Abstract
For inexperienced brains, some stimuli are more attractive than others. Human neonates and newly hatched chicks preferentially orient towards face-like stimuli, biological motion, and objects changing speed. In chicks, this enhances exposure to social partners, and subsequent attachment trough filial imprinting. Early preferences are not steady. For instance, preference for stimuli changing speed fades away after 2 days in chicks. To understand the physiological mechanisms underlying these transient responses, we tested whether early preferences for objects changing speed can be promoted by thyroid hormone 3,5,3′-triiodothyronine (T3). This hormone determines the start of imprinting’s sensitive period. We found that the preference for objects changing speed can be re-established in female chicks treated with T3. Moreover, day-1 chicks treated with an inhibitor of endogenous T3 did not show any preference. These results suggest that the time windows of early predispositions and of sensitive period for imprinting are controlled by the same molecular mechanisms.
Keywords: animacy, thyroid hormone, avian, sensitive period, plasticity, T3
Introduction
Early experience plays a crucial role in shaping neural and behavioural development. However, early experience effects are stronger during certain periods of development (Hensch, 2005). An example is provided by filial imprinting (Spalding, 1873; Hess, 1959; Bateson, 1979; Horn, 2004; McCabe, 2013; Vallortigara and Versace, 2018). Shortly after birth or hatching, the young of some animals, usually of precocial species, learn to recognise their social partners (e.g., the mother and siblings) by simply being exposed to them. In the young domestic fowl (Gallus gallus), for instance, imprinting usually occurs within 24–48 h from hatching. Lorenz (1935) used the term “critical period” to refer to the fact that rather than being available throughout the lifespan, filial imprinting is shown only during a limited period of life. This time window can be extended for a few more days when a proper stimulus is not immediately available (Bolhuis, 1991). Being a flexible period with the characteristics of a self-terminating process (Bolhuis, 1991), imprinting is now better known as a “sensitive period.” Sensitive periods are windows of plasticity during brain development, in which experience has a powerful effect (Hensch, 2005; Dehorter and Del Pino, 2020).
The thyroid hormone 3,5,3′-triiodothyronine (T3) is implicated in the timing of the sensitive period for imprinting (Yamaguchi et al., 2012). In domestic chicks, imprinting causes a rapid inflow of T3 in the brain, particularly in the intermediate medial mesopallium (IMM), an associative telencephalic region involved in learning the features of the imprinting object (Horn, 2004; Yamaguchi et al., 2012). Via Wnt-signalling pathway, the enzyme Dio2 (type 2 iodothyronine deiodinase), localised in vascular endothelial cells of the brain, converts the inactive form thyroxine (T4) into the active form T3 (Yamaguchi et al., 2012, 2018). Endogenous T3 level in the brain peaks around the peri-hatch period, and decays within a few days if not boosted by imprinting. Injection of iopanoic acid (IOP), a potent inhibitor of Dio2, impairs visual imprinting during the sensitive period (Yamaguchi et al., 2012). After the sensitive period, administration of exogenous T3 allows non-imprinted chicks to imprint, re-opening the sensitive period for memory formation (Yamaguchi et al., 2012).
Re-opening of sensitive periods has been obtained also by other pharmacological agents (Batista et al., 2016, 2018; Yamaguchi et al., 2016; Aoki et al., 2018), and it has been discovered for phenomena other than filial imprinting, such as ocular dominance in non-human mammals (Hensch and Quinlan, 2018) and absolute pitch in humans (Gervain et al., 2013).
Although imprinting can occur with either naturalistic stimuli (resembling a conspecific) or artificial objects, a large amount of evidence shows the existence of spontaneous unlearned preferences for animate features (Rosa-Salva et al., 2021). These preferences act as a sort of canalisation mechanism to direct the newborns’ attention, favouring exposure to stimuli that are more likely to be social partners (Di Giorgio et al., 2017; Versace et al., 2018; Rosa-Salva et al., 2021). Preferences for animacy cues, that set apart animate from non-animate objects, have been described in newly hatched chicks, comprising, e.g., preferences for face-like stimuli (Rosa-Salva et al., 2010), biological motion stimuli (Vallortigara et al., 2005; Miura and Matsushima, 2016; Miura et al., 2020) and self-propelled objects that move with variable speed (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016; for review see: Di Giorgio et al., 2017; Lorenzi and Vallortigara, 2021; Vallortigara, 2021). The same animacy cues operate on human newborns and other species, in particular for the preference for speed changes we are dealing with here (see in the human newborns Di Giorgio et al., 2021 and see for reviews: Di Giorgio et al., 2017; Lorenzi and Vallortigara, 2021; Rosa-Salva et al., 2021; Vallortigara, 2021).
