Abstract
Posterior cortical atrophy is an atypical form of Alzheimer’s disease characterized by visuospatial impairments and predominant tissue loss in the posterior parieto-occipital and temporo-occipital cortex. Whilst episodic memory is traditionally thought to be relatively preserved in posterior cortical atrophy, recent work indicates that memory impairments form a common clinical symptom in the early stages of the disease. Neuroimaging studies suggest that memory dysfunction in posterior cortical atrophy may originate from atrophy and functional hypoconnectivity of parietal cortex. The structural connectivity patterns underpinning these memory impairments, however, have not been investigated. This line of inquiry is of particular interest, as changes in white matter tracts of posterior cortical atrophy patients have been shown to be more extensive than expected based on posterior atrophy of grey matter. In this cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging MRI study, we examine the relationship between white matter microstructure and verbal episodic memory in posterior cortical atrophy. We assessed episodic memory performance in a group of posterior cortical atrophy patients (n = 14) and a group of matched healthy control participants (n = 19) using the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test with Immediate Recall. Diffusion tensor imaging measures were obtained for 13 of the posterior cortical atrophy patients and a second control group of 18 healthy adults. Patients and healthy controls demonstrated similar memory encoding performance, indicating that learning of verbal information was preserved in posterior cortical atrophy. However, retrieval of verbal items was significantly impaired in the patient group compared with control participants. As expected, tract-based spatial statistics analyses showed widespread reductions of white matter integrity in posterior cortical regions of patients compared with healthy adults. Correlation analyses indicated that poor verbal retrieval in the patient group was specifically associated with microstructural damage of the splenium of the corpus callosum. Post-hoc tractography analyses in healthy controls demonstrated that this splenial region was connected to thalamic radiations and the retrolenticular part of the internal capsule. These results provide insight into the brain circuits that underlie memory impairments in posterior cortical atrophy. From a cognitive perspective, we propose that the association between splenial integrity and memory dysfunction could arise indirectly via disruption of attentional processes. We discuss implications for the clinical phenotype and development of therapeutic aids for cognitive impairment in posterior cortical atrophy.
Keywords: posterior cortical atrophy, diffusion tensor imaging, white matter, memory, splenium
Using diffusion tensor imaging data, Overman et al. identified a specific relationship between white matter integrity of the splenium and memory retrieval in posterior cortical atrophy. These results fit within a framework in which memory deficits in posterior cortical atrophy are underpinned by disruption of attentional pathways.
Graphical Abstract
Graphical Abstract.
Introduction
Posterior cortical atrophy (PCA) is a neurodegenerative syndrome characterized by prominent impairments of visuospatial abilities, independent from ocular disease. PCA is most commonly associated with Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology, although some patients present with features more closely resembling Lewy body dementia, corticobasal degeneration or prion disease.1,2 Tissue loss and hypometabolism resulting from these processes are predominantly observed in posterior parieto-occipital and temporo-occipital cortex.3–6 In contrast to the amnestic presentation of Alzheimer’s disease, episodic memory is traditionally thought to be relatively preserved in PCA until later stages of the disease.7,8 However, recent work has shown that memory processes are compromised in the majority of PCA patients, and form an oft-reported early clinical symptom.9–11 The emerging profile of memory impairment is one of decreased encoding and retrieval of verbal information.10,12 Recognition and sensitivity to cues in memory retrieval appear to be less affected, with PCA patients achieving scores within the normal range when cues to recall are utilised.13,14
Episodic memory dysfunction is classically associated with the medial temporal lobe circuitry and has long been considered the hallmark feature of typical Alzheimer’s disease,15,16 which is characterized by impaired storage. In contrast, neuroimaging research consistently shows that the medial temporal lobe system is comparatively intact in the early to moderate stages of PCA.6,8,17,18 Instead, research suggests that the neural underpinnings of memory impairment in PCA originate in the posterior parietal neural networks of the brain. A recent voxel-based morphometry study demonstrated that impairments of immediate recall, delayed recall and recognition on a verbal memory task were associated with reduced grey matter density in the left postcentral gyrus in PCA patients.12 Rather than being implicated in memory performance, this region has previously been identified as a node of the frontoparietal cortical network with a role in orienting attention.19 Medial temporal regions were largely spared and did not correlate with memory performance. Resting-state analyses of PCA patients further indicated that reduced verbal recall performance was related to hypoconnectivity between the insula and the dorsal attention network, which encompasses the intraparietal sulcus and superior parietal lobule.14 Although the default mode network, which is typically disrupted in Alzheimer’s disease, also showed hypoconnectivity in PCA patients, this alteration was not associated with impaired performance on the memory task.
Longstanding views of the parietal lobe have traditionally ascribed its roles in support of planning and control of movement, attention to spatial information and construction.20–22 More recent neuroimaging studies, however, suggest that the posterior parietal cortex may also contribute to the neurocircuitry of episodic memory and, in particular, to encoding and retrieval mechanisms (for reviews, see Refs.23–25). Specifically, differential parietal activations have been observed for recognition of previously encountered items versus new items,26–30 recollection- and familiarity-driven recognition,31–33 and orienting retrieval to specific aspects of memory (e.g. source versus item34,35) Lesion studies provide convergent evidence for a salient role of parietal cortex in memory functioning. Significant impairments in spontaneous but not cued recall of autobiographical memories were reported in two patients with bilateral lesions in the inferior and superior parietal lobules as well as the intraparietal sulcus.36 Similarly, five patients with focal lesions in parietal cortex demonstrated significant impairments on verbal recall and recognition tasks on neuropsychological testing.37 In a large-scale study of 48 stroke patients with posterior parietal damage, significant associations were found between angular gyrus damage and cued recall on a verbal paired associates learning task.38 Damage to parietal cortex can result in specific memory retrieval deficits which differ from the dense amnesia observed after hippocampal damage.39
The underlying mechanisms linking these posterior cortical regions to memory remain under debate (for reviews, see Refs.23,40). One candidate hypothesis, entertained by a number of researchers previously,23,41 would propose that the lateral parietal cortex supports attentional processes during memory encoding and retrieval. The Attention to Memory (AtoM) model24,42 builds on prior theories of attention, which suggest that a dorsal frontoparietal system guides bottom-up attention whereas a ventral frontoparietal network is responsible for directing top-down visual attention.22 The AtoM postulates that these attentional mechanisms can be extended to the memory domain. Thus, a dorsal attention network with key nodes in the superior parietal lobe and intraparietal sulcus is presumed to mediate goal-directed attention to memories (i.e. top-down attention), whereas a ventral network encompassing the inferior parietal lobe including the temporoparietal junction and angular gyrus controls reflexive capture of attention to salient memory cues (i.e. bottom-up attention). Similar to this hypothesis, Gilmore et al.43 posit that a reliable parietal memory network which largely depends on bottom-up attentional capture of stimuli can be identified from the fMRI literature, consisting of the precuneus, mid-cingulate gyrus and the posterior inferior lobule/dorsal angular gyrus. Disrupted attentional processes could therefore be a driving force behind the memory deficits observed in PCA, with atrophy and functional hypoconnectivity of parietal cortex in PCA.12,14
In the present study, we aimed to further explore the neurocognitive underpinnings of memory impairment in PCA. Currently, no study has investigated structural connectivity changes in posterior neural networks which may contribute to memory functioning in PCA, yet there are compelling reasons to add this line of inquiry. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies have indicated that PCA is associated with widespread changes in white matter tracts, which are more extensive than expected when based on the posterior atrophy observed in structural imaging.44 It is therefore possible that reduced integrity of these tracts, and consequent disconnection of cognitive networks, may contribute to the memory symptoms observed in PCA, distinct from the effects of grey matter atrophy. Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in PCA have identified white matter damage in the corpus callosum, cingulum, fornix, parietal lobe, and occipital lobe, as well as major tracts including the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), inferior longitudinal fasciculus (ILF), inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and parahippocampal tracts (see Table 1 for summary). Compared to typical Alzheimer’s disease, PCA showed greater involvement of the SLF, posterior cingulate, splenium of the corpus callosum, and posterior ILF and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus.45 An examination of the memory neuroimaging literature reveals that many of the regions which are compromised in PCA have also been associated with memory performance in healthy adults (see Table 2). At present, however, it is unknown whether microstructural changes in these tracts contribute to the memory impairments observed in PCA.
