Abstract
Structural covariance conceptualizes how morphologic properties of brain regions are related to one another (across individuals). It can provide unique information to cortical structure (e.g., thickness) about the development of functionally meaningful networks. The current study investigated how structural covariance networks develop during the transition from childhood to adolescence, a period characterized by marked structural re-organization. Participants (N = 192; scans = 366) completed MRI assessments between 8.5 and 14.5 years of age. A sliding window approach was used to create “age-bins”, and structural covariance networks (based on cortical thickness) were created for each bin. Next, generalized additive models were used to characterize trajectories of age-related changes in network properties. Results revealed nonlinear trajectories with “peaks” in mean correlation and global density that are suggestive of a period of convergence in anatomical properties across the cortex during early adolescence, prior to regional specialization. “Hub” regions in sensorimotor cortices were present by late childhood, but the extent and strength of association cortices as “hubs” increased into mid-adolescence. Moreover, these regional changes were found to be related to rates of thinning across the cortex. In the context of neurocognitive networks, the frontoparietal, default mode, and attention systems exhibited age-related increases in within-network and between-network covariance. These regional and modular developmental patterns are consistent with continued refinement of socioemotional and other complex executive functions that are supported by higher-order cognitive networks during early adolescence.
Subject terms: Neural circuits, Development of the nervous system
Introduction
Converging evidence from longitudinal neuroimaging research highlights patterns of cortical thinning during childhood and adolescence1, reflective of synaptic pruning, myelination and/or cortical morphology2–4. These normative developmental trajectories do not, however, occur simultaneously across the cortex; i.e., there is regional variation in grey matter changes5–7. Importantly, this variability suggests that structural cortical networks are also likely to mature during this period8. Understanding the changing relationships between cortical regions may provide unique information beyond univariate studies of grey matter development, as covariance in structural properties is thought to arise from experience dependent plasticity and mutually trophic processes9–11, such that regions that fire together, wire together12. In particular, the transition between childhood and adolescence, and associated pubertal processes, brings about a second wave of structural “re-organization” that is second only to infancy13–15. However, targeted investigations of the development of structural cortical networks during this period are lacking.
Structural covariance is a multivariate analysis technique that conceptualizes how morphological properties of different brain regions relate to each other at the group-level. To do so, properties such as cortical thickness are measured for each brain region in a group of subjects, and correlations between these regional estimates are calculated for each pair of regions across the group. While most research has characterized topological properties of structural covariance across one period of the lifespan (e.g., infancy16, childhood/adolescence17, aging18), a smaller set of studies have shown that these properties also change over time. Studies of early childhood to late adolescence have found patterns of increasing integration (i.e., capacity to facilitate the combination of information from distributed brain regions) and decreasing segregation (i.e., capacity to facilitate specialized processing within groups of regions) of networks until late childhood, followed by inverse trajectories or plateaus during adolescence19–21. Those focusing on later adolescence and young adulthood have shown that the strength of overall cortical correlations decreases between 14 and 20 years of age before plateauing8, which is hypothesized to reflect inter-individual variability in the timing of maturation of different brain regions. Taken together, these studies provide growing evidence that the transition between childhood and adolescence may be particularly characterised by evolving structural network properties, and that these global changes may be nonlinear in pattern.
There is also emerging evidence for regional variability in covariance patterns; for example, association cortices have been found to exhibit continued increases in connectivity strength and efficiency from early childhood to young adulthood19,21, while paralimbic and sensorimotor cortices exhibit increases and decreases, respectively, that plateau19. Others have shown that neurocognitive systems (i.e., regions within functional communities) have differential developmental patterns during later adolescence and young adulthood, with the frontoparietal network exhibiting the greatest decrease in covariance8. Those using a lifespan approach have also found that higher-order cognitive systems exhibit structural covariance changes that differentiate young adulthood from both childhood/adolescence and older adults22. Prior literature has thus identified regional variability in covariance patterns when using used wide age ranges or a life-span approach. However, extending such analyses to specifically focus on the transition from childhood to adolescence may provide novel insight into prominent models of neurodevelopment that purport a mismatch between neurocognitive systems that begins during this transition period e.g. 23,24.
Early adolescence is also characterized by significant sex differences in biological development, which are often postulated to underlie prominent sex differences in the prevalence of psychopathology that emerge during this period25,26. While research has largely failed to identify such differences in structural covariance patterns in neonates16, young8 and older adults10, targeted investigations of the transition from childhood to adolescence may provide novel insight into sexual dimorphism. Pubertal hormone changes that support physical maturation also act on receptors in the brain27. Based on earlier pubertal maturation in females, some have suggested they may undergo earlier cortical28,29 and white matter30 maturation relative to males. Although a number of studies have failed to identify sex differences in cortical thinning, some support comes from research using multivariate approaches31. Thus, the examination of changes in network properties may provide novel insight into potentially differential patterns of cortical maturation in males and females during early adolescence.
The current study extends the literature on the network properties of brain structure during the transition from childhood to adolescence. As highlighted by Váša and colleagues8, prior literature has typically provided coarse-grained resolution of structural network development by categorizing participants into discrete (and wide) age groups. Arbitrary definitions of age-defined groups may also be contributing to inconsistencies in the literature. As such, we utilize a sliding window approach (as employed in recent studies8,21) to precisely characterize changes in structural covariance networks during this transition period (i.e., how does the relationship between structural properties of regions change over time). Our age-defined structural covariance networks are based on sliding windows of larger participant numbers and narrower age ranges relative to prior literature e.g. 19,20, thus increasing the robustness of our correlation estimates32. This was achievable with an overall sample size that was considerable for the confined developmental period that was examined. We hypothesized an increase in global connection density during the transition from late childhood and early adolescence, followed by either a plateau or decrease by mid-adolescence. We also investigated regional variation in topological properties, and characterized these changes in the context of neurocognitive networks. We hypothesized that association cortices, and particularly frontoparietal networks, may exhibit the greatest changes in covariance patterns during this period. Next, we examined associations between cortical thinning and covariance, speculating that regions that exhibit the greatest thinning during early adolescence may also have the greatest covariance with the rest of the brain. Finally, we explored sex differences in global properties and neurocognitive networks.
