Abstract
Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID) include structural and functional blood vessel injuries linked to poor neurocognitive outcomes. Smoking might indirectly increase the likelihood of cognitive impairment by exacerbating vascular disease risks. Sex disparities in VCID have been reported, however, few studies have assessed the sex-specific relationships between smoking and memory performance and with contradictory results. We investigated the associations between sex, smoking, and cardiovascular disease with verbal learning and memory function. Using MindCrowd, an observational web-based cohort of ~ 70,000 people aged 18–85, we investigated whether sex modifies the relationship between smoking and cardiovascular disease with verbal memory performance. We found significant interactions in that smoking is associated with verbal learning performance more in women and cardiovascular disease more in men across a wide age range. These results suggest that smoking and cardiovascular disease may impact verbal learning and memory throughout adulthood differently for men and women.
Subject terms: Cognitive ageing, Human behaviour
Introduction
Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID) include blood vessel injuries that can cause significant changes to memory, thinking, and behavior1. Vascular diseases are also associated with increased risk for Alzheimer’s disease (AD)2, which is the 6th leading cause of death in the US with increasing numbers and financial tolls3,4. Vascular-related dementia is the second most common cause of cognitive decline, with only AD-related dementia being more prevalent5. Several extensive cohort-based studies have found that cardiovascular disease risk factors such as tobacco smoking are associated with cognitive decline and increased risk of dementia in older age6.
Despite early reports suggesting that smoking tobacco might have beneficial effects on cognition7,8 and a reduced risk of dementia9, more recent evidence clearly suggests that active smoking has neurotoxic effects on the brain10–12 and is associated with a doubling of dementia risk for older adults13. Smoking tobacco might indirectly increase risk of cognitive impairment by exacerbating the subclinical risks associated with underlying vascular disease. Smoking causes vascular damage, including carotid artery disease, atherosclerotic plaque formation, increased platelet aggregation, compromised endothelial cell function, arterial stiffness and increased systolic blood pressure, which all contribute to stroke risk14–16. The presence of smoking with hypertension is one of the greatest risk factors for acute myocardial infarction and stroke, according to a meta-analysis of 10 cohort studies which included more than 60,000 people17. Tobacco smoking is also a major risk factor for cardiovascular disease and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD), both of which can decrease cognitive function18. Importantly, prevalence rates of any dementia and AD may be greater in women than men19–21. A recent study found that women had a stronger association between vascular risk factors and worse cognition than men in a middle-aged Hispanic/Latino population22. However, it is currently unknown if the effects of smoking on verbal learning and memory function are different between men and women.
Several studies have found an association between smoking, cognitive decline, and dementia12,23,24. Nevertheless, the majority of these studies do not assess biological sex as a main variable of interest but instead use sex as a covariate. As a covariate, the variance due to sex is only being controlled for and not directly explored. Thus, the question whether smoking affects cognition in men and women differently remains unresolved. One methodological issue that might explain why so few studies have directly examined this important question pertains to the need for relatively large sample sizes to be adequately powered for interaction, or moderation, analyses. It is estimated that detecting an interaction requires at least four times the sample size than needed to detect a main effect25,26.
The few studies that assess a smoking by sex interaction or test women and men separately, have typically been underpowered and have found contradicting results. Some studies suggest no sex effect or that smoking may affect men’s cognition more strongly than women’s12,13,27–29; however, other studies have found a larger effect of smoking on cognition in women30,31. It does appear, however, that smoking often impacts women’s risk to a greater extent for diseases that occur outside of the central nervous system. A meta-analysis of more than 2.4 million people suggests that, compared with nonsmokers, women who smoke have a 25% greater relative risk of coronary heart disease than men who smoke, independent of other cardiovascular risk factors32. Women smokers may also have a greater relative risk of lung cancer than men who smoke33. Others have found the effects of smoking in various disease models such as bone fragility and Crohn’s disease differ between men and women34,35. Furthermore, there appears to be a sex difference in the behavioral response to the acute effect of smoking in prepulse inhibition, a measure of startle reflex36. Importantly, human and animal studies point to many structural and functional sex differences in nicotinic acetylcholinergic brain physiology (for review see37). Collectively, these data suggest sex-specific effects should be considered a high priority of inquiry when assessing the relationship between smoking status and cognition.
The primary objective of this study was to determine whether biological sex moderates the relationship between smoking and memory performance in healthy adults aged 18–85 years old. We performed down sampling analyses to estimate the minimum sample size required to detect this interaction. In addition, given the known deleterious effects of smoking on the cardiovascular system and the established relationship between cardiovascular disease and cognitive impairment and dementia38–41, we further tested if sex moderates the combined influence of diabetes, heart disease, hypertension, and stroke history on memory performance. We hypothesize that both smoking and an integrated cardiovascular disease index would have a greater detrimental relationship with paired-associate learning in women compared to men. For this study, we leveraged MindCrowd (www.mindcrowd.org), a web-based cohort of more than 70,000 persons aged 18 to 85 years from which memory and demographic data were collected.