These biological priors, whose main function seems to speed up learning by canalising imprinting, also operate only during transient windows of sensitivity in development. Visually naïve chicks show a spontaneous preference for the head (face-like) region of a stuffed hen during the first 2 days post-hatching, which then fades away on day 3 (Johnson et al., 1989). The spontaneous preference for objects moving with visible speed changes (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016) shows a window of sensitivity in three genetically selected and isolated breeds of chicks for only the first day of life, then disappearing on day 3 (Versace et al., 2019). Similarly, the biological motion preference occurs only within the first few days of life (Miura and Matsushima, 2012). Importantly, appearing slightly later on day 2 of life, biological motion preference exhibits also sex differences, with females being choosier than males when approaching a biological motion stimulus (Miura and Matsushima, 2012).
From the second day of life, precocious sexually dimorphic behaviours start to emerge in chicks (Andrew, 1966). Due to different levels of social motivation and aggression, males and females exhibit different attitudes towards familiar and unfamiliar individuals (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara et al., 1990; Vallortigara, 1992; Versace et al., 2017; Lemaire et al., 2020; Santolin et al., 2020). Progressively, females develop strong social cohesive behaviours with familiar subjects, while males engage more in aggressive and explorative ones (McBride and Foenander, 1962; McBride et al., 1969). At the neurobiological level, imprinting-related gene expression in the brain shows remarkable differences between sexes (Yamaguchi et al., 2012); among others, an upregulation of Dio2 gene enabling imprinting, which appears to be quantitatively different between males and females (Yamaguchi et al., 2012).
Here we report that the thyroid hormone T3 can modulate the timing of one window of early preferences for animacy cues, i.e., change of speed. Different from the sensitive period for imprinting, this preference does not depend on a particular experience with stimuli but rather canalises the animal’s attention towards particular stimuli, working as a spontaneous, unlearned biological prior.
We devised three different experimental conditions. The first was aimed to confirm that the same window of sensitivity for the spontaneous animacy preference conveyed by visible speed changes shown in genetically selected strains (Versace et al., 2019) also exists in the strain of broiler chicks we were using in the lab. We confirmed that the preference is there on post-hatching day 1, but fades away on day 2. To check whether T3 influences the duration of this window of sensitivity, we performed two experiments. First, in order to show that inhibition of endogenous T3 action can abolish the animacy preference, we injected the inhibitor IOP on post-hatching day 1, and compared IOP-injected chicks with vehicle-injected peers. Second, in order to show that the animacy preference can be re-established by exogenous T3 administration, we injected chicks with T3 on post-hatching day 3 and compared T3-injected chicks with vehicle-injected peers.
Results
The results for the not-injected condition are shown in Figure 1. The permutation test revealed a significant main effect of testing Day (F(1,67) = 5.40, p < 0.05), but did not reveal any effect of Sex (F(1,67) = 1.17, p = 0.28) nor interaction (F(1,67) = 0.05, p = 0.83; see Supplementary Table 1 for the number of subjects tested in each condition for each sex, and for results split by sex). Chicks tested on day 1 showed a significant preference for animacy (V(35) = 458, p < 0.05, d = 0.44), whereas chicks tested on day 3 did not show any preference (V(36) = 310, p = 0.72, d = 0.08).
The results for chicks treated on day 1 with the IOP (Dio2-inhibitor) are shown in Figure 2. The permutation test revealed a significant main effect of Treatment (F(1,54) = 5.74, p < 0.05), but did not reveal any effect of Sex (F(1,54) = 1.10, p = 0.30) nor interaction (F(1,54) = 0.66, p = 0.42; see Supplementary Table 1). The vehicle-injected group showed a significant preference for animacy (V(28) = 347, p < 0.001, d = 1.01), whereas the IOP-injected group did not show any preference (V(30) = 268.5, p = 0.46, d = 0.14). As expected, at this age no sex differences were apparent.