Methods
Participants
Participants were from the community in Melbourne, Australia, and were recruited into one of two longitudinal projects: i) Neuroimaging of the Children’s Attention Project (NICAP), and ii) imaging brain development in the Childhood to Adolescence Transition Study (iCATS). NICAP participants were recruited as typically developing controls into a study of a community-based cohort of children with and without ADHD. Further details on the NICAP and iCATS samples is presented in Silk et al.33 and Simmons et al.34, respectively. Exclusion criteria for these analyses included MRI contraindications, developmental disability, history of a neurological or serious medical disorder (e.g., diabetes, kidney disease), and psychotropic medications. For both cohorts, written informed consent was obtained from the parent/guardian of all participants. Ethics approval was granted by the Royal Children’s Hospital Human Research Ethics Committee, Melbourne (NICAP #34,071; iCATS #32,171). The iCATS protocol was additionally ratified by the University of Melbourne Human Research Ethics Office (#1238745), and the NICAP protocol was ratified by the Deakin University Human Research Ethics Office (#2016–394). Methods were performed in accordance with these approved protocols.
The NICAP sample underwent up to 3 repeated assessments between the ages of 9.5 and 14.5 years, with approximately 18-month intervals (M = 1.432, SD = 0.222, 1.021–2.330 years) between assessments. The iCATS sample underwent 2 repeated assessments between the ages of 8.5 and 13.5 years, with approximately 36-month intervals (M = 2.763, SD = 0.243, 2.158–3.344 years) between assessments. The two samples did not differ in sex (χ2 = 1.342, df = 1, p = 0.247), pubertal stage (based on parent-report Sexual Maturity Scale35; Mean: iCATS = 1.282, NICAP = 1.316, t(161) = − 0.431, p = 0.666), or intelligence (based on Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence—Matrix Reasoning T-score; Mean: iCATS = 54.096, NICAP = 52.607, t(179) = 1.225, p = 0.222). However, the iCATS sample was significantly younger than the NICAP sample at baseline (Mean: iCATS = 9.556, NICAP = 10.425; t(157) = − 14.928, p < 0.001) and had higher socioeconomic status (based on Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas—Index of Relative Socio-economic Advantage and Disadvantage, based on Australian census data; Mean: iCATS = 1056.175, NICAP = 1018.326; t(198) = 4.887, p < 0.001).
Following exclusions during quality-control (see below for further detail), the final sample comprised 366 scans from 192 participants (96 females, 90 NICAP) aged 8.5–14.5 years, which was used to create cross-sectional windows (see “Statistical analyses” for further detail). Specifically, 59 participants (28 males) provided a single observation, 92 (46 males) provided two observations, and 41 (22 males) provided three observations. For further breakdown of these numbers by cohort, and distributions of age and sex at each wave, refer to Vijayakumar et al.36).
MRI acquisition and processing
Neuroimaging data for both projects were acquired on a 3 T Siemens scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) at the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute in Melbourne, Australia. Participants completed a mock-scan prior to their actual scan at wave 1 (and was repeated at subsequent waves if the participant wished or researcher deemed it appropriate). They were also given information on MRI (including a video) prior to participating in order to familiarize them with the procedure and minimize anxiety as much as possible. Both waves of iCATS, and waves 1 and 2 of NICAP, were collected on a TIM Trio scanner. The final wave of NICAP was collected after an upgrade to a MAGNETOM Prisma scanner, which has been accounted for in statistical modelling. Refer to the Vijayakumar et al.36 for further detail of an investigation into potential cortical differences related to scanner upgrade, which found that only one region (the right insula) exhibited significant differences when comparing pre- and post-upgrade estimates in a sample of age- and sex-matched participants (N = 22).
Participants lay supine in a 32-channel head coil during the MRI scan. Structural T1-weighted images were acquired as follows: iCATS. MPRAGE with repetition time = 1900 ms, echo time = 2.24 ms, flip angle = 9°, field of view = 230 mm2, resulting in 176 contiguous slices with voxel dimensions 0.9 mm3. NICAP. MEMPRAGE with repetition time = 2530 ms, echo time = 1.77, 3.51, 5.32, 7.2 ms, flip angle = 7°, field of view = 230 mm2, resulting in 176 contiguous slices with voxel dimensions 0.9 mm3.
