaviors included.

We have included additional information in the Methods about how dominance rank is measured and calculated in the Amboseli baboons (lines 474-482).

4. The absence of certain effects seems to be as interesting as the presence of the rank result. Recent manuscripts from these researchers and this population have shown that early-life adversity is extremely important in shaping a variety of measures of biological condition and life history. Some of the discussion in the present manuscript does consider potential reasons why there were no such effects here, but I thought more could be done – as the Authors state, it’s surprising given their prior results. I also felt that the significance of the absence of other effects on epigenetic age could usefully be discussed – for example, nothing is really made of the fact that female rank does not predict epigenetic age. As the Authors note, the result presented here seems most similar to the results found on GC concentrations and male dominance in this study population. What do the present results tell us about how aging effects can operate differently within and between biological domains, and about the interaction between different elements of allostatic load? The Amboseli Baboon Project has published a large number of studies relating social behavior and social status to measures of health and fitness. For those of us on the outside trying to understand the overall picture, it would be useful to read more explicitly how the authors feel that the present results fit together (or not, where appropriate) with their other findings involving other measures and markers.

Thanks; we agree that the absence of significant associations is interesting in and of itself. In response, we now discuss two themes emerging from work on the Amboseli baboons (lines 262-283; 303-321).

First, dominance rank effects on physiological and molecular outcomes—epigenetic aging, in this paper, and also immune gene expression (Lea et al. 2018, PNAS) and glucocorticoid levels (Levy et al., in review at Hormones and Behavior; Gesquiere et al. 2011, Science)—are consistently more detectable and/or stronger in males than in females. This difference likely arises from major differences in how rank is attained and maintained in male versus female baboons (lines 303-321). Second, major predictors of lifespan (e.g., cumulative early adversity and social integration, in this population) are not necessarily major predictors of physiological and molecular measures. This may be because they act through entirely distinct pathways, because their effects are tissue, cell type, or environment-specific, or because they are sensitive to specific types of early adversity, but not others. Further research will be necessary to tease apart these possibilities (lines 262-283).

Reviewer #3:

Anderson et al. have developed a “DNA methylation-based age predictor”, i.e. an epigenetic clock, for the wild baboons at Amboseli, Kenya and they find that male dominance rank is associated with significant differences between predicted age and chronological age. The study is well-designed and well-executed, and the paper is well-written and a pleasure to read. The results are fascinating and have broad relevance for aging in humans and other non-primates as well as addressing bigger questions of evolution. It’s fascinating that neither cumulative early life adversity nor social bond strength explained variation in the difference between predicted and chronological age, but male dominance rank did. Higher social status in males is associated with more rapid epigenetic aging even though higher social status yields increased resource access and is generally associated with a better body condition, higher fertility and better competitive advantage. These results are novel and only possibly in a population like the Amboseli baboons that have been studied for so long with a wealth of data available on each individual. Specifically, this study provides new results comparing the impact of different psychosocial stressors on epigenetic age and furthermore suggests that the impact of epigenetic aging lasts only as long as the stressor exists.

Thank you.

The paper should be published and I have only a few comments:

– Could the authors speculate on why their clock was more accurate in males than females – sample sizes are comparable so it must be something else. The faster rate of biological aging in males relative to females would seem to suggest that the clock would be less accurate, not more accurate, in males than females.

Thanks for this question. Given that female baboons live longer than males (Colchero et al. 2016, PNAS), we think it is perhaps unsurprising that they show slower epigenetic aging age than males in adulthood. Notably, if we fit a clock only for animals up to age 8, when the slopes for males and females diverge, they are similarly accurate (lines 130133). Thus, the relative flattening of the slope in older females likely accounts for better overall prediction in males relative to females (although we cannot exclude some degree of viability selection in females). We now explain this interpretation in lines 146-149.

If choosing different CpG sites would create a better epigenetic clock for females, what does it mean that different CpG sites are methylated differently in response to aging in males vs females?

We believe that the observed sex differences in clock performance reflect changes that occur at the same CpG sites, but with higher variance in females. Indeed, if we estimate the effect of age on DNA methylation for the 573 clock sites in males versus females, these estimates are well-correlated between sexes (Pearson’s r = 0.91, p = 3.35 x 10204), but are estimated with more certainty, per site, in males than in females (e.g., the standard error are systematically smaller in males; now reported in lines 143-146 and new Supplementary Figure 4).