Results
The entire demographic, health, lifestyle, and medical variable composition of our study cohort can be found in Supplemental Table S1. Additionally, chi square test of differences between the smoker and non-smoker groups for all covariates can be found in Supplemental Table S2. Briefly, the groups significantly differed in all covariates, which is expected based on our large sample sizes.
Multivariate linear regression
Smoking
In women and men 18–85 years old our linear model (LM) revealed a significant omnibus effect of smoking (F(30, 81,670) = 560. 58, p = 0e + 00; Fig. 1). The model also revealed a significant sex × smoking interaction (β = 0.99, SE = 0.22, p = 4e−6). Simple effects analyses revealed that women’s memory is negatively related to smoking (β = − 1.01 word pairs, SE = 0.14, p = 7.83e−13; Fig. 2) whereas men’s memory was not significantly related (β = − 0.27 word pairs, SE = 0.18, p = 0.135: Fig. 2).
Cardiovascular disease
In the whole cohort, 77.4% of women and 78.1% of men had a CVD sum of 0, 17.9% of women and 16.6% of men had a CVD sum of 1, 4.1% of women and 4.3% of men had a CVD sum of 2, and 0.6% of women and 1% of men had a CVD sum score of 3+ . In women and men 18–85 years old our linear model revealed a significant omnibus effect of cardiovascular disease (F(31, 81,669) = 542.88, p = 0e+00: Fig. 3). The model also revealed a significant sex × cardiovascular disease interaction in the 1 group (β = − 0.84 word pairs, SE = 0.14, p = 6.41e−9), 2 group (β = − 1.38 word pairs, SE = 0.27, p = 2.08e−7), and 3+ group (β = − 1.64 word pairs, SE = 0.54, p = 0.002). Simple effects analyses revealed the negative relationship between cardiovascular disease and memory had slightly larger effect sizes in men compared to women in the 1 group (men: β = − 0.64 word pairs, SE = 0.12, p = 1.22e−7; women: β = − 0.41 word pairs, SE = 0.09, p = 3.74e−6), the 2 group (men: β = − 0.99 word pairs, SE = 0.23, p = 1.2e−5; women: β = − 0.67 word pairs, SE = 0.16, p = 3.18e−5), and the 3 group (men: β = − 1.80 word pairs, SE = 0.41, p = 1.3e−5; women: β = − 1.27 word pairs, SE = 0.35, p = 0.0003; Fig. 4).
Propensity score matching
When collapsed across age, PSM suggested there is no relationship between smoking and memory in men (β = − 0.21 (− 0.62 to 0.19, 95% credible interval)) and a negative relationship in women (β = − 0.54 (− 0.14 to − 0.93, 95% credible interval)) (Fig. 5).
Down sampling
The horizontal red line in Fig. 6A indicates the effect size estimated by the largest-sized sample. Green filled circles indicate an individual down-sampled comparison that resulted in a statistically significant association (p < 0.05). We performed 1000 of these simulations at each down-sampled size. At a cohort size of approximately 10,000 samples, 50% of down-sampled comparisons resulted in the observation of a significant smoking × sex interaction (Fig. 6B). Even at the largest sample size shown, there are still some observed down-sampled comparisons that result in a non-significant association (black circles). These data illustrate why there is a concern about small sample sizes and their ability to result in misestimated β values and the probability of type 2 errors. Additionally, note that at sample sizes below 6000 it is possible to observe significant associations in the opposite direction of the actual effect (e.g. smoking enhances performance).
Artificial error introduction
A Monte Carlo simulation (Fig. 7) was used to determine the effect of introducing artificial error (in addition to any real self-report error already in the data). The error was introduced in 1% increments (x-axis) by randomizing smoker status. Each box represents 10,000 model simulations, and plotted are the p-values for each simulation. The red line represents a significance level of α = 0.05. At 0% simulated error, we report our measured p-value. In the whole cohort (Fig. 7A) and women specifically (Fig. 7B), we measured a significant effect in > 90% of simulations after 10% additional self-report error was introduced. For the sex × smoking interaction term, 75% of simulations were statistically significant after 10% additional self-report error was introduced (Fig. 7C).
Permutations
From one million permutations performed for the main effect of smoking in the whole cohort (Fig. 8A) and the main effect of smoking in women only (Fig. 8B), not a single t-statistic was observed to be more extreme than our reported t-statistic. This suggests that the odds of our findings being observed due to chance alone is less than one in a million. However, our reported t-value for the men only analysis did overlap with values calculated in the permutation tests demonstrating confidence in our non-significant finding (Fig. 8C). Further, we ran the same permutation tests on the sex × cardiovascular interaction term and showed that our full model t-value did not overlap with any permutation test (Fig. 8D).
