Abstract
Background: While the COVID-19 pandemic has affected the lives of almost all people worldwide, many people observed also positive changes in their attitudes and behaviors. This can be seen in the context of posttraumatic growth. These perceived changes refer to five main categories: Nature/Silence/Contemplation, Spirituality, Relationships, Reflection on life, and Digital media usage. A previous study with persons recruited in June 2020 directly after the lockdown in Germany showed that the best predictors of these perceived changes related to the Corona pandemic were the ability to mindfully stop and pause in distinct situations, to be “spellbound at the moment” and to become “quiet and devout,” indicating moments of wondering awe, with subsequent feelings of gratitude. Now, we intended to analyze (1) by whom and how strongly awe/gratitude was experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic, and (2) how these feelings relate to perceived changes and experienced burden, and (3) whether or not feelings of awe/gratitude contribute to participants' well-being or may buffer perceived burden in terms of a resilience factor.
Methods: Online survey with standardized questionnaires [i.e., WHO-Five Well-being Index (WHO5), Life satisfaction (BMLSS), Awe/Gratitude scale (GrAw-7), and Perceived Changes Questionnaire (PCQ)] among 2,573 participants (68% women; mean age 48.7 ± 14.2 years, 74% with a Christian affiliation) from Germany recruited between June and November 2020.
Results: Awe/Gratitude scored significantly higher particularly among women (Cohen's d = 0.40), older persons (d = 0.88), persons who rely on their faith as a “stronghold in difficult times” (d = 0.99), those with higher well-being (d = 0.70), and lower perceptions of loneliness (d = 0.49). With respect to perceived changes during the pandemic, more intense feelings of Awe/Gratitude were particularly related to Nature/Silence/Contemplation (r = 0.41), Spirituality (r = 0.41), and Relationships (r = 0.33). Regression analyses revealed that the best predictors of Awe/Gratitude (R2 = 0.40) were the frequency of meditation, female gender, life satisfaction and well-being, faith as a stronghold, and perceived burden and also life reflection, while Nature/Silence/Contemplation and Relationships had a further, but weaker, impact on Awe/Gratitude as a dependent variable. Awe/Gratitude was moderately associated with well-being (r = 0.32) and would predict 9% of participants' well-being variance. The best predictors of participants' well-being were multidimensional life satisfaction and low perceived burden (related to the pandemic), and further Awe/Gratitude and Nature/Silence/Contemplation; these would explain 47% of variance in well-being scores. However, Awe/Gratitude cannot be regarded as a buffer of the negative aspects of the COVID-19 pandemic, as it is only marginally (though negatively) related to perceived burden (r = −0.15). Mediation analysis showed that Awe/Gratitude mediates 42% of the link between well-being as a predictor on Nature/Silence/Contemplation as an outcome and has a direct effect of β = 0.15 (p < 0.001) and an indirect effect of β = 0.11 (p < 0.001). Further, Awe/Gratitude mediates 38% (p < 0.001) of the link between Nature/Silence/Contemplation as a predictor on well-being as the outcome; the direct effect is β = 0.18 (p < 0.001), and the indirect effect is β = 0.11 (p < 0.001).
Conclusions: The general ability to experience Awe/Gratitude particularly during the COVID-19 pandemic may sensitize to perceive the world around (including nature and concrete persons) more intensely, probably in terms of, or similar to, posttraumatic growth. As this awareness toward specific moments and situations that deeply “touch” a person was higher in persons with more intense meditation or prayer practice, one may assume that these practices may facilitate these perceptions in terms of a training. However, the experience of Awe/Gratitude does not necessarily buffer against adverse events in life and cannot prevent perceived burden due to the corona pandemic, but it facilitates to, nevertheless, perceive positive aspects of life even within difficult times. As Awe/Gratitude is further mediating the effects of Nature/Silence/Contemplation on well-being, intervention programs could help to train these perceptions, as these self-transcendent feelings are also related to prosocial behaviors with respectful treatment of others and commitment to persons in needs, and well-being.
Keywords: awe, gratitude, spirituality, resilience, burden, perceived changes, COVID-19 pandemic
Introduction
The COVID-19 pandemic has changed the lives of almost all people worldwide in one way or the other. Many infected persons have died, others have recovered but have to cope with chronic health affections, and others recovered without relevant restrictions; a lot of people have lost their jobs, or are working in short-time, while others are in home-office or continue at their workplace. Many persons at risk avoid social contacts and feel isolated (1, 2), while others do not care too much about social restrictions (3), or even discuss why face-mask usage might be harmful (4). There are, in fact, heterogeneous ways to cope with the implications of the pandemic (5). A recent systematic review underlines that in the general population, there is a high prevalence of stress, anxiety, and depression because of the COVID-19 pandemic (6). Clinicians meet patients in depressive states and others with signs of “defeat stress” (7). Tumor patients, as an example of persons at risk, reported to be in fear of being infected, of having and complicated courses of disease and of staying helpless; they were irritated about conflicting information about the danger and about the course of the COVID-19 infection in the media (1, 2).
Nevertheless, related to the “extra time” at home during the lockdown, many people not only had fears and worries; they also perceived positive changes in their attitudes and behaviors. Tumor patients, for example, perceived nature and silence and also their relationships more intensely, some had a stronger interest in spiritual issues, while others perceived loneliness and ruminated worrying reflections (2). In a survey among a more general population from Germany, these perceived changes were operationalized and measured in terms of (1) Nature/Silence/Contemplation, (2) Spirituality, (3) Relationships, (4) Reflection on life, and (5) Digital media use (8). Strongest changes were observed for Relationships and Nature/Silence/Contemplation. All perceived changes were stronger among older persons, among those with higher well-being, and those who relied on their faith as a resource. Interestingly, best predictors of these perceived changes were the ability to mindfully stop and pause in distinct situations, to be “spellbound at the moment” and to become “quiet and devout,” indicating moments of wondering awe, with subsequent feelings of gratefulness (8).
Perceptions of wondering awe, which can be considered as a variety of religious experience (9) can also be interpreted as an experiential aspect of “secular spirituality” as no specific religious belief is required and thus can be perceived also by non-religious persons (10, 11). Awe in terms of admiration (as a weaker experience) or even of overwhelming astonishment (as a stronger experience) is an emotional perception triggered by “outstanding” experiences, situations, nature, music, and persons (12–14). Keltner and Haidt (12) assumed that perceptions of awe might be experienced also in “times of tremendous social change;” in consequence, this perception is probably relevant particularly in times of the COVID-19 pandemic, too.