T1-weighted images were processed through FreeSurfer 6.0, a freely available image analysis suite for cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/). Specifically, images were processed with the submillimeter reconstruction37 and the longitudinal stream that creates an unbiased within-subject template space from all available data using robust, inverse consistent registration. The template is used as an estimate to initialize segmentation processes for each time point, providing common information regarding anatomical structures, and has been found to significantly increase reliability and statistical power38,39. The quality of i) raw images and ii) (longitudinal) cortical reconstructions was visually inspected and rated for all scans. Raw images were rated on a 4-point scale for “ringing” (1: no ringing; 2: slight ringing restricted to a small cortical area; 3: more ringing extending into white matter and/or covering more brain regions; 4: extensive ringing) and “blurriness” (1: sharply defined images; 2: slight blurriness; 3: or considerable blurriness; 4: blurring throughout). Ratings of “3” and “4” on either scale were excluded. Processed images were rated on a 3-point scale on the accuracy of the white and pial surfaces (1: near perfect reconstruction; 2: minor reconstruction issues limited to small areas of the brain; 3: poor reconstruction with consistent under-estimation of white matter or extensive areas of CSF included as grey matter). Ratings of “3” were excluded. Images were also processed through MRIQC (v0.14.2) to supplement the visual inspection40. This resulted in the exclusion of a total of 37 scans from 34 participants (i.e., 3 participants had 2 scans removed). No manual edits were made to the remaining (included) data (further details of the quality control procedure have been reported previously36) . Mean cortical thickness estimates from 360 regions of the Human Connectome Pipeline’s multimodal parcellation atlas (HCP-MMP1)41 were extracted42 and used in subsequent analyses. Supplemental analyses examined mean cortical thickness of 62 regions of the Desikan Killiany Tourville (DKT) atlas43.
Statistical analyses
Structural covariance networks (SCN)
The sliding window approach involves creating a series of overlapping “bins” of participants while incrementally sliding across the age range of this sample. Bins were defined by i) an equal sample size, and ii) an incremental step size. As this sliding window approach requires an arbitrary definition of sample and step sizes, we ran analyses across a series of these parameters. Bin sizes of n = 70, 80 and 90, in conjunction with overlap of 70%, 75%, and 80% of the sample (i.e., incremental age-based “steps” of 20%, 25%, and 30%), were examined. Every combination of bin and step sizes were used, thus producing 9 configurations. A visual representation of the data for one sliding window configuration is presented in Fig. 1.
For each of these 9 sliding window configurations, structural covariance networks were created for each age-bin, and the median age of each age-bin was assigned to the respective matrix. Each participant contributed a single scan to each age-defined network. In instances where more than one scan from a participant fell into a given age bin, the scan closest to the median age was selected. The 3 bin sizes and 3 step parameters were chosen based on the trade-off between bin size and repeated assessments per participant within each bin. In other words, larger bin sizes had wider age ranges, and thus participants were more likely to contribute two scans to each age-bin.
Given sample differences in demographics and scanner protocols described above, prior to creating structural covariance networks for each bin, we performed linear regression, modelling cortical thickness of each region as a function of sex, cohort (iCATS, NICAP) and scanner (pre-upgrade, post-upgrade) across the full sample. The standardized residuals of this model were calculated using the “rstandard” function in R, and used to create the structural covariance networks. That is, for the subsample of participants in each bin, we correlated cortical thickness across 360 regions defined by the HCP-MMP1 parcellation, and as supplemental analyses for the 62 regions defined by the DKT parcellation.
There are concerns of noise when estimating structural covariance networks from small sample sizes, although knowledge of appropriate sample size remains limited32. Prior research has used a probabilistic bootstrap thresholding procedure to address this issue8, whereby connections are retained on the basis of a criteria of connection “likelihood” rather than connection weights or fixed edge density. The bootstrap thresholding approach identifies the most statistically robust connections in a network, which are least likely to represent false positives. Within each age-bin, 1000 sets of participants were sampled with replacement, to re-estimate the structural network. We retained edges that were consistently positive across bootstraps (at false discovery rate [FDR] corrected α = 0.05) and set the remaining edges to zero (note, negative edges were identified in an average of 0.04% (range: 0.06–0.1) of the edges across all window configurations). Thresholded networks were binarized prior to graph analyses.
Graph metrics
Graph metrics of each thresholded and binarized SCN were examined using global, nodal and modular properties. We focused on basic metrics as bootstrap thresholding produces variable density for each network, which can confound differences between networks when comparing higher-order graph metrics44. Thus, we examined global density (i.e., percentage of total connections) and node degree (i.e., number of connections per region). Hubs were defined as nodes with standardized degree greater than 1 (i.e., > 1SD from the mean of a given SCN)45,46. SCNs were also decomposed into a functional modular structure using the Yeo 7 parcellation scheme47, with each cortical region of the HCP-MMP1 atlas assigned to one Yeo module based on maximum overlap of vertices48. For this structure, we calculated i) intra-modular density, defined as the density of connections within each module (i.e., number of connections between nodes within a module, relative to all possible connections) and ii) inter-modular density, defined as the density of connections between each pair of modules (i.e., number of connections between nodes in each pair of modules, relative to all possible connections)8. Given changes in global network properties over age windows, these modular metrics were normalized (i.e., divided by) global density of the respective SCN.
Development of structural covariance networks
Changes in the topological properties of SCNs between 8 and 15 years of age were examined with generalized additive models (GAM), using the “mgcv” package49 in R50. Specifically, a given property of each bin was assigned to the median age of participants within the bin. We then examined changes in the property with respect to the (median) age. In a “smooth” model, (e.g.) density was predicted by a smooth age term with a basis function of 3 (i.e., the maximum possible degrees of freedom allowed for the smooth term) given the constrained time frame (e.g., density ~ s(age, k = 3)). This model was compared to a “linear” model (e.g., density ~ age) and a “null” model (e.g., density ~ 1). All models were examined with maximum likelihood (ML) estimation, and model comparisons were used to identify best-fitting developmental trajectories. Model comparisons were based on AIC values, with more complex models selected if AIC was at least 3 less than all lower-order models (i.e., a “smooth” model was only selected if AIC was lower than both “linear” and “null” models).