Discussion
In this large web-based study, we found current cigarette smoking and cardiovascular disease are associated with worse memory performance in adults as young as 18 years. Furthermore, we found significant sex-modification of these associations showing that the impact of smoking on verbal recall was worse in women whereas the impact of cardiovascular disease on memory performance was worse in men. These findings are important because according to the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, cigarette smoking is the leading cause of preventable disease and death in the United States and accounts for about 1 in every 5 deaths42. In 2018, nearly 14 of every 100 U.S. adults aged 18 years or older (13.7%) smoked cigarettes, which translates to about 34.2 million adult smokers43. In addition, cardiovascular disease is the leading cause of morbidity and mortality worldwide, and an important predictor of cognitive decline and VCID.
We found that sex modifies the relationship between smoking and verbal recall in that women are negatively affected to a larger degree than men. This finding is in agreement with studies that reported a larger effect of smoking on cognition in women30,31, but stands in contrast with other smaller studies that found no sex effect or that smoking might affect cognition more strongly in men12,13,27–29. These results also align well with studies that suggest smoking impacts coronary heart disease and lung cancer more in women than in men32,33. We used down sampling analyses to demonstrate the importance of large sample sizes to ensure reproducible interaction effects of our model. These analyses suggest that a study sample of at least 10,000 is needed to observe a significant sex by smoking interaction at least 50% of the time. This finding highlights the possibility that many previous studies may have been underpowered to find the interaction.
In the United States, men (15.6%) were more likely to be current cigarette smokers than women (12.0%) according to a 2018 report43 and we replicated this finding within our MindCrowd cohort (Fig. 9). In addition, women on average smoke fewer cigarettes per day and have lower salivary cotinine levels compared with men. However, smoking rates for women have increased relative to smoking rates for men in the US and the popularity of smoking in women from low to middle-income countries may be increasing44,45. This is of particular interest since there are higher numbers of Alzheimer’s disease cases in women than men19–21 and these data suggest that smoking could potentially accelerate these trends. While men may be at a slightly higher risk for VCID throughout most of the lifespan, some risk factors for VCID more adversely affect women such as preeclampsia, menopause, and hormone replacement46. Not only does the risk of dementia increase with age, but normative decreases in many cognitive abilities occur across the lifespan47, therefore a better understanding of modifiable contributors, such as smoking, to cognitive function is essential.
In addition to sex differences in smoking behaviors, sex differences in the cholinergic system are possible biological mechanistic explanations for why smoking may have a more substantial impact on cognition in women compared to men. Animal studies have shown both nicotine-exposed and non-nicotine exposed female rats exhibit higher cholinergic receptor (NAChR) densities than their male counterparts48. Since NAChR innervation influences several cognitive functions and neurotransmitters49, perhaps higher expression in women exacerbates the effects of smoking on cognition. However, it is not clear whether smoking-related changes in cognition are primarily due to nicotine exposure or the complex chemical makeup of cigarette tobacco and its additives42. This is becoming an important distinction as the prevalence of adult e-cigarette use/vaping increased from 2.8% in 2017 to 3.2% in 201843. However, since we did not ask about vaping specifically, further research is needed to monitor the relationship between e-cigarette use and memory performance since it is possible that the effects of e-cigarette use will differ compared to smoking tobacco due to the differing mix of chemical exposures.
Given the established relationships between smoking and cardiovascular disease, we also tested whether sex moderates the impact of cardiovascular disease on memory performance. Although much of the prior research in this domain has focused on cardiovascular risk scores (for review see6), we used number of CVD disease incidents as opposed to risk factors. Nevertheless, we can draw some comparisons between these investigations. The existing cross-sectional associations between cardiovascular disease risk and cognitive function in the literature are largely consistent with results obtained in this study between cardiovascular disease and memory performance. However, comparison with these findings for the sex effect is limited because of differences in study analyses. As with smoking studies, the majority of this research investigates sex as a covariate in the primary analyses instead of comparing men and women separately or investigating an interaction (for review see6). We found that sex moderates the relationship between cardiovascular disease and cognitive performance in that men are affected to a larger degree. However, the few studies that investigated men and women separately have found women’s cognitive performance to be slightly more impacted. For example, Kaffashian et al. investigated the Framingham cardiovascular risk profile and cognitive function and 10-year decline separately between men and women and reported larger effects in women across their cognitive batteries in 35–55 year-olds50. In a cohort of community-dwelling adults without clinical heart disease, the Framingham Cardiac Risk Score (FCRS) was associated with the rate of cognitive decline in women, but not men51. Yet another cohort of Mexican Americans demonstrated that higher predicted cardiovascular disease risk was associated with greater change in errors on multiple cognitive tests and that these associations were larger in women than men52. Lastly, a recent study found women had a higher association between vascular risk factors and worse cognition compared to men in a middle-aged Hispanic/Latino population22. It is unclear why we found larger effects in men, however, it could be due to the broad age range included in our study. The relationship between cardiovascular disease and cognitive function is primarily studied in older adults, with a few examples including 35 and older41,50,53,54, and one study including 18–30 year-olds55. Understanding the relationship between cardiovascular health and cognitive function in young adulthood may be necessary for understanding possible treatment and intervention opportunities.