In line with the findings of Yaden et al. (15), qualitative analyses revealed that the range of awe triggers is wide, encompassing, e.g., Experience of nature, Perceiving the sacred in creation, Encounter with impressive people, Birth/own children, Accompanying the dying, Professional situations/Crises, Times of silence, Art and Music, Sacred buildings, and Special spiritual practices/ceremonies (Büssing et al., in preparation). Depending on the triggers, the resulting emotions can be overwhelming, while smaller moments of wonder (or admiration) are more common. The readiness to be touched and to mindfully perceive such moments seems to be higher in religious persons compared with non-religious/non-spiritual persons (10, 11). As Awe/Gratitude is related to the frequency of spiritual practices (i.e., meditation and praying), these practices can be considered as sensitizers for mindful encounters with nature, others, and with whatever a person may regard as sacred (10). Praying and meditation seem to facilitate such perceptions of connectedness with nature and others. Such a sense of connection and belonging was observed by Prade and Saraougly (16), too.
In Franciscan brothers and sisters, who have committed themselves to live a devoted life, which may facilitate such feelings, Awe/Gratitude was related to their intention to seek God in silence and prayer, with peaceful attitudes and respectful treatment of others and nature, and special care for persons in need (17). In terms of an “inner transformation,” these perceptions may have consequences for a person's life, as they may change their behaviors and attitudes (12, 17). Self-transcendent emotions may also intensify a person's spirituality and their religious and spiritual feelings (18). Further, persons with a gratefulness disposition seem to be more able to perceive these feelings (10), and even persons with depressive diseases can experience them (19).
At any rate, Awe/Gratitude as a construct of spiritual awareness can be regarded as an additional dimension of a persons' quality of life. However, Awe/Gratitude is weakly associated with well-being and marginally only correlated to multidimensional life satisfaction (17). When one perceives awe in terms of the Sacred (God), then one may also feel more connected with others and may be more satisfied with life (20). In addition, gratitude as a life orientation is associated with well-being and positive social relationships (21), and with lower risk of major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, phobia, and drug abuse (22), as it may help to reframe the negative emotions (23). For religious persons, gratitude is also related with perceived closeness to God and to a secure attachment to God (24). Furthermore, spiritual transcendence and connectedness with transcendent sources of meaning outside is higher in grateful persons (25).
On the other hand, therefore, well-being is not necessarily a prerequisite to perceive Awe/Gratitude. Feelings of Awe/Gratitude may arise because of a more open and mindful awareness for those things in life that are of value and thus can emotionally touch a person—in spite of difficulties in life (i.e., coping with the outcomes of the COVID-19 pandemic). Such an awareness shift has been frequently observed in persons who undergo difficult life situations or phases of illness (26). In this perspective, one can even relate the outcomes of these experiences to the concept of posttraumatic growth (27, 28) or of spiritual transformation (29, 30), where people change their attitudes and make new resolutions, become (at least for some time) more conscious, more attentive, more mindful, more spiritually minded, etc.
As perceptions of awe and gratitude were among the best predictors of persons' perceived changes due to the Corona pandemic (2), we intended to analyze whether Awe/Gratitude could be regarded as a resilience factor to cope with the impacts of the pandemic on their lives. Theoretical considerations drawn from experimental studies would indicate that awe may buffer negative feelings (31, 32). Also in an experimental group of people waiting for the results of an intelligence test or peer feedback, positive emotions and less anxiety were observed when they experienced “awe conditions” compared with neutral conditions, and these findings were independent from a person's predisposition to experience awe (33). As positive emotions usually help to adapt in difficult situations, it was suggested that also gratitude could be a resilience factor [(34); cf. (35)]. We therefore intended to analyze (1) how strongly and by whom Awe/Gratitude was perceived during the corona pandemic, and (2) how these feelings relate to perceived changes and experienced burden during the pandemic, and (3) whether or not feelings of awe/gratitude contribute to participants' well-being or may buffer perceived burden.
Methods
Recruitment of Participants
Participants were recruited within 6 months (from June 9 to November 30, 2020). The snowball sampling started in different networks in Germany, i.e., university students and staff, research collaborators, religious orders and church communities, Rotary Club members, Facebook sites, diocesan websites, etc. All contacted persons and networks were invited to share the information and link where possible. Within this time frame, we were able to include persons from the first wave of the corona pandemic, from the “relaxation” time in summer, and in the meantime from the second wave of fall and winter 2020.
Participants were assured confidentially and were informed about the purpose of the study and data protection information at the starting page of the online survey. By filling in the anonymous questionnaire, interested persons consented to participate. Neither concrete identification of personal details nor IP addresses were recorded to realize and guarantee full anonymity. Therefore, we were unable to control for multiple entries.
Awe and Gratitude
To address times of pausing for “wonder” (Awe) in specific situations as a perceptive aspect of spirituality, we used the seven-item Awe/Gratitude scale (GrAw-7) (10). This single-factor scale has good psychometric properties (Cronbach's alpha = 0.82) and uses items such as “In certain places, I become very quiet and devout,” “I stop and am captivated by the beauty of nature,” “I pause and stay spellbound at the moment,” “I stop and then think of so many things for which I'm really grateful.” The scale thus addresses a person's emotional reaction toward an immediate and “captive” experience. All items were scored on a four-point scale (0—never; 1—seldom; 2—often; 3—regularly), referred to as a 100-point scale. In this sample, Cronbach's alpha is 0.87, and the single-factor structure was confirmed (explaining 57% of variance).
Perception of Changes
Perceived changes due to the Corona pandemic were measured with the 24-item Perceived Changes Questionnaire (PCQ) (8). This newly developed instrument differentiates five dimensions (factors) with good internal consistence: (1) Nature/Silence/Contemplation (Cronbach's alpha = 0.87), (2) Spirituality (Cronbach's alpha = 0.83), (3) Relationships (Cronbach's alpha = 0.80), (4) (worrying) Reflection on life (Cronbach's alpha = 0.74), (5) Digital media usage (Cronbach's alpha = 0.74). The respective items refer to perceptions that were reported by various persons at the start of the COVID-19-related lockdown. The respective items were introduced by the phrase “Due to the current situation…,” (referring to the Corona pandemic) and scored on a five-point agreement scale (0—does not apply at all; 1—does not truly apply; 2—neither yes nor no; 3—applies quite a bit; 4—applies very much). Specific items are “I perceive the relationship with my partner/family more intensely,” “I pay more attention to what's really important in life,” “I perceive nature more intensely,” “I enjoy quiet times of reflection,” “I am connected to friends via digital media,” “I deal more with spiritual/religious questions,” “I pray/meditate more than before,” “I'm more concerned about the lifetime that I have,” etc.
Well-Being Index
Participants' well-being (within the last 2 weeks) was measured with the WHO-Five Well-being Index (WHO-5) (36). It uses items such as “I have felt cheerful and in good spirits” or “My daily life has been filled with things that interest me.” The frequency of these experiences is scored from at no time (0) to all of the times (5). Here, we report the sum scores ranging from 0 to 25 and also the 100% level scores ranging from 0 to 100. Scores <13 (or <50) would indicate reduced well-being or even depressive states. In this sample, Cronbach's alpha = 0.89.