This model fitting procedure was first used to examine changes in the mean correlation of unthresholded networks and global density of thresholded networks. Second, we examined changes in the connectivity of “hubs” by modelling age-related changes in the (standardized) degree of hub regions. We limited these analyses to regions that were classified as hubs in at least two age-bins, in order to minimize potential noise from regions classified as hubs in one age-bin alone. Third, we examined changes in (normalized) intramodular and intermodular density of functional networks using the same model fitting procedure. We additionally checked the significance of smooth age terms when correcting for multiple comparisons using Benjamini & Hochberg FDR correction51 in nodal and modular analyses.
GAM models were run for 9 combinations of bin and step size, and only findings that were identified in more than 50% of these sliding window configurations are presented. Model coefficients and illustrations of significant developmental patterns are presented for the bin size of 80 and 25% step, as it represents the median of all 9 configurations that were examined. Whole brain maps in Figs. 2C and 3 (as well as Supplementary Figures S1C and S4) were created using PySurfer v0.10.0 (https://pysurfer.github.io/).
Associations with cortical thinning
Rates of thinning were calculated for each region using linear mixed models using the “lmer” package in R. We modelled linear trajectories based on prior work examining cortical development in this dataset36: Y = Intercept + di + β1 (sex) + β2 (cohort) + β3 (scanner) + β4 (age) + ei. Models were conducted within each ith subject, with a random intercept (di) to account for the repeated observations per subject. The ei represents the normally distributed residual error term. The β coefficients for age were extracted as effect sizes of regional thinning. In order to understand whether rates of cortical thinning were associated with covariance properties in mid-adolescence, we correlated the β coefficients with standardized node degree in the oldest age-bins. Significant associations were followed up by correlating β coefficients to node degree of the youngest age-bins, to determine whether potential associations were unique to mid-adolescence (and thus reflective of changes with age). To calculate significance of these correlations, BrainSMASH was used to simulate 1000 surrogate brain maps that preserve the spatial autocorrelation of the original cortical map (based on the geodesic distance matrix of parcels in each hemisphere)52. Correlations between thinning and covariance in each hemisphere were then calculated for each surrogate map, and non-parametric p values were calculated as the proportion of surrogate maps that generated correlations equal to or greater than the empirical value.
Exploratory sex differences
Using the same sliding window configurations as above, SCNs were recreated for males and females within each age-window. To do so, linear regression was first run within each sex to remove the influence of variables of non-interest (cohort (iCATS, NICAP) and scanner (pre-upgrade, post-upgrade)) from cortical thickness. Next, SCNs for males and females were created for each age-window. They were bootstrap-thresholded, binarized, and graph metrics were calculated. Generalized additive models examined sex differences in age-related trajectories for mean correlations and global density of SCNs. Finally, we examined changes in (normalized) intramodular and intermodular density of functional networks within each sex, and GAMs examined “null”, “linear” and “smooth” change. Sex differences in modular density were not statistically examined as normalized metrics are dependent on global density, which differed between males and females.
Results
Global development
Generalized additive modelling of non-thresholded SCNs revealed nonlinear change in edge strength (i.e., mean of all correlations) between the youngest and oldest age windows, characterized by a “peak” around 11.5 years of age. This pattern of nonlinear change in mean correlation was consistent across 8 out of 9 sliding window configurations. When focusing on the most statistically robust connections (i.e., bootstrap-thresholded and binarized SCNs), GAMs revealed a similar pattern of nonlinear change with maximum global edge density at 11.5 years of age (Fig. 2A). This pattern of nonlinear change was consistent across all 9 configurations. Thresholded SCNs also exhibited increased variability in degree (i.e., degree distribution) at the middle age windows (~ 11.5 years) relative to the youngest and oldest windows (Fig. 2B). Refer to Supplementary Table S1 for model fit and coefficients. Nonlinear changes in global metrics over the age windows was replicated for the DKT parcellation, although degree distribution was negatively skewed (i.e., most of the 62 regions were correlated with one another; see Supplementary Fig. S1).
Nodal development
At a regional level, a similar pattern of nonlinear change was identified for nodal degree (Fig. 2C); across the cortex, regions exhibited a pattern of increasing numbers of connections between the youngest and middle age windows (i.e., roughly 9.5 to 11.5 years of age), followed by reductions through to the oldest age-window (~ 13.5 years). Hubs, defined as regions with high degree (> 1 SD) in at least two age windows, were identified across sensorimotor and association cortices during early adolescence (Fig. 3). While GAMs revealed that a number of these hubs did not exhibit change in standardized degree across the age-bins, significant decreases were identified in motor regions and increases were identified in multiple frontal and parietal regions (see Supplementary Table S2 for model comparisons and coefficients). Additionally, correlational analyses conducted across the entire parcellation revealed that rates of cortical thinning were associated with standardized degree in the oldest age-bin, such that regions exhibiting greater thinning had greater degree (Left hemisphere: r = − 0.270, p = 0.011, 9/9 window configurations; Right hemisphere: r = − 0.241, p = 0.044, 6/9 configurations). However, there were no such associations in the youngest age-bin (Left: r = − 0.160, p = 0.182, 9/9 configurations; Right: r = − 0.185, p = 0.171, 9/9 configurations).
Modular development
In the context of functional communities, “peaks” were identified for mean density across all modules, consistent with patterns of global density. To further understand community-level changes, we examined “normalized” metrics of modular density that accounted for global density. Normalized intramodular density (i.e., connections of regions within a network) of the dorsal attention and frontoparietal networks exhibited nonlinear increases over age-windows (Fig. 4A,B), while that of the visual and somatomotor networks exhibited linear decreases over age. Normalized intermodular density (i.e., connections of regions between networks) exhibited predominantly linear increases between the ventral attention, dorsal attention, frontoparietal, and default mode networks (see Fig. 4A,C). Comparatively, changes involving the visual network were largely characterized by “peaks”, while those involving the limbic network were characterized by “troughs”. Model comparisons and coefficients for modular density are presented in Supplementary Table S3. Post-hoc illustration of differences between hemispheres are presented in Supplementary Fig. S5.