Due to the large, widely available, and electronic nature of our study cohort, we rely on self-report answers to demographic, lifestyle, and health questions56. Current studies comparing self-report data given over the Internet versus data collected in-person show anywhere from a 0.3 to 20% discrepancy for height and weight measurements57,58. For some socially unacceptable measures (like smoking) internet self-report may actually have higher accuracy since the pressure to “perform” well in the presence of an investigator is removed when answering questions electronically59. To investigate the potential role that false-report error may play on our smoking effect, we re-analyzed the smoking effect after introducing additional error into the smoking self-report response. The additional error was added by randomly flipping the smoking response to various percentages of the cohort (stepwise from 1 to 10% of individuals) and re-analyzing the effect of smoking using our complete statistical model a total of 10,000 times for each error percentage. These results suggest it is unlikely that smoking self-report error is driving our results. Lastly, PAL was tested cross-sectionally in the cohort; therefore, determinations about the influence of collected factors on trajectories of change in performance across time within an individual subject are not possible. Additional longitudinal-based studies will be necessary to identify this class of variables.
There are limitations of this study to acknowledge. First, the smoking rate in MindCrowd is lower than national averages which may reflect the tendency of healthier people to participate in observational research60. However, since we used propensity matching to confirm the sex effect, this suggests our results are generalizable to the broader population. Next, the primary outcome measure was based on a single verbal memory and learning test with a ceiling and a floor effect. Using a measure with a more comprehensive score range may have tracked subtle differences between ages and our results may not be generalizable to different cognitive functions. Further, our study design is dependent on self-report of smoking and cardiovascular disease and we did not attempt to verify these with medical records. However, previous studies have reported discrepancies between self-reported smoking habits and serum cotinine concentration, a biomarker of nicotine absorption, especially in girls and women, suggesting that more women under-report smoking than do men61–63. A higher rate of inaccurate smoking status self-report by women in our dataset would have attenuated our results. This suggests that our results may actually underestimate the difference between men and women who smoke. Additionally, we were unable to assess a dose–response of smoking since we only asked for smoking status and did not collect requisite smoking history information in order to calculate pack years. Finally, the cross-sectional design of this study does not allow a causal conclusion and there may be other factors driving this association which were not measured (e.g. social class, diet, etc.). A longitudinal study design should follow to verify our results and assess a causal interpretation.
Despite these limitations, there are several advantages of using a large web-based study cohort. Our cohort includes a wide range of adults aged 18–85, which allowed us to assess the relationship between smoking and cardiovascular disease and verbal memory in the broadest single study age range used to date. The MindCrowd cohort has a considerable number of variables assessed, which enabled us to control for many potential confounding variables in addition to conducting propensity matching made possible due to the cohort size. Both regression and propensity analyses indicate that the independent effect of smoking over and above other health factors on verbal memory performance is robust in women. Propensity score matching attempts to mimic randomization by matching smokers and non-smokers on all observed covariates. Because smoking is correlated with many other lifestyle factors (e.g. education), propensity score matching helps isolate the effect of smoking on memory performance. Another advantage of using a web-based study design is the ability to use an identical study wide protocol as opposed to attempting to harmonize protocols and data across sites, which is often required when combining smaller, local cohorts. Furthermore, this cohort is geographically diverse with participants in both rural and urban settings, which allows a higher degree of generalizability. In addition to smoking status, we also explored the synergistic effects of cardiovascular disease on verbal learning and memory using a composite. Composites have the advantage of weighing multiple risks with a single summary variable and can be more sensitive and robust than single variables64. Therefore, composite scores may be more biologically relevant and have advantages in both clinical practice and cardiovascular research.
In summary, we report that sex moderates the relationship between smoking status and verbal learning and memory performance based on results from the largest study to date. Furthermore, we report down sampling tests that suggest the minimum sample required to dependably detect this interaction 50% of the time is 10,000. Based on these results, prior study sample sizes may have produced less reliable results due to small sample sizes. Our results highlight the importance of investigating sex as a variable of interest in understanding environmental influences on verbal learning and memory performance across development and aging. The results suggest smoking may impact women’s cognitive health to a greater degree than men.
Methods
Study participants
In January of 2013 we launched our observational study site at www.mindcrowd.org. Website visitors, who were 18 years or older, were asked to consent to our study before any data collection via an electronic consent form. Approval for this study was obtained from the Western Institutional Review Board (WIRB study number 1129241) and all experiments were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
As of March 17, 2020 MindCrowd has recruited 84,260 qualified participants from around the world aged 18–85 with 64.3% women and 35.7% men. An overrepresentation of women has been previously described in studies drawn from the general population65. Across the entire sample, 7.9% of participants reported being a current smoker (Fig. 9).
