Abstract
Experiencing pain with a familiar individual can enhance one’s own pain sensitivity, a process known as pain contagion. When experiencing pain with an unfamiliar individual, pain contagion is suppressed in males by activating the endocrine stress response. Here, we coupled a histological investigation with pharmacological and behavioral experiments to identify enhanced glucocorticoid receptor activity in the prelimbic subdivision of the medial prefrontal cortex as a candidate mechanism for suppressing pain contagion in stranger mice. Acute inhibition of glucocorticoid receptors in the prelimbic cortex was sufficient to elicit pain contagion in strangers, while their activation prevented pain contagion in cagemate dyads. Slice physiology recordings revealed enhanced excitatory transmission in stranger mice, an effect that was reversed by pre-treating mice with the corticosterone synthesis inhibitor metyrapone. Following removal from dyadic testing, stranger mice displayed enhanced affective-motivational pain behaviors when placed on an inescapable thermal stimulus, which were reversed by metyrapone. Together, our data suggest that the prelimbic cortex may play an integral role in modulating pain behavior within a social context and provide novel evidence towards the neural mechanism underlying the prevention of pain contagion.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Empathy
Introduction
Emotional contagion—matching an emotional state between individuals—is a primitive form of empathy and serves as an important social cue in humans and animals [1–3]. In addition to distress [4], fear [5], and anxiety [6], pain is an emotionally contagious phenomenon [7, 8]. Pain contagion represents a sensory tuning process by which the social observation of a painful experience in another individual heightens one’s own pain sensitivity. In this respect, pain behaviors are highly salient and evolutionarily adaptive cues to inform others about distress within a social context and warn members about potentially dangerous situations [9]. The mere presence of another individual impacts the sufferer’s pain experience, with the specific audience playing a critical role. Human and rodent studies have shown that observing and experiencing pain with a familiar, but not unfamiliar (i.e., stranger) conspecific enhances pain behavior and sensitivity in both members of the dyad [7, 10, 11]. Moreover, stranger conspecifics’ social interaction causes the prevention of pain contagion by activating the endocrine stress response [12]. In mice, the lack of pain contagion is specific to male stranger dyads in which both mice received the pain stimulus. We suspect that the prevention of pain contagion in male mouse stranger dyads is related to the potential threat of an aggressive encounter with an unfamiliar male mouse since female–female aggression rarely occurs in group-housed laboratory mice [13].
The influence of social threat on pain sensitivity is a burgeoning field with perceived social threat level shown to modulate pain behavior in mice [14] and humans [15, 16]. In response to potentially threatening situations, a signaling cascade is initiated that culminates in the release of adrenal glucocorticoid hormones (mainly corticosterone (CORT) in animals and cortisol in humans). These glucocorticoid hormones exert their effects through glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors (GR and MR, respectively), with GRs predominately mediating the response to acute stress [17]. In the brain, GRs are present in both subcortical and cortical structures exhibiting high distribution in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) [18, 19]. Following exposure to an acute stressor, corticosterone is elevated in the mPFC and remains elevated for up to 90 min [20]. Moreover, recent evidence points toward a role for the mPFC in behavioral responses to socially threatening [21] and stressful situations [22], while bilateral lesions of the mPFC abolish the social facilitation of pain [10]. Thus, the current study sought to determine whether the social threat of an unfamiliar mouse suppresses pain contagion through GR signaling in the mPFC. Also, we view the prevention of pain contagion in strangers as an outright suppression of pain behavior. This led us to explore the secondary hypothesis that detecting a social threat causes the inhibition of pain behavior in unfamiliar male mice, and this can lead to an enhancement of pain sensitivity once the social threat has been removed.
In the present study, we measured the expression of c-Fos, an immediate early gene product, and phosphorylated GR (p-GR) across multiple cortical brain regions in mice tested alone or with a stranger/cagemate following exposure to a nociceptive assay. Immunohistochemical analysis showed elevated c-Fos and p-GR activity in the mPFC of stranger dyads. Local infusion of RU-486, a GR antagonist, into the prelimbic cortex of stranger dyads increased pain behavior, while a local infusion of corticosterone, a GR agonist, decreased pain behavior in cagemate dyads. To determine whether synaptic transmission of cortical neurons in the prelimbic cortex was altered in stranger dyads, we measured spontaneous activity in layers II/III following pain testing in mice treated with or without metyrapone, a drug that blocks the synthesis of CORT. Finally, we also demonstrate that stranger and cagemate mice display different pain behaviors when removed from their dyadic condition, and tested alone on an inescapable thermal apparatus.
Materials and methods
Mice
Mice were bred in-house and consisted of young (6–12 weeks) adult male CD-1 mice. All mice were housed in non-ventilated cages in groups of 4–5, maintained in a temperature-controlled (20 ± 1 °C) environment with 12:12-h light: dark cycle (lights on at 7 am and off at 7 pm) with access to food (Harlan Teklad 8604) and water ad libitum. Experiments were conducted only during the light period. All procedures were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Canadian Council on Animal Care and approved by the University of Toronto Animal Care Committee.
Acetic acid test and social conditions
Pain behavior was assessed using the acetic acid abdominal constriction test, under three social conditions: (1) alone, (2) in a dyadic condition where mice were drawn from the same home cage (cagemates), or (3) in a dyadic condition where mice were drawn from different home cages (strangers). All testing occurred near mid-photoperiod (10 am–4 pm). At the beginning of the test day, mice were brought to the experimental room and left undisturbed for 1 h before testing began. Mice were then placed on a glass surface within red Plexiglas cylinders (30 cm high × 15 cm diameter) either alone or with a cagemate/stranger social partner and allowed to habituate to the cylinder for 30 min. Mice were then briefly removed and injected intraperitoneal with acetic acid (0.6% in physiological saline; 10 ml/kg). Mice were placed back in their cylinders and observed continuously for 30 min. Stereotypical abdominal constrictions or “writhes” (lengthwise constrictions of the torso with a concomitant concave arching of the back) were counted over this period, sampling for 5 s every 20 s for a total of 90 observations. The presence of a “writhe” within the 5 s observation period was considered a positive sample. The percentage of writhing behavior was calculated by dividing the number of positive samples by total samples and multiplying by 100 %. Sampling in this manner allows for a higher degree of accuracy and higher interrater reliability than counting constrictions [7]. In experiments where drug delivery was required, mice were pretreated systemically or via microinjection 30 min before acetic acid injection (i.e., at the beginning of habituation to the test cylinders). The synchronization of writhing was calculated using joint probability, a statistical measure that calculates the likelihood of two events occurring together and at the same point in time. Since probabilities are combined using multiplication, the “expected” joint probability of writhing was calculated by multiplying the writhing score of mouse #1 by the writhing score of mouse #2. The observed writhing sample for a mouse was then divided by the expected joint probability of its dyad to calculate the percentage of synchronized writhing above expected levels. In all experiments (behavioral, immunohistochemistry, and electrophysiological), both mice within a dyad were injected with acetic acid. Behavior was video recorded for offline analyses, and all experimenters were blind to social condition and drug condition being coded.