Table 1.
Tracts and white matter regions demonstrating degeneration in patients with Posterior Cortical Atrophy (PCA)
First author | Journal | PCA patient sample size | Analysis method | Outcome measure | Tracts/regions identified |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Agosta44 | NeuroImage: Clinical | 21 | Probabilistic tractography | FA, MD, AxD, and RD |
Cingulum Corpus callosum, body Corpus callosum, splenium ILF, left SLF, bilateral |
Caso46 | Radiology | 13 | ROI-based TBSS | FA |
Cingulum, posterior Corpus callosum IFOF ILF Parahippocampal tracts SLF |
Cerami3 | Journal of Alzheimer's Disease | 6 | Whole-brain TBSS | FA, MD, AxD, and RD |
Cingulum bundle Corpus callosum, genu Corpus callosum, splenium Forceps minor Fornix IFOF ILF SLF Thalamic radiations Uncinate fasciculus |
Duning47 | Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry | 1 | ROI-based TBSS | FA |
Occipital lobe Parietal lobes |
Glick-Shames48 | Brain Imaging and Behavior | 10 | Probabilistic tractography | FA, AxD, and RD |
Optic radiations Splenial fibers |
Madhavan45 | Journal of Alzheimer's Disease | 18 | Whole-brain voxel-based analysis | FA, MD, AxD, and RD |
Cingulum Corpus callosum, body Corpus callosum, splenium Fornix IFOF ILF Internal capsule SLF Thalamic radiation, left posterior Uncinate fasciculus |
Migliaccio49 | Cortex | 1 | Tractography | FA, MD, AxD, and RD |
Corpus callosum IFOF ILF SLF, fronto-parietal |
Migliaccio50 | Alzheimer's & Dementia | 13 | Voxel-based morphometry analysis | WM atrophy |
Cingulum, posterior Corpus callosum, posterior Occipital region, bilateral Parietal region, bilateral Temporal region, bilateral |
Migliaccio51 | Neurobiology of Aging | 7 | Tractography and voxel-based morphometry analysis |
FA, MD, AxD, and RD WM atrophy |
Tractography Corpus callosum IFOF, bilateral ILF, left SLF, right fronto-parietal Voxel-based morphometry Bilateral ventral occipitotemporal region |
Millington52 | NeuroImage: Clinical | 10 | Whole-brain and ROI-based TBSS | FA and MD |
Corpus callosum, genu Corpus callosum, splenium Occipital lobe |
Yoshida53 | European Neurology | 1 | ROI-based TBSS | FA | Splenium of the corpus callosum |
ADC, apparent diffusivity coefficient; AxD, axial diffusivity; FA, fractional anisotropy; IFOF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; MD, mean diffusivity; RD, radial diffusivity; ROI, region-of-interest; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; TBSS, tract-based spatial statistics.
Table 2.
Overview of neuroimaging studies of healthy adults demonstrating relationships between memory performance and white matter structures consistently affected by PCA
Structure | First author | Memory type | Imaging outcome measure | Direction of correlation |
---|---|---|---|---|
Cingulum | Alm54 | Verbal episodic memory | MD and RD | ↑ |
Ezzati55 | Verbal episodic memory | FA | ↑ | |
Sasson56 | Verbal and visual episodic memory | ADC | ↓ | |
Sasson57 | Verbal and visual episodic memory | ADC and AxD | ↓ | |
ILF | Hodgetts58 | Semantic aspects of autobiographical memory | MD | ↓ |
Sasson57 | Verbal and visual episodic memory | FA and RD | ↑ | |
SLF | Begre59 | Visual episodic memory | Intervoxel coherence | ↑ |
Sasson57 | Verbal and visual episodic memory | ADC and RD | ↑ | |
Splenium of the corpus callosum | Begre59 | Visual episodic memory | Intervoxel coherence | ↑ |
Cox60 | Verbal episodic memory | FA | ↑ | |
Voineskos61 | Verbal and visual episodic memory | FA | ↑ | |
Uncinate fasciculus | Alm54 | Verbal episodic memory | White matter volume | ↑ |
Metzler-Baddeley62 | Verbal episodic memory | FA | ↑ | |
Niogi63 | Verbal episodic memory | FA | ↑ | |
Sasson57 | Verbal and visual episodic memory | ADC | ↓ |
indicates a positive correlation, ↓ indicates a negative correlation.
ADC, apparent diffusivity coefficient; AxD, axial diffusivity; FA, fractional anisotropy; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; MD, mean diffusivity; RD, radial diffusivity; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus.
The aim of this study, therefore, is to investigate the relationship between verbal memory performance and white matter diffusivity in patients with PCA. We assessed white matter damage as a function of microstructural properties (fractional anisotropy, FA; mean diffusivity, MD). We predicted lower FA and higher MD within regions which are both consistently affected in PCA and strongly overlap with memory networks identified in healthy adults, namely the cingulum, ILF, SLF, splenium of the corpus callosum and uncinate fasciculus. More precisely, we hypothesised that white matter damage in tracts originating from these posterior structures would be specifically associated with poorer verbal memory retrieval.
Materials and methods
Participants
Eighteen participants fulfilling the diagnostic criteria for PCA (n = 18) were recruited from the Oxford Cognitive Disorders Clinic, Oxford, UK. Diagnoses were made by a senior behavioural neurologist (CB) and neuropsychologists (IB and SA) based on clinical, neuropsychological, and brain imaging features and in accordance with consensus criteria.1,2 These criteria included core features of simultanagnosia with or without optic ataxia or ocular apraxia, constructional apraxia, visual field defect, elements of Gestmann's syndrome or environmental disorientation, and the presence of the supportive features of alexia, ideomotor or dressing apraxia, or propopagnosia. Focal atrophy in the occipital and parietal lobes was confirmed in all patients with clinical MRI scans. Detailed grey matter assessments of the patients are reported in a previous voxel-based morphometry study.12 Neuropsychological assessment showed that patients presented with salient visuospatial and perceptual impairment, in line with the clinical phenotype (Table 3). Detailed memory assessments and MRI DTI data were available for 14 PCA patients. Scans from one PCA patient were excluded from the DTI analyses due to artefacts.