After consenting to the study and answering five demographic questions (age, sex, years of education, primary language, and country of residence), participants completed a web-based paired-associates learning (PAL) task. For this cognitive task, during the learning phase, participants were presented 12 word-pairs, one word-pair at a time (2 s/word-pair). During the recall phase, participants were presented with the first word of each pair and were asked to use their keyboard to type (i.e., recall) the missing word. This learning-recall procedure was repeated for two additional trials. Before beginning the task, each participant received one practice trial consisting of three word-pairs not contained in the 12 used during the test. Word-pairs were presented in different random orders during each learning and each recall phase. The same word pairs and orders of presentation were used for all participants. The dependent variable/criterion was the total number of correct word pairs entered across the three trials (i.e., 36 is a perfect score). Upon completing the PAL task, participants were directed to a webpage asking them to fill out an additional 17 demographic and health/disease risk factor questions including if they are a current smoker. Other questions included: marital status, handedness, race, ethnicity, number of daily prescription medications, a first-degree family history of dementia, and yes/no responses to the following: seizures, dizzy spells, loss of consciousness (more than 10 min), high blood pressure, diabetes, heart disease, cancer, stroke, alcohol/drug abuse, brain disease, and memory problems. Next, participants were shown their results and were provided with different comparisons to other test takers based on the average scores across all participants, as well as across sex, age, education, etc. On the same page, participants were also provided with the option to provide contact information if they wanted to be recontacted for future research.
Statistical analyses
Multivariate linear regression
To investigate the sex × smoking interaction on memory performance we ran a linear model (LM) controlling for various health and lifestyle factors from self-report including: age, race, ethnicity, marital status, handedness, education attainment, number of daily medications, history of diabetes, seizures, cancer, stroke, hypertension, heart disease, family history of Alzheimer disease, drug abuse, loss of consciousness, and dizziness. In order to justify the use of all covariates in both models, we ran stability selection using a Lasso regression model. We chose a common cut-off value for stability selection of 70%, all covariates passed this cutoff. The dependent variable was the total number of correct word pairs entered across the three trials of PAL tests (range of 0–36). We report standardized beta coefficients, standard error (SE), and p values.
To assess the interaction between cardiovascular disease × sex on PAL performance, we created a cardiovascular disease composite score by computing a sum number of CVD factors including heart disease, hypertension, diabetes, and stroke history (range of 0–4). Individuals were treated as groups based on their composite score (0 as the control group compared to 1, 2, and 3+ of these conditions). Participants with a sore of 3 or 4 were combined into one group to create an adequate sample size. We ran a linear model (LM) controlling for age and education level. We report standardized beta coefficients, standard error (SE), and p values.
Propensity score matching
In addition to running a multivariate linear regression model, we also conducted propensity score matching. Propensity score matching (PSM) has the benefit of reducing bias and variance66; however, this is often achieved at the expense of sample size compared to regression analysis. We matched participants on all variables listed as covariates in the multivariate linear model. Matching was performed using the R package, MatchIt (version 3.0.2). Effect sizes were estimated from the matched cohort using the R package, Zelig (version 5.1.6.1). Due to constraints on sample sizes, we conducted the PSM model collapsed across all ages on men and women separately. Zelig uses least squares regression on matched data to estimate the partial effect on an outcome of interest, in our case, total word pairs correct67,68. We also report effect sizes with credible intervals. The credible interval is the Bayesian version of a confidence interval and can be interpreted as a probability (i.e., there is a 95% probability that the effect size is between X and Y word pairs).
Down sampling
In order to demonstrate the importance of our large sample size for generating reliable effects, we ran 1000 down-sample linear regression models of the MindCrowd cohort between the ages of 18 and 85 years for the interaction effect of smoking × sex on paired-associate learning (PAL) for each indicated total sample size. Total sample sizes ranged from 268 to 13,400 and each group (smoker and non-smoker and sex) was sampled at equal size per age 18–85.
Artificial error introduction
To investigate the potential role false-report error may play on the smoking effect, we used a Monte Carlo simulation to determine the effect of introducing artificial error (in addition to any real self-report error already in the data). The additional error was added by randomly re-assigning the self-reported smoking status (smoker or non-smoker) to various percentages of the cohort (stepwise from 1 to 10% of individuals) and re-analyzing the effect of smoking using our complete statistical model. This process was performed a total of 10,000 times for each error percentage, and the resulting influences on the p-value are reported using boxplots.