Thermal nociceptive reflexes and affective-motivational behavior
To evaluate whether pain behaviors—other than writhing—were altered following social interactions or removal from dyadic pain testing, a subset of mice were placed on an inescapable thermal stimulus for 45 s at three different time points throughout the testing session. Briefly, mice were placed singly on a 52.5 °C hotplate for 45 s before any social interaction (baseline), following a 30 min social interaction with a cagemate or stranger (social-no pain) and following acetic acid dyadic testing (social-pain). Baseline responses were collected immediately following 1-h habituation to the test environment and before the social interaction/acetic acid test. The acetic acid dyadic testing was performed as described above, with each mouse receiving an acetic acid injection. In a subset of stranger dyads, metyrapone (50 mg/kg, s.c.) was injected 30 min before baseline testing. For these experiments, the same pair of mice were used for the social-no pain phase and then the social-pain phase. We categorized nociceptive reflexive (rapid flicking of the limb) and affective-motivational (paw licking, paw guarding, and escape jumping) pain behaviors over the entire 45 s duration of the trial based on previous studies [23–25]. The primary distinction is that rapid reflexive retraction of the paw occurs in response to nociceptive sensory information but ceases once the stimulus has been removed—these responses mainly involve spinal and brainstem circuits. In contrast, affective-motivational responses are temporally-delayed—relative to noxious stimulation—and characterized by hyperlocomotion, rearing, and escape behavior. Affective responses are complex and rely on limbic and cortical circuits in the brain, which may indicate motivation to reduce aversive simulation by licking tissue, protecting tissue, or attempting to escape [23]. Video files were coded using BORIS (Behavioral Observation Research Interactive Software, http://www.boris.unito.it). The number of cumulative reflexive responses were counted, while the cumulative duration of the affective-motivational responses was summed over the entire duration of the trial. The area under the curve (AUC) of the cumulative responses was calculated using the trapezoidal rule.
Tissue preparation and immunohistochemistry
Ninety-minutes (for c-Fos) or 30-min (for GR phosphorylation) following acetic acid injection, a subset of mice, were deeply anesthetized with sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg, i.p.) and underwent transcardiac perfusion with 0.1 M phosphate-buffered saline (PBS, pH = 7.4) followed by 4% (w/v) buffered paraformaldehyde. The brains were extracted and post-fixed in the same fixative for 4 h at 4 °C before undergoing PBS sucrose infiltration (30% w/v sucrose) and sectioning on a cryostat (Cryostar NX50, ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) at a section thickness of 40 µm. All serial sections were collected and stored at −20 °C in a cryoprotectant solution consisting of 30% (w/v) sucrose, 1% (w/v) polyvinylpyrrolidone, and 30% (v/v) ethylene glycol in 0.1 M PBS until use. Immunostaining for the immediate early gene product c-Fos or GR phosphorylation at Ser(211) was performed on free-floating coronal sections as described previously [26, 27].
c-Fos immunohistochemistry
All procedures were performed under gentle agitation and at room temperature unless noted otherwise. Sections were first washed several times in 0.1 M PBS (pH 7.4; 6 times for 10 min each). To minimize endogenous peroxidase activity, slices were incubated in a solution of 0.3% (v/v) H2O2/PBS for 30 min. The sections were then rinsed several times in 0.1 M PBS before being placed in a blocking buffer containing 5% (v/v) Normal Horse Sera, 1% (w/v) Bovine Serum Albumin, and 0.3% (v/v) Triton X-100 dissolved in 0.1 M PBS for 1 h. After blocking, sections were incubated with a primary anti-rabbit polyclonal c-Fos antibody (1:10,000, ABE457 Millipore) and dissolved in the same blocking buffer for 48 h at 4 °C, followed by incubation with a secondary biotinylated antibody (goat anti-rabbit, 1:500, 2 h, room temperature, Vector Laboratories) and then treatment with avidin-biotin-peroxidase complex (ABC, 1:500, 1 h, Vector Laboratories). After rinsing first in PBS then in 0.175 M sodium acetate (pH = 7.0), the antibody-peroxidase complex was visualized with a solution of 2.5% (w/v) nickel ammonium sulfate, 0.2% (w/v) 3,3-diaminobenzidine HCl, and 0.083% (v/v) H2O2 in aqueous 0.175 M sodium acetate to yield a blue-black reaction product. After approximately 15–20 min, the tissue was rinsed in the acetate solution, washed in PBS, and then placed into saline. The sections were mounted onto charged glass slides (Fisher SuperFrost Plus, Fisher Scientific) and left to air dry overnight. Slides were dehydrated through an ascending series of alcohols, cleared in xylene, and coverslipped with Entellan mounting medium.
Phosphorylated glucocorticoid receptor immunohistochemistry
Slices were rinsed with 1× PBS and then washed with PBS-T three times. Slices were then incubated with 4% normal donkey serum in PBS-T for 2 h at room temperature. Following three washes with PBS-T, slices were then transferred to 1:1000 solution of the anti-rabbit phosphorylated glucocorticoid receptor antibody (Cat. #4161, Cell Signaling Technology) for 48 h at 4 °C. After rinsing with PBS-T three times, slices were then incubated in anti-rabbit Cy3 secondary antibody (Jackson laboratories) for 2 h at room temperature. Slices were then mounted onto VWR SuperFrost slides, dried and dipped to rehydrate in a 1:50,000 DAPI solution. Slides were then coverslipped with an aqueous mounting medium.