The results for the chicks injected with T3 on day 3 are shown in Figure 3. As expected, the permutation test revealed at this age a significant interaction between Treatment and Sex (F(1,57) = 25.02, p < 0.001) but did not reveal any main effect of Treatment (F(1,57) = 1.27, p = 0.26) or Sex (F(1,57) = 0.20, p = 0.65). Females and males showed a different pattern within each group, T3-injected (W(29) = 166.5, p < 0.01, d = 1.17) and vehicle-injected (W(32) = 39.5, p < 0.001, d = 1.40). T3- and vehicle-injected females showed a significant difference in their preferences (W(29) = 183.5, p < 0.001, d = 1.59). T3-injected females showed a significant preference for animacy (V(14) = 85, p < 0.05, d = 0.63), whereas vehicle-injected females showed a significant preference for the non-animacy stimulus (V(15) = 11, p < 0.01, d = 1.0). T3- and vehicle-injected males also showed a marginally significant difference in their preferences (W(32) = 61, p < 0.05, d = 0.98). However, in spite of a trend for an inverted pattern with respect to females, T3-injected males did not show any significant preference for the stimuli (V(15) = 27, p > 0.05, d = 0.53) nor did vehicle-injected males (V(17) = 116, p > 0.05, d = 0.47).
Discussion
Untreated chicks showed a clear spontaneous preference for visible speed changes, a self-propelled cue to animacy (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016), on post-hatching day 1 that disappeared on day 3. Restricted sensitivity windows to animacy motion cues seem thus to exist in chicks similar to those for the head region of the mother hen and for biological motion (Johnson et al., 1989; Miura and Matsushima, 2012). Similar sensitivity windows have been described in humans as well. During the first month of life infants preferentially look at schematic face-like configurations over identical stimuli in a non-face configuration, whereas the same is not observed at 3 and 5 months of age (Johnson et al., 1991). Also, the intensity of selective EEG responses to face-like patterns tends to decrease over time after the first hours of life (Buiatti et al., 2019).
The thyroid hormone T3 appears to play a crucial role in the sensitivity window for spontaneous motion animacy preference. Animacy preference disappeared on day 1 in chicks injected with IOP, an inhibitor of Dio2 the enzyme converting the inactive form T4 into the active T3. While vehicle-injected day 1 chicks had a significant preference for animacy, consistently with previous findings (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016).
On day 3, administration of T3 seems to restore animacy preference, at least in females. Sex differences, which are well-known to be associated with critical period regulation by thyroid hormones (see Miura and Matsushima, 2012; Yamaguchi et al., 2012; Batista and Hensch, 2019), seem to be complicated at this age also by the effect of the injecting needle in itself. T3-injected females preferred the animacy stimulus, while vehicle-injected females preferred the non-animacy one. Given that we do not expect any physiological effect of vehicle (as indeed this is the case for day-1 treated chicks), it seems that in older chicks the mere, though mild, pain experience of injecting the needle interacts with the expression of animacy preferences at least in females (males showed a somewhat similar effect but with inverted direction).
Precocious sex differences in social motivation and aggression are commonly observed in chicks (Andrew, 1966). Functionally, it has been argued that they may arise from different levels of social motivation (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara et al., 1990) and attitudes towards novelty (Vallortigara, 1992) in the two sexes. Females tend to engage more than males in social reinstatement and usually prefer to approach familiar individuals (Cailotto et al., 1989; Vallortigara et al., 1990; Vallortigara, 1992), whereas males tend to approach unfamiliar ones (Vallortigara, 1992; Versace et al., 2017; Santolin et al., 2020). In natural populations fowls exhibit territorial behaviour wherein single dominant cocks maintain and patrol a large territory within which several highly social-aggregated females live (McBride and Foenander, 1962; McBride et al., 1969). This social organisation may favour the prevalence of gregarious and affiliative behaviours in females and aggressive and exploratory behaviours in males.
Sex differences have also been described for biological motion preferences, whose window of sensitivity occurs exactly around day 3 (Miura and Matsushima, 2012). Therefore, it could be that the injection of T3 affected both sexes similarly, but that they exhibited different behavioural responses coherent with their natural preferences that become apparent from day 3. Females would thus preferentially approach the animacy stimulus, engaging in social reinstatement with the most familiar, as predisposed, object. Whereas males, less socially motivated, would engage in explorative responses towards the unfamiliar, non-predisposed, object.
It could also be that the efficiency of T3 transport from muscle through blood vessels and finally to the brain is different between males and females. This may point towards a possible limitation of the method adopted here and suggests different T3 administration strategies.
From a neurobiological perspective, sex differences related to imprinting have been found in the brain. Several genes upregulate during imprinting, but only some do it in a sex dimorphic way. The Dio2 gene expresses more in males than in females during imprinting, while Tubby-like protein 1 gene, producing an upstream cell signalling protein, expresses more in females than males (Yamaguchi et al., 2012). Such differences might underly the behavioural differences between the two sexes observed in the present study in older chicks.