– It was interesting to read about the complexities of male high rank and age, i.e. high rank exists primarily between 7-12 years of age whereas low rank exists across all ages.

Furthermore, it was fascinating that lower rank consistently associated with lower predicted age even when the lower rank was in a formerly high-ranking male, e.g. two males who were sampled later in life when they were of a lower rank both showed a decrease in predicted age, despite greater chronological age. This suggests that the increased epigenetic aging due to high rank is not a permanent change, i.e. does not result in increased aging across the lifespan but just during the tenure of high rank. The implications for the ‘recency’ vs ‘accumulation’ model are highly relevant. I look forward to seeing future work by Tung’s group with more longitudinal samples.

– The methods used are robust and the methods section, as well as the results, are very detailed and well-written.

We appreciate the positive feedback!

– The Results and Discussion section ends abruptly with the discussion of BMI and the costs associated with investment in body condition. I suggest a concluding paragraph that brings the reader back to the results on male dominance rank.

We have restructured the results and discussion based on comments from Reviewers 1 and 2, and now discuss BMI-related results in lines 284-302. The revised manuscript now concludes as suggested, by focusing on the implications of our results for understanding the consequences (and potential costs) of dominance rank in different types of hierarchies.

– I could not find project PRJNA607996 in the NCBI SRA but I assume that’s because access is limited until the paper is published.

Yes, that’s correct. However, a reviewer link is available: https://tinyurl.com/y5xuhdxf.

Response to second decision letter

Reviewer #1:

I do not agree with the author’s response about the correlation between age-related methylation status and BMI. In humans, even in the cases with normal BMI (<25), there were tendencies of accelerations of epigenetic aging as the BMI increased (at least several reports; PMID: 28289477, PMID: 28089957, PMID: 25313081, however it would depend on sample sets). I assumed that there would be the possibility of this variation existing in wild baboons as well.

Thank you for this perspective and the additional references. We highlight two important points that support our original response: specifically, that the effect of BMI on epigenetic age in male baboons is distinct from the epigenetic age-BMI associations in published human studies.

First, we explicitly test the hypothesis that BMI predicts Δage (our measure of biological age: predicted age – chronological age). We report that male baboons with higher age-adjusted BMI do appear somewhat old-for-age, although only when dominance rank is accounted for (lines 223-224, 230-232, 330-332; Table 1 and Table S5). This pattern is only observed in males: there is no evidence, in any of our analyses, that BMI predicts epigenetic age in baboon females (lines 223-224; Table 1).

Second, all of the papers cited above include individuals that are overweight and/or obese, and none perform analyses on subsets of the data that exclude those individuals. Indeed, two of them explicitly focus on the effect of obesity in their titles (PMID: 28289477, “Obesity accelerates epigenetic aging in middle-aged but not in elderly individuals”; PMID: 25313081, “Obesity accelerates epigenetic aging of human liver”). Because these papers focus on the range of BMI variation in developed human populations, we do not feel that they provide strong enough priors to override the actual data analysis performed in our study.

However, the reviewer’s comment brings up an important caveat that we now clarify in the text—BMI in humans is calculated differently from how we calculate BMI in baboons (baboon BMI = body mass divided by the square of crown-rump length; human BMI = body mass divided by the square of height; our approach follows the precedent set in Altmann et al. 1993, AJPA; Altmann et al. 2010, ANYAS; and citations therein). Thus, the scales of BMI are species-specific, such that a BMI of 45 in humans represents extreme obesity, but a BMI of 45 in baboons represents a healthy weight. To ensure that readers do not default to expectations set by human studies, we have placed additional emphasis on the distinction between baboons and humans in the revised manuscript, including explaining the difference in BMI calculation and noting that body fat percentages are very low in Amboseli animals (lines 189-190, 334-338, 570-573).

However more importantly, according to this study, I noticed an acceleration of epigenetic aging in the lower BMI group (paraphrased as young age group) as well which I personally have never seen before, despite the significant correlations in BMI/rank and BMI/age. The lowest BMI group included animals with lower rank which cannot be explained by their claim of aging accelerating in relation to the rise in rank. It seems like that the aging acceleration occurred not only in high-ranked animals but in low-ranked animals as well. The trend showing that the younger animals have a higher epigenetic aging speed is visible in Figure 1B in the manuscript when checked carefully. The fact cannot be ignored and I believe the authors should change the title of this manuscript.