Image analysis and quantification
c-fos
Fos immunoreactivity was examined in the medial prefrontal cortex (infralimbic, prelimbic, and anterior cingulate), amygdala (basolateral and central amygdala), hippocampus (CA1, CA2/CA3, and dentate gyrus), and hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus with each region being delineated based on cytoarchitectonic boundaries found in the Mouse Brain Atlas (Franklin and Paxinos, 1997). Microscopic images of the regions of interest were photographed with a 1600 × 1200-megapixel digital camera (Optronics MicroFire) attached to a light microscope (Nikon Eclipse 80i). For each region, images were taken at 4x magnification to identify the regions and 20x magnification to quantify immunostaining. All photomicrographs were exported in TIFF format and processed in ImageJ (http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/; National Institutes of Health, USA). Images were converted to (8-bit) grayscale, and Fos+ immunolabeled cells were counted using the Particle Analyzer tool in ImageJ with the application of the same image, threshold, size, and circularity settings for each brain region. For each animal, cell counts were obtained from between three and four sections. Each region of interest was outlined and measured using ImageJ to calculate the area. The number of Fos+ cells for each region of interest was then expressed as cells per square millimeter.
Phosphorylated glucocorticoid receptor (p-GR)
The expression of p-GR in rostral ACC, prelimbic cortex, and anterior insular cortex across three brain slices per ROI were examined. Images were taken using the Cytation 5 Imaging Multi-Mode Reader (BioTek, Winooski, VT). Images were taken at 4× for regional identification and 20× for quantification. Using the Gen5 software cell counter tool, cells expressing p-GRs were counted using the same threshold, size, and circularity settings for each brain region. Each region of interest was outlined and measured using Gen5. The number of p-GR+ cells for each region of interest was expressed as cells per square millimeter.
Plasma corticosterone
Following behavior, mice were sacrificed, and trunk blood was collected. Blood samples were kept on ice and centrifuged at 4 °C at 15,000 r.p.m. for 15 min. Plasma was extracted from the samples and frozen at −80 °C until processing. Corticosterone levels were computed using enzyme immunoassay (Cayman Chemical Company, Kit 500655). Samples and standards were assayed in duplicate at a 1:800 dilution according to the manufacturer’s protocol. Single absorbance readings for standards and samples were obtained at 405 nm using a Biotek Plate reader (Synergy HT, Winooski, VT). These values were used to calculate plasma corticosterone levels (ng/ml) based on linear regression of the standard curve using a log–logit transformation.
Prelimbic and ACC microinfusions
Stainless steel injection guide cannulas (C315GS-5/spc, P1 Technologies, Roanoke, VA) were bilaterally implanted into the prelimbic cortex (AP1 2.33 mm, ML 0.40 mm, and DV 1.30 mm) or the anterior cingulate cortex (AP1 2.00 mm, ML 0.40 mm, and DV 1.15 mm) at 6 weeks of age, DV was measured relative to brain surface [28]. The internal cannulas extended 0.5 mm from the guide cannula. Mice were allowed to recover for 7–8 days, after which microinjections were done under slight isoflurane anesthesia, 30 min preceding acetic acid injection. The internal cannulas were connected to a microliter syringe (1700 series Hamilton syringe, 100 μl, Harvard Apparatus, Montreal, QC) and pump (Pump 11 Elite Nanomite, Harvard Apparatus, Montreal, QC) via calibrated tubing. To limit diffusion, RU-486 or corticosterone was infused in a volume of 0.25 μl per side over 5 min, after which the injectors were left in position for an additional 1 min.
Verification of cannulae placement
After experiments, animals were killed by decapitation, and brains were rapidly removed and post-fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde. Coronal sections (40 μm) were then cut at the level of the mPFC using the Cryostar NX50 and mounted onto VWR superfrost slides, nissl stained or dipped in DAPI, and placed under coverslips to verify probe placements.
Drugs
Metyrapone (2-methyl-1,2-di-3-pyridyl-1-propanone), RU-486 (11β-(4-dimethyl-amino)-phenyl-17β-hydroxy-17-(1-propynyl)-estra-4,9-dien-3-one) and corticosterone were purchased from Sigma Aldrich (St. Louis, MO). Drug dose and pre-treatment timing were chosen based on our previous work [11], and pilot experiments to identify proper microinjection dosing parameters in which outright writhing behavior was not affected. Metyrapone was dissolved in saline and injected, 30 min before acetic acid injection, subcutaneously at a concentration of 50 mg/kg. Corticosterone was dissolved in aCSF to a concentration of 20 ng/μl. RU 486 was not dissolvable in aCSF or saline, and thus aliquots were prepared in DMSO (dimethyl sulfoxide) to obtain a final concentration of 2 μg/μl. In all dyadic conditions, both mice received the same drug treatment.