Table 3.
Demographic and clinical characteristics of control and patient groups. Standard deviations are given in brackets.
HC1 | PCA | P-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Demographics | |||
N | 19 | 14 | – |
Age (years) | 63.1 (6.3) | 63.8 (7.0) | 0.75 |
Education (years) | 14.5 (2.2) | 13.5 (2.3) | 0.21 |
Gender (m: F) | 8:11 | 8:6 | 0.39 |
Symptom duration | – | 3.7 (1.9) | – |
VOSP | |||
Dot count (max. 10)a | 10.0 (0.0) | 4.7 (3.4) | <0.001 |
Position discrimination (max. 20)a | 19.6 (1.1) | 13.5 (4.1) | <0.001 |
Cube analysis (max. 10)a | 9.4 (1.3) | 1.9 (2.4) | <0.001 |
RCFT | |||
Copy (max. 18)a | 17.6 (0.7) | 2.0 (3.0) | <0.001 |
Immediate Recall (max. 18)a | 10.6 (3.7) | 0.8 (1.0) | <0.001 |
Delayed recall (max. 18)a | 10.5 (3.2) | 0.2 (0.4) | <0.001 |
FCSRT-IR | |||
Immediate cued recall (max. 16) | 14.8 (2.9) | 14.1 (1.9) | 0.13 |
Total recall (max. 48) | 47.6 (1.0) | 19.9 (8.7) | <0.001 |
Free recall (max. 48) | 32.7 (4.6) | 21.8 (5.1) | <0.001 |
Cued recall (max. 48) | 14.9 (4.3) | 41.7 (5.2) | <0.001 |
Cue sensitivity (%) | 98.1 (4.8) | 80.1 (14.2) | <0.001 |
Imaging cohort | HC2 | PCA | P-value |
N | 18 | 13 | – |
Age (years) | 68.0 (6.2) | 62.6 (13.7) | 0.02 |
Education (years) | 14.1 (3.5) | 13.7 (1.9) | 0.76 |
Gender (m: F) | 11:7 | 7:6 | 0.69 |
Symptom duration (years) | – | 3.7 (2.0) | – |
FCSRT-IR, Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test with Immediate Recall; PCA, posterior cortical atrophy; RCFT, Rey Complex Figure Test; VOSP, Visual Object and Space Perception Battery.
Missing data: Data of PCA patients was missing for some tests due to refusal or inability to complete. Reduced sample sizes were present for: RCFT Copy and Immediate Recall (n = 9), RCFT Delayed Recall (n = 8), VOSP Dot Count (n = 12), VOSP Position Discrimination (n = 10), and VOSP Cube Analysis (n = 9).
Nineteen healthy volunteers were recruited from the local community as a behavioural control group (herein referred to as healthy control group 1; HC1) for cross-sectional comparisons. Exclusion criteria were a history of psychiatric illness, head injury, cerebrovascular disease or current medication known to affect cognition. A separate group of 18 healthy control subjects (herein referred to as healthy control group 2; HC2) was recruited from the Oxford project to Investigate Memory and Ageing and the Memory and Amnesia Project, University of Oxford, UK (for further details of original cohorts see Refs.12,14,64,65). Control participants had no objective cognitive impairments, as indicated by scores within the normal range on the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination (ACE66,67) or the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE68). HC2 participants underwent DTI scanning on the same MRI scanner with identical sequences as the PCA group.65 Demographic and clinical characteristics of all participants are provided in Table 3. Within the imaging cohort, there was a significant difference in age between the PCA patient and HC2 groups. All neuroimaging analyses were therefore corrected for age. HC1 and HC2 groups did not differ in either age (t(32) = −2.02, P = 0.052) or gender (χ2 (1, N = 35) = 0.24, P = 0.621). Ethical approval was obtained from the National Research Ethics Service South Central—Hampshire B and Oxford C. All participants provided written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Memory assessment
Episodic memory was assessed using the Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test with Immediate Recall (FCSRT-IR69). In the study phase of this task, participants were presented with cards containing four black-and-white line drawings, as per standard administration of the task. Taking into account the visuo-perceptual impairments in PCA, the examiner additionally read the name of each item concurrent with the picture presentation and asked the participant to repeat it. This procedure ensured that visuo-perceptual difficulties did not obstruct encoding of the items. The examiner then provided a verbal semantic category cue (e.g. fruit) and the participant was asked to name the associated item (e.g. grapes). Once each of the items had been correctly identified, immediate cued recall of the four items was assessed by verbally presenting the four category cues. If an item was not recalled the card was shown again and the categories verbally repeated. The participant was asked to name and select the relevant item, after which immediate cued recall was reattempted. When the participant had recalled all four items, the next set of items was presented as above, until all 16 items had been learned in this way. Following the study phase, there were three recall trials, each involving two minutes of spontaneous free recall followed by cuing with the semantic category for words that were not retrieved. Scores were recorded for Immediate Cued Recall (number of items recalled directly after presentation), Free Recall (number of items spontaneously recalled), Cued Recall (number of items retrieved with cues), Total Recall (number of items retrieved with or without cues), and Cue Sensitivity (efficacy of semantic category cues in facilitating recall) calculated as (Total Recall—Free Recall)/(48—Free Recall). Statistical analyses were undertaken with the following metrics: Immediate Cued Recall, to assess encoding processes; Total Recall scores, to investigate delayed memory; and Free Recall and Cue Sensitivity to assess memory performance requiring high and low levels of attentional control, respectively.
DTI data acquisition and analyses
MRI data were acquired with a 3T Siemens Trio scanner using a 32-channel head coil. Sixty non-collinear diffusion directions (b = 1000 s/mm2) and five non-diffusion (b = 0 s/mm2) volumes were obtained using a transverse 2D echo planar DTI sequence (repetition time = 9300 ms; echo time = 94 ms; slice thickness = 2.5 mm; voxel size = 2.0 mm × 2.0 mm × 2.0 mm; FOV = 192 mm). Sixty-five axial slices covering whole brain were acquired for each participant.
All imaging data were analysed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL70; Version 5.0.10). Pre-processing steps of diffusion-weighted images included correction for participant movements and eddy currents with the ‘eddy’ command.71 Maps of FA and MD were derived by fitting the diffusion tensor model within each voxel using FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox (FDT). Voxel-wise statistical analyses of the FA and MD data were carried out using tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS72). All participants’ FA data were aligned into a common space using the nonlinear registration tool FNIRT,73 which uses a b-spline representation of the registration warp field.74 Next, the mean FA image was created and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton representing the centres of all tracts common to the participant group. Each participant's aligned FA data were then projected onto the skeleton. The final non-linear warp and skeleton projection procedures were then repeated with the MD data.