Male baboons do not achieve full adult body size until several years after reproductive maturation (i.e. testicular enlargement). This produces a correlation between raw BMI and chronological age, as we show in new Supplementary Figure 7 (note that the correlation emerges entirely from having younger males, including a small number of pre-maturation males [n = 11] in the sample). Δage (predicted age – chronological age) is also correlated with chronological age because the epigenetic clock tends to overestimate the ages of young baboons and underestimate the ages of old baboons (as shown in Figures 1A-B and explained in lines 205-212). We now note that this compression effect has precedent in epigenetic clock and elastic net regression analyses (see for example Levine et al., Aging, 2019; Engebretsen and Bohlin, Clinical Epigenetics, 2019]), including some of the foundational work on epigenetic age prediction (the “Hannum clock:” Hannum et al., Molecular Cell, 2013; lines 208-212).

Together, the raw BMI-chronological age relationship and the Δage-chronological age relationship produce the patterns reported by the reviewer, which were based on simple bivariate correlations. However, these spurious patterns are eliminated in our multivariate linear modeling approach because we explicitly control for chronological age as a covariate in our linear models. Indeed, if we do not correct chronological age in this manner, any variable that is correlated with chronological age will predict Δage (see simulations that demonstrate this pattern, in response to the next reviewer comment).

Nevertheless, the comments above motivated us to perform a fifth additional analysis to check whether our findings are confounded by BMI (new results in lines 232-235, complementing four other alternative checks; Supplementary Table S5). We now show that if we drop all low BMI samples from our analysis (BMI < 41 and 31% of our data set for males, chosen to eliminate all males who clearly have not completed full growth; see retained points above the dashed line in Author response image 2A), we eliminate the correlation between BMI and Δage altogether in the remaining sample (Pearson’s r = 0.04, p = 0.67; Author response image 2A, C). However, eliminating low BMI/young males does not change any of our findings about the relationship between male rank, age-adjusted BMI, and epigenetic age (rank effect pvalue = 7.14 x 10-3 in the reduced data set; Author response image 2B, D; model now presented in Supplementary Table S5). This result is consistent with the idea that the relationship observed between Δage and BMI pointed out by the reviewer is driven by young individuals who are small and, because of their young age, tend to be overpredicted for their age due to the compression effect in the elastic net regression.

Author response image 2. Results of the full male dataset are qualitatively the same as results when excluding low BMI/young males.

Author response image 2.

(A,C) Chronological age in years at the time of sampling versus body mass index (kilograms/meters2) for (A) all males in our sample or (C) only males with BMI > 41 (i.e., only males above the dashed line in (A). (B,D) Results from the analysis including (B) all males in our sample (as presented in our main model) or (D) only males with BMI > 41.

Finally, we were confused about how the reviewer observed a relationship between age-adjusted BMI and Δage, because we do not (using age-adjusted BMI in Supplementary Table 1, Pearson’s r = 0.13, p = 0.18). We could only replicate for comparison in Author response image 3 by summing our age-adjusted BMI value and the original raw BMI values to construct a new variable that we never analyzed in our study. Indeed, this approach effectively reverses the age adjustment (raw BMI versus the reviewer’s “age-adjusted BMI” in males: Pearson’s r = 0.94, p = 6.71 x 10-60).

Author response image 3. (A) Age-adjusted BMI (as reported in the manuscript and provided in Supplementary Table 1) does not predict the difference between predicted age and chronological age (p=0.

Author response image 3.

18). (B) We found that Reviewer 1’s correlation, as shown in the review, can be recreated (C) only by adding raw BMI to our age-adjusted BMI values, which essentially removes the age-adjustment.

Once receiving the additional data that was not included in the original manuscript (which I asked in Minor comment 5), I was able to perform several valuations/verifications (including assays described above) to evaluate the author’s conclusion. Then I realized that the authors’ decision (statement in line 183-186) is inaccurate and has greatly affected the results. The authors should not include chronological age as a predictor in their model, even if the systematically (?) overpredicted for young and underpredicted for old animals were detected.

As we outline above, we agree that inclusion/exclusion of chronological age does greatly affect the results. However, inclusion of chronological age is in fact essential to producing interpretable results. This is because, due to the systematic compression effect described in lines 206-214, any random age-correlated variable would be spuriously associated with epigenetic age without correction for chronological age.