Permutations
We examined the smoking effect on PAL through the use of permutation testing. Permutation testing is an approach utilized to determine the probability of a false-positive finding if the null hypothesis were true. To create the permuted datasets, we randomly assigned the smoking status for each participant before analyzing the complete model in the whole cohort, women only, and men only. We also ran permutation tests on the interaction between sex × smoking on PAL. This process was performed one million times per model.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
The MindCrowd site was built by The Lavidge Company (Scottsdale, Arizona) and we acknowledge their support through the donation of significant pro bono employee hours and resources. We also acknowledge the administrative support of Kara Karaniuk, Bethine Moore, and Danielle Metz (for IRB and CRC activities); Galen Perry, Jeffrey Watkins, Steve Yozwiak, and Murphy Raine (for marketing and communications activities). The Alzheimer’s Association TrialMatch is recognized for directing participants to MindCrowd. The Alzheimer’s Prevention Initiative at Banner Alzheimer’s Institute (Phoenix, Arizona) also helped with recruitment, and we acknowledge their support as well, including the Program Director Jessica Langbaum. Recruitment via social media was greatly facilitated by the efforts of Target Latino (Havi Goffan, CEO, Atlanta, Georgia). We would also like to thank our MindCrowd Facebook friends, Twitter followers, Redditors, and other social media participants.
Author contributions
C.R.L.: literature search, writing, and data interpretation. J.S.T.: data collection, data interpretation, and writing. M.D.D.B.: figures, data collection, and data analysis. A.M. Schmidt: literature search, and figures. M.A. Naymik: figures, data collection, and data analysis. A.K.H., T.R., B.E.L., S.H., Y.B., R.D.B., M.H., C.A.B., E.G. and L.R.: writing. M.J. Huentelman: study design, data collection, data interpretation, and writing.
Funding
The authors wish to acknowledge the Mueller Family Charitable Trust for funding the initial creation of the MindCrowd site and to Mike Mueller for his significant donation of personal time during the development, testing, and ongoing evolution of the study. This work was also supported in part by the State of Arizona DHS in support of the Arizona Alzheimer’s Consortium (PI; Eric Reiman), the Flinn Foundation (PI; Matt Huentelman), The McKnight Brain Research Foundation, and NIH-NIA (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services) Grant R01-AG049465 (PI; Carol Barnes). We also acknowledge the support of many anonymous individual donors and institutional support of TGen, City of Hope, and the University of Arizona. Philanthropic fundraising efforts for MindCrowd were led by the TGen Foundation staff (Erin Massey, Chief Development Officer).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-88923-z.
References
- 1.Corriveau RA, et al. The science of vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID): A framework for advancing research priorities in the cerebrovascular biology of cognitive decline. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2016;36:281–288. doi: 10.1007/s10571-016-0334-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Snyder HM, et al. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia including Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2015;11:710–717. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Heron M. Deaths: Leading causes for 2014. Natl. Vital Stat. Rep. 2016;65:1–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gaugler J, James B, Johnson T, Scholz K, Weuve J. 2016 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2016;12:459–509. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Seshadri S, Wolf PA. Lifetime risk of stroke and dementia: Current concepts, and estimates from the Framingham Study. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:1106–1114. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70291-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Harrison SL, et al. Cardiovascular disease risk models and longitudinal changes in cognition: A systematic review. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:1–14. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Elrod K, Buccafusco JJ, Jackson WJ. Nicotine enhances delayed matching-to-sample performance by primates. Life Sci. 1988;43:277–287. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(88)90318-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Salomon AR, Marcinowski KJ, Friedland RP, Zagorski MG. Nicotine inhibits amyloid formation by the β-peptide. Biochemistry. 1996;35:13568–13578. doi: 10.1021/bi9617264. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lee PN. Smoking and Alzheimer’s disease: A review of the epidemiological evidence. Neuroepidemiology. 1994;13:131–144. doi: 10.1159/000110372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Swan GE, Lessov-Schlaggar CN. The effects of tobacco smoke and nicotine on cognition and the brain. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2007;17:259–273. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9035-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Reitz C, Luchsinger J, Tang MX, Mayeux R. Effect of smoking and time on cognitive function in the elderly without dementia. Neurology. 2005;65:870–875. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000176057.22827.b7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Sabia S, et al. Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: The Whitehall II cohort study. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2012;69:627–635. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rusanen M, Kivipelto M, Quesenberry CP, Zhou J, Whitmer RA. Heavy smoking in midlife and long-term risk of Alzheimer disease and vascular dementia. Arch. Intern. Med. 2011;171:333–339. doi: 10.1001/archinternmed.2010.393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Papathanasiou G, Mamali A, Papafloratos S, Zerva E. Effects of smoking on cardiovascular function: The role of nicotine and carbon monoxide. Health Sci. J. 2014;8:274. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pittilo M. Cigarette smoking, endothelial injury and cardiovascular disease. Int. J. Exp. Pathol. 2000;81:219–230. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2613.2000.00162.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Groppelli A, Giorgi D, Omboni S, Parati G, Manciat G. Persistent blood pressure increase induced by heavy smoking. J. Hypertens. 