Note that vehicle-injected females on day 3 preferred the non-animacy stimulus, whereas males had a trend to prefer the animacy one. From day 3, chicks start to be more fearful due to the rising of hormonal levels (Schaller and Emlen, 1962; Rogers, 1995). Avoidance behaviours increase progressively from day 3 on, but with different timings in the two sexes. Males show a weaker avoidance response than females until post-hatching day 4 (Schaller and Emlen, 1962). Sex differences observed in the vehicle group on day 3 might arise from the different reactions to the needle’s stressful event. The fear caused by the injection in females might have evoked an avoidance response towards the most animate object, the animacy stimulus, making them walk to the opposite end of the apparatus. In males, less avoidant and more prone to aggression at this age, the same event could have evoked a tendency to approach the most animate stimulus.
It is also worth noting that early during development the levels of different thyroid hormones (T3 and T4, thyroxine) in the brain show sex-dependent differences. The onset of the surge of T4 in male zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) precedes that of females, while the onset of T3 in females precedes that of males (Yamaguchi et al., 2017). Strengthening the hypothesis of an interaction between T3 and other sexually dimorphic hormones, response to animacy in visually naïve chicks involves brain regions rich in sex steroid hormone receptors (Mayer et al., 2016, 2017a,2017b; Lorenzi et al., 2017), part of the so-called Social Behavior Network, which appears to be highly conserved in vertebrates (Newman, 1999; O’Connell and Hofmann, 2011; Goodson and Kingsbury, 2013; Lorenzi et al., 2017).
Thyroid hormones are key regulators of vertebrates’ brain development (Van Herck et al., 2013). Among others, T3 directly influences the expression of genes by binding to its nuclear receptors, which act as transcription factors (Harvey and Williams, 2002). Therefore, abnormalities in the level of T3 during development may result in permanent impairments. In the present study, inhibiting T3 function gave rise to a lack of the spontaneous approach behaviour towards animacy stimuli. Abnormalities in spontaneous preferences for animacy at birth have been linked to autistic spectrum disorder (Sgadò et al., 2018; Lorenzi et al., 2019). Interestingly, human neonates at high familial risk for autism exhibit anomalous preferential looking patterns to animacy cues provided by schematic face-like and biological motion stimuli (Di Giorgio et al., 2016).
Conclusion
In conclusion, we showed that providing exogenous T3 restores the predisposition to orient towards motion-animacy cue on day 3 post-hatch, whereas inhibiting the endogenous T3 conversion prevents the predisposition to appear on day 1. The similarity of thyroid hormone effects on imprinting and on early predispositions is unlikely to be coincidental. Furthermore, re-opening the neural plasticity to the effects of environmental stimuli without restoring at the same time the biological priors for proper environmental stimulation could appear worthless. The two processes should be functionally and temporally linked, and probably for this reason they share the same molecular ground.
Materials and Methods
Animals
All experiments were carried out in compliance with the applicable European Union and Italian laws, and guidelines for animals’ care and use. All the experimental procedures were approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Trento OPBA and by the Italian Health Ministry (permit number 1139/2015).
Fertilised eggs of the Aviagen Ross 308 strain were provided by a commercial hatchery (Azienda Agricola Crescenti, Brescia, Italy). Upon arrival, eggs were incubated under controlled temperature (37.7°C until post-hatching day 1, 33°C until post-hatching day 3) and humidity (60%) within incubators (FIEM MG140/200 Rural LCD EVO). Incubators were kept in darkness, preventing the chicks from any visual experience during incubation and hatching prior to testing. We employed a total of 190 domestic chicks (G. gallus; 95 females; the exact number of chicks in each condition and sex is shown in Supplementary Table 1). Sex was determined by feather dimorphism.