Electrophysiology
Brain slices were prepared based on previous work [29]. Mice were anesthetized with 5% isoflurane and killed by decapitation. The brains were quickly removed and placed in cold (4 °C) oxygenated (95% O2; 5% CO2) artificial cerebrospinal fluid (aCSF) consisting of 124 mM NaCl, 4.4 mM KCl, 2 mM CaCl2, 1 mM MgSO4, 25 mM NaHCO3, 1 mM NaH2PO4, and 10 mM glucose. Brain slices (300 μm) containing coronal sections of the prelimbic cortex were prepared with a VT1200S tissue slicer (Leica, Concord, ON). Slices recovered for a minimum of 60 min in a submerged holding chamber (25 °C) before recording. Following recovery, slices were placed in a recording chamber where they were continuously perfused with oxygenated (95% O2; 5% CO2) aCSF at a rate of 2 ml per min. Whole-cell voltage-clamp recordings from layer II/III pyramidal neurons of the prelimbic cortex was obtained under visual guidance using a 40× objective on a Zeiss Axioskop FS upright microscope. Layer II/III was specifically targeted because p-GR expression was quantified in these layers, and synaptic transmission in response to an acute stressor is enhanced in layers II/III [29]. Recordings were made with electrodes (4–6 MΩ) fabricated using a horizontal puller (P1000; Sutter, Novato, CA) and filled with an internal solution containing, cesium methanesulfonate-based intracellular solution (120 mM Cs-MeSO3, 5 mM NaCl, 10 mM HEPES, 1 mM MgCl2, 0.5 mM EGTA, 2 mM MgATP, 0.1 mM Na3GTP, 10 mM HEPES, and pH 7.3, 285–290 mOsmol). QX314 (5 mM) was included in the intracellular solution to block postsynaptic sodium currents. Neurons were held at −60 mV, the reversal potential for chloride, to assess glutamatergic synaptic transmission. In the same cell, neurons were held at 0 mV, the reversal potential for sodium, to assess GABAergic synaptic transmission. Electrophysiological recordings were performed using an Axon 700B amplifier (Axon Instruments, Foster City, CA), low pass filtered at 1 kHz, and digitized at 10 kHz with Clamplex (version 10.6; Molecular Devices). Spontaneous synaptic currents (sEPSC or sIPSC) were detected using Mini Analysis v. 6.0.3 (Synaptosoft, Decatur, GA, USA) with a detection threshold set three times higher than the level of baseline noise. The accuracy of detection was visually confirmed for each event in each recording. Spontaneous currents were selected so that the rise and decay phases did not contain any overlapping events. For individual neurons, the amplitude and frequency were analyzed during 5-min bins. Given that the frequency of events (Freq) measures the probability of neurotransmitter release and the amplitude (Amp) measures the response of postsynaptic site to the released neurotransmitter, their product gives the synaptic driving force of the neurotransmitters involved [29, 30]. Thus, we calculated synaptic drive as previously reported [29] using the formula:
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS v24 software. Behavioral data (percent abdominal constrictions) were analyzed using one-way or two-way ANOVAs, corrected for multiple comparisons, with between-subject factors of social context or drug. c-Fos (neurons/mm2) and p-GR labeling (neurons/mm2) were averaged for each animal across 2–3 tissue sections, and then group means calculated. In most experiments, the vehicle-treated alone, stranger, and cagemate conditions were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA to compare cagemate and stranger responses directly. In experiments where the effects of metyrapone were assessed, cagemates were not inputted into the two-way ANOVA because of the unbalanced experimental design. The thermal stimuli experiment was analyzed using a mixed two-way ANOVA with the between-subject factor of social context and the repeated measure factor of phase of testing (baseline, social-no pain, and social-pain). For all analyses, significance was set at p < 0.05. Tukey’s post-hoc tests were conducted; significant comparisons are only shown for analogous drug-treated groups between (i.e., saline-alone vs. saline-strangers) or within (i.e. saline-strangers vs. metyrapone-strangers) the social context.
Results
c-Fos expression following social interactions and pain
As shown in Fig. 1a, writhing behavior (i.e., % of positive samples with a “writhe”) in the dyadic condition increased significantly in cagemate, but not stranger pairs, replicating our previous findings [11]. To determine whether writhing behavior was synchronized, as described elsewhere [7], we calculated the joint probability of total writhing, expressed as a percentage of the group mean. In line with contagion effects, writhing co-occurred in time more in cagemate dyads than stranger dyads (Fig. 1b). Further, cagemate, but not stranger dyads, exhibited highly correlated writhing behavior during the abdominal constriction test (Fig. 1c). In general, observed behaviors other than writhing were similar across all conditions during social interactions. However, there was a higher incidence of hair-nibbling between stranger pairs (Supplementary Fig. 1), which may reflect an attempt to establish social dominance among unfamiliar mice [31].
To understand the neural basis for reduced writhing behavior in stranger dyads when compared with cagemate dyads, a subset of mice from each social condition was selected for brain region-specific mapping of c-Fos expression. We considered several brain areas known to participate in pain modulation, social cognition, and threat, including the mPFC, ACC, and amygdala [6, 32]. There was a significant main effect of social context (alone vs. cagemate vs. stranger) for the prelimbic subdivision of the mPFC (Fig. 1d–f) and the ACC (Fig. 1d–f), with no other brain region reaching statistical significance (All Fs <2.086, all ps > 0.144; Supplementary Fig. 2). Fos labeling was significantly increased in stranger dyads for both brain regions compared to mice tested alone and cagemate dyads, suggesting enhanced neural activity within these brain regions (Fig. 1f). In the stranger condition, there was a significant negative correlation between writhing behavior and c-Fos labeling for both the prelimbic (Fig. 1g) and the ACC (Fig. 1h). There was also a significant positive correlation between writhing behavior and c-Fos labeling in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) of the hypothalamus (Fig. 1i). However, cell-type-specific activation of these regions was not measured; therefore, it remains unknown whether certain cell-types or receptor populations are involved in inhibiting pain behaviors in strangers.
Glucocorticoid receptor activity is enhanced in the prelimbic cortex of stranger dyads
Since we previously showed that stress blocks pain contagion in strangers [11], we next sought to identify whether social interactions with a stranger mouse were sufficient to enhance glucocorticoid receptor activity in specific brain regions, and determine whether blocking glucocorticoid synthesis reversed these changes. Mice were pre-treated with the glucocorticoid synthesis inhibitor metyrapone (50 mg/kg, s.c.), at a dose that enhances co-writhing in stranger dyads [11, 33]. As shown in Fig. 2a, metyrapone enhanced writhing in stranger dyads compared with vehicle-treated dyads, replicating our previous work [11]. Metyrapone also enhanced the co-occurrence of writhing behavior (Fig. 2b), and metyrapone-treated dyads exhibited highly correlated writhing behavior (Fig. 2c). Corticosterone levels were not higher in stranger dyads than in cagemate dyads or mice tested alone (Fig. 2d), but metyrapone reduced blood corticosterone levels in both the alone and stranger condition (Fig. 2d). To assess whether metyrapone specifically reduced GR activity in the brain, we measured GR phosphorylation (p-GR) at Ser(211), a known biomarker for GR activation in vivo [34] in regions identified by our c-Fos analysis (i.e., prelimbic cortex, ACC) and the insula, a region important for vicarious pain responses in humans [35]. Of the regions examined, only the prelimbic cortex of stranger mice exhibited more p-GR staining than the alone condition and cagemate dyads, which was abolished by pre-treatment with metyrapone (Fig. 2e, f). The presence of a stranger mouse did not enhance p-GR in the ACC (Fig. 2g, h) or insula (Fig. 2g, h), but metyrapone reduced p-GR in these regions in stranger dyads (Fig. 2i, j). Immunoblotting supported these data, where stranger dyads showed increased p-GR in the mPFC (but not other regions) (Supplementary Fig. 3), which was significantly reduced by metyrapone pre-treatment (Supplementary Fig. 4) and negatively correlated with corticosterone in saline-treated stranger dyads (r = −0.588, p < 0.05; Supplementary Fig. 5). In the absence of a nociceptive stimulus, blood corticosterone and p-GR in the prelimbic cortex were not increased in stranger dyads indicating that social context and nociceptive processes interact to enhance GR activity, and potentially modulate writhing behavior within a social context (Supplementary Fig. 6).