Sex differences
Exploratory analyses failed to identify any consistent sex differences in age-related changes of non-thresholded mean correlation (“smooth” AIC: − 143, “smooth * sex” AIC: − 145) and thresholded global density (“smooth” AIC: − 136, “smooth * sex” AIC: − 137). Next, changes in the density of connections between functional communities were examined within each sex, normalized by their respective global densities. In females, increases in intra- and inter-modular density were limited to the dorsal and ventral attention networks, particularly in connection with the frontoparietal and default mode networks. In comparison, a number of nonlinear reductions were identified for the visual and somatomotor networks (Fig. 5A,C). In males, increases in modular density were present for the dorsal attention and frontoparietal networks, while decreases were limited to intramodular connections of ventral attention and limbic networks (Fig. 5B,D). See Supplementary Table S4 for results.
Discussion
The current investigation revealed age-related changes in structural covariance networks during the transition from childhood to adolescence. As hypothesized, there was a “peak” in global covariance between 9.5 and 14.5 years of age based on correlations in regional thickness across subjects. There was also regional variability beyond this global pattern, with association cortices exhibiting greater increases in covariance. Relatedly, regions within higher-order neurocognitive systems exhibited greater within-network and between-network covariance with age, compared to sensorimotor networks. Exploratory analyses also indicated that these patterns were more prominent in females relative to males. Finally, as hypothesized, regions exhibiting the greatest thinning during this period had the greatest covariance with the rest of the brain.
Findings indicate that the transition from childhood to adolescence is characterized by global increases in structural covariance, followed by reductions into mid-adolescence. This pattern was identified for the strength of non-thresholded connections (i.e., mean correlations), as well as the density of thresholded connections. Similar nonlinear trajectories have been identified during this period for mean correlations19,21,53 and mean local efficiency (a measure of communication between the nodes)19. These global “peaks” may be reflective of a transient period of convergence in anatomical properties across the cortex during early adolescence, followed by a divergence that reflects greater inter-individual variability in the rates or timing of regional maturation over the course of the second decade. Somewhat consistent with this speculation, others have shown that global covariance continues to decrease through late adolescence before plateauing in the early 20 s, which corresponds to the protracted maturation of association cortices8. Future research is needed to understand whether such a divergence may be related to the onset of socioemotional problems during adolescence, as purported by mismatch models of neurodevelopment24,54.
Our analyses also highlighted regional differences in covariance properties of sensorimotor and association cortices during early adolescence. Examination of (standardized) degree revealed the prominence of sensorimotor regions as highly connected “hubs” by late childhood. The connectivity of visual hubs remained consistent across early adolescence, while motor hubs exhibited significant reductions with age. Although there were some hub regions present in associations cortices by late childhood, the extent and strength of hubs within frontal and parietal cortices increased with age. These results are consistent with prior literature that has highlighted the prominence of hubs within association cortices during adolescence19, although this has been limited to parietal regions in some studies20. Findings are also supported by regional variability in group-level trajectories, with frontal, parietal and temporal cortices exhibiting greater changes than occipital and motor cortices in adolescents55,56 and exhibiting later maturation in macaques57. Moreover, increasing covariance was mostly limited to regions that support abstract higher-order cognitive skills that continue to mature during adolescence58. Interestingly, others have shown that mean correlation strength is related to (group-level) working memory during adolescence21. However, our findings suggest potential value in examining the contribution of specific regions’ covariance properties in relation to cognitive maturation during early adolescence.
When accounting for the global “peaks” in density, there were also age-related (linear) increases in intramodular (within network) correlations in frontoparietal and ventral attention networks, suggesting that regions within these networks become increasingly connected with one another relative to average connectivity of the cortex. There were also age-related linear increases in intermodular (between network) connections between the ventral attention, dorsal attention, frontoparietal and default mode networks. Comparatively, there was a decoupling of connections with visual and somatomotor networks with age. Taken together, higher-order cognitive networks appear to undergo coordinated structural development that supports their specialization and segregation across adolescence, within the context of a generalized divergence across the cortex. These findings are consistent with seed-based analyses of covariance that have found primary sensory and motor networks to be well-developed in early childhood, and later maturation of salience and executive control networks during adolescence17. Others have found that higher-order networks continue to mature into young adulthood, with the frontoparietal network exhibiting the greatest reduction in covariance during the late teens and early 20 s, whilst the default mode and ventral attention networks are the last to reach adult levels of maturity8. A similar developmental pattern is also postulated for functional connectivity, with visual and sensorimotor areas developing earlier59 and exhibiting less variability in their trajectories60 than other networks. Findings are also consistent with continued maturation of between-network connectivity of task-positive (frontoparietal, attention) and task-negative networks (default mode) during adolescence61–64. Developmental patterns in both nodal and modular properties are therefore consistent with earlier mastery of basic sensory and motor skills, but continued refinement of emotion regulation, social cognition and other complex executive functions that are supported by frontoparietal, default mode, and attention networks.