Apparatus and Stimuli
We used the same apparatus and stimuli as in previous studies investigating motion-animacy preference in newly hatched chicks (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016, 2018; Lorenzi et al., 2017, 2019). A detailed description of the apparatus, stimuli and procedure used can be found in Rosa-Salva et al. (2016) (Exp. 2). Briefly, the apparatus consisted of a white corridor (85 × 30 × 30 cm, see Figure 4) with two opposite high-frequency monitor screens (ASUS MG248Q, 24″, 120 Hz). Three areas subdivided the corridor: a central one (starting area: 45 cm long) and two lateral ones (choice areas: 20 cm long each). A small step (1.5 cm high) on each side delimited the boundaries between starting and choice areas. A video-camera, centrally located above the apparatus, recorded animals’ behaviour. The two stimuli were displayed on the screens and represented a red circle (diameter 3 cm) moving horizontally. One stimulus was moving at a constant speed (≈4.64 cm/s on our monitors) while the other one was visibly changing speed (the slower speed being ≈3.37 cm/s and the faster one being ≈19.64 cm/s), a reliable cue to animacy (Rosa-Salva et al., 2016). We counterbalanced the position of the stimuli on the screens between subjects.
Intramuscular Injections
Iopanoic acid (IOP 10 mM, TCI I0300, Tokyo Chemical Industry Co., Ltd., Tokyo, Japan) was dissolved in 0.05M NaOH solution at 1 mM and rebuffered to pH = 8.5 by 6M HCl. 3,3′,5-Triiodo-L-thyronine (T3, 100 μM, Sigma-Aldrich, T-2877) was dissolved in 0.002M NaOH and 0.9% NaCl. Vehicle solutions were also prepared to control for any effect of the injections. Respectively, the vehicle for IOP was a 0.05M NaOH solution buffered to pH = 8.5 by 6M HCl, while the vehicle for T3 was a 0.9% NaCl and 0.002M NaOH solution (vehicle-injected groups). One hour before testing, each subject was carefully taken from the incubator in complete darkness. A black hood on the head prevented any source of visual stimulation during the intramuscular injection to thigh. Control chicks underwent the same procedure without receiving any injection (not-injected groups). Immediately after, each chick was placed back to the dark incubator. To distinguish single individuals in the darkness while keeping the same auditory environment experienced before injection, we placed the chicks in individual compartments within the same incubator.
Testing
One hour after injection (according to the different groups assigned), each chick was individually tested for the spontaneous preference for animacy for 10 min. After placing each subject in the starting area, the time spent in each sector of the corridor was recorded. In order to measure the animal preference for motion-animacy, we considered the ratio (%) of time spent near the animacy stimulus over the total choice time using the formula:
The preference score could range from 0% (full preference for the non-animacy stimulus) to 100% (full preference for animacy), while 50% represented the absence of preference. Permanence in the starting area for the total duration of the test was considered as no choice and led to the exclusion from further analyses (this occurred in about 47% of the cases). Sexing of the subjects occurred at the end of the procedure of testing.
Statistical Analysis
The statistical analyses were performed using RStudio v1.1 and the following packages: goftest (Faraway et al., 2019), nlme (Pinheiro et al., 2020), tidyr (Wickham et al., 2020), plyr (Wickham, 2011), dplyr (Wickham et al., 2020), reshape (Wickham, 2007), lsr (Navarro, 2013), ggplot2 (Wickham, 2016), and lmPerm (Wheeler and Torchiano, 2016).
The number of subjects required in each group was a priori determined with a power analysis (Champely et al., 2020) with an effect size of 0.96, and an alpha of 0.05. Results showed that 18 individuals were required per group and per sex to achieve a power of 0.80.
To detect the presence of outliers, we used a Multivariate Model Approach using Cook’s distance. Subjects having a Cook’s distance four times greater than the group mean were considered as outliers and discarded from further analyses (Kannan and Manoj, 2015). We identified six outliers from different groups. To assess the normality of data distribution, we looked at the distribution of residuals (Q–Q plot).
As parametric assumptions were not met, we used non-parametric tests. In not-injected condition, to determine whether the testing Day (two levels: day 1 and day 3) and Sex (two levels: female and male) affected animacy preference, we performed a permutation test using F-test probabilities. A similar test was conducted for the IOP- and T3-injected conditions, to determine whether the treatments (two levels: IOP/T3 and respective vehicles) and Sex (Two levels: female and male) affected animacy preference.
To determine whether the preference was statistically different between groups within each condition (not-, IOP-, T3-injected), we conducted two-sample Wilcoxon tests. To examine whether each group had a significant preference for either stimulus, we conducted one-sample Wilcoxon tests against chance level (50%). We calculated Cohen’s d (d) for each Wilcoxon test performed.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Trento OPBA and by the Italian Health Ministry.