The PCA and HC2 groups were compared with a two-sample unpaired t-test using the ‘randomise’ tool run on a whole-brain mask.75 Next, within the resulting regions of significant difference between the two groups, correlational analyses were run to test for specific associations between FA and MD measures and the degree of memory impairment in PCA patients (no FCSRT-IR data were available from the HC2 control group). For this purpose, binary masks were created from the group comparison at a threshold of P < 0.1 and the ‘randomise’ tool used to test for correlations between FCSRT-IR scores and FA and MD measures within the binary masks, adding age as a covariate of no interest to the model. For all analyses, a threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) P < 0.05 correction was applied, with the number of permutations set at 5000. To ascertain the specificity of correlations with the memory domain, analyses were re-run with inclusion of patients' visual function as a co-variate, as estimated from scores on the visual subdomain of Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination.
Finally, given the relatively modest size of the PCA sample, we applied region-of-interest (ROI) analyses to complement and confirm the results from the whole-brain TBSS approach. Seven ROIs which have consistently been identified as showing degeneration with PCA in prior studies (see Table 1) and are associated with memory performance (see Table 2) were defined for further investigation within the patient cohort. These were the right and left SLF, right and left ILF, right and left cingulum, and splenium of the corpus callosum. To create the ROIs, masks of the regions were extracted based on the JHU ICBM-DTI-81 white matter labels and JHU white-matter tractography atlas within the FSL atlas tool and multiplied with the mean skeleton. The average FA and MD values were extracted for each participant and region, and correlations with memory scores were assessed using statistical analyses as described below.
Post-hoc tractography analyses
After identification of the regions associated with memory performance in PCA patients, we carried out post-hoc tractography analyses to determine the associated white matter pathways in healthy controls.
Fibre orientation distributions were computed at each voxel for two fiber directions with the bedpostx tool.76,77 Probtrackx2 was used to perform probabilistic tractography, with the number of individual streamlines set at 5000 and a curvature threshold of 0.2.76,77 We generated a binary mask from voxels correlated with memory scores within the patient group in TBSS analyses (P < 0.05). This mask was projected onto each control participant's native DTI space and used as a seed for tractography, with resulting distributions being transformed back into MNI space. For each participant, results were divided by the total number of streamlines and thresholded at 0.01 to exclude low-probability pathways. Finally, thresholded distributions were binarized and averaged across individuals to obtain a group map.
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed using R software (version 3.6.0). Between-group differences on continuous demographic variables and task performance were assessed with two-tailed t-tests or Mann–Whitney U-tests for non-normal distributions, and comparisons of categorical variables were performed using χ2 tests.
The relationship between episodic memory scores and FA or MD values extracted from regions of interest was assessed using Spearman's partial correlation analyses adjusted for age. Bonferroni corrections were performed to control for Type I errors.
Data availability
The data are not publicly available due to privacy and ethical restrictions.
Results
Memory assessment
There was no significant difference in Immediate Cued Recall scores between PCA patients and HC1 (U = 184, P = 0.131), suggesting that encoding of verbal items was spared in PCA. However, PCA patients obtained significantly lower scores on Total FCSRT-IR (U = 266, P < 0.001), Free Recall (t(31) = 6.32, P < 0.001), and cue sensitivity (U = 234, P < 0.001) compared to the HC1 group. Semantic categories continued to support word retrieval in PCA, as indicated by a cue sensitivity of 80.1%.
Whole-brain voxel-based comparison between PCA and healthy controls
The group comparison demonstrated lower FA and higher MD in the forceps major, ILF, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, SLF, anterior thalamic radiation, cingulum, and body and splenium of the corpus callosum in PCA patients relative to HC2 participants (Fig. 1). There were no increases in FA or decreases in MD in PCA patients relative to the HC2 group.
Figure 1.
Fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity differences between groups (A) Significant reduction of FA (blue) in PCA patients compared to controls (x = −29, y = 59, z = 101). (B) Significant increase of MD (red) in PCA patients compared to control participants (x = −3, y = −35, z = 18). MNI coordinates are in millimetres and the skeletonised results have been thickened to improve visibility of the affected white matter regions for illustrative purposes.
Voxel-based correlations of FCSRT-IR with measures of white matter integrity in PCA
Within the PCA patient group, a significant positive correlation was observed between FA values in the splenium and Free Recall scores on the FCSRT-IR in TBSS analyses (Fig. 2A). In addition, a negative correlation for splenium MD and Free Recall was found (Fig 2B). Including visual functioning as a covariate of interest decreased significance but did not change the overall pattern of results. No further significant associations were observed for either FA or MD measures and Immediate Cued Recall, Cue Sensitivity or Total Recall scores.
Figure 2.
White matter microstructure in the splenium associated with memory retrieval. Higher FCSRT-IR scores on the Free Recall component were associated with (A) higher FA in left-sided splenium (x = −19, y = −50, z = 19) and (B) lower MD in bilateral splenium of the corpus callosum (x = −18, y = −49, z = 21). MNI coordinates are in millimetres and the skeletonised results have been thickened to improve visibility of the affected white matter regions for illustrative purposes.
ROI analyses of FCSRT-IR performance
Consistent with the whole-brain analyses, the ROI analyses indicated that there was a negative relationship between FCSRT-IR Free Recall scores and MD values in the splenium of the corpus callosum (see Table 4). No significant correlations were observed for MD or FA in any of the ROIs with FCSRT-IR Immediate Cued Recall, Cue Sensitivity or Total Recall scores.
Table 4.
Age-adjusted Spearman’s partial correlations between FCSRT-IR and ROI white matter microstructure in PCA patients.
Immediate cued recall | Free recall | Total recall | Cue sensitivity | |
---|---|---|---|---|
FA | ||||
Left SLF | −0.05 | −0.05 | 0.06 | 0.18 |
Right SLF | −0.39 | −0.16 | −0.34 | −0.30 |
Left ILF | −0.35 | −0.33 | −0.23 | −0.12 |
Right ILF | −0.39 | −0.43 | −0.42 | −0.39 |
Left cingulum | 0.06 | 0.36 | 0.20 | 0.19 |
Right cingulum | 0.37 | 0.44 | 0.43 | 0.34 |
Splenium | 0.16 | 0.48 | 0.30 | 0.35 |
MD | ||||
Left SLF | −0.11 | 0.44 | 0.30 | 0.08 |
Right SLF | 0.00 | 0.28 | 0.26 | 0.31 |
Left ILF | 0.36 | −0.31 | −0.29 | −0.35 |
Right ILF | 0.25 | −0.06 | −0.07 | −0.17 |
Left cingulum | 0.06 | −0.22 | 0.03 | 0.08 |
Right cingulum | 0.20 | −0.04 | 0.09 | 0.20 |
Splenium | −0.40 | −0.71a | −0.57 | −0.46 |
FA, fractional anisotropy; MD, mean diffusivity; ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; SLF; superior longitudinal fasciculus. r represents Spearman's correlation coefficient.