Targeted inhibition of glucocorticoid receptors in the prelimbic cortex reveals pain contagion in stranger dyads
To test whether GR activity specifically within the prelimbic cortex of stranger mice modulates pain expression within a social context, we bilaterally microinjected RU 486, a GR antagonist, into the prelimbic cortex of stranger dyads. Figure 3a illustrates the microinjection sites for each surgery, color-coded depending on whether vehicle or RU 486 was administered. Strikingly, inhibition of GRs in the prelimbic cortex of stranger dyads enhanced pain contagion but did not change writhing behavior in mice tested alone (Fig. 3b). In contrast, RU 486 microinjection into the ACC did not enhance pain contagion in strangers relative to vehicle-injected controls (Fig. 3c). In some mice (n = 3), cannula placement was located in the infralimbic cortex, where RU 486 did not enhance writhing behavior in stranger dyads (Supplementary Fig 7). Finally, to show that increased GR activity within the prelimbic cortex suppressed pain contagion, we activated GRs in the prelimbic cortex of cagemate dyads with bilateral microinfusions of corticosterone (Fig. 3d). As shown in Fig. 3e, prelimbic microinfusions of corticosterone blocked pain contagion in cagemate dyads. Representative images for the spread of a fluorescently labeled ligand microinjected into the prelimbic cortex are shown in Supplementary Fig 8. Together, these results suggest that GR activity in the prelimbic cortex bidirectionally modulates pain contagion within a social context.
Increased synaptic drive of prelimbic neurons in stranger dyads and reversal by metyrapone pre-treatment
We next sought to determine whether neuronal function was altered in the prelimbic cortex of mice following social interactions with another mouse in pain and assess whether pre-treatment with metyrapone normalized this activity. Thus, we used slice electrophysiology and recorded spontaneous synaptic transmission in layer II/III of the prelimbic cortex immediately following behavioral testing. For each condition, four mice were used with sample sizes ranging between 9 and 11 cells/group. Representative traces for sEPSCs (Fig. 4a) and sIPSCs (Fig. 4b) in the prelimbic cortex after behavioral testing are shown for each of the social conditions with or without metyrapone (50 mg/kg, s.c.) pre-treatment. The frequency, but not the amplitude of sEPSCs, was significantly increased in stranger dyads when compared with mice tested alone (Fig. 4b, c), and metyrapone pre-treatment reversed this increase (Fig. 4b, c). There was no effect for social context or metyrapone pre-treatment on sIPSC frequency (Fig. 4e); however, sIPSC amplitude was increased by metyrapone treatment but only in stranger dyads (Fig. 4f). The increased sIPSC amplitude in metyrapone-treated strangers was comparable to that of cagemate dyads (t19 = 1.78, p > 0.05). To better understand the impact of social context and metyrapone pre-treatment on synaptic transmission, we calculated the synaptic drive, an overall measure of synaptic functioning for each neuron. Consequently, synaptic drive was enhanced in stranger dyads, and this was reversed by metyrapone-treatment (Fig. 4g).
Social interactions affect the expression of pain differently in cagemate and stranger mice
In stranger mice, we reasoned that enhanced GR activity and synaptic functioning in the prelimbic cortex might reflect the detection of a social threat, which adaptively may work to suppress overt pain behavior in the presence of an unfamiliar conspecific. Since social threat leads to decreased pain expression, but a simultaneous increase in reported pain intensity [15, 16], we assessed whether pain behavior was increased in strangers following removal from dyadic conditions. Thus, mice were individually placed on an inescapable thermal stimulus for 45 s [23] and assessed for nociceptive reflexes and affective-motivational responses at three timepoints during the testing session (baseline, following a social interaction (social-no pain) and acetic acid testing (i.e., social-pain) (Fig. 5a–e). The cumulative summation of nociceptive responses and cumulative duration of affective-motivational behavior was similar between mice in the cagemate and stranger groups during the 45 s baseline assessment (nociceptive: t17 = 1.09, p = 0.29; affective-motivational behavior: t17 = 1.44, p = 0.17, Fig. 5a–e). Nociceptive reflexes were increased in stranger dyads following social-pain; however, metyrapone pre-treatment did not change nociceptive responding in stranger dyads (Fig. 5a–c). Also, affective-motivational behaviors were increased in stranger dyads following social-no pain and social-pain, which were prevented with metyrapone treatment (Fig. 5a, d, e). In addition, we examined the latency to first response (i.e., the most commonly used hotplate metric) for either nociceptive (i.e., flinch) or affective-motivational behavior (i.e., hind paw lick). The latency to hindpaw flinch was not significantly different between the conditions at any phase of testing (Supplementary Fig. 9a); however, the latency to hindpaw lick was lower in stranger dyads following social-no pain and social-pain with metyrapone treatment reversing this trend (Supplementary Fig. 9b).