Author Contributions
EL, BL, TM, GV, and EV: conceptualisation. EL, BL, TM, and GV: methodology. BL: software. EL and BL: formal analysis, investigation, writing–original draft, and visualization. GV and TM: resources. GV, TM, and EV: writing-review and editing. GV: funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Authors are particularly thankful to Orsola Rosa-Salva for fruitful discussion and advices.
Footnotes
Funding. This work was supported by a grant from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013)/ Advanced Grant ERC PREMESOR G.A. (n 295517) to GV.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.675994/full#supplementary-material
References
- Andrew R. J. (1966). Precocious adult behaviour in the young chick. Anim. Behav. 14 485–500. 10.1016/s0003-3472(66)80050-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aoki N., Yamaguchi S., Fujita T., Mori C., Fujita E., Matsushima T., et al. (2018). GABA-A and GABA-B receptors in filial imprinting linked with opening and closing of the sensitive period in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Front. Physiol. 9:1837. 10.3389/fphys.2018.01837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bateson P. (1979). How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Anim. Behav. 27:470–486. 10.1016/0003-3472(79)90184-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Batista G., Hensch T. K. (2019). Critical period regulation by thyroid hormones: potential mechanisms and sex-specific aspects. Front. Mol. Neurosci. 12:77. 10.3389/fnmol.2019.00077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batista G., Johnson J. L., Dominguez E., Costa-Mattioli M., Pena J. L. (2016). Translational control of auditory imprinting and structural plasticity by eIF2α. eLife 5:e17197. 10.7554/eLife.17197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Batista G., Johnson J. L., Dominguez E., Costa-Mattioli M., Pena J. L. (2018). Regulation of filial imprinting and structural plasticity by mTORC1 in newborn chickens. Sci. Rep. 8:8044. 10.1038/s41598-018-26479-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolhuis J. J. (1991). Mechanisms of avian imprinting: a review. Biol. Rev. 66 303–345. 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1991.tb01145.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buiatti M., Giorgio E. D., Piazza M., Polloni C., Menna G., Taddei F., et al. (2019). Cortical route for facelike pattern processing in human newborns. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116 4625–4630. 10.1073/pnas.1812419116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cailotto M., Vallortigara G., Zanforlin M. (1989). Sex differences in the response to social stimuli in young chicks. Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1 323–327. 10.1080/08927014.1989.9525502 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Champely S., Ekstrom C., Dalgaard P., Gill J., Weibezahl S., Anandkumar A., et al. (2020). Package ‘pwr’. [Google Scholar]
- Dehorter N., Del Pino I. (2020). Shifting developmental trajectories during critical periods of brain formation. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 14:283. 10.3389/fncel.2020.00283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Giorgio E., Frasnelli E., Rosa-Salva O., Maria Luisa S., Puopolo M., Tosoni D., et al. (2016). Difference in visual social predispositions between newborns at low – and high-risk for Autism. Sci. Rep. 6:26395. 10.1038/srep26395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Giorgio E., Loveland J. L., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Versace E., Vallortigara G. (2017). Filial responses as predisposed and learned preferences: early attachment in chicks and babies. Behav. Brain Res. 325(Pt B) 90–104. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Giorgio E., Lunghi M., Vallortigara G., Simion F. (2021). Newborns’ sensitivity to speed changes as a building block for animacy perception. Sci. Rep. 11:542. 10.1038/s41598-020-79451-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faraway J., Marsaglia G., Marsaglia J., Baddeley A. (2019). Goftest: Classical Goodness-of-Fit Tests for Univariate Distributions (Version 1.2-2) [Computer software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=goftest (accessed December 2, 2019). [Google Scholar]
- Gervain J., Vines B. W., Chen L. M., Seo R. J., Hensch T. K., Werker J. F., et al. (2013). Valproate reopens critical-period learning of absolute pitch. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:102. 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodson J. L., Kingsbury M. A. (2013). What’s in a name? Considerations of homologies and nomenclature for vertebrate social behavior networks. Horm. Behav. 64 103–112. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harvey C. B., Williams G. R. (2002). Mechanism of thyroid hormone action. Thyroid 12 441–446. 10.1089/105072502760143791 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensch T. K. (2005). Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6 877–888. 10.1038/nrn1787 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hensch T. K., Quinlan E. M. (2018). Critical periods in amblyopia. Vis. Neurosci. 35:E014. 10.1017/S0952523817000219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hess E. H. (1959). Imprinting. Science 130 133–141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horn G. (2004). Pathways of the past: the imprint of memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5 108–120. 