P = 0.038, corrected for multiple comparisons.
Post-hoc tractography
Splenial voxels significantly associated with FCSRT-IR Free Recall scores in PCA were used as a seed mask for probabilistic tractography in the healthy control group. As shown in Fig. 3, streamlines from the voxels of interest in the control participants passed bilaterally through the thalamic radiations and the retrolenticular part of the internal capsule.
Figure 3.
Average probabilistic tractography results in healthy adults seeded from the splenial mask derived from PCA patients. Thresholded streamlines (red) passed through the thalamic radiations (light blue) and retrolenticular internal capsule (dark blue) as defined by the JHU-DTI atlas.
Discussion
In this study, PCA patients presented with impaired free recall and relatively spared cued retrieval on a task of verbal episodic memory. In line with previous studies,12,64 these results are suggestive of a different cognitive basis for the memory dysfunction associated with PCA compared with the typical storage deficits observed in amnestic Alzheimer’s disease. DTI showed that poor memory retrieval was specifically associated with reduced FA and increased MD of posterior regions of the cortex, namely the splenium of the corpus callosum.
The splenium is highly interconnected with posterior parietal, occipital and temporal regions of the brain, all adversely affected in PCA.78,82 As part of the corpus callosum, the splenium forms a major commissural area of the brain, with the dorsal region of the splenium forming connections between bilateral parietal lobes and the ventral regions connecting the temporal cortices.80,81 Degeneration of the splenium can therefore result in disconnection of these posterior regions of the cerebral cortex, with resulting disruption of cognitive functioning.83 The splenium of the corpus callosum has consistently been found to be vulnerable in PCA.3,44,45,50,52,53 Neuropsychological studies have linked symptoms of visuospatial neglect to splenial lesions84 and marked perceptual difficulties in patients with tumours of the splenium,85 and it is therefore plausible that the splenial changes contribute to the characteristic visuospatial deficits which define PCA. The splenium has not previously been considered in connection to memory impairments in PCA. The memory literature, however, presents a strong evidence base for its role in episodic memory. In healthy older adults, integrity of the splenial fibres has been shown to relate to measures of immediate and delayed verbal memory recall.60,61 In Alzheimer’s disease, decreases in FA of the posterior corpus callosum were associated with poor delayed verbal recall and visual memory.86 Of particular relevance, a study of patients with tumours of the splenium observed memory impairments alongside visuospatial deficits,85 presenting a symptom profile similar to PCA.
From a mechanistic point of view, we propose that there are at least three different pathways through which splenial degeneration could influence memory performance. A first explanation is that since the corpus callosum plays an integral role in transferring information between the two hemispheres, damage to this region results in slowed information processing. Corpus callosum integrity has been found to significantly predict processing speed across various neuropsychological tasks.87,88 One study even proposed that reduced processing speed mediates the relationship between age and episodic memory decline.89 Slowed processing speed could therefore be hypothesized to be the driving factor in the association of the splenium with memory processes. However, this theory cannot explain the presentation of impaired memory retrieval with relatively preserved encoding skills observed in PCA. Reduced processing speed would be expected to interfere more generally with cognitive processes90 rather than being limited to retrieval mechanisms.
A second possibility is that the effects of splenium integrity are indirect and mediated by regions which are more strongly implicated in memory retrieval. For instance, Rudge and Warrington85 suggested that memory impairments in patients with tumours of the splenium are due to consequent damage to the fornix. The fornix forms a major efferent pathway from the medial temporal lobe, with posterior fibres projecting to the cerebral cortex and brainstem through the splenium of the corpus callosum.91 As such, it provides an important connection between the hippocampus and posterior regions affected in PCA. It is well-established that the fornix is linked to memory performance.54,57,58,92–95 Metzler-Baddeley et al.62 showed that FA values in the fornix were positively correlated with Free Recall and Total Recall measures of the FCSRT-IR in healthy older adults. In the present study, there was no evidence for damage to the fornix in patients compared with healthy controls, indicating that white matter integrity of this structure is not directly linked to memory performance in PCA. However, one might propose that damage of the splenium results in reduced functioning of connected structures such as the fornix, which in turn affects memory. Additional research is warranted to further explore potential interactions between the splenium and the fornix.
Finally, memory impairments may stem from disruption of attentional processes. The corpus callosum has been found to play a modulatory role in visuospatial attention networks. In particular, microstructural properties of the splenium have been related to the orienting component of attention in healthy adults.96 Reductions of white matter connectivity in posterior corpus callosum are correlated with reduced quality of orienting of attention.97 Furthermore, lesions of and reduced FA in the splenium have been associated with hemispatial neglect,84 a disorder which at its core is thought to involve impaired orienting of attention. Congruent with the AtoM model,24 it is possible that such changes in visual attention can be extrapolated to memory performance, that is, damage of the splenium may lead to difficulties in focussing on relevant stimuli, whether this concerns external (i.e. visual) or internal (i.e. memories) information. In the context of previous investigations of memory in PCA, this explanation is particularly compelling. Voxel-based morphometry and resting-state fMRI analyses provide converging evidence for a critical role of parietal attention networks as opposed to classic memory circuitry in retrieval dysfunction.12,14 Of particular relevance, voxel-based morphometry analyses of the same patient cohort found largely spared temporal regions, with significant correlations between autobiographical memory performance and atrophy of lateral parietal cortex but not hippocampal damage.12 Notably, post-hoc tractography analyses in the present study demonstrated that the splenial region associated with memory performance in PCA was connected to the internal capsule in healthy adults, a structure which has previously been linked to orienting of attention.96,97 These results are suggestive of a specific disruption of white matter pathways supporting attentional processing, rather than being caused by reduced integrity of connections with regions directly implicated in memory function (e.g. fornix). Crucially, the distinction between bottom-up and top-down attentional processes central to the AtoM model could also account for the pattern of impaired free recall in the face of comparatively intact cue sensitivity. Within this framework, specific disruptions of top-down attention would result in difficulties with spontaneously directing attention to memories, whereas patients could fare better when provided with external input (i.e. category cues). An attentional mechanism driving memory impairments in PCA could therefore provide a unifying account for the findings across these MRI modalities. Imaging studies explicitly testing for non-spatial attentional control processes in relation to the corpus callosum are needed to clarify this potential role for splenial fibres within the AtoM account of episodic memory.