Life Satisfaction
Life satisfaction was measured with the Brief Multidimensional Life Satisfaction Scale (BMLSS) (37). It covers five main topics: intrinsic (oneself and life in general), social (friendships and family life), external (work situation and where one live), prospective dimensions (financial situation and future prospects), and health (health situation and abilities to deal with daily life concerns). All items were scored on a seven-point scale from dissatisfaction to satisfaction (0—very dissatisfied; 1—dissatisfied; 2—mostly dissatisfied; 3—mixed (about equally satisfied and dissatisfied); 4—mostly satisfied; 5—satisfied; 6—very satisfied). The BMLSS score was referred to as a 100% level (transformed scale score). The internal consistency of the instrument was found to be good in the validation study (Cronbach's alpha = 0.87). In this sample, Cronbach's alpha = 0.82.
Perception of Burden
Perceived restrictions of daily life, of being under pressure/stressed, anxiety/insecurity, loneliness/social isolation, and financial-economic situation due to corona pandemic were measured with five numeric rating scales (5NRS), ranging from 0 (not at all) to 100 (very strong) as described (8). These five variables can be combined to a factor termed “Perceived burden” (“Stressors”) with good internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.80).
Indicators of Spirituality
To measure reliance on faith, item A37 from the Reliance on God's Help scale (38) was used as a differentiating variable to assess intrinsic religiosity in terms of an attitude. It states “faith as a stronghold in difficult times” and can be scored on a three-point scale (0—disagreement; 2—indifference; 3—agreement). The frequency of spiritual/religious practices such as meditation or praying was assessed with a four-grade scale ranging from never, to at least once per month, at least once per week, and at least once per day as described (2, 8).
Physical Activities
We addressed physical activity/sporting and walking outside in nature with a four-grade scale (never, at least once per month, at least once per week, and at least once per day) as described (2, 8). When awe is indeed triggered by experience in nature, etc., then a positive association with walking outside in nature can be expected, but intuitively, not that intensely, with physical activity/sporting.
Statistical Analyses
Descriptive statistics for demographic variables and for factors are presented as frequencies for categorical variables and mean (±standard deviation, SD) for numerical variables. Analyses of variance (ANOVA) as well as first-order correlation (Spearman rho) and linear regression analyses with stepwise variable selection method based on probabilities (p-values) were computed with SPSS 23.0. To investigate possible interactions as mediation and moderation of independent variables on dependent factors, the Mediation and Moderation Analyses were performed with software R (4.0.3) packages “mediation” (39) and “olsrr” (40). Given the exploratory character of this study, we set a stricter significance level at p < 0.01 (41). With respect to classifying the strength of the observed correlations, we adjusted the recommended thresholds (42) to r > 0.5 as a strong correlation, an r between 0.3 and 0.5 as a moderate correlation, an r between 0.2 and 0.3 as a weak correlation, and r < 0.2 as negligible or no correlation. Cohen's d effect sizes were used to report differences between groups (43).
Results
Description of the Sample
Within the sample (N = 2.573), women (68%) and persons with a Christian affiliation (74%) were predominating (Table 1). Participants' mean age was 48.7 ± 14.2 years. Their area of profession was heterogeneous, ranging from Administration, Economy, Education, Medicine/Health, Church, and other (incl. students and retired persons). Despite a predominance of Christian and other religious affiliations (78%), only 43% stated to have faith as a stronghold in difficult times, 29% were undecided, and 28% disagreed. In line with this proportion, 38% were praying at a daily level (indicating a religious person) and 22% meditating at a daily level, while 44% were never meditating and 35% never praying. Physical activities/sporting and walking outside in the nature were practiced by 71 and 70%, respectively, at least once per week (Table 1).
Table 1.
n | % of responders | Mean ± SD | Range | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | ||||
Women | 1,443 | 67.9 | ||
Wen | 821 | 32 | ||
Age (years) | 1,261 | 48.7 ± 14.2 | 15–92 | |
Living conditions | ||||
Family household | 1,664 | 64.7 | ||
Shared house | 229 | 8.9 | ||
Single | 515 | 20 | ||
Monastery/community | 180 | 7 | ||
Profession* | ||||
Administration | 368 | 14.3 | ||
Economy | 243 | 9.4 | ||
Education | 285 | 11.1 | ||
Medicine/health | 527 | 20.5 | ||
Church | 434 | 16.9 | ||
Other | 913 | 35.6 | ||
Religious affiliation | ||||
Catholics | 1,331 | 51.7 | ||
Protestant | 569 | 22.6 | ||
Other | 105 | 4.1 | ||
None | 570 | 22.2 | ||
Faith as stronghold in difficult times | ||||
Disagreement | 705 | 28,1 | ||
Undecided | 728 | 29.1 | ||
Agreement | 1,072 | 42.8 | ||
Meditation | 1.2 ± 1.2 | 0–3 | ||
Never | 1,054 | 44.3 | ||
At least once per month | 345 | 14.5 | ||
At least once per week | 449 | 18.9 | ||
At least once per day | 530 | 22.3 | ||
Praying | 1.6 ± 1.3 | 0–3 | ||
Never | 821 | 34.6 | ||
At least once per month | 267 | 11.3 | ||
At least once per week | 378 | 15.9 | ||
At least once per day | 905 | 38.2 | ||
Physical activity/sporting | 1.8 ± 0.9 | 0–3 | ||
Never | 354 | 14.9 | ||
At least once per month | 348 | 14.6 | ||
At least once per week | 1,217 | 51.1 | ||
At least once per day | 461 | 19.4 | ||
Walking outside in nature | 2.1 ± 0.8 | 0–3 | ||
Never | 83 | 3.5 | ||
At least once per month | 388 | 16.2 | ||
At least once per week | 1,236 | 51.6 | ||
At least once per day | 690 | 18.8 | ||
Wellbeing and burden | ||||
Life satisfaction (BMLSS-10) | 2,573 | 67.2 ± 16.1 | 0–100 | |
Satisfaction with Support (BMLSS Support) | 2,571 | 60.6 ± 18.2 | 0–100 | |
Wellbeing (WHO-5 100) | 2,572 | 58.7 ± 22.2 | 0–100 | |
Wellbeing (WHO-5 sum) | 2,573 | 14.7 ± 5.5 | 0–25 | |
Low wellbeing (WHO-5 sum scores <13) | 835 | 32.5 | ||
Moderate wellbeing (WHO-5 sum scores 13–18) | 973 | 37.8 | ||
High wellbeing (WHO-5 sum scores >18) | 765 | 29.7 | ||
Perceived burden (“Stressors”) (5NRS) | 2,572 | 31.0 ± 20.9 | 0–100 | |
Loneliness/social isolation (NRS) | 2,572 | 25.0 ± 28.5 | 0–100 | |
No loneliness (NRS scores = 0) | 900 | 35 | ||
Low to moderate loneliness (NRS cores 10–50) | 1,245 | 48.4 | ||
High loneliness (NRS scores 50–100) | 427 | 16.6 | ||
Perceived changes (PCQ) | ||||
Nature/Silence/Contemplation | 2,549 | 56.6 ± 21.0 | 0–100 | |
Spirituality | 2,549 | 41.8 ± 26.0 | 0–100 | |
Relationships | 2,551 | 63.3 ± 18.9 | 0–100 | |
Reflection on life | 2,549 | 52.3 ± 25.0 | 0–100 | |
Digital media usage | 2,548 | 54.5 ± 23.7 | 0–100 |
Some ascribed themselves to multiple professions, and thus, the response rate is >100%.