10.1038/nrn1324 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson M. H., Davies D. C., Horn G. (1989). A sensitive period for the development of a predisposition in dark-reared chicks. Anim. Behav. 37 1044–1046. 10.1016/0003-3472(89)90148-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson M. H., Dziurawiec S., Ellis H., Morton J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition 40 1–19. 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kannan K. S., Manoj K. (2015). Outlier detection in multivariate data. Appl. Math. Sci. 9 2317–2324. 10.12988/ams.2015.53213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lemaire B. S., Rugani R., Regolin L., Vallortigara G. (2020). Response of male and female domestic chicks to change in the number (quantity) of imprinting objects. Learn. Behav. 49 54–66. 10.3758/s13420-020-00446-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenz K. (1935). Der kumpan in der umwelt des vogels. J. Ornithol. 83 137–213. 10.1007/BF01905572 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzi E., Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Vallortigara G. (2017). Dynamic features of animate motion activate septal and preoptic areas in visually naïve chicks (Gallus gallus). Neuroscience 354 54–68. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzi E., Pross A., Rosa-Salva O., Versace E., Sgadò P., Vallortigara G. (2019). Embryonic exposure to valproic acid affects social predispositions for dynamic cues of animate motion in newly-hatched chicks. Front. Physiol. 10:501. 10.3389/fphys.2019.00501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lorenzi E., Vallortigara G. (2021). “Evolutionary and neural bases of the sense of animacy,” in Cambridge Handbook of Animal Cognition, eds Kaufman A. B., Call J., Kaufman J. C. (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Lorenzi E., Vallortigara G. (2016). Social predisposition dependent neuronal activity in the intermediate medial mesopallium of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Behav. Brain Res. 310 93–102. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.05.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Morbioli F., Vallortigara G. (2017a). The motion of a living conspecific activates septal and preoptic areas in naive domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Eur. J. Neurosci. 45 423–432. 10.1111/ejn.13484 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayer U., Rosa-Salva O., Vallortigara G. (2017b). First exposure to an alive conspecific activates septal and amygdaloid nuclei in visually-naïve domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). Behav. Brain Res. 317 71–81. 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.09.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McBride G., Foenander F. (1962). Territorial behaviour in flocks of domestic fowls. Nature 194 102–102. 10.1038/194102a014456379 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McBride G., Parer I. P., Foenander F. (1969). The social organization and behaviour of the feral domestic fowl. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 2 125–181. 10.1016/S0066-1856(69)80003-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe B. J. (2013). Imprinting. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4 375–390. 10.1002/wcs.1231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miura M., Matsushima T. (2012). Preference for biological motion in domestic chicks: sex-dependent effect of early visual experience. Anim. Cogn. 15 871–879. 10.1007/s10071-012-0514-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miura M., Matsushima T. (2016). Biological motion facilitates filial imprinting. Anim. Behav. 116 171–180. 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.03.025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Miura M., Nishi D., Matsushima T. (2020). Combined predisposed preferences for colour and biological motion make robust development of social attachment through imprinting. Anim. Cogn. 23 169–188. 10.1007/s10071-019-01327-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Navarro D. (2013). Learning Statistics with R: A Tutorial for Psychology Students and Other Beginners: Version 0.5. Adelaide, SA: University of Adelaide. [Google Scholar]
- Newman S. W. (1999). The medial extended amygdala in male reproductive behavior a node in the mammalian social behavior network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877 242–257. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connell L. A., Hofmann H. A. (2011). The Vertebrate mesolimbic reward system and social behavior network: a comparative synthesis. J. Comp. Neurol. 519 3599–3639. 10.1002/cne.22735 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinheiro J., Bates D., Roy S. D., Sarkar D., Heisterkamp S. (2020). nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models (Version 3.1-148) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme (accessed February 4, 2021). [Google Scholar]
- Rogers L. J. (1995). The Development of Brain and Behaviour in the Chicken. Wallingford: CAB International. [Google Scholar]