To illustrate this statistical confounding problem, we simulated a random variable that is correlated with chronological age to the same degree that BMI is correlated with chronological age in real data (r = 0.6, see code at Github (https://github.com/janderson94/BaboonEpigeneticAging/tree/master/Simulations_for_re viewers). First, we verify that this simulated variable and chronological age are significantly positively correlated at r = 0.6 (Author response image 4A). Second, we show that this simulated variable, which has no true biological relationship with Δage, nevertheless artifactually predicts Δage because of its correlation with chronological age (r = 0.32, p = 1.0 x 10-4; Author response image 4B). Third, we show that this spurious relationship is eliminated by controlling for chronological age, as expected (r = 0.12, p = 0.16; Author response image 4C).

Author response image 4. (A) Our simulation (based on sampling from the multivariate normal) produces the same correlation between the simulated variable and chronological age as observed for chronological age and the BMI in the real data (r = 0.60).

Author response image 4.

(B) The simulated variable significantly predicts Δage (predicted epigenetic age-chronological age) due to its correlation with chronological age. (C) Correcting for the chronological age compression effect (“residual epigenetic age”), as performed in the main manuscript, eliminates this artifactual correlation.

This spurious relationship, which occurs in the absence of chronological age correction, directly accounts for the reviewer’s findings. To demonstrate this, we repeated our simulations 1000 times to show that we consistently see a relationship with Δagebefore, but not after, correction for chronological age (Author response image 5A-B). Controlling for chronological age is therefore essential for unbiased inference, as has been previously recognized in the literature (e.g.,Hannum et al., Molecular Cell, 2013). Because this observation is an outcome of variable correlation structure and is not specific to this data set, we have not included these simulations in the revised manuscript; however, we have posted the underlying code on Github and are happy to include them at the editor’s request.

Author response image 5. (A) 1000 simulations of this random variable consistently result in a negative correlation with Δage.

Author response image 5.

(B) The spurious negative correlation is consistently eliminated by using residual epigenetic age. Histogram shows data from the same 1000 simulations, except with Δage corrected for the compression effect shown in main manuscript Figure 1. Histograms are colored based on the significance of the correlation between the simulated variable and either Δage or residual epigenetic age (p < 0.05 in blue, 0.05<p<0.10 in grey-blue, and p > 0.10 in grey).

By plotting real age and predicted age with their ranking information, again, I could see the epigenetic aging acceleration not only in high ranked animals (red/green dots) but also in low-ranked animals (white/blue dots) in the young to middle age range.

The reviewer is absolutely correct that, if one does not control for chronological age, young individuals will appear to have accelerated ages. This is the result of the compression effect outlined above and described in the manuscript (lines 206-214), and it is visible by eye in Figures 1A – B, which do not include the chronological age correction. As explained above, if chronological age is not taken into account, low rank (or any variable correlated with chronological age, see simulations above) will produce a spurious association with Δage.

If authors included chronological age as a predictor in the model, although variations (epigenetic aging accelerations) were seen in young-middle aged animals (mostly low-ranked), the targets of assays was limited around the central range of the plotting, which is mostly high-ranked animals. That model misled the authors and affected their conclusions.

We agree that including chronological age in the model affects the results, but as outlined in detail above, it is essential (see also lines 206-214). We also now provide a new supplementary figure (Supplementary Figure S8) that shows how rank maps onto the relationship between epigenetic age and chronological age (this figure corrects for the compression effect shown in Figure 1).

I recommend that the authors use the exact same ranges on the x and y-axis and include ranking information in Figure 1 to avoid misleading the readers.

We show the x = y line on each panel, which we hope addresses this concern. However, we have retained the axis ranges in Figure 1 because we believe it is important to enable the reader to clearly discriminate the variation we are depicting rather than condensing it along the y-axis.

Then I checked the intra-individual changes. In AMB_133 and AMB_69, I could see a higher decline of epigenetic aging speed in higher rankings (rank 3 to 4, and 5 to 5, respectively) than when it changed from 4 to 14 (in AMB_133) and from 11 to 5 (in AMB_69). I also could detect the drastic decline in epigenetic aging when there was a change in rank, from 4 to 6, but none when the rank was lowered from 6 to 11 in AMB_152. I further detected that the slope of AMB_230 (2nd to 3rd time point on rank 1 to 1) was similar to AMB_198 (rank 2 to 7), showing drastic aging-speed down even though the animal kept its highest rank. In addition, the cases that showed the epigenetic aging speed slowing while keeping the same high rank (for example, AMB_230; 1 to 1, and AMB_69; 5 to 5) seems to have been omitted from the assay in Figure 3, reducing the accuracy and reliability of the assay.