Discussion
The importance of brain regions such as the medial prefrontal cortex [10], anterior cingulate cortex [4, 5], and insular cortex [36] are not only critical for rodent empathy but also human empathy [37]. This suggests conserved neuronal mechanisms are likely to be found between the species. The experiments described here identify the prelimbic subdivision of the medial prefrontal cortex as an important site for bidirectionally modulating pain contagion in mice. Glucocorticoid receptor activity within the prelimbic cortex modulated writhing behavior when a social partner was present but did not when mice were tested alone. We further showed using electrophysiology, that following a social interaction with a stranger in pain, there was increased excitatory synaptic transmission in the prelimbic cortex of mice while pre-treatment with metyrapone reversed these changes. Removing stranger mice from dyadic testing revealed latent pain behavior when placed singly on an inescapable thermal surface, which was reduced by metyrapone.
Socially affective behaviors like pain contagion [7, 11], fear learning [5, 38], social approach [39, 40], and stress contagion [41]—especially in mice—are largely dependent on familiarity. In particular, pain contagion is determined by the nature of the social relationship [7], type of social interaction [14, 42], and pain status of those interacting [39]. We previously reported that social stress prevented pain contagion in stranger dyads through greater HPA activity, but these observations were based on behavioral pharmacology [11]. The current data now show that plasma corticosterone levels were not higher in stranger dyads than in cagemate dyads or mice tested alone, but rather strangers have a different pattern of GR activity in prefrontal cortical regions. This activity is further highlighted by increased excitatory synaptic transmission in the prelimbic cortex of stranger mice following dyadic pain testing and the subsequent reversal of this effect with metyrapone pre-treatment. As different stressors result in unique levels of catecholamines (noradrenaline and adrenaline) and corticosterone release, GR activity patterns may differ based on the type of stressor (social vs. non-social). While corticosterone is just one physiological marker of stress reactivity, it is an important endpoint of the HPA axis and an index of limbic activity [43]. Social stress would expectedly increase limbic activity at the level of stress-appraisal or social threat [44], which would involve brain regions important for social cognition that would, in turn, trigger the hypothalamus [45]. Further, our results align with previous observations showing that behavioral stressors cause long-lasting changes in glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the mPFC [29, 46].
Analysis of c-Fos immunoreactivity across several limbic ROIs identified only two regions (prelimbic cortex and ACC), where c-Fos expression was elevated in the stranger, but not cagemate dyads. Activity within these regions typically serves as important areas for acute and chronic pain modulation [47, 48] and social threat detection [6, 29, 49]. While c-Fos expression was increased in the prelimbic and ACC of stranger dyads, only inhibition of GRs within the prelimbic cortex revealed pain contagion. Microinjections of RU-486 into the prelimbic, but not the ACC revealed pain contagion in stranger dyads, while corticosterone microinjections into the prelimbic cortex blocked pain contagion in cagemate dyads. Given the proximity of the prelimbic and ACC (approximately 350 μm) and highly lipophilic properties of RU 486 and corticosterone (i.e., ability to diffuse from prelimbic to ACC), it could be possible for the ligands to traverse cytoarchitectural boundaries. To provide confidence that the ligands did not cross the prelimbic and ACC boundary, we microinjected a fluorescent ligand (muscimol, another highly lipophilic substance) into the prelimbic allowing us to visualize injection spread, which was mostly contained to the prelimbic cortex.
We suspect that the prelimbic cortex may operate as a gate to either allow or suppress the expression of nociceptive behavior in the presence of a social partner. In humans, the dorsal medial PFC—analogous to the rodent prelimbic cortex—is consistently activated by social behaviors requiring the perception of dissimilar others [49]. Another possibility is that centrally released glucocorticoids blocked pain contagion in stranger dyads through selective activation of the prelimbic cortex. For instance, corticotrophin-releasing factor (CRF) is known to play a role in anxiogenic responses through a mPFC mechanism [50], and exposure to predator scent leads to CRF receptor 1-dependent changes in synaptic function, which are specific to the prelimbic cortex [29]. However, the central release of glucocorticoids is not a likely mechanism because systemic metyrapone reduced p-GR in the prelimbic and normalized synaptic drive in stranger dyads. GRs within the prelimbic cortex act as a major site for feedback of HPA responses because local glucocorticoid infusions inhibit anticipatory (but not reflexive) responses to stressors [51]. Activation of GRs within the prelimbic cortex of stranger dyads—when both mice were injected with acetic acid—may suggest enhanced negative feedback to the HPA axis in strangers, which would counteract any physiological corticosterone increase in strangers. However, in the absence of a nociceptive stimulus, p-GR was similar for all social conditions suggesting that pain contagion was inhibited through a specific feedback mechanism. The anticipatory nature of the unfamiliar social interaction may increase the neural control of innate defense programs that would suppress pain expression in the presence of a social threat [52].
Since displaying pain is a liability for many animals because it signals weakness [53], we explored whether stranger dyads may be suppressing overt pain expression while exhibiting sensitivity to other pain modalities. The influence of social threat on overt pain behavior (i.e., acetic acid writhing) was separated from nociceptive and affective-motivational pain responses by momentarily removing mice from the dyad and testing them on an inescapable thermal apparatus. In strangers, nociceptive behavior in response to noxious heat on the hotplate was increased immediately after the acetic acid test, while affective behaviors were increased following social interactions and immediately after acetic acid testing. Increased nocifensive behavior on the hotplate following the acetic acid test may be due to a latent effect of acetic acid on the mouse’s nociceptive responses as we have shown that 0.6% acetic acid—the concentration used in the current study—increased thermal pain sensitivity [54]. However, we would like to point out that all mice in the current study received acetic acid before hotplate testing and observed differences were only noted within stranger dyads. Further, metyrapone pre-treatment reversed affective-motivational behaviors in strangers following social interactions but did not affect nociceptive responses. Since metyrapone also reversed increased synaptic drive in the prelimbic and targeted inhibition of GRs in the prelimbic revealed pain contagion in strangers, we believe that our data collectively support the notion that GR activity in the prelimbic cortex modulates social interaction-induced changes in nociceptive behavior.