The findings presented here have several key clinical implications. From a diagnostic perspective, the finding of splenial contributions to memory deficits that may be linked to attentional processes can help to inform which memory assessments are used to profile PCA patients. Although memory is not systematically examined in PCA, a body of research has begun to delineate the nature of memory disruption this group, highlighting it as a common supporting feature of the early clinical phenotype.2 Targeted memory assessment can therefore be used as a supplementary marker to increase diagnostic accuracy. Our results suggest that, although PCA patients are able to store verbal information, they have difficulties in recalling the items without external prompts. Direct comparisons between PCA and typical Alzheimer’s disease patients show that the former tend to learn word lists more rapidly, but that the two groups are equally impaired on delayed recall measures.12 Tasks which clearly distinguish between encoding and retrieval difficulties may therefore be particularly useful in differentiating PCA from other dementia phenotypes. Potential examples include the FCSRT-IR used here, as well as classic list learning tasks such as the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT98) and California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT99). Our findings also have implications for the identification and development of effective therapeutic approaches to support memory functioning in PCA. For example, if the role of the splenium in attentional components of memory is further supported, PCA patients may derive greater benefit from strategies aiming to help direct attention and minimise distractions, rather than classic compensatory aids to memory. The delivery of appropriate memory support is particularly important as deficits have been associated with greater difficulties in performing activities of daily living100,101 and increased caregiver burden102 in dementia, and specifically in PCA.103
Several limitations to the present study must be acknowledged. First, the neuropsychological battery did not incorporate detailed assessments of different aspects of memory or attention. Whilst we cautiously suggest that attentional processes may lie at the root of the memory deficits in PCA, additional testing with both attention and memory tasks will be essential to directly test this proposal as well as competing hypotheses. Despite this shortfall, the FCSRT-IR has a well-evidenced base and is widely posited as a go-to memory assessment in dementia.104 We are confident, therefore, that our findings remain a robust addition to our understanding of memory in PCA. Second, although the sample size of the patient group was not small given the relative rarity of the disorder, a larger sample would provide more power to detect subtle associations between white matter and memory performance. Future studies using a larger sample size would therefore be valuable in confirming and extending the present findings. Third, our hypotheses regarding the association between white matter integrity and memory in PCA focussed only on those mechanisms which are reliably linked to both the splenium and memory, and are therefore non-exhaustive. For example, an intriguing new line of work on autobiographical memory in PCA has uncovered a relationship between visual imagery and memory deficits in posterior parietal regions.12,105 It would be of interest to explore whether such associations extend to episodic memory tasks such as the FCSRT-IR, which in contrast to autobiographical memory assessments do not hinge on the retrieval of rich contextual information. Finally, as the healthy control cohort undergoing MRI scanning (HC2) did not complete the FCSRT-IR, it was not possible to conduct between-group comparisons for the relationship between DTI measures and memory performance. It is therefore unclear whether the proposed contributions of the splenium to memory extend to the wider population. As the HC1 control group performed near ceiling on the FCSRT-IR, it is unlikely that healthy adults show sufficient variability on the task to detect significant associations with white matter integrity. However, further explorations with other patient groups (e.g. PCA versus typical Alzheimer’s disease) are warranted to directly contrast the pathways associated with memory deficits across types of dementia.
Conclusions
In summary, the results of this study show that memory deficits in PCA are specifically associated with diffusivity changes in the splenium of the corpus callosum. Potential mechanisms by which the splenium could contribute to retrieval processes include indirect effects via the limbic system or attention networks, with the latter presenting a particularly compelling line of future inquiry. The findings have significant translational value in increasing our understanding of the broader clinical phenotype of PCA, aiding in diagnostic assessment and signposting to tailored post-diagnostic support.
Glossary
- DTI =
diffusion tensor imaging
- FA =
fractional anisotropy
- FCSRT-IR =
Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test with Immediate Recall
- ILF =
inferior longitudinal fasciculus
- MD =
mean diffusivity
- PCA =
posterior cortical atrophy
- ROI =
region-of-interest
- SLF =
superior longitudinal fasciculus
- TBSS =
tract-based spatial statistics
Funding
C.R.B. received funding from the Medical Research Council (Clinician Scientist Fellowship MR/K010395/1).
Competing interests
The authors report no competing interests.
References
- 1. Tang-Wai DF, Graff-Radford NR, Boeve BF, et al. Clinical, genetic, and neuropathologic characteristics of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology. 2004;63(7):1168–1174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Crutch SJ, Schott JM, Rabinovici GD, Murray M, et al. ; Alzheimer's Association ISTAART Atypical Alzheimer's Disease and Associated Syndromes Professional Interest Area. Consensus classification of posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(8):870–884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Cerami C, Crespi C, Della Rosa PA, et al. Brain changes within the visuo-spatial attentional network in posterior cortical atrophy. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;43(2):385–395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Ossenkoppele R, Cohn-Sheehy BI, La Joie R, et al. Atrophy patterns in early clinical stages across distinct phenoypes of Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36(11):4421–4437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Peng G, Wang J, Feng Z, et al. Clinical and neuroimaging differences between posterior cortical atrophy and typical amnestic Alzheimer’s disease patients at an early disease stage. Sci Rep. 2016;6:29372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Firth NC, Primativo S, Marinescu RV, et al. Longitudinal neuroanatomical and cognitive progression of posterior cortical atrophy. Brain. 2019;142(7):2082–2095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. McMonagle P, Deering F, Berliner Y, Kertesz A.. The cognitive profile of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurology. 2006;66(3):331–338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Alves J, Soares JM, Sampaio A, Goncalves OF.. Posterior cortical atrophy and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analytic review of neuropsychological and brain morphometry studies. Brain Imaging Behav. 2013;7(3):353–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Shakespeare TJ, Yong KC, Foxe D, Hodges J, Crutch SJ.. Pronounced impairment of everyday skills and self-care in posterior cortical atrophy. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;43(2):381–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Ahmed S, Baker I, Husain M, et al. Memory impairment at initial clinical presentation in posterior cortical atrophy. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;52(4):1245–1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Mendez MF, Ghajarania M, Perryman KM.. Posterior cortical atrophy: Clinical characteristics and differences compared to Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2002;14(1):33–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Ahmed S, Loane C, Bartels S, et al. Lateral parietal contributions to memory impairment in posterior cortical atrophy. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;20:252–259. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Putcha D, McGinnis SM, Brickhouse M, Wong B, Sherman JC, Dickerson BC.. Executive dysfunction contributes to verbal encoding and retrieval deficits in posterior cortical atrophy. Cortex. 2018;106:36–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Veldsman M, Zamboni G, Butler C, Ahmed S.. Attention network dysfunction underlies memory impairment in posterior cortical atrophy. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101773- [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Dickerson BC, Sperling RA.. Functional abnormalities of the medial temporal lobe memory system in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: Insights from functional MRI studies. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(6):1624–1635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Terry DP, Sabatinelli D, Puente AN, Lazar NA, Miller LS.. A meta-analysis of fMRI activation differences during episodic memory in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Neuroimaging. 2015;25(6):849–860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Whitwell JL, Jack CR, Kantarci K, et al. Imaging correlates of posterior cortical atrophy. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28(7):1051–1061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Schott JM, Rabinovici GD, Rossor MN, Fox NC.. Posterior cortical atrophy. Lancet Neurol. 2012;11(2):170–178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Corbetta M. Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1998;95(3):831–838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Posner MI, Petersen SE.. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1990;13:25–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Nobre AC. Orienting attention to instants in time. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39(12):1317–1328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Corbetta M, Shulman GL.. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2002;3(3):201–215. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Wagner AD, Shannon BJ, Kahn I, Buckner RL.. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(9):445–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Cabeza R, Ciaramelli E, Olson IR, Moscovitch M.. The parietal cortex and episodic memory: An attentional account. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(8):613–625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Hutchinson JB, Uncapher MR, Wagner AD.. Posterior parietal cortex and episodic retrieval: Convergent and divergent effects of attention and memory. Learn Mem. 2009;16(6):343–356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Konishi S, Wheeler ME, Donaldson DI, Buckner RL.. Neural correlates of episodic retrieval success. Neuroimage. 2000;12(3):276–286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Kahn T, Davachi L, Wagner AD.. Functional-neuroanatomic correlates of recollection: Implications for models of recognition memory. J Neurosci. 2004;24(17):4172–4180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Shannon BJ, Buckner RL.. Functional-anatomic correlates of memory retrieval that suggest nontraditional processing roles for multiple distinct regions within posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24(45):10084–10092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Henson RNA, Hornberger M, Rugg D.. Further dissociating the processes involved in recognition memory: An fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17(7):1058–1073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Iidaka T, Matsumoto A, Nogawa J, Yamamoto Y, Sadato N.. Frontoparietal network involved in successful retrieval from episodic memory. Spatial and temporal analyses using fMRI and ERP. Cereb Cortex. 2006;16(9):1349–1360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Henson RNA, Rugg MD, Shallice T, Josephs O, Dolan RJ.. Recollection and familiarity in recognition memory: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 1999;19(10):3962–3972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Vilberg KL, Rugg MD.. Dissociation of the neural correlates of recognition memory according to familiarity, recollection, and amount of recollected information. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(10):2216–2225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Wheeler ME, Buckner RL.. Functional-anatomic correlates of remembering and knowing. Neuroimage. 2004;21(4):1337–1349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Dobbins IG, Foley H, Schacter DL, Wagner AD.. Executive control during episodic retrieval: Multiple prefrontal processes subserve source memory. Neuron. 2002;35(5):989–996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Dobbins IG, Rice HJ, Wagner AD, Schacter DL.. Memory orientation and success: Separably neurocognitive components underlying episodic recognition. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(3):318–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Berryhill ME, Olson IR.. Is the posterior parietal lobe involved in working memory retrieval? Evidence from patients with bilateral parietal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1775–1786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Davidson PSR, Anaki D, Ciaramelli E, et al. Does lateral parietal cortex support episodic memory? Evidence from focal lesion patients. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1743–1755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Ben-Zvi S, Soroker N, Levy DA.. Parietal lesion effects on cued recall following pair associate learning. Neuropsychologia. 2015;73:176–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Spiers HJ, Maguire EA, Burgess N.. Hippocampal amnesia. Neurocase. 2001;7(5):357- 382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Olson IR, Berryhill M.. Some surprising findings on the involvement of the parietal lobe in human memory. Neurobiol Learn Memory. 2009;91(2):155–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Naghavi HR, Nyberg L.. Common fronto-parietal activity in attention, memory, and consciousness: Shared demands on integration? Conscious Cogn. 2005;14(2):390–425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Ciaramelli E, Grady CL, Moscovitch M.. Top-down and bottom-up attention to memory: A hypothesis (AtoM) on the role of the posterior parietal cortex in memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(7):1828–1851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, McDermott KB.. A parietal memory network revealed by multiple MRI methods. Trends Cogn Sci. 2015;19(9):534–543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Agosta F, Mandic-Stojmenovic G, Canu E, et al. Functional and structural brain networks in posterior cortical atrophy: A two-centre multiparametric MRI study. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;19:901–910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Madhavan A, Schwarz CG, Duffy JR, et al. Characterizing white matter tract degeneration in syndrome variants of Alzheimer’s disease: A diffusion tensor imaging study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;49(3):633–643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Caso F, Agosta F, Mattavelli D, et al. White matter degeneration in atypical Alzheimer disease. Radiology. 2015;277(1):162–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Duning T, Warnecke T, Mohammadi S, et al. Pattern and progression of white-matter changes in a case of posterior cortical atrophy using diffusion tensor imaging. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80(4):432–436. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Glick-Shames H, Backner Y, Bick A, Raz N, Levin N.. The impact of localized grey matter damage on neighboring connectivity: Posterior cortical atrophy and the visual network. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019;13(5):1292–1301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Migliaccio R, Agosta F, Toba MN, et al. Brain networks in posterior cortical atrophy: A single case tractography study and literature review. Cortex. 2012;48(10):1298–1309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Migliaccio R, Agosta F, Possin KL, Rabinovici GD, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML.. White matter atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease variants. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8:S78–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Migliaccio R, Agosta F, Scola E, et al. Ventral and dorsal visual streams in posterior cortical atrophy: A DT MRI study. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(11):2572–2584. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Millington RS, James-Galton M, Maia da Silva MN, Plan GT, Bridge H.. Lateralized occipital degeneration in posterior cortical atrophy predicts visual field deficits. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;14:242–249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Yoshida T, Shiga K, Yoshikawa K, Yamada K, Nakagawa M.. White matter loss in the splenium of the corpus callosum in a case of posterior cortical atrophy: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Eur Neurol. 2004;52(2):77–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Alm KH, Faria AV, Moghekar A, et al. Medial temporal lobe white matter pathway variability is assocaited with individual differences in episodic memory in cognitively normal older adults. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;87:78–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Ezzati A, Katz MJ, Lipton ML, Zimmerman ME, Lipton RB.. Hippocampal volume and cingulum bundle fractional anisotropy are independently associated with verbal memory in older adults. Brain Imaging Behav. 2016;10(3):652–659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Sasson E, Doniger GM, Pasternak O, Tarrasch R, Assaf Y.. Structural correlates of cognitive domains in normal aging with diffusion tensor imaging. Brain Struct Funct. 2012;217(2):503–515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Sasson E, Doniger GM, Pasternak O, Tarrasch R, Assaf Y.. White matter correlates of cognitive domains in normal aging with diffusion tensor imaging. Front Neurosci. 2013;7:32. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Hodgetts CJ, Postans M, Warne N, Varnava A, Lawrence AD, Graham KS.. Distinct contributions of the fornix and inferior longitudinal fasciculus to episodic and semantic autobiographical memory. Cortex. 2017;94:1–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Begré S, Frommer A, von Känel R, Kiefer C, Federspiel A.. Relation of white matter anisotropy to visual memory in healthy subjects. Brain Res. 2007;1168:60–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Cox SR, Bastin ME, Ferguson KJ, et al. Compensation or inhibitory failure? Testing hypotheses of age-related right frontal lobe involvement in verbal memory ability using structural and diffusion MRI. Cortex. 2015;63:4–15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Voineskos AN, Rajji TK, Lobaugh NH, et al. Age-related decline in white matter tract integrity and cognitive performance: A DTI tractography and structural equation modeling study. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33(1):21–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Metzler-Baddeley C, Jones DK, Belaroussi B, Aggleton JP, O’Sullivan MJ.. Frontotemporal connections in episodic memory and aging: A diffusion MRI tractography study. J Neurosci. 2011;31(37):13236–13245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Niogi SN, Mukherjee P, Ghajar J, et al. Structural dissociation of attentional control and memory in adults with and without mild traumatic brain injury. Brain. 2008;131(Pt 12):3209–3221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Ahmed S, Irish M, Loane C, et al. Association between precuneus volume and autobiographical memory impairment in posterior cortical atrophy: Beyond the visual syndrome. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;18:822–834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Zamboni G, Wilcock GK, Douaud G, et al. Resting functional connectivity reveals residual functional activity in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(5):375–383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Arnold R, Hodges JR.. The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination Revised (ACE-R): A brief cognitive test battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21(11):1078–1085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Hsieh S, Schubert S, Hoon C, Mioshi E, Hodges JR.. Validation of the Addnebrooke’s Cognitive Examination III in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;36(3-4):242–250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR.. “Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Grober E, Buschke H.. Genuine memory deficits in dementia. Dev Neuropsychol. 1987;3(1):13–36. [Google Scholar]
- 70. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM.. FSL. Neuroimage. 2012;62(2):782–790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN.. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage. 2016;125:1063–1078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. 2006;31(4):1487–1505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Andersson JLR, Jenkinson M, Smith S. Non-linear optimisation, aka spatial normalisation. FMRIB technical report TR07JA1. https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FNIRT. Accessed 21 April 2020.