Within the sample, life satisfaction and well-being were in a moderate range. Thirty-two percent have WHO-5 scores <13, indicating low well-being. Furthermore, 17% have strong feelings of loneliness/social isolation. However, participants' general perceived burden (5NRS) scored in the lower third, indicating that the majority of the participants were (only) “somewhat” affected (Table 1).
Participants perceived changes in their attitudes and behaviors due to the Corona pandemic, particularly with respect to Nature/Silence/Contemplation and Relationships, while changes in Digital media usage and Reflection of life were less strong; in contrast, changes in Spirituality were rather not perceived (Table 1).
Perception of Awe and Gratitude
Within the sample, stopping and being “captivated by the beauty of nature” and “experience and value of beauty” were experienced most often (often to very often by 90 and 86%, respectively), followed by feelings of “great gratitude” (often to very often by 77%), while feelings of “wondering awe” were experienced less often (often to very often by 47%) (Table 2).
Table 2.
Never (%) | Seldom (%) | Often (%) | Very often (%) | Mean score (0–3) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ED1 | I have a feeling of great gratitude. | 3 | 21 | 52 | 25 | 1.99 ± 0.75 |
ED2 | I have a feeling of wondering awe. | 10 | 44 | 36 | 11 | 1.48 ± 0.82 |
ED3 | I still have learned to experience and value beauty. | 1 | 9 | 57 | 33 | 2.23 ± 0.63 |
ED4 | I stop and am captivated by the beauty of nature. | 1 | 13 | 46 | 40 | 2.24 ± 0.71 |
ED5 | I pause and stay spellbound at the moment. | 3 | 38 | 43 | 17 | 1.74 ± 0.76 |
ED6 | In certain places I become very quiet and devout. | 3 | 31 | 46 | 21 | 1.84 ± 0.78 |
ED7 | I stop and then think of so many things for which I am really grateful. | 3 | 28 | 48 | 21 | 1.88 ± 0.77 |
As shown in Table 3, Awe/Gratitude scored significantly higher particularly among women (Cohen's d = 0.40), older persons (d = 0.88), persons who rely on their faith as a “stronghold in difficult times” (d = 0.99), religious brothers and sisters (d = 0.60), those with higher well-being (d = 0.70), and lower perceptions of loneliness (d = 0.49). Further, Awe/Gratitude was moderately related to a person's meditation and praying frequency, and weakly to life satisfaction (particularly satisfaction with life in general, r = 0.30) and to walking outside in nature, and marginally only to physical activities/sporting (Table 4).
Table 3.
Awe/Gratitude | |
---|---|
All | 63.74 ± 18.79 |
Gender | |
Female | 66.13 ± 18.32 |
Male | 58.72 ± 18.79 |
F value | 89.85 |
p value | <0.0001 |
Cohen's d (f/m) | 0.40 |
Age cohorts | |
<30 years | 57.10 ± 17.92 |
30–40 years | 57.20 ± 19.27 |
41–50 years | 62.25 ± 19.06 |
51–60 years | 67.04 ± 17.84 |
61–70 years | 69.03 ± 16.96 |
>70 years | 72.52 ± 16.62 |
F value | 37.90 |
p value | <0.0001 |
Cohens' d (<30/>70) | 0.88 |
Religious | |
Living in Monastery | 73.02 ± 15.32 |
All other | 63.04 ± 18.84 |
F value | 48.11 |
p value | <0.0001 |
Cohens' d (monastery/not) | 0.60 |
Faith as stronghold | |
Does not apply | 53.67 ± 18.11 |
Partly | 62.00 ± 16.25 |
Applies | 71.36 ± 17.58 |
F value | 225.56 |
p value | <0.0001 |
Cohens' d (yes/no) | 0.99 |
Well-being (WHO-5) | |
Scores <13 | 57.03 ± 18.50 |
Scores 13–18 | 63.85 ± 16.94 |
Scores >18 | 70.92 ± 18.68 |
F value | 119.05 |
p value | <0.0001 |
Cohens' d (high/low) | 0.70 |
Loneliness (NRS) | |
Scores 50–100 | 57.67 ± 19.95 |
Scores 10–50 | 63.23 ± 17.17 |
Scores = 0 | 67.32 ± 19.56 |
F value | 40.31 |
p value | <0.0001 |
Cohens' d (high/low) | 0.49 |
Table 4.
Awe/Gratitude | |
---|---|
Perceived Changes (PCQ) | |
Nature/Silence/Contemplation | 0.408** |
Spirituality | 0.407** |
Relationships | 0.333** |
Reflection on life | 0.257** |
Digital media usage | 0.146** |
Restrictions | −0.174** |
Well-being and burden | |
Life satisfaction (BMLSS-10) | 0.289** |
Well-being (WHO-5) | 0.316** |
Perceived burden (5NRS) | −0.153** |
Frequency of activities | |
Physical activities/sporting | 0.143** |
Walking outside in nature | 0.237** |
Meditation | 0.442** |
Praying | 0.365** |
p < 0.001 (Spearman rho); moderate associations are highlighted.
Relation of Awe/Gratitude to Perceived Changes Due to the Corona Pandemic
With respect to perceived changes in attitudes and behaviors because of the Corona pandemic, Awe/Gratitude was moderately related to Nature/Silence/Contemplation, Spirituality and Relationships, weakly also to Reflection of life, and marginally only to Digital media usage or perceived Restrictions (Table 4). Also, Perceived burden (“Stressors”) related to the Corona pandemic was marginally (and inversely) related to Awe/Gratitude scores.
Predictors of Awe/Gratitude
As there are several variables significantly related to perceptions of Awe/Gratitude, both inherent and in response to the pandemic, regression analyses were performed to analyze which independent variables would predict Awe/Gratitude (as a dependent variable). These regression analyses were performed in different steps, which refer to previous findings (8): (1) including gender and age as independent variables, (2) adding spirituality-related variables (faith as a strong hold, meditation, and praying), (3) adding well-being and life satisfaction (which is related also to the feelings during the pandemic), and (4) adding perceived changes and burden because of the pandemic.