In sum, we find that GRs in the prelimbic cortex are critical for modulating pain responses within a social context. Stranger mice do not display pain contagion but following removal from the social context show enhanced nociceptive reflexes. This enhanced nociceptive processing is consistent with pain processing during social threat in humans [16]. The inhibition or activation of GRs in the prelimbic cortex enhanced or inhibited pain contagion in stranger and cagemate dyads, respectively. We also identified enhanced excitatory synaptic transmission in the prelimbic cortex of stranger dyads. Together this may indicate that increased glucocorticoid input increases synaptic drive in the prelimbic cortex of strangers to reduce pain when in the presence of a social partner but may enhance latent nociceptive sensitivity once removed from the dyad. In the future, it will be essential to determine whether there are specific downstream targets for activated cells within the prelimbic cortex, that modulate pain contagion and compare specific cellular populations that become activated during the observation of a familiar or unfamiliar conspecific in pain.
Funding and disclosure
This research was supported by the Ontario Ministry of Innovation (Early Career Researcher Award to LJM), Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NMF, LJM), Canadian Foundation for Innovation (NMF, LJM), and the Canada Research Chairs program (LJM). All authors have nothing to disclose.
Supplementary information
Author contributions
LJM conceived of the project. NKL, NMF, LJM designed research. NKL, SDG, SS, SK, FW, HT, MS, NMF, and LJM performed research; NKL, SDG, SS, NMF, and LJM analyzed data; and NKL, NMF, and LJM wrote the paper.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information accompanies this paper at (10.1038/s41386-020-00912-4).
References
- 1.Sivaselvachandran S, Acland EL, Abdallah S, Martin LJ. Behavioral and mechanistic insight into rodent empathy. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;91:130–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Acland EL, Lidhar NK, Martin LJ. Bridging the gap between people and animals: the roots of social behavior and its relationship to pain. In: Vervoort T, Karos K, Trost Z, Prkachin KM, editors. Social and interpersonal dynamics in pain: we don’t suffer alone. Cham: Springer; 2018. p. 197–217.
- 3.Sivaselvachandran S, Sivaselvachandran M, Abdallah S, Martin LJ. Lost in translation: improving our understanding of pain empathy. In: Meyza KZ, Knapska E, editors. Neuronal correlates of empathy. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2018. p. 123–35.
- 4.Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FB, Young LJ. Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science. 2016;351:375–8. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Jeon D, Kim S, Chetana M, Jo D, Ruley HE, Lin SY, et al. Observational fear learning involves affective pain system and Cav1.2 Ca2+ channels in ACC. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13:482–8. doi: 10.1038/nn.2504. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Eilam D, Izhar R, Mort J. Threat detection: behavioral practices in animals and humans. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35:999–1006. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Langford DJ, Crager SE, Shehzad Z, Smith SB, Sotocinal SG, Levenstadt JS, et al. Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science. 2006;312:1967–70. doi: 10.1126/science.1128322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Smith ML, Hostetler CM, Heinricher MM, Ryabinin AE. Social transfer of pain in mice. Sci Adv. 2016;2:e1600855. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.1600855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Craig KD. Social communication model of pain. Pain. 2015;156:1198–9. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Li Z, Lu YF, Li CL, Wang Y, Sun W, He T, et al. Social interaction with a cagemate in pain facilitates subsequent spinal nociception via activation of the medial prefrontal cortex in rats. Pain. 2014;155:1253–61. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Martin LJ, Hathaway G, Isbester K, Mirali S, Acland EL, Niederstrasser N, et al. Reducing social stress elicits emotional contagion of pain in mouse and human strangers. Curr Biol. 2015;25:326–32. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pitcher MG-C, Gonzalez-Cano R, Vincent K, Lehmann M, Cobos EJ, Coderre, T et al. Mild social stress in mice produces opioid-mediated analgesia in visceral but not somatic pain states. J Pain. 2017;18: 716–725. [DOI] [PubMed]
- 13.Novotny M, Harvey S, Jemiolo B, Alberts J. Synthetic pheromones that promote inter-male aggression in mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1985;82:2059–61. doi: 10.1073/pnas.82.7.2059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Langford DJ, Tuttle AH, Briscoe C, Harvey-Lewis C, Baran I, Gleeson P, et al. Varying perceived social threat modulates pain behavior in male mice. J Pain. 2011;12:125–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Peeters PA, Vlaeyen JW. Feeling more pain, yet showing less: the influence of social threat on pain. J Pain. 2011;12:1255–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2011.07.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Karos K, Meulders A, Goubert L, Vlaeyen JWS. Hide your pain: social threat increases pain reports and aggression, but reduces facial pain expression and empathy. J Pain. 2019;21:334–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2019.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Godoy LD, Rossignoli MT, Delfino-Pereira P, Garcia-Cairasco N, de Lima Umeoka EH. A comprehensive overview on stress neurobiology: basic concepts and clinical implications. Front Behav Neurosci. 2018;12:127. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Diorio D, Viau V, Meaney MJ. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex (cingulate gyrus) in the regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. J Neurosci. 1993;13:3839–47. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.13-09-03839.1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.McEwen BS, De Kloet ER, Rostene W. Adrenal steroid receptors and actions in the nervous system. Physiol Rev. 1986;66:1121–88. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1986.66.4.1121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Dominguez G, Faucher P, Henkous N, Krazem A, Piérard C, Béracochéa D. Stress induced a shift from dorsal hippocampus to prefrontal cortex dependent memory retrieval: role of regional corticosterone. Front Behav Neurosci. 2014;8:166. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Franklin TB, Silva BA, Perova Z, Marrone L, Masferrer ME, Zhan Y, et al. Prefrontal cortical control of a brainstem social behavior circuit. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20:260–70. doi: 10.1038/nn.4470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McKlveen JM, Myers B, Herman JP. The medial prefrontal cortex: coordinator of autonomic, neuroendocrine and behavioural responses to stress. J Neuroendocrinol. 2015;27:446–56. doi: 10.1111/jne.12272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Corder G, Tawfik VL, Wang D, Sypek EI, Low SA, Dickinson JR, et al. Loss of mu opioid receptor signaling in nociceptors, but not microglia, abrogates morphine tolerance without disrupting analgesia. Nat Med. 2017;23:164–73. doi: 10.1038/nm.4262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Corder G, Ahanonu B, Grewe BF, Wang D, Schnitzer MJ, Scherrer G. An amygdalar neural ensemble that encodes the unpleasantness of pain. Science. 2019;363:276–81. doi: 10.1126/science.aap8586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Manglik A, Lin H, Aryal DK, McCorvy JD, Dengler D, Corder G, et al. Structure-based discovery of opioid analgesics with reduced side effects. Nature. 2016;537:185–90. doi: 10.1038/nature19112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Fournier NM, Botterill JJ, Marks WN, Guskjolen AJ, Kalynchuk LE. Impaired recruitment of seizure-generated neurons into functional memory networks of the adult dentate gyrus following long-term amygdala kindling. Exp Neurol. 2013;244:96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2012.11.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Arango-Lievano M, Lambert WM, Bath KG, Garabedian MJ, Chao MV, Jeanneteau F. Neurotrophic-priming of glucocorticoid receptor signaling is essential for neuronal plasticity to stress and antidepressant treatment. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2015;112:15737–42. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509045112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Franklin KBJ, Paxinos G. The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 4th edition. Amsterdam: Academic Press, an imprint of Elsevier; 2013.