The exact mechanisms that underlie structural covariance networks remain uncertain, but it is frequently postulated to arise from coordinated functioning of distributed brain regions. In support, there is some overlap between structural covariance and functional networks65,66. Work in neonates has also found that SCNs develop later than functional networks, suggesting that coactivation of functional networks may guide the development of SCNs16. There is also partial convergence with structural connectivity networks67, suggesting a role of mutually trophic effects mediated by underlying axonal connections10. Others have highlighted similarities with maturational covariance networks (i.e., correlations of longitudinal regional trajectories)65, as well as coordinated gene expression during brain development68,69. Moreover, higher covariance between regions at shorter distances may also relate to shared gene expression from common embryonic origins69. Importantly these mechanisms are not mutually exclusive, as trophic, genetic and neurodevelopmental processes are most likely interconnected influences on SCNs9,11.
Less is known of the mechanisms that may contribute to developmental changes in structural covariance. However, we found that regions exhibiting greater cortical thinning had greater degree by mid-adolescence. As rates of cortical thinning were not associated with node degree during late childhood, findings imply that thinning specifically contributes to changes in covariance networks between late childhood and mid-adolescence. During later adolescence, greater cortical thinning has conversely been show to relate to more reductions in nodal degree8. As association cortices exhibited the greatest cortical thinning across both datasets, it appears that underlying neural mechanisms, such as synaptic pruning and myelination, may have nonlinear effects on the network properties of these regions across the extended period of adolescence. Váša and colleagues8 also show that intracortical myelination has stronger associations with regional changes in covariance than cortical thinning, highlighting the need for multimodal analyses to unpack the evolving relationship between structural connectivity and structural covariance networks during development.
Exploratory investigations of sexual dimorphism failed to identify consistent differences in age-related changes of global covariance (i.e., mean correlation and global density), although nonlinear age-related differences in a number of sliding window configurations suggest that females may have steeper “peaks” than males in global density during early adolescence. Given these inconsistencies, continued research with larger sample sizes per group, within this targeted age range, is thus needed to further investigate sex differences. Our finding of overall greater mean correlation in females is also inconsistent with prior research that has found greater correlations of subcortical volumes in males70, suggesting that sex differences in anatomical networks may differ by regions-of-interest and/or morphologic properties during development. When accounting for potential global differences, females exhibited a general pattern of increased covariance of (dorsal and ventral) attention networks with one another and the frontoparietal and default mode networks, while males had fewer such increases that were concentrated in the dorsal attention and frontoparietal network. Moreover, females exhibited a number of decreases in the covariance of primary sensorimotor networks. Prior seed-based analysis has shown sex-specific associations between testosterone levels and prefrontal-hippocampal covariance during adolescence71. Moreover, covariance has been found to differ between peri- and post-menopausal women72, and is also related to estradiol levels in adults73. Together with the lack of sex differences in the structural covariance of neonates and later adolescents/young adults8,16, it appears that sex differences in covariance may be specific to periods of rapidly changing hormone levels. Our pattern of findings may also be suggestive of earlier maturation in females, as they had more extensive segregation of later maturing higher-order cognitive networks, and also exhibit “decoupling” of earlier maturing sensorimotor networks. However, it is important to note that differences in modular density are qualitative, and further investigations incorporating hormones and a more extended period of adolescence is needed to test our hypotheses.
Limitations
Our findings need to be considered in light of certain strengths and limitations. The current investigation of age-related changes infers developmental processes from group-level networks. As discussed above, changes in cross-sectional correlations are suggestive of individual differences in developmental trajectories and longitudinal research is needed to fully understand these underlying neurodevelopmental processes. Our analyses are also dependent on the configuration of age-windows based on window and step/overlap sizes. However, we conducted statistical analyses on a range of window and step sizes so as to identify results that were consistent across the majority of configurations. Windows were also comprised of large participant numbers and narrow age ranges relative to prior literature, but further increases in sample size will continue to decrease noise within age-defined SCNs, and also create additional age-bins/estimates to model age-related changes. Another limitation of the sliding window approach is that each window’s topological properties that are modelled within GAMs are not independent of one another. The MRI scanner was also upgraded between waves in one of the cohorts, but we additionally modelled this upgrade as a covariate in our analyses. Moreover, comparison on age-matched participants pre- and post- scanner upgrade failed to identify significant differences36. We also note that stringent quality control procedures were undertaken to minimize the influence of head motion, but future studies with estimates of head motion for T1-weighted images are needed to confirm findings while statistically controlling for motion confounds. Finally, our sex differences are considered exploratory given the smaller sample size when differentiating males and females, although we note that similar sample sizes have been used frequently in the literature. Nevertheless, given concerns of the reliability of estimates for small samples32, future work with larger samples of this age range are needed to corroborate our findings. The incorporation of pubertal measures may also provide novel insight into the biological processes that may underlie the identified changes in structural covariance networks during this period.
Conclusions
The transition from childhood to adolescence is characterized by nonlinear trajectories with “peaks” in global covariance of cortical thickness. While occipital and motor regions are more highly connected hubs during late childhood, the prominence of frontal and parietal regions as hubs increases with age. Beyond global patterns, attention, frontoparietal and default mode networks exhibit increasing covariance, suggesting greater specialization and segregation of regions that support higher-order cognitive processes during early adolescence.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Prof George Patton (Murdoch Children’s Research Institute) for comments on the manuscript, and A/Prof Richard Beare (Monash University) for methodological discussions. We also thank all the families that have participated in both NICAP and iCATS. NICAP was funded by the National Medical Health and Research Council of Australia (NHMRC; project grant #1065895). iCATS was funded by a Discovery Project grant from the Australian Research Council (ARC; DP120101402). SW was supported by a NHMRC Career Development Fellowship (#1125504). Cohorts were supported by the Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, The Royal Children’s Hospital, The Royal Children’s Hospital Foundation, Department of Paediatrics at The University of Melbourne and the Victorian Government's Operational Infrastructure Support Program. We also thank The Royal Children’s Hospital’s Medical Imaging staff for their assistance and expertise in the collection of the MRI data included in this study.