1992;10:495–499. doi: 10.1097/00004872-199205000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nakamura K, et al. Influence of smoking combined with another risk factor on the risk of mortality from coronary heart disease and stroke: Pooled analysis of 10 Japanese cohort studies. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2012;33:480–491. doi: 10.1159/000336764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (US), National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion (US), Office on Smoking and Health (US) How Tobacco Smoke Causes Disease: The Biology and Behavioral Basis for Smoking-Attributable Disease: A Report of the Surgeon General. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Beam CR, et al. Differences between women and men in incidence rates of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2018;64:1077–1083. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Snyder HM, et al. Sex biology contributions to vulnerability to Alzheimer’s disease: A think tank convened by the Women’s Alzheimer’s research initiative. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2016;12:1186–1196. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hebert LE, Scherr PA, McCann JJ, Beckett LA, Evans DA. Is the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease greater for women than for men? Am. J. Epidemiol. 2001;153:132–136. doi: 10.1093/aje/153.2.132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Tarraf W, et al. Cardiovascular risk and cognitive function in middle-aged and older hispanics/latinos: Results from the hispanic community health study/study of latinos (HCHS/SOL) J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2020;73:103–116. doi: 10.3233/JAD-190830. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Peters R, et al. Smoking, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly, a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2008;8:1–7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-8-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Anstey KJ, Von Sanden C, Salim A, O’Kearney R. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007;166:367–378. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Heo M, Leon AC. Sample sizes required to detect two-way and three-way interactions involving slope differences in mixed-effects linear models. J. Biopharm. Stat. 2010;20:787–802. doi: 10.1080/10543401003618819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Heo M, Leon AC. Sample size requirements to detect an intervention by time interaction in longitudinal cluster randomized clinical trials. Stat. Med. 2009;28:1017–1027. doi: 10.1002/sim.3527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Launer LJ, et al. Rates and risk factors for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: Results from EURODEM pooled analyses. Neurology. 1999;52:78. doi: 10.1212/WNL.52.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Aggarwal NT, et al. The relation of cigarette smoking to incident Alzheimer’s disease in a biracial urban community population. Neuroepidemiology. 2006;26:140–146. doi: 10.1159/000091654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Deal JA, et al. Relationship of cigarette smoking and time of quitting with incident dementia and cognitive decline. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2020;68:337–345. doi: 10.1111/jgs.16228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Prince M, Lewis G, Bird A, Blizard R, Mann A. A longitudinal study of factors predicting change in cognitive test scores over time, in an older hypertensive population. Psychol. Med. 1996;26:555–568. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700035637. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Hotta R, et al. Cigarette smoking and cognitive health in elderly Japanese. Am. J. Health Behav. 2015;39:294–300. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.39.3.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Huxley RR, Woodward M. Cigarette smoking as a risk factor for coronary heart disease in women compared with men: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Lancet. 2011;378:1297–1305. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60781-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Papadopoulos A, et al. Heavy smoking and lung cancer: Are women at higher risk? Result of the ICARE study. Br. J. Cancer. 2014;110:1385–1391. doi: 10.1038/bjc.2013.821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sutherland LR, Ramcharan S, Bryant H, Fick G. Effect of cigarette smoking on recurrence of Crohn’s disease. Gastroenterology. 1990;98:1123–1128. doi: 10.1016/0016-5085(90)90324-T. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Høidrup S, et al. Tobacco smoking and risk of hip fracture in men and women. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2000;29:253–259. doi: 10.1093/ije/29.2.253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Della Casa V, Höfer I, Weiner I, Feldon J. The effects of smoking on acoustic prepulse inhibition in healthy men and women. Psychopharmacology. 1998;137:362–368. doi: 10.1007/s002130050631. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rhodes ME, Rubin RT. Functional sex differences ('sexual diergism’) of central nervous system cholinergic systems, vasopressin, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal. Brain Res. Rev. 1999;30:135–152. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00011-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Newman AB, et al. Dementia and Alzheimer’s disease incidence in relationship to cardiovascular disease in the cardiovascular health study cohort. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005;53:1101–1107. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53360.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dregan A, Stewart R, Gulliford MC. Cardiovascular risk factors and cognitive decline in adults aged 50 and over: A population-based cohort study. Age Ageing. 2013;42:338–345. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afs166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Harrison SL, et al. Predicting risk of cognitive decline in very old adults using three models: The Framingham stroke risk profile; the cardiovascular risk factors, aging, and dementia model; and oxi-inflammatory biomarkers. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2017;65:381–389. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Joosten H, et al. Cardiovascular risk profile and cognitive function in young, middle-aged, and elderly subjects. Stroke. 2013;44:1543–1549. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.111.000496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.United States Department of Health and Human Services . The Health Consequences of Smoking—50 Years of Progress a Report of the Surgeon General. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2014. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Creamer MLR, et al. Tobacco product use and cessation indicators among adults—United States, 2018. Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2019;68:1013. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6845a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Warren CW, et al. Change in tobacco use among 13–15 year olds between 1999 and 2008: Findings from the Global Youth Tobacco Survey. Glob. Health Promot. 2009;16:38–90. doi: 10.1177/1757975909342192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Gilmore A, et al. Prevalence of smoking in 8 countries of the former Soviet Union: Results from the living conditions, lifestyles and health study. Am. J. Public Health. 2004;94:2177–2187. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.94.12.2177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Gannon OJ, Robison LS, Custozzo AJ, Zuloaga KL. Sex differences in risk factors for vascular contributions to cognitive impairment & dementia. Neurochem. Int. 2019;127:38–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2018.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tucker-Drob EM. Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 2011;47:331. doi: 10.1037/a0021361. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Koylu E, Demirgören S, London ED, Pöǧün Ş. Sex difference in up-regulation of nicotine acetylcholine receptors in rat brain. Life Sci. 1997;61:185–190. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3205(97)00665-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gotti C, Clementi F. Neuronal nicotinic receptors: From structure to pathology. Prog. Neurobiol. 2004;74:363–396. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kaffashian S, et al. Predictive utility of the Framingham general cardiovascular disease risk profile for cognitive function: Evidence from the Whitehall II study. Eur. Heart J. 2011;32:2326–2332. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehr133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Laughlin GA, et al. Sex differences in the association of Framingham cardiac risk score with cognitive decline in community-dwelling elders without clinical heart disease. Psychosom. Med. 2011;73:683. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31822f9089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Zeki-Al-Hazzouri A, et al. Cardiovascular risk score, cognitive decline, and dementia in older Mexican Americans: The role of sex and education. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2013;2:1–9. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.113.004978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kaffashian S, et al. Predicting cognitive decline: A dementia risk score vs the Framingham vascular risk scores. Neurology. 2013;80:1300–1306. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828ab370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kaffashian S, et al. Midlife stroke risk and cognitive decline: A 10-year follow-up of the Whitehall II cohort study. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2013;9:572–579. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Reis JP, et al. Cardiovascular health through young adulthood and cognitive functioning in midlife. Ann. Neurol. 2013;73:170–179. doi: 10.1002/ana.23836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Huentelman MJ, Talboom JS, Lewis CR, Chen Z, Barnes CA. Reinventing neuroaging research in the digital age. Trends Neurosci. 2020;43:17–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Maukonen M, Männistö S, Tolonen H. A comparison of measured versus self-reported anthropometrics for assessing obesity in adults: A literature review. Scand. J. Public Health. 2018;46:565–579. doi: 10.1177/1403494818761971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Nikolaou CK, Hankey CR, Lean MEJ. Accuracy of on-line self-reported weights and heights by young adults. Eur. J. Public Health. 2017;27:898–903. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckx077. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Demetriou C, Ozer BU, Essau CA. Self-report questionnaires. In: Cautin RL, Lilienfeld SO, editors. The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology. Wiley; 2014. pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Shrank WH, Patrick AR, Brookhart MA. Healthy user and related biases in observational studies of preventive interventions: A primer for physicians. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2011;26:546–550. doi: 10.1007/s11606-010-1609-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Hwang JH, Kim JY, Lee DH, Jung HG, Park SW. Underestimation of self-reported smoking prevalence in Korean adolescents: Evidence from gold standard by combined method. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2018;15:689. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jung-Choi KH, Khang YH, Cho HJ. Hidden female smokers in Asia: A comparison of self-reported with cotinine-verified smoking prevalence rates in representative national data from an Asian population. Tob. Control. 2012;21:536–542. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2011-050012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Florescu A, et al. Methods for quantification of exposure to cigarette smoking and environmental tobacco smoke: Focus on developmental toxicology. Ther. Drug Monit. 2009;31:14. doi: 10.1097/FTD.0b013e3181957a3b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Song MK, Lin FC, Ward SE, Fine JP. Composite variables: When and how. Nurs. Res. 2013;62:45. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0b013e3182741948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Krokstad S, et al. Cohort profile: The HUNT study, Norway. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2013;42:968–977. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ho DE, Imai K, King G, Stuart EA. MatchIt: Nonparametric preprocessing for parametric causal inference. J. Stat. Softw. 2011;42:1–28. doi: 10.18637/jss.v042.i08. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Imai K, King G, Lau O. Toward a common framework for statistical analysis and development. J. Comput. Graph. Stat. 2009;17:892–913. doi: 10.1198/106186008X384898. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Imai K, King G, Lau O, Imai M. The Zelig package. Polit. Sci. 2007;15:199. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.