- 29.Hwa LS, Neira S, Pina MM, Pati D, Calloway R, Kash TL. Predator odor increases avoidance and glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the prelimbic cortex via corticotropin-releasing factor receptor 1 signaling. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2019;44:766–75. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0279-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar] [Retracted]
- 30.Guillamon A, McLaughlin DW, Rinzel J. Estimation of synaptic conductances. J Physiol Paris. 2006;100:31–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2006.09.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Long SY. Hair-nibbling and Whisker-trimming as indicators of social hierarchy in mice. Anim Behav. 1972;20:10. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(72)80167-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lee E, Rhim I, Lee JW, Ghim JW, Lee S, Kim E, et al. Enhanced neuronal activity in the medial prefrontal cortex during social approach behavior. J Neurosci. 2016;36:6926–36. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0307-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Carrillo M, Migliorati F, Bruls R, Han Y, Heinemans M, Pruis I, et al. Repeated witnessing of conspecifics in pain: effects on emotional contagion. PLoS ONE. 2015;10:e0136979. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Rogatsky I, Logan SK, Garabedian MJ. Antagonism of glucocorticoid receptor transcriptional activation by the c-Jun N-terminal kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:2050–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.5.2050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Corradi-Dell’Acqua C, Tusche A, Vuilleumier P, Singer T. Cross-modal representations of first-hand and vicarious pain, disgust and fairness in insular and cingulate cortex. Nat Commun. 2016;7:10904. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10904. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rogers-Carter MM, Varela JA, Gribbons KB, Pierce AF, McGoey MT, Ritchey M, et al. Insular cortex mediates approach and avoidance responses to social affective stimuli. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:404–14. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0071-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Singer T, Lamm C. The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2009;1156:81–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Knapska E, Nikolaev E, Boguszewski P, Walasek G, Blaszczyk J, Kaczmarek L, et al. Between-subject transfer of emotional information evokes specific pattern of amygdala activation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2006;103:3858–62. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0511302103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Langford DJ, Tuttle AH, Brown K, Deschenes S, Fischer DB, Mutso A, et al. Social approach to pain in laboratory mice. Soc Neurosci. 2010;5:163–70. doi: 10.1080/17470910903216609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rogers-Carter MM, Djerdjaj A, Culp AR, Elbaz JA, Christianson JP. Familiarity modulates social approach toward stressed conspecifics in female rats. PLoS ONE. 2018;13:e0200971. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sterley TL, Baimoukhametova D, Fuzesi T, Zurek AA, Daviu N, Rasiah NP, et al. Social transmission and buffering of synaptic changes after stress. Nat Neurosci. 2018;21:393–403. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0044-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Feng CL, Li ZH, Feng X, Wang LL, Tian TX, Luo YJ. Social hierarchy modulates neural responses of empathy for pain. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2016;11:485–95. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.McEwen BS, Weiss JM, Schwartz LS. Uptake of corticosterone by rat brain and its concentration by certain limbic structures. Brain Res. 1969;16:227–41. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(69)90096-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Gable SL, Hilmert CJ, Lieberman MD. Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. Neuroimage. 2007;35:1601–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Sesack SR, Deutch AY, Roth RH, Bunney BS. Topographical organization of the efferent projections of the medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: an anterograde tract-tracing study with Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. J Comp Neurol. 1989;290:213–42. doi: 10.1002/cne.902900205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Yuen EY, Liu W, Karatsoreos IN, Feng J, McEwen BS, Yan Z. Acute stress enhances glutamatergic transmission in prefrontal cortex and facilitates working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106:14075–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0906791106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ong WY, Stohler CS, Herr DR. Role of the prefrontal cortex in pain processing. Mol Neurobiol. 2019;56:1137–66. doi: 10.1007/s12035-018-1130-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bliss TV, Collingridge GL, Kaang BK, Zhuo M. Synaptic plasticity in the anterior cingulate cortex in acute and chronic pain. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17:485–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bicks LK, Koike H, Akbarian S, Morishita H. Prefrontal cortex and social cognition in mouse and man. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1805. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Miguel TT, Gomes KS, Nunes-de-Souza RL. Tonic modulation of anxiety-like behavior by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) type 1 receptor (CRF1) within the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in male mice: role of protein kinase A (PKA) Horm Behav. 2014;66:247–56. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Akana SF, Chu A, Soriano L, Dallman MF. Corticosterone exerts site-specific and state-dependent effects in prefrontal cortex and amygdala on regulation of adrenocorticotropic hormone, insulin and fat depots. J Neuroendocrinol. 2001;13:625–37. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2001.00676.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Herman JP, Figueiredo H, Mueller NK, Ulrich-Lai Y, Ostrander MM, Choi DC, et al. Central mechanisms of stress integration: hierarchical circuitry controlling hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2003;24:151–80. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2003.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.de C Williams CWAC, Gallagher E, Fidalgo AR, Bentley PJ. Pain expressiveness and altruistic behavior: an exploration using agent-based modeling. Pain. 2016;157:759–68. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Tansley SN, Macintyre LC, Diamond L, Sotocinal SG, George N, Meluban L, et al. Conditioned pain modulation in rodents can feature hyperalgesia or hypoalgesia depending on test stimulus intensity. Pain. 2019;160:784–92. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.