All of our analyses rely on the full population sample or population subsets. We did not pursue the types of analyses the reviewer outlines because any estimate of the slope relating change in epigenetic age and change in rank will be highly inaccurate when based on connecting only two data points.

However, the comment raises an important point about how we performed our analysis for the small subset of individuals sampled three times (n = 5). For four of the five males that were sampled three times, we only included the two samples that were sampled the farthest apart in time (i.e., excluded the temporal middle sample) to maximize the age change between sample dates. For the fifth male that was sampled three times, we included the first two samples collected in time because BMI information was missing for the third sample. We made this decision to avoid including all possible pairs from the individuals sampled three times, which would place extra weight on the data from those particular individuals. We now clarify our procedure in lines 610-614.

The correlation assay gave the exact answer to the author’s hypothesis, however the results did not match their manuscript title.

Please see our responses above about the fundamental problem with analyzing Δage without controlling for chronological age.

Most importantly, as I have mentioned in Major comments, additional supportive data using other sample set/cohort/species (and/or age-BMI-range-matched cases) are essential/necessary when making a claim in a high-impact journal. Although I understand the difficulties of collecting samples, I regret to say that the manuscript with only a single sample data (without any supportive data) is not appropriate to publish in a high-impact journal, especially when the authors are trying to make a claim this impactful.

We appreciate this perspective, but respectfully disagree. Many impactful studies, especially those that require true population-based data across the life course, are conducted in single species. This has certainly been our experience and is not uncommon at Nature Communications or other flagship journals. For example, work on the Amboseli baboon population that sets the foundation for this paper includes Lea et al. 2018 (PNAS), Gesquiere et al. 2011 (Science), and Tung et al. 2016 (Nature

Communications), and motivating work on the consequences of nonhuman primate social status in single populations includes Silk et al. 2010 (Current Biology) and Snyder Mackler et al. 2016 (Science). Outside of primates, other work on single long-term field studies has been seminal in understanding evolution and ecology in nature (e.g., Johnston et al. 2013 Nature and Graham et al. 2010 Science, on the wild Soay sheep of St. Kilda; Grant and Grant et al. 2006 Science, on Darwin’s finches on Daphne Major; Huchard et al. 2016 Nature on the Kalahari meerkats).

I do not agree with the statement (line 191-192, 313-314) and will not be able to judge the authors claimed point unless I am provided exact evidence of ranking effects on age-related methylation using other sample sets or other species. Therefore, I have to say that the phenomenon the authors detected could just be a variation in population found in this sample group.

Please see our responses above. We note that our title explicitly refers to male wild baboons.

In addition, I found many discrepancies in the Suppl Table 1 between the original version and revised version (see orange-colored cells in attachment; xls file) and felt it was sloppy.

The reviewer is correct that there were several differences in Supplementary Table 1 between versions. During the upload process of our data to NCBI, after all analyses were complete and prior to initial submission, we mistakenly swapped several sample IDs within the subset of individuals who were sampled longitudinally (i.e., no samples were assigned to the wrong individual in the Table, but some metadata for the same individual were swapped by date). We caught this error during the first round of revisions and corrected it both in the revised Supplementary Table 1 and in the NCBI Sequence Read Archive (project accession PRJNA648767, which we removed and re-uploaded at that time). This error did not impact any of the analyses or manuscript results.

I also cannot understand why “NA”s were found in “Accuracy of birth date” of multiple sampled animals in Suppl Table 1 (see attachment), and was then deleted from the assay that used 286 samples.

Thanks for pointing this out. We had neglected to add the full metadata information for the nine samples that were added for the purposes of the longitudinal analysis (described in lines 595-597). This information has now been added to Supplementary Table 1.

I believe that the authors still can analyze data using age-range and BMI-range matched cases. I highly recommend that the authors include simple/primitive analyses, such as the ones I have provided in Suppl materials, when preparing the next version of this manuscript for submission to scientific journals.

We appreciate the time taken to revisualize and reanalyze our data. However, as our results above show, simple bivariate analyses can often be systematically misleading. Further, as outlined above and in Supplementary Table 5, removing low BMI/younger males from our data set produces qualitatively unchanged results.