Author contributions
N.V.: Designed research, Performed research, Analyzed data, Wrote the paper. S.W., T.S.: Designed research, Wrote the paper. G.B., M.L.S., LM.: Wrote the paper.
Data availability
The datasets analysed in the current study are not publicly available as we do not have consent from participants in the Imaging of the Children’s Attention Project (iCATS) cohort to share individual data. However, it can be made available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Custom analysis scripts can be found at https://github.com/nandivij/struct_cov_dev.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
These authors jointly supervised this work: Sarah Whittle and Tim Silk.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-88918-w.
References
- 1.Mills KL, et al. Structural brain development between childhood and adulthood: Convergence across four longitudinal samples. Neuroimage. 2016;141:273–281. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.07.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hogstrom LJ, Westlye LT, Walhovd KB, Fjell AM. The structure of the cerebral cortex across adult life: age-related patterns of surface area, thickness, and gyrification. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:2521–2530. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs231. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Natu VS, et al. Apparent thinning of human visual cortex during childhood is associated with myelination. PNAS. 2019;116:20750–20759. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1904931116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Petanjek Z, Judas M, Kostović I, Uylings HBM. Lifespan alterations of basal dendritic trees of pyramidal neurons in the human prefrontal cortex: a layer-specific pattern. Cereb. Cortex. 2008;18:915–929. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tamnes CK, et al. Development of the cerebral cortex across adolescence: a multisample study of inter-related longitudinal changes in cortical volume, surface area, and thickness. J. Neurosci. 2017;37:3402–3412. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3302-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Vijayakumar N, et al. Brain development during adolescence: a mixed-longitudinal investigation of cortical thickness, surface area, and volume. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37:2027–2038. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Wierenga LM, Langen M, Oranje B, Durston S. Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuroimage. 2014;87:120–126. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Váša F, et al. Adolescent tuning of association cortex in human structural brain networks. Cereb. Cortex. 2018;28:281–294. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Alexander-Bloch A, Giedd JN, Bullmore E. Imaging structural co-variance between human brain regions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013;14:322–336. doi: 10.1038/nrn3465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mechelli A, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS, Price CJ. Structural covariance in the human cortex. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:8303–8310. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0357-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Vértes PE, Bullmore ET. Annual research review: growth connectomics–the organization and reorganization of brain networks during normal and abnormal development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2015;56:299–320. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Hebb DO. The Organization of Behavior; A Neuropsychological Theory. Wiley; 1949. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Blakemore S-J, Burnett S, Dahl RE. The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2010;31:926–933. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Dumontheil I. Development of the social brain during adolescence. Psicol. Educ. 2015;21:117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.pse.2015.08.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Konrad K, Firk C, Uhlhaas PJ. Brain development during adolescence. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 2013;110:425–431. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2013.0425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Geng X, et al. Structural and maturational covariance in early childhood brain development. Cereb. Cortex. 2016 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Zielinski BA, Gennatas ED, Zhou J, Seeley WW. Network-level structural covariance in the developing brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2010;107:18191–18196. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003109107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Montembeault M, et al. The impact of aging on gray matter structural covariance networks. Neuroimage. 2012;63:754–759. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Khundrakpam BS, et al. Developmental changes in organization of structural brain networks. Cereb. Cortex. 2013;23:2072–2085. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Nie J, Li G, Shen D. Development of cortical anatomical properties from early childhood to early adulthood. Neuroimage. 2013;76:216–224. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sandini C, et al. Development of structural covariance from childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal study in 22q11.2DS. Front. Neurosci. 2018;12:327. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.DuPre E, Spreng RN. Structural covariance networks across the life span, from 6 to 94 years of age. Netw. Neurosci. 2017;1:302–323. doi: 10.1162/NETN_a_00016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nelson EE, Jarcho JM, Guyer AE. Social re-orientation and brain development: an expanded and updated view. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 2016;17:118–127. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shulman EP, et al. The dual systems model: review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 2016;17:103–117. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jones SA, Morales AM, Lavine JB, Nagel BJ. Convergent neurobiological predictors of emergent psychopathology during adolescence. Birth Defects Res. 2017;109:1613–1622. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Kaczkurkin AN, Raznahan A, Satterthwaite TD. Sex differences in the developing brain: insights from multimodal neuroimaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:71–85. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0111-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sisk CL. Development: pubertal hormones meet the adolescent brain. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:R706–R708. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lenroot RK, et al. Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage. 2007;36:1065–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sowell ER, et al. Sex differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 and 87 years of age. Cereb. Cortex. 2007;17:1550–1560. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Genc S, Malpas CB, Ball G, Silk TJ, Seal ML. Age, sex, and puberty related development of the corpus callosum: a multi-technique diffusion MRI study. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223:2753–2765. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1658-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nebli A, Rekik I. Gender differences in cortical morphological networks. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019 doi: 10.1007/s11682-019-00123-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Carmon J, et al. Reliability and comparability of human brain structural covariance networks. Neuroimage. 2020;220:117104. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Silk TJ, et al. Developmental brain trajectories in children with ADHD and controls: a longitudinal neuroimaging study. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16:59. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0770-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Simmons JG, et al. Study protocol: imaging brain development in the childhood to adolescence transition study (iCATS) BMC Pediatr. 2014;14:1–10. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-14-115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Morris NM, Udry JR. Validation of a self-administered instrument to assess stage of adolescent development. J. Youth Adolesc. 1980;9:271–280. doi: 10.1007/BF02088471. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Vijayakumar N, et al. A longitudinal analysis of puberty-related cortical development. Neuroimage. 2021;228:117684. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Zaretskaya N, Fischl B, Reuter M, Renvall V, Polimeni JR. Advantages of cortical surface reconstruction using submillimeter 7 Tesla MEMPRAGE. Neuroimage. 2018;165:11–26. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.09.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Reuter M, Schmansky NJ, Rosas HD, Fischl B. Within-subject template estimation for unbiased longitudinal image analysis. Neuroimage. 2012;61:1402–1418. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Reuter M, Fischl B. Avoiding asymmetry-induced bias in longitudinal image processing. Neuroimage. 2011;57:19–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Esteban O, et al. MRIQC: advancing the automatic prediction of image quality in MRI from unseen sites. PLoS ONE. 2017;12:e0184661. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Glasser MF, et al. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature. 2016;536:171–178. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mills, K. L. HCP-MMP1.0 projected on fsaverage (2016) 10.6084/m9.figshare.3498446.v2
- 43.Klein A, Tourville J. 101 labeled brain images and a consistent human cortical labeling protocol. Front. Neurosci. 2012;6:171. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00171. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.van Wijk BCM, Stam CJ, Daffertshofer A. Comparing brain networks of different size and connectivity density using graph theory. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e13701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Fornito A, Zalesky A, Bullmore E. Fundamentals of Brain Network Analysis. Academic Press; 2016. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Oldham S, Fornito A. The development of brain network hubs. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 2019;36:100607. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Yeo BT, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2011;106:1125–1165. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Baum GL, et al. Modular segregation of structural brain networks supports the development of executive function in youth. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:1561–1572.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.051. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Wood, S. Generalized Additive Models: An Introduction With R. vol. 66 (2006).