- Rosa-Salva O., Fiser J., Versace E., Dolci C., Chehaimi S., Santolin C., et al. (2018). Spontaneous learning of visual structures in domestic chicks. Animals 8:135. 10.3390/ani8080135 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosa-Salva O., Grassi M., Lorenzi E., Regolin L., Vallortigara G. (2016). Spontaneous preference for visual cues of animacy in naïve domestic chicks: the case of speed changes. Cognition 157 49–60. 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosa-Salva O., Mayer U., Versace E., Hébert M., Lemaire B. S., Vallortigara G. (2021). Sensitive periods for social development: interactions between predisposed and learned mechanisms. Cognition 104552. 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104552 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosa-Salva O., Regolin L., Vallortigara G. (2010). Faces are special for newly hatched chicks: evidence for inborn domain-specific mechanisms underlying spontaneous preferences for face-like stimuli. Dev. Sci. 13 565–577. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00914.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Santolin C., Rosa-Salva O., Lemaire B. S., Regolin L., Vallortigara G. (2020). Statistical learning in domestic chicks is modulated by strain and sex. Sci. Rep. 10:15140. 10.1038/s41598-020-72090-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaller G. B., Emlen J. T. (1962). The ontogeny of avoidance behaviour in some precocial birds. Anim. Behav. 10 370–381. 10.1016/0003-3472(62)90060-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sgadò P., Rosa-Salva O., Versace E., Vallortigara G. (2018). Embryonic exposure to valproic acid impairs social predispositions of newly-hatched chicks. Sci. Rep. 8:5919. 10.1038/s41598-018-24202-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spalding D. (1873). Instinct, with original observations on young animals. Macmillan’s Mag. 27 282–293. [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G. (1992). Affiliation and aggression as related to gender in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus). J. Comp. Psychol. 106 53–57. 10.1037/0735-7036.106.1.53 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G. (2021). Born Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G., Cailotto M., Zanforlin M. (1990). Sex differences in social reinstatement motivation of the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) revealed by runway tests with social and nonsocial reinforcement. J. Comp. Psychol. 104 361–367. 10.1037/0735-7036.104.4.361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G., Regolin L., Marconato F. (2005). Visually inexperienced chicks exhibit spontaneous preference for biological motion patterns. PLoS Biol. 3:e208. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallortigara G., Versace E. (2018). “Filial imprinting,” in Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, eds Vonk J., Shackelford T. (Cham: Springer; ). [Google Scholar]
- Van Herck S. L. J., Geysens S., Delbaere J., Darras V. M. (2013). Regulators of thyroid hormone availability and action in embryonic chicken brain development. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 190 96–104. 10.1016/j.ygcen.2013.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Versace E., Martinho-Truswell A., Kacelnik A., Vallortigara G. (2018). Priors in animal and artificial intelligence: where does learning begin? Trends Cogn. Sci. 22 963–965. 10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Versace E., Ragusa M., Vallortigara G. (2019). A transient time window for early predispositions in newborn chicks. Sci. Rep. 9:18767. 10.1038/s41598-019-55255-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Versace E., Spierings M. J., Caffini M., Ten Cate C., Vallortigara G. (2017). Spontaneous generalization of abstract multimodal patterns in young domestic chicks. Anim. Cogn. 20 521–529. 10.1007/s10071-017-1079-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheeler B., Torchiano M. (2016). lmPerm: Permutation Tests for Linear Models (Version 2.1.0) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmPerm (accessed August 02, 2016). [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. (2007). Reshaping data with the reshape package. J. Stat. Softw. 21 1–20. 10.18637/jss.v021.i12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. (2011). The split-apply-combine strategy for data analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 40 1–29. 10.18637/jss.v040.i01 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Cham: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Wickham H. Henry L. RStudio. (2020). tidyr: Tidy Messy Data (Version 1.1.1) [Computer Software]. Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=tidyr (accessed March 03, 2021). [Google Scholar]
- Yamaguchi S., Aoki N., Kitajima T., Iikubo E., Katagiri S., Matsushima T., et al. (2012). Thyroid hormone determines the start of the sensitive period of imprinting and primes later learning. Nat. Commun. 3:1081. 10.1038/ncomms2088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamaguchi S., Aoki N., Matsushima T., Homma K. J. (2018). Wnt-2b in the intermediate hyperpallium apicale of the telencephalon is critical for the thyroid hormone-mediated opening of the sensitive period for filial imprinting in domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Horm. Behav. 102 120–128. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.05.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamaguchi S., Aoki N., Takehara A., Mori M., Kanai A., Matsushima T., et al. (2016). Involvement of nucleotide diphosphate kinase 2 in the reopening of the sensitive period of filial imprinting of domestic chicks (Gallus gallus domesticus). Neurosci. Lett. 612 32–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamaguchi S., Hayase S., Aoki N., Takehara A., Ishigohoka J., Matsushima T., et al. (2017). Sex differences in brain thyroid hormone levels during early post-hatching development in Zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). PLoS One 12:e0169643. 10.1371/journal.pone.0169643 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.