- 74. Rueckert D, Sonoda LI, Hayes C, Hill DLG, Leach MO, Hawkes DJ.. Non-rigid registration using free-form deformations: Application to breast MR images. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 1999;18(8):712–721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Winkler AM, Ridgway GR, Webster MA, Smith SM, Nichols TE.. Permutation inference for the general linear model. Neuroimage. 2014;92:381–397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Jenkinson M, et al. Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR imaging. Magn Reson Med. 2003;50(5):1077–1088. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MFS, Woolrich MW.. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: What can we gain? Neuroimage. 2007;34(1):144–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Huang H, Zhang J, Jiang H, et al. DTI tractography based parcellation of white matter: Application to the mid-sagittal morphology of corpus callosum. Neuroimage. 2005;26(1):195–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Hofer S, Frahm J.. Topography of the human corpus callosum revisited – comprehensive fiber tractography using diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage. 2006;32(3):989–994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Park HJ, Kim JJ, Lee SK, et al. Corpus callosal connection mapping using cortical gray matter parcellation and DT-MRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 2008;29(5):503–516. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Chao YP, Cho KH, Yeh CH, Chou KH, Chen JH, Lin CP.. Probabilistic topography of human corpus callosum using cytoarchitectural parcellation and high angular resolution diffusion imaging tractography. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30(10):3172–3187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Wei PH, Mao ZQ, Cong F, et al. Connection between bilateral temporal regions: Tractography using human connectome data and diffusion spectrum imaging. J Clin Neurosci. 2017;39:103–108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. Blaauw J, Meiners LC.. The splenium of the corpus callosum: Embryology, anatomy, function and imaging with pathophysiological hypothesis. Neuroradiology. 2020;62(5):563–585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Kesayan T, Gasoyan H, Heilman KM.. Left hemispatial neglect with a splenial lesion. Neurocase. 2018;24(4):220–226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Rudge P, Warrington EK.. Selective impairment of memory and visual perception in splenial tumours. Brain. 1991;114(1):349–360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Kavcic V, Ni H, Zhu T, Zhong J, Duffy CJ.. White matter integrity linked to functional impairments in aging and early Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2008;4:391–399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Kumar R, Saksena S, Husain M, et al. Serial changes in diffusion tensor imaging metrics of corpus callosum in moderate traumatic brain injury patients and their correlation with neuropsychometric tests: a 2-year follow-up study. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2010;25(1):31–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Chopra S, Shaw M, Shaw T, Sachdev PS, Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N.. More highly myelinated white matter tracts are associated with faster processing speed in healthy adults. Neuroimage. 2018;171:332–340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Bucur B, Madden DJ, Spaniol J, et al. Age-related slowing of memory retrieval: Contributions of perceptual speed and cerebral white matter integrity. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29(7):1070–1079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103(3):403–428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Jang SH, Kwon HG.. Neural connectivity of the posterior body of the fornix in the human brain: Diffusion tensor imaging study. Neurosci Lett. 2013;549:116–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Kinnunen KM, Greenwood R, Powell JH, et al. White matter damage and cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury. Brain. 2011;134(Pt 2):449–463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Palacios EM, Fernandez-Espejo D, Junque C, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging differences relate to memory deficits in diffuse traumatic brain injury. BMC Neurol. 2011;11:24- [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Mielke MM, Okonkwo OC, Oishi K, et al. Fornix integrity and hippocampal volume predict memory decline and progression to Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8(2):105–113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Ly M, Adluru N, Destiche DJ, et al. Fornix microstructure and memory performance is associated with altered neural connectivity during episodic recognition. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(2):191–204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Niogi S, Mukherjee P, Ghajar J, McCandliss BD.. Individual differences in distinct components of attention are linked to anatomical variations in distinct white matter tracts. Front Neuroanat. 2010;4:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Yin X, Zhao L, Xu J, et al. Anatomical substrates of the alerting, orienting and executive control components of attention: Focus on the posterior parietal lobe. PLoS One. 2012;7(11):e50590. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Rey A. Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test. In PUF, ed. L’examen Clinique en Psychologie. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 99. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA.. California Verbal Learning Test – second edition. Adult Version. Manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 100. Jefferson AL, Cahn-Weiner D, Boyle P, et al. Cognitive predictors of functional decline in vascular dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2006;21(8):752–754. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Farias ST, Cahn-Weiner DA, Harvey DJ, et al. Longitudinal changes in memory and executive functioning are associated with longitudinal change in instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. Clin Neuropsychol. 2009;23(3):446–461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Berger G, Bernhardt T, Weimer E, Peters J, Kratzsch T, Frolich L.. Longitudinal study on the relationship between symptomatology of dementia and levels of subjective burden and depression among family caregivers in memory clinic patients. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2005;18(3):119–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Ahmed S, Culley S, Blanco-Duque C, Hodges JR, Butler C, Mioshi E.. Pronounced impairment of activities of daily living in posterior cortical atrophy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2020;49(1):48–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Teichmann M, Epelbaum S, Samri D, et al. Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test – accuracy for the differential diagnosis of Alzheimer's and neurodegenerative diseases: a large-scale biomarker-characterised monocenter cohort study (ClinAD). Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(8):913–923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Ramanan S, Alaeddin S, Goldberg ZL, Strikwerda-Brown C, Hodges JR, Irish M.. Exploring the contribution of visual imagery to scene construction – Evidence from posterior cortical atrophy. Cortex. 2018;106:261–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data are not publicly available due to privacy and ethical restrictions.