- 50.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria (2013).
- 51.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. B (Methodol.) 1995;57:289–300. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Burt JB, Helmer M, Shinn M, Anticevic A, Murray JD. Generative modeling of brain maps with spatial autocorrelation. bioRxiv. 2020 doi: 10.1101/2020.02.18.955054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Moura LM, et al. Coordinated brain development: exploring the synchrony between changes in grey and white matter during childhood maturation. Brain Imaging Behav. 2017;11:808–817. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9555-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Casey BJ, Galván A, Somerville LH. Beyond simple models of adolescence to an integrated circuit-based account: a commentary. Dev. Cognit. Neurosci. 2016;17:128–130. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ball G, Beare R, Seal ML. Charting shared developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and structural connectivity in childhood and adolescence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019;40:4630–4644. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Krongold M, Cooper C, Bray S. Modular development of cortical gray matter across childhood and adolescence. Cereb. Cortex. 2017;27:1125–1136. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Scott JA, et al. Longitudinal analysis of the developing rhesus monkey brain using magnetic resonance imaging: birth to adulthood. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016;221:2847–2871. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1076-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Larsen B, Luna B. Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2018;94:179–195. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Grayson DS, Fair DA. Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: a guide to neuroimaging literature. Neuroimage. 2017;160:15–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Váša F, et al. Conservative and disruptive modes of adolescent change in human brain functional connectivity. PNAS. 2020;117:3248–3253. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1906144117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Anderson JS, Ferguson MA, Lopez-Larson M, Yurgelun-Todd D. Connectivity gradients between the default mode and attention control networks. Brain Connect. 2011;1:147–157. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Barber AD, Caffo BS, Pekar JJ, Mostofsky SH. Developmental changes in within- and between-network connectivity between late childhood and adulthood. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:156–167. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Chai XJ, Ofen N, Gabrieli JDE, Whitfield-Gabrieli S. Selective development of anticorrelated networks in the intrinsic functional organization of the human brain. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 2014;26:501–513. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Uddin LQ, Supekar KS, Ryali S, Menon V. Dynamic reconfiguration of structural and functional connectivity across core neurocognitive brain networks with development. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:18578–18589. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4465-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Alexander-Bloch A, Raznahan A, Bullmore E, Giedd J. The convergence of maturational change and structural covariance in human cortical networks. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:2889–2899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3554-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Paquola C, Bennett MR, Lagopoulos J. Structural and functional connectivity underlying gray matter covariance: impact of developmental insult. Brain Connect. 2018;8:299–310. doi: 10.1089/brain.2018.0584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Gong G, He Y, Chen ZJ, Evans AC. Convergence and divergence of thickness correlations with diffusion connections across the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage. 2012;59:1239–1248. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Romero-Garcia R, et al. Structural covariance networks are coupled to expression of genes enriched in supragranular layers of the human cortex. Neuroimage. 2018;171:256–267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Yee Y, et al. Structural covariance of brain region volumes is associated with both structural connectivity and transcriptomic similarity. Neuroimage. 2018;179:357–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wierenga LM, Sexton JA, Laake P, Giedd JN, Tamnes CK. A key characteristic of sex differences in the developing brain: greater variability in brain structure of boys than girls. Cereb. Cortex. 2018;28:2741–2751. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Nguyen T-V, et al. Sex-specific associations of testosterone with prefrontal-hippocampal development and executive function. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2017;76:206–217. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Seitz J, et al. Impact of sex and reproductive status on memory circuitry structure and function in early midlife using structural covariance analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2019;40:1221–1233. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Zsido RG, et al. Association of estradiol and visceral fat with structural brain networks and memory performance in adults. JAMA Netw. Open. 2019;2:e196126–e196126. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.6126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets analysed in the current study are not publicly available as we do not have consent from participants in the Imaging of the Children’s Attention Project (iCATS) cohort to share individual data. However, it can be made available from the corresponding author on reasonable request. Custom analysis scripts can be found at https://github.com/nandivij/struct_cov_dev.