As shown in Table 5, higher age and female gender predicted 11% of Awe/Gratitude score variance (with age being the best predictor). Adding the three spirituality-related variables, the explained variance increased to 28% (now meditation frequency became the best predictor). Adding well-being and life satisfaction to the model in the third step, the included variables would explain 35% of variance (now age loses its relevance as a predictor). Adding the topics of perceived changes and perceived burden due to the corona pandemic raises the predictive power to 40%. Finally, the best predictors of Awe/Gratitude were frequency of meditation practice, female gender, life satisfaction and well-being, faith as a stronghold, perceived burden, and life reflection because of the pandemic. Perceived Restrictions and changes in spiritual practices and perceptions (Spirituality), and changed Digital media usage had no independent predictive relevance in this final model.
Table 5.
Beta | T | p | Beta | T | p | Beta | T | p | Beta | T | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Dependent variable: GrAw-7 scores | Model 1: F = 135.6. R2 = 0.11 | Model 2: F = 157.7. R2 = 0.28 | Model 3: F = 154.6. R2 = 0.35 | Model 4: F = 95.5 R2 = 0.40 | ||||||||
Constant | 32.891 | <0.0001 | 33.405 | <0.0001 | 19.082 | <0.0001 | 8.355 | <0.0001 | ||||
Male gender | −0.199 | −10.081 | <0.0001 | −0.177 | −9.300 | <0.0001 | −0.190 | −10.482 | <0.0001 | −0.162 | −9.157 | <0.0001 |
Age cohorts | 0.266 | 13.450 | <0.0001 | 0.100 | 4.931 | <0.0001 | 0.070 | 3.560 | <0.0001 | 0.054 | 2.842 | 0.005 |
Faith as hold | 0.180 | 6.669 | <0.0001 | 0.144 | 5.584 | <0.0001 | 0.105 | 3.972 | <0.0001 | |||
Meditation | 0.286 | 13.393 | <0.0001 | 0.285 | 13.963 | <0.0001 | 0.232 | 11.180 | <0.0001 | |||
Praying | 0.080 | 2.911 | 0.004 | 0.088 | 3.333 | 0.001 | 0.075 | 2.763 | 0.006 | |||
Well-being (WHO-5) | 0.134 | 5.945 | <0.0001 | 0.161 | 6.721 | <0.0001 | ||||||
Life satisfaction (BMLSS-10) | 0.164 | 7.382 | <0.0001 | 0.184 | 8.019 | <0.0001 | ||||||
Nature/Silence/Contemplation (PCQ) | 0.084 | 3.190 | 0.001 | |||||||||
Spirituality (PCQ) | 0.025 | 0.805 | 0.421 | |||||||||
Relationships (PCQ) | 0.082 | 3.668 | <0.0001 | |||||||||
Reflection on life (PCQ) | 0.101 | 4.232 | <0.0001 | |||||||||
Digital media usage (PCQ) | −0.020 | −1.048 | 0.295 | |||||||||
Perceived restrictions (PCQ) | −0.035 | −1.582 | 0.114 | |||||||||
Perceived burden (5NRS) | 0.103 | 4.210 | <0.0001 |
Predictors of Well-Being
Which of the analyzed variables would predict best a persons' well-being (as dependent variable) during the Corona pandemic? Awe/Gratitude (as an influencing variable) alone would predict 9% of participants' variance in their well-being, as regression analyses indicated. Stepwise regression analyses including the abovementioned independent variables revealed that it is foremost life satisfaction and low perceived burden (related to the pandemic), which would together explain 44% of well-being variance. Next, relevant variables were Awe/Gratitude and Nature/Silence/Contemplation (PCQ), which would add 3% of the explained variance. The next six significant predictors (low Reflection of life, higher age, walking outside in nature, low Relationships, female gender, physical activities/sporting) were of even less relevance as all of them together would add only 2% of the explained variance (Table 6). Not significant in this prediction model were frequency of Meditation and Praying, Spirituality (PCQ), and Digital media usage (PCQ).
Table 6.
Dependent Variable: | |||
---|---|---|---|
Well-being (WHO-5) | |||
Model 10: F = 196.3, p < 0.0001; R2 = 0.49 | Beta | T | p |
(constant) | 5.984 | <0.0001 | |
Life satisfaction (BMLSS-10) | 0.339 | 17.154 | <0.0001 |
Perceived burden (5NRS) | −0.320 | −16.578 | <0.0001 |
Awe/Gratitude (GrAw-7) | 0.123 | 6.535 | <0.0001 |
Perceived Changes: Nature/Silence/Contemplation (PCQ) | 0.168 | 7.251 | <0.0001 |
Perceived Changes: Reflection of life (PCQ) | −0.115 | −5.651 | <0.0001 |
Age cohort | 0.062 | 3.715 | <0.0001 |
Walking outside in nature | 0.049 | 2.907 | 0.004 |
Perceived Changes: Relationships (PCQ) | −0.059 | −2.912 | 0.004 |
Female gender | 0.046 | 2.801 | 0.005 |
Physical activities/sporting | 0.039 | 2.407 | 0.016 |
Not significant in the model: Praying, Meditation, Satisfaction with support, Perceived Changes: Spirituality; Perceived Changes: Digital media usage.
Meditation and Moderator Analyses
It is supposed that Awe/Gratitude could play a role in the analysis either as mediator or moderator. For this purpose, both causal relationships will be investigated. Mediation analysis describes a causal sequence of effects from the predictor variable on the outcome. Moderator analysis evaluates if a given variable affects the direction and/or strength of the causal relationship (in terms of an “enhancer” or “buffer”). Mediation analysis revealed that Awe/Gratitude mediates 42% (p < 0.001) of the link between well-being as a predictor on Nature/Silence/Contemplation as an outcome and has a direct effect of β = 0.15 (p < 0.001) and an indirect effect of β = 0.11 (p < 0.001) (Figure 1A). Furthermore, Awe/Gratitude mediates 38% (p < 0.001) of the link between Nature/Silence/Contemplation as a predictor on well-being as the outcome; the direct effect is β = 0.18 (p < 0.001), and the indirect effect is β = 0.11 (p < 0.001) (Figure 1B). For Relationships as an outcome, Awe/Gratitude mediates 62% (p < 0.001) of the effect of well-being as predictor, with small direct and indirect effects of β = 0.05 (p < 0.01) and β = 0.08 (p < 0.001), respectively (Figure 1C). Finally, Awe/Gratitude mediates 95% (p < 0.001) of the effect of well-being on Reflection of life; interestingly, the direct effect β = −0.26 (p < 0.001) has a negative influence decreasing the intensity of Reflections, while the indirect effect β = 0.12 (p < 0.001) remains positive (Figure 1D). All other perceived changes were not significantly mediated by Awe/Gratitude. However, Awe/Gratitude was a significant mediator, but not a significant moderator of the link between perceived changes and well-being (data not shown).
Next, we analyzed whether well-being could moderate interaction effects between perceived changes (namely, Nature/Silence/Contemplation, Spirituality, and Relationships, and also Meditation or Praying) on Awe/Gratitude. Interestingly, Awe/Gratitude can be estimated through the variables Nature/Silence/Contemplation (β = 0.11), Spirituality (β = 0.09), Relationships (β = 0.26), well-being (β = 0.26), and meditation frequency (β = 0.10), but not significantly through frequency of praying (β = 0.01). The β values represent the standardized estimates of the regression model and are in the interval (0,1). In this model, well-being was moderating weakly the relationship between Awe/Gratitude and meditation (β = 0.15), praying (β = 0.13) and inversely also Relationship (β = −0.21), but not Spirituality or Nature/Silence/Contemplation (Figure 2). The respective model has a moderate adequacy explaining 36% of the variance in the data. Furthermore, no significant moderation model was observed for well-being as response variable (data not shown).
Course of Well-Being, Perceived Burden, and Awe/Gratitude Within the 6-Month Observation Period
Time of course may have significant influence on several variables. The main recruiting phase was within June and July 2020 (n = 2,046); nevertheless, we further recruited participants within the summer months August and September (n = 288) and during the autumn months October and November (n = 242), which went along with the start of the second wave of the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. Referring to different cohorts, within these 6 months, participants' well-being decreased significantly [F(2, 2, 575) = 70.4, p < 0.0001], while Perceived burden increased [F(2, 2, 574) = 100.8, p < 0.0001] (Figure 3). Within this time span, feelings of Awe/Gratitude were significantly declining [F(2, 2, 572) = 54.6, p < 0.0001]. This decline is more related to the decrease in well-being rather than to the increase in Perceived burden, as well-being and Awe/Gratitude are moderately correlated (r = 0.32, p < 0.001; Spearman rho), while the association between Perceived Burden and Awe/Gratitude is marginal only (r = –0.15, p < 0.001; Spearman rho). It has to be noted, however, that the participants of October to November are, on average, 5 years younger (mean age) than the participants of the months June to July [F(2, 2, 546) = 28.6, p < 0.0001], while the gender proportion was similar (p = 0.68, Chi2).
Discussion
Answers to Our Research Questions
Referring to our research questions, first, we can state that particularly women, older persons, and religious/spiritual persons perceived Awe/Gratitude more often (and intensely); this is also true for those with higher well-being and lower perceptions of loneliness. It seems that both, low scores of well-being and feelings of loneliness may distract a person's awareness for the Sacred and the “beauty” in life. In persons with depressive states, their perception of beauty (in nature) was found to be lower compared with those of the other patients, while their general ability to stand in wondering awe and to be grateful was not significantly different (19). In this study, well-being was particularly related to feelings of gratefulness (item ED1: r = 0.33) and to the experience and value of beauty (ED3: r = 0.30), to stopping in awe and then thinking “of so many things for which I am really grateful” (ED7: r = 0.28). Thus, low emotional well-being is associated with low feelings (or felt reasons) of gratefulness in life, in general—and probably also during the pandemic, in particular. In this study, reduced well-being was strongly associated with dissatisfaction with oneself (r = 0.56), dissatisfaction with life, in general (r = 0.54), and dissatisfaction with own abilities to cope with daily life situations (r = 0.50). These more “personal” variables could be related to the underlying personality structure.
With respect to perceived changes during the pandemic as second focus, more intense feelings of Awe/Gratitude were particularly related to Nature/Silence/Contemplation, Spirituality and Relationships. This means that the general ability to experience Awe/Gratitude particularly during the Corona pandemic may sensitize to perceive the world around (including nature and concrete persons) more intensely. Furthermore, well-being modified the relation between Relationships and Awe/Gratitude decreasing the intensity of those feelings. This interaction indicates that Relationships might be a protective factor, but if well-being fluctuates in a lower range of scores, the potency of Awe/Gratitude degenerates. Apart from moderation effects, we found evidence that Awe/Gratitude is mediating the link between Nature/Silence/Contemplation and well-being and also in the reverse order between well-being and Nature/Silence/Contemplation. This means that the effect of well-being on the perception of changes related to nature, silence, and contemplation can be enhanced by Awe/Gratitude, on the one hand, and that the perceived changes related to nature, silence, and contemplation may positively influence a person's well-being, mediated by the ability to perceived Awe/Gratitude. Moreover, Awe/Gratitude weakly mediates the relationship between well-being and Relationships and negatively between well-being and Reflection of life. Well-being itself is a buffer for Reflection of life (which implies worrying thoughts about meaning and purpose in life and the lifetime one has, and more intensive perceptions of loneliness) and the mediator and may thus reduce the intensity of this effect, acting as an enhancer.
The underlying dynamics for the realization of this ability to perceive beauty in nature, to stand in wondering awe and finally to be grateful might be comparable or even the same as in posttraumatic growth (27, 28) or spiritual transformation (29, 30), processes in which people change their attitudes and make new resolutions because of specific experiences. Yaden et al. (15) likewise observed that the experience of awe may result in feelings of connectedness with others and nature. The realization of such dynamics seems to depend on various factors. The best predictors of Awe/Gratitude were the frequency of meditation (which may indicate that the awareness can be trained), female gender (women are usually more aware of their emotions and more sensitive toward spiritual issues) (44), life satisfaction, and well-being (which may indicate that positive emotional states may facilitate awareness), faith as a stronghold (which may imply that whatever may come, one has unconditional trust in God or another source of hope), and inversely and marginally only perceived burden and also life reflection (which has a negative worrying connotation in this context), too. Nature/Silence Contemplation and Relationships had a further, but weaker, impact on Awe/Gratitude as a dependent variable.
The third research question was whether or not feelings of awe and subsequent gratitude contribute to participants' well-being, as awe was suggested to increase well-being and personal change (45) and to be related to openness and extroversion as a personality structure (46). In our study, Awe/Gratitude was indeed moderately associated with well-being and would predict 9% of participants' well-being variance. Best predictors of participants' emotional well-being were multidimensional life satisfaction and low perceived burden (related to the pandemic), and further Awe/Gratitude and Nature/Silence/Contemplation. Thus, these perceptions have their role in relation to well-being, but not in the forefront.
Even when it is true that Awe/Gratitude facilitates to be more aware of positive changes in attitudes and behaviors due to the COVID-19 pandemic, it, nevertheless, does not relevantly buffer against the perceived burdens and restrictions as the associations are marginal only. Moderator analyses further indicated that Awe/Gratitude was not a significant moderator of the link between perceived changes and well-being. It cannot be called a buffering resilience factor, but rather an ability to perceive the positive aspects in life—in spite of the stressors. Actually, this could be considered an interesting process of inner development similar or closely related to the concept of mindfulness: being aware of the situation as it is, and deal with the situation as it is without judgment, as judgmental processes would result in negative emotions (47–49).
The results show that the practice of meditation and praying is related to Awe/Gratitude and (in terms of training) may sensitize to be more aware of the underlying moments and situations that cause feelings of wondering awe, and thus, it was of interest whether well-being could moderate these pathways. We found that the effect of both spiritual practices (meditation and praying), when moderated by a person's well-being, increases the levels of Awe/Gratitude more than if they were evaluated separately, while the interaction between more intense Relationship and well-being decreases Awe/Gratitude scores. This result could be due to a range of participants who reported lower well-being but high scores for perceived changes in terms of more intense Relationship (which may become more relevant as a stabilizing resource). The variables with the strongest contribution to higher levels of Awe/Gratitude were perceived changes in Relationships and well-being, and the strongest moderation was observed between frequency of meditation practices and well-being. Praying as a separate variable did not contribute to the model.
Limitations
We are aware that the data are not representative for all parts of German society, as the recruitment process may have favored persons with Internet access, academic contexts, and persons with a Christian background. However, while this selection bias is acceptable to address the research questions, it would be interesting to analyze persons from others contexts.
Due to the cross-sectional design of the study, no causal conclusions can be drawn. To account for this, we added data from different recruitment months (resulting in different cohorts). Data from these cohorts (Figure 3) indicate that a decrease in well-being and Awe/Gratitude may precede an increase in perceived burden. However, one cannot fully exclude the possibility that perceptions of Awe are better triggered during spring times instead of late autumn.
Conclusions
Perceptions of Awe and subsequent Gratitude are higher in persons with a religious background (11), and in those with more intense meditation or prayer practice. Such spiritual practices may facilitate these perceptions in terms of “training” and attitude. However, these experiences of Awe and Gratitude do not usually buffer against adverse events in life and cannot prevent perceived burden due to the pandemic; rather, they facilitate to, nevertheless, perceive the positive aspects of life, particularly Nature/Silence/Contemplation, Spirituality, and Relationships. This indicates higher awareness of a connectedness with the world around and with concrete others (horizontal direction of relations) and with the Sacred (vertical direction of relations). As Awe/Gratitude is further mediating the effects of Nature/Silence/Contemplation on well-being, intervention programs to train these perceptions could be considered in order to support people particularly in the time of the COVID-19 pandemic, as these self-transcendent feelings are also related to prosocial behaviors with respectful treatment of others and commitment to persons in need (17).
Data Availability Statement
According to the data protection regulations, the data set cannot be made publicly available. Data are however available from the first author upon reasonable request.
Ethics Statement
Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on healthy human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was not required for this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements. Participants were assured confidentially and were informed about the purpose of the study and data protection information at the starting page of the online survey. By filling in the anonymous questionnaire, interested persons consented to participate.
Author Contributions
AB designed the study, set up the online survey, and wrote the first draft of the paper. DR and AB undertook the statistical analyses. KB and JS were actively involved in writing and revising the manuscript. All authors provided feedback and approved the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We highly appreciate the support of all friends and colleagues who have forwarded the questionnaire, and to all who responded to it.
References
- 1.Büntzel J, Klein M, Keinki C, Walter S, Büntzel J, Hübner J. Oncology services in corona times: a flash interview among German cancer patients and their physicians. J Cancer Res Clin Oncol. (2020) 146:2713–5. 10.1007/s00432-020-03249-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Büssing A, Hübner J, Walter S, Gießler W, Büntzel J. Tumor patients' perceived changes of specific attitudes, perceptions and behaviors due to the Corona pandemic and its relation to reduced wellbeing. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:574314. 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.574314 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Campos-Mercade P, Meier MN, Schneider FH, Wengström E. Prosociality Predicts Health Behaviorsduring the COVID-19 Pandemic. ECON - Working Papers 346. Department of Economics - University of Zurich. Available online at: https://ideas.repec.org/p/zur/econwp/346.html (accessed February 22, 2021). [Google Scholar]
- 4.Scheid JL, Lupien SP, Ford GS, West SL. Commentary: physiological and psychological impact of face mask usage during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:6655. 10.3390/ijerph17186655 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Mukhtar S. Psychological health during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic outbreak. Int J Soc Psychiatry. (2020) 66:512–6. 10.1177/0020764020925835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Salari N, Hosseinian-Far A, Jalali R, Vaisi-Raygani A, Rasoulpoor S, Mohammadi M, et al. Prevalence of stress, anxiety, depression among the general population during the COVID-19 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Global Health. (2020) 16:57. 10.1186/s12992-020-00589-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Shader RI. COVID-19 and depression. Clin Ther. (2020) 42:962–3. 10.1016/j.clinthera.2020.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Büssing A, Recchia DR, Hein R, Dienberg T. Perceived changes of specific attitudes, perceptions and behaviors during the Corona pandemic and their relation to wellbeing. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2020) 18:374. 10.1186/s12955-020-01623-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.James W. The Varieties of Religious Experience. A Study of Human Nature. Introduction by Reinhold Niebuhr. New York: Touchstone; (1997). [Google Scholar]
- 10.Büssing A, Rechia DR, Baumann K. Validation of the gratitude/awe questionnaire and its association with disposition of gratefulness. Religions. (2018) 9:117. 10.3390/rel9040117 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Büssing A. Ehrfurcht/Dankbarkeit als säkulare Form der Spiritualität bei jungen Erwachsenen und Ordens-Christen. Spiritual Care. (2020) 9:3–11. 10.1515/spircare-2019-0057 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Keltner D, Haidt J. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cogn Emotion. (2003) 17:297–314. 10.1080/02699930302297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pearsall P. Awe: The Delights and Dangers of Our Eleventh Emotion. Deerfield Beach, FL: Health Communications, Inc. (2007). [Google Scholar]
- 14.Silvia PJ, Fayn K, Nusbaum EC, Beaty RE. Openness to experience and awe in response to nature and music: personality and profound aesthetic experiences. Psychol Aesthetic Creat Arts. (2015) 9:376–84. 10.1037/aca0000028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Yaden DB, Kaufman SB, Hyde E, Chirico A, Gaggioli A, Zhang JW, et al. The development of the Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S): a multifactorial measure for a complex emotion. J Posit Psychol. (2018) 14:474–88. 10.1080/17439760.2018.1484940 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Prade C, Saroglou V. Awe's effects on generosity and helping. J Posit Psychol. (2016) 11:1–9. 10.1080/17439760.2015.112799226640507 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Büssing A, Recchia DR, Dienberg T. Attitudes and behaviors related to Franciscan-inspired Spirituality and their associations with compassion and altruism in Franciscan brothers and sisters. Religions. (2018) 9:324. 10.3390/rel9100324 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Van Cappellen P, Saroglou V. Awe activates religious and spiritual feelings and behavioral intentions. Psychol Relig Spirituality. (2012) 4:223–36. 10.1037/a0025986 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Büssing A, Wirth AG, Reiser F, Zahn A, Humbroich K, Gerbershagen K, et al. Experience of gratitude, awe and beauty in life among patients with multiple sclerosis and psychiatric disorders. Health Qual Life Outcomes. (2014) 12:63. 10.1186/1477-7525-12-63 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Krause N, Hayward RD. Assessing whether practical wisdom and awe of God are associated with life satisfaction. Psychol Relig Spirituality. (2015) 7:51–9. 10.1037/a0037694 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wood AM, Froh JJ, Geraghty AW. Gratitude and well-being: a review and theoretical integration. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 30:890–905. 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kendler KS, Liu XQ, Gardner CO, McCullough ME, Larson D, Prescott CA. Dimensions of religiosity and their relationship to lifetime psychiatric and substance use disorders. Am J Psych. (2003) 160:496–503. 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.3.496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lambert NM, Fincham FD, Stillman TF. Gratitude and depressive symptoms: the role of positive reframing and positive emotion. Cogn Emot. (2012) 26:615–33. 10.1080/02699931.2011.595393 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Watkins PC, Xiong I, Kolts RI. How grateful processing brings closure to troubling memories. Paper Presented at the 20th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science. Chicago, I. L. (2008). [Google Scholar]
- 25.Diessner R, Lewis G. Further validation of the Gratitude, Resentment, and Appreciation Test (GRAT). J Soc Psychol. (2007) 147:445–7. 10.3200/SOCP.147.4.445-448 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Li L, Li S, Wang Y, Yi J, Yang Y, He J, et al. Coping profiles differentiate psychological adjustment in Chinese women newly diagnosed with breast cancer. Integr Cancer Ther. (2017) 16:196–204. 10.1177/1534735416646854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ai AL, Hall D, Pargament K, Tice TN. Posttraumatic growth in patients who survived cardiac surgery: the predictive and mediating roles of faith-based factors. J Behav Med. (2013) 36:186–98. 10.1007/s10865-012-9412-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tedeschi RG, Shakespeare-Finch J, Taku K, Calhoun LG. Posttraumatic growth. Theory, research, and applications. Milton Park: Taylor and Francis; (2018). 10.4324/9781315527451 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kremer H, Ironson G. Everything changed: spiritual transformation in people with HIV. Int J Psychiatry Med. (2009) 39:243–62. 10.2190/PM.39.3.c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ironson G, Kremer H. Spiritual transformation, psychological well-being, health, and survival in people with HIV. Int J Psychiatry Med. (2009) 39:263–81. 10.2190/PM.39.3.d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Atamba C. Restorative effects of awe on negative affect after receiving negative performance feedback. J Psychol Afr. (2019) 29:95–103. 10.1080/14330237.2019.1594640 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Koh AHQ, Tong EMW, Yuen AYL. The buffering effect of awe on negative affect towards lost possessions. J Posit Psychol. (2017) 14:1–10. 10.1080/17439760.2017.1388431 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rankin K, Andrews SE, Sweeny K. Awe-full uncertainty: easing discomfort during waiting periods. J Posit Psychol. (2019) 15:1–10. 10.1080/17439760.2019.1615106 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Algoe SB, Stanton AL. Gratitude when it is needed most: social functions of gratitude in women with metastatic breast cancer. Emotion. (2012) 12:163–8. 10.1037/a0024024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Seligman MEP. Authentic Happiness. Using the Positive Psychology to Realize Your Potential for Lasting Fulfillment. New York, NY: The Free Press; (2002). [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bech P, Olsen LR, Kjoller M, Rasmussen NK. Measuring well-being rather than the absence of distress symptoms: a comparison of the SF-36 mental health subscale and the WHO-Five well-being scale. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2013) 12:85–91. 10.1002/mpr.145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Büssing A, Fischer J, Haller A, Heusser P, Ostermann T, Matthiessen PF. Validation of the brief multidimensional life satisfaction scale in patients with chronic diseases. Eur J Med Res. (2009) 14:171–7. 10.1186/2047-783X-14-4-171 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Büssing A, Recchia DR, Baumann K. The reliance on God's help scale as a measure of religious trust – a summary of findings. Religions. (2015) 6:1358–67. 10.3390/rel6041358 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Tingley D, Yamamoto T, Hirose K, Keele L, Imai K. mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. J Stat Soft. (2014) 59:1–38. 10.18637/jss.v059.i05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Hebbali A. olsrr: Tools for Building OLS Regression Models. R Package Version 0.5.3. (2020). Available online at: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=olsrr (accessed February 22, 2021).
- 41.Dahiru T. P - value, a true test of statistical significance? A cautionary note. Ann Ibadan Postgrad Med. (2008) 6:21–6. 10.4314/aipm.v6i1.64038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Akoglu H. User's guide correlation coefficients. Turk J Emerg Med. (2018) 18:91–3. 10.1016/j.tjem.2018.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Sullivan GM, Feinn R. Using effect size-or why the P value is not enough. J Grad Med Educ. (2012) 4:279–82. 10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Luna MJ, Ameli R, Sinaii N, Cheringal J, Panahi S, Berger A. Gender differences in psycho-social-spiritual healing. J Womens Health. (2019) 28:11. 10.1089/jwh.2019.7837 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Haidt J, Keltner D. Appreciation of beauty and excellence. In: Peterson Cand Seligman MEP, editor. Character Strengths and Virtues. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; (2004). p. 537–51. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Dong R, Ni SG. Openness to experience, extraversion, and subjective well-being among chinese college students: the mediating role of dispositional awe. Psychol Rep. (2020) 123:903–28. 10.1177/0033294119826884 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hayes SC, Follette VM, Linehan MM. Mindfulness and Acceptance: Expanding the Cognitive-Behavioral Tradition. New York, NY: Guilford Publications; (2004). [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kabat-Zinn J. Full Catastrophe Living, Revised Edition: How to Cope With Stress, Pain and Illness Using Mindfulness Meditation. London: Piatkus; (2013). [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kabat Zinn J. Mindfulness for Beginners: Reclaiming the Present Moment–And Your Life. Boulder: Sounds True Inc; (2016). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
According to the data protection regulations, the data set cannot be made publicly available. Data are however available from the first author upon reasonable request.