Reviewer #2:

I read the revised version of the manuscript, and found it much improved. Thanks to the authors for taking the comments so seriously, and for a thorough revision.

I only have one comment left, which is that the structure still has issues, and is more difficult to follow than necessary. There are now 3 aims at the end of the Introduction (not numbered, but a “First” then a “Second”, then a “Finally”), but there are 4 subsections of the Results section. The language used in the Introduction to describe any of the individual aims does not map directly onto the language used for any of the Results subheadings. All of this is making it needlessly hard to track. If you have 4 results subsections, then you should have 4 aims. Easiest would be to number them, but it’s not essential. Then, use *exactly* (not similar) the same wordings to allow readers to easily track the manuscript’s aims through the Results section and onwards. For example, your first Results subsection heading is “Epigenetic clock calibration and composition”, but this is *not* how you describe your first aim in your Introduction. Why not just write in your Introduction: “Our first aim was to undertake an epigenetic clock calibration and composition”, or something similar. Set up the aims and wording structure, map it straight on to the Results section with the same wording, and it will make it easy to follow.

I hope these comments are helpful. Congratulations on an excellent manuscript – James Higham

Thank you for this helpful suggestion; we’ve followed it here in the revised manuscript (lines 63-81).

Reviewer #3:

The authors have done an excellent and thorough job of addressing all three sets of reviewers’ comments. I found the new analyses and discussion about BMI vs dominance rank interesting and directly relevant to the authors' findings, particularly the fact that human BMI is more variable up to unhealthy ranges in contrast to BMI in wild baboon populations. The revised manuscript presents novel findings about the epigenetic impact of rank dominance in male wild baboons. The fact that the effect is not universal is very interesting and I find the authors’ explanation of why the epigenetic aging effect of dominance rank is not found in female wild baboons to be fascinating and exactly the type of data-driven, nuanced dissection of the results needed to push the field forward. The additional analyses and more detailed explanation of the results and their implications improve an already excellent manuscript. The manuscript presents valuable new data and meticulous analyses on a unique population of wild baboons with decades of supporting data and they provide two novel results – the first epigenetic clock in a wild primate population and compelling evidence of the epigenetic aging effect of dominance rank in males – these are valuable contributions to the growing field of social and behavioral epigenetic.

Thank you for these supportive comments.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Anderson JA, Johnston RA, Lea AJ, Campos FA, Voyles TN, Akinyi MY, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. 2021. Baboon Epigenetic Aging. NCBI BioProject. PRJNA648767 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    2. Anderson JA, Johnston RA, Lea AJ, Campos FA, Voyles TN, Akinyi MY, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. 2021. High social status males experience accelerated epigenetic aging in wild baboons. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
    3. Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Tung J. 2015. Papio cynocephalus Epigenomics. NCBI BioProject. SRP058411

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Samples used for generating the RRBS dataset.
    elife-66128-supp1.xlsx (45KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 2. Genomic coordinates, average annual methylation level change (averaged across the 277 leave-one-out models), and genomic context for the 573 CpG sites in the epigenetic clock.
    elife-66128-supp2.xlsx (51.3KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 3. Results from site-by-site models (for each of 534 testable clock sites) predicting chronological age, controlling for relatedness.
    elife-66128-supp3.xlsx (43.1KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 4. Pearson correlations among covariates for females (above diagonal) and males (below diagonal), with p-values in parentheses.
    elife-66128-supp4.xlsx (9.3KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 5. Linear models for Δage, fit for males, with significant results shown in bold.
    elife-66128-supp5.xlsx (10.6KB, xlsx)
    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    All sequencing data generated during this study are available in the NCBI Sequence Read Archive (project accession PRJNA648767), and processed counts data are available in the Dryad digital repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.qjq2bvqf0).

    The following datasets were generated:

    Anderson JA, Johnston RA, Lea AJ, Campos FA, Voyles TN, Akinyi MY, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. 2021. Baboon Epigenetic Aging. NCBI BioProject. PRJNA648767

    Anderson JA, Johnston RA, Lea AJ, Campos FA, Voyles TN, Akinyi MY, Alberts SC, Archie EA, Tung J. 2021. High social status males experience accelerated epigenetic aging in wild baboons. Dryad Digital Repository.

    The following previously published dataset was used:

    Lea AJ, Altmann J, Alberts SC, Tung J. 2015. Papio cynocephalus Epigenomics. NCBI BioProject. SRP058411


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES