Abstract
Introduction
Experimental models of neuropsychiatric disorders, for example, ADHD, are used to mimic specific phenotypic traits of a complex human disorder. However, it remains unresolved to what extent the animal phenotype reflects the specific human trait. The null mutant mouse of the serotonin‐synthesizing tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 (Tph2 ‐/‐) gene has been proposed as experimental model for ADHD with high face validity for impulsive, aggressive, and anxious behaviors. To validate this ADHD‐like model, we examined the Tph2‐/‐ phenotype in humans when considering allelic variation of TPH2 function (“reverse phenotyping”).
Methods
58 participants (6 females, 8–18 years) were examined, of whom 32 were diagnosed with ADHD. All participants were phenotyped for impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety using questionnaires, behavioral tests, and MRI scanning while performing the 4‐choice serial reaction time task. Additionally, participants were genotyped for the TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) polymorphism. To analyze the relation between TPH2 G‐703T variants and the impulsive/aggressive/anxious phenotype, mediation analyses were performed using behavioral and MRI data as potential mediators.
Results
We found that the relation between TPH2 G‐703T and aggression as part of the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype was mediated by structure and function of the right middle and inferior frontal gyrus.
Conclusion
At the example of trait aggression, our results support the assumption that the Tph2 null mutant mouse reflects the TPH2 G‐703T‐dependent phenotype in humans. Additionally, we conclude that “reverse phenotyping” is a promising method to validate experimental models and human findings for refined analysis of disease mechanisms.
Keywords: aggression, anxiety, attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), impulsivity, TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) polymorphism, Tph2‐/‐ mouse, tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 (TPH2)
The Tph2‐/‐ mouse has been proposed as an experimental model for ADHD. Translation of an animal phenotype into humans to examine its face validity (reverse phenotyping). Phenotyping included impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety, reflecting comorbid conditions of ADHD. Use of the RDoC approach to address phenotype dimensions in an isolated manner.
1. INTRODUCTION
Attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a highly heterogenous disorder that frequently presents with comorbidities such as oppositional defiant and conduct disorder (prevalence: 67%, 46%) and anxiety disorders (prevalence: 44%) (Steinhausen et al., 2006). Translational studies have gained importance in neuropsychiatric research, improving our understanding of mechanisms and pathways to comorbidities. In the case of the serotonin‐synthesizing tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 (TPH2) gene, the null mutant mice (Tph2 ‐/‐) have been discussed as an experimental model for ADHD. Functional variants of the human TPH2 gene have been associated with ADHD in numerous studies (Manor et al., 2008; McKinney et al., 2008; Sheehan et al., 2005; Walitza et al., 2005), and Tph2 ‐/‐ mice are characterized by increased impulsivity (Lesch & Merschdorf, 2000; Sachs et al., 2013), which is a core symptom of ADHD. However, as the Tph2 ‐/‐ mouse also demonstrates increased aggression and decreased anxiety‐like behavior (Lesch et al., 2012; Waider et al., 2019), it is not known to what extent the Tph2 ‐/‐ mouse reflects ADHD symptomatology. To investigate whether the alterations following constitutive Tph2 gene inactivation in mice can also be found in humans via the way the TPH2 influences human (impulsive, aggressive, and anxious) behavior, we employ the “reverse phenotyping” approach “where phenotypes are refined based on genetic marker data” (Schulze & McMahon, 2004). Based on the assumption that the animal phenotype reflects ADHD‐associated symptoms, we examined children and adolescents with and without ADHD. Thus, in this study, the reverse Tph2 ‐/‐ phenotype refers to the examination of the animal phenotype (i.e., altered impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety) in humans.
TPH2 catalyzes the rate‐limiting step in the biosynthesis of serotonin (Zhang et al., 2004). It is primarily expressed in the serotonergic neurons of the raphe nuclei, which project to brain regions including the hippocampus (Brivio et al., 2018; Migliarini et al., 2013; Zill et al., 2004; Zill et al., 2004), amygdala (Brown et al., 2005; Haghighi et al., 2008), the prefrontal cortex (Baehne et al., 2009; Haghighi et al., 2008), and the anterior cingulate (Canli et al., 2008). To “explore the question of what traits or neuropsychiatric disorders are attributable to TPH2 dysfunction across the life span” (Lesch et al., 2012), three different Tph2‐deficient mouse lines were generated close in time with a targeted inactivation of Tph2 (Alenina et al., 2009; Gutknecht et al., 2008; Savelieva et al., 2008). Reduced Tph2 expression is associated with an impulsivity‐, aggression‐, and anxiety‐related phenotype combined with alterations in prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate, and amygdala (Coccaro et al., 2011; Ko et al., 2018; Lesch et al., 2012; Mark et al., 2019; Waider et al., 2019).
Altered impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety also correspond to phenotypes frequently observed in patients with ADHD, especially in the context of the abovementioned comorbidities. In humans, the influence of TPH2 is often addressed using either the tryptophan depletion approach or by stratification for the functional TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) polymorphism. In patients with ADHD and typically developing children (TDC), tryptophan deficiency has been associated with increased impulsivity (Stoltenberg et al., 2012), heightened aggression (Duke et al., 2013), and altered anxiety (Lowry et al., 2008). For example, carriers of the TPH2 T allele (T+) presented increased risk of impulse control disorders (Gizer et al., 2009), disturbed affective behavior (Canli et al., 2008), and reduced response inhibition (Stoltenberg et al., 2012). Studies using tryptophan depletion on aggression in TDC and ADHD (Kotting et al., 2013; Polier et al., 2013) showed that depleted patients reacted strongest in comparison with nondepleted patients and TDC. In socially anxious T+ patients, an overactive presynaptic serotonergic neurotransmission in the amygdala and anterior cingulate was described (Furmark et al., 2016), and in a sample of patients with ADHD with and without comorbid anxiety, anxiety was related to a reduction in impulsivity and response inhibition deficits suggesting that a “mildly elevated trait anxiety confers a protective influence by reducing the degree of impairment seen in ADHD” (Ruf et al., 2016).
In this fMRI study, we examined children with and without ADHD. Participants were genotyped for TPH2 G‐703T and phenotyped according to trait impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety. In addition, structural and functional MRI was acquired using the 4‐choice serial reaction time task (4‐CSRTT) (Voon et al., 2014). Waiting impulsivity as measured via the 4‐CSRTT is one form of impulsivity, defined as the tendency to premature responding, that is, to respond before target onset. Thus, it involves the aspects of response inhibition and top‐down control, mediated by motivational aspects and reward processing (Robinson et al., 2009; Voon, 2014). The underlying brain network includes the ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex as well as the anterior cingulate cortex representing motor or response inhibition (Mechelmans et al., 2017), the reward‐related nucleus accumbens (Neufang et al., 2016), and hippocampus and amygdala responsible for reward‐based learning (Dalley et al., 2011). We used the dimensional approach as suggested by the rDoC (https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research‐funded‐by‐nimh/rdoc/index.shtml), to address neuropsychiatric impairments in terms of a gradual change rather than a binary factor (ADHD versus. TDC). Thus, all subjects were examined as one sample using regression‐based statistical analyses.
The aim of the study was to examine how TPH2 G‐703T was related to trait impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety (i.e., the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype) in children and adolescent with and without ADHD. Additionally, the question was if brain structure and function mediated this relation. Therefore, TPH2 G‐703T variant group comparisons were performed in a first step, to see whether TPH2 G‐703T directly influences phenotype variables. In a second step, mediation analyses were performed to reveal whether the influence of TPH2 G‐703T on phenotype variables indeed was direct or rather indirect via neural parameters.
Based on the earlier reported findings, we assumed that TPH2 G‐703T modulated all three phenotype traits: considering the protective effect of anxiety on ADHD, we hypothesized a positive effect on anxiety (i.e., GG > T+). In contrast, the influence of aggression and impulsivity was supposed to be negative, that is, T+ presented higher impulsive and aggressive trait scores compared with GG. However, we did not expect to find significant differences between TPH2 G‐703T variants on impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety (direct effect), but assumed that the relation between TPH2 G‐703T and impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety was mediated by neural structure and/or function (indirect effects), for example, of the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala.
2. METHODS
2.1. Subjects
We examined 59 subjects (6 females), comprising 32 patients with ADHD (3 females) and 26 TDC (3 females). One ADHD patient did not finish MRI scanning and was excluded from statistical analyses. Remaining 58 subjects were aged from 8 to 18 years. M = 13.5 ± 2.2 years showed normal physical maturation (Tanner stages: M = 3.1 ± 1.2), (Marshall & Tanner, 1969, 1970) and intelligence was screened via the "Culture Fair Intelligence Test" (Weiß, 2006) (M = 105.0 ± 14.4, range: 80–153). Healthy participants were recruited within the CRC‐TRR‐58, and patients with ADHD were recruited from in/outpatient clinics of the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Würzburg, within the research network ESCAlife. All patients were diagnosed with ADHD according to the DSM‐V by trained clinicians (Table S1). Eighteen patients were medicated with methylphenidate (9 retard, daily dosage: M = 30±10.8 mg), and unmedicated patients were either medication‐naïve (n = 8) or stopped medication use for more than a year (n = 6). Medicated patients underwent a washout phase of 48 hr (N = 11) or longer (N = 7, M = 22 ± 22.8 day) prior to scanning.
This study is in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki in its latest version and was approved by the ethics committee of the Faculty of Medicine, University of Würzburg, Germany. Participants and their parents/legal guardians gave written consent.
2.2. Reverse Tph2‐/‐ Phenotyping
The reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype was determined via the (a) “hyperactivity/impulsivity” scale of the "Diagnostic System for Mental Disorders in Children and Adolescents according to ICD‐10 and DSM‐V" (Döpfner & Görtz‐Dorten, 2017), (b) the questionnaire for aggressive behavior in children and adolescents (Goertz‐Dorten & Döpfner, 2010), and (c) the trait scale of German version of the State‐Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (Unnewehr et al., 1992). All three instruments were administered at the examination day, and completion was supervised by an examiner.
2.3. Genotyping
Blood sampling and genotyping were performed in all subjects. Genomic DNA was extracted from whole‐blood samples according to a standard desalting protocol. Genotyping procedures were performed using PCR and gel electrophoresis. Genotyping for the TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) polymorphism was performed according to published protocols (Hahn et al., 2013). TPH2 G‐703T distribution (TT = 0, 0%; GT = 25, 43.1%; GG = 33, 56.9%; p(Exact) = .7845) did not deviate significantly from the expected numbers calculated according to the Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium using the program DeFinetti provided (https://wpcalc.com/en/equilibrium‐hardy‐weinberg/). Based on the findings showing that TPH2 expression is decreased in carriers of the T allele (Lin et al., 2007), we defined two groups, subjects homozygous for the TPH2 G allele (GG, n = 33) and carriers of at least one T allele (T+, n = 25).
2.4. Experimental paradigm
The 4‐CSRTT examines waiting impulsivity defined as the capability to inhibit a response to earn a reward. A trial begins with a short presentation of 4 boxes, which starts the waiting period. After a certain cue–target interval, the target appears as a green dot, located in one of the four boxes. Correct and quick responding was remunerated by a monetary reward. Premature responses were defined as reactions before target onset in the anticipation of reward. The task consisted of one block outside the scanner (2.5 min) and five blocks within the MR scanner (14 min) (Neufang et al., 2016).
All participants were trained outside the scanner and prior to the actual task. After training sessions, a first baseline block was conducted to determine the individual mean reaction time window (rt, Mrt ± 2SD). The baseline block consisted of 20 trials and did not include a reward. The individual rt windows were used for reward definition in all consecutive blocks, which were performed in the MR scanner. Reward was provided in terms of a win of 10 euro cents, when the answer was correct and within the individual rt window, or a win of 1 euro when subjects answered correctly and were faster than their individual rt window. Subjects encountered a loss of 1 euro when response times for correct responses were longer than the rt window. Incorrect answers were neither rewarded nor punished.
The experiment in the scanner included 5 further blocks, 4 blocks with the opportunity to earn a reward and a second baseline block without reward, with each having 20 trials. The 4 reward blocks had a hierarchical structure to increase the tendency for premature responses. In detail, in the 1st block in the scanner, the presentation duration of the target was 64 ms, and the cue–target interval was 2 s and only green, that is, correct targets were presented. In block 3, target presentation duration was decreased to 32 ms. In block 4, presentation duration remained 32 ms; in addition, cue–target interval varied from 2 s to 6.5 s. Finally in block 5, in addition to short presentation duration and varying cue–target intervals, distractor targets were presented in terms of blue or yellow dots (for further detail see (Neufang et al., 2016)). Total task duration was 14 min in the MR scanner and 2.5 min for the first baseline block.
2.5. Data acquisition
Scanning was performed on a 3 Tesla TIM Trio Scanner (Siemens). Whole‐brain T2*‐weighted BOLD images were recorded with a gradient echo‐planar imaging sequence (repetition time = 2000 ms, echo time = 30 ms, 36 slices, 3 mm thickness, field of view = 192 mm, flip angle = 90°, 425 volumes). In addition, an isotropic high‐resolution T1‐weighted three‐dimensional structural MR image was acquired (magnetization prepared rapid gradient echo, 176 slices, 1 × 1 × 1 mm3, repetition time = 2400 ms, echo time = 2.26 ms, field of view = 256 mm, flip angle = 9°).
2.6. fMRI processing
Data processing was performed using the Statistical Parametric Mapping Software Package (SPM12, Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK, Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging; http:// www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Data preprocessing in the native space included the steps of temporal and spatial alignment: All images were slice time‐corrected, realigned to the first functional image, and unwarped. Images were then spatially normalized into a standard stereotactic space (Montreal Neurological Institute), resampled to an isotropic voxel size of 2 × 2×2 mm3, and spatially smoothed with a Gaussian kernel of 8mm full width at half maximum. Statistical analysis on the individual first level (single‐subject level) was based on the general linear model (GLM) approach. Model specification included the definition of experimental condition, in our case “cue,” “target,” and “reward.” Reward trials were subdivided into “win” and “loss” trials. Break periods were defined as “rest.” In addition to the experimental conditions, nuisance regressors were specified, that is, “error trials” and “realignment parameters” (i.e., six regressors containing movement in three spatial and three rotational axes), to correct for error variance and movement artifacts. For each condition, onset times were determined from log files with onsets of the cue condition were determined at the time when the cue picture was presented. Onset times of target trials were defined in terms of the appearance of the target picture, and onset times of reward trials (win and loss) were the time points when the reward feedback picture appeared on the screen. The onsets of error trials were defined as the target onsets of incorrect trials. On the single subjects, three contrasts of interest were calculated, “cue–rest” to identify cue‐specific brain activation, “target–rest” to isolate target‐induced brain activation, and “reward” in terms of “win–loss” to identify brain activation associated with the receipt of monetary reward. Resulting contrast images entered statistical group analysis. For mediation analyses, beta values of brain areas which were significantly related to the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype were extracted.
2.7. sMRI processing
Structural MRI data were analyzed using the FreeSurfer (versions 5.3) software (Fischl et al., 2002). Analysis and quality‐control protocols of the ENIGMA consortium (http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols /imaging‐protocols) were applied including the recon‐all ‐all stream and the segmentations of 68 (34 left and 34 right) cortical gray matter regions based on the Desikan–Killiany atlas (Desikan et al., 2006). In this study, we focused on regions associated with waiting impulsivity, that is, the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG, triangular/opercular/orbital part), the middle frontal gyrus (MFG), the anterior cingulate, hippocampus, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens. Regional volume scores were corrected for global brain volume in terms of % of intracranial volume.
2.8. Mediation analyses
In line with earlier publications (e.g., (Bi et al., 2017)), we performed mediation analyses to address the complex mechanism how TPH2 G‐703T was related to the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype and which roles brain structure and activation played. We used the Macro PROCESS for SPSS developed by Hayes (Hayes, 2017) (http://www.processmacro.org/download.html), which has been used in numerous earlier scientific reports, for example, (Kneer et al., 2020; Mertens et al., 2018). The simple mediation model includes two consequent variables; thus, two linear models are required and defined as follows:
(1) |
(2) |
with i M and i Y being regression constants; eM and eY, error estimates of M and Y; and a, b, and c’, the regression coefficients given to the antecedent variables in the model (Figure S1 X, Y, M1, a1, b1 and c’). When using control variables (e.g., in our case diagnosis and age), the simple mediation model changes as follows:
(3) |
(4) |
with y’ representing the direct effect of X on Y, αβ defining the indirect effect of X on Y as mediated via M, and the sum of y’ + αβ the total effect of X on Y.
In this study, we defined three models, one for anxiety, one for impulsivity, and one for aggression, with TPH2 G‐703T as independent factor (X) and the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype as dependent variables (Y) and four mediator variables. Mediator variables were the two impulsivity/aggression/anxiety‐specific activation scores (Table 2) and the corresponding regional volume (i.e., impulsivity‐associated brain activation in the right IFG pars triangularis (M1) and the right MFG (M2), and the volumes of IFGtri (M3) and MFG (M4); aggression‐related brain activation in the same regions and their structural volumes; and activation correlated with anxiety in the right IFG pars opercularis and the MFG, and the regional volumes of the IFGop and the MFG] (Figure S1). All three models were tested twice varying between parallel (PROCESS, model 4) and serial mediation effects (PROCESS, model 6). In the case of serial mediation, the total effect of c is dissected into five different indirect effects:
(5) |
TABLE 2.
Multiple regressions of the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype on brain activation using multiple regression models (independent factors: impulsivity, aggression, anxiety)
Task condition | Regressor | k | x | y | z | Z | region |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cue | Impulsivity(+) | 15 | −46 | 6 | 16 | 3.2 | Left IFGop |
Cue | Aggression(+) | 32 | 46 | 12 | 10 | 3.0 | Right IFGop |
Target | Anxiety(+) | 43 | −44 | 22 | 6 | 3.5 | Right IFGop |
Reward | Impulsivity(+) | 169 | 40 | 50 | 8 | 3.6 | Right MFG |
38 | 34 | 8 | 3.4 | Right IFGtri | |||
Reward | Aggression(‐) | 71 | 40 | 36 | 8 | 3.1 | Right IFGtri |
44 | 48 | 4 | 3.1 | Right MFG | |||
Reward | Anxiety(+) | 20 | 38 | 22 | 54 | 3.4 | Right MFG |
(+): positive correlation, (‐): negative correlation, IFGop, opercular part of the inferior frontal gyrus, IFGtri, triangular part of the inferior frontal gyrus, MFG, middle frontal gyrus, FDR correction p<.05 on voxel level.
Statistical significance of direct effects was reached when p < .05, FDR‐corrected for 15 comparisons (for a detailed description of direct effects, Table 3). Indirect effects were tested for significance using bootstrapping (no of bootstrap samples: 10,000). In this case, the test for significance was one‐sided assuming that indirect effects (i.e., αβ as the indirect effect of X on Y, mediated via M) were > 0. Therefore, bootstrapped confidence intervals of 95% were defined. If confidence intervals did not include 0, the indirect effect was considered as significant.
TABLE 3.
Mediation model with the independent factor X = TPH2 G‐703T, the dependent variable Y = aggression, potentially mediating variables M1 = aggression‐related activation in the right IFGtri, M2 = aggression‐associated activation in the MFG, M3 = right IFGtri volume, and M4 = right MFG volume, as well as nuisance variables: age and diagnostic group
Variable 1 | Variable 2 | Path | Coeff | SE | T, p | LLCI | ULCI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Direct effects | |||||||
TPH2(X) | actrIFGtri(M1) | a1 | 1.60 | 0.66 | 2.4, p=.02 | 0.27 | 2.92 |
actrMFG(M2) | a2 | −0.10 | 0.73 | 0.1, p=.89 | −1.56 | 1.36 | |
volrIFGtri(M3) | a3 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.2, p=.90 | −0.03 | 0.03 | |
volrMFG(M4) | a4 | 0.06 | 0.04 | 1.4, p=.16 | −0.02 | 0.14 | |
aggression(Y) | c’ | 8.13 | 6.65 | 1.2, p=.23 | −5.24 | 21.50 | |
actrIFGtri(M1) | actrMFG(M2) | d21 | 0.98 | 0.14 | 6.8, p=.00 | 0.69 | 1.26 |
volrIFGtri(M3) | d31 | −0.01 | 0.01 | 0.3, p=.78 | −0.01 | 0.01 | |
volrMFG(M4) | d41 | −0.02 | 0.01 | 1.9, p=.06 | −0.04 | 0.01 | |
aggression(Y) | b1 | −1.26 | 1.83 | 0.7, p=.49 | −4.94 | 2.41 | |
actrMFG(M2) | volrIFGtri(M3) | d32 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.1, p=.95 | −0.01 | 0.01 |
volrMFG(M4) | d42 | 0.02 | 0.01 | 2.2, p=.04 | 0.01 | 0.03 | |
aggression(Y) | b2 | −1.70 | 1.30 | 1.3, p=.20 | −4.10 | 0.91 | |
volrIFGtri(M3) | volrMFG(M4) | d34 | 0.58 | 0.41 | 1.4, p=.16 | −0.24 | 1.40 |
aggression(Y) | b3 | 28.99 | 67.56 | 0.4, p=.67 | −106.79 | 164.76 | |
volrMFG(M4) | aggression(Y) | b4 | 39.25 | 23.04 | 1.7, p=.10 | −7.05 | 85.54 |
Effect | bootSE a | bootLLCI a | bootULCI a | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Indirect effects (Ind) | |||||
Ind1: | XM1Y | −2.02 | 3.27 | −9.93 | 3.27 |
Ind2: | XM1M2Y | −2.64 | 2.58 | −9.62 | 1.03 |
Ind3: | XM1M3Y | −0.05 | 0.58 | −1.72 | 0.82 |
Ind4: | XM1M4Y | −1.31 | 1.35 | −6.27 | −0.02 |
Ind5 | XM1M2M3Y | 0.01 | 0.31 | −0.56 | 0.65 |
Ind6: | XM1M2M4Y | 1.01 | 0.97 | 0.01 | 4.50 |
Ind7: | XM1M3M4Y | −0.04 | 0.25 | −0.95 | 0.21 |
Ind8: | XM1M2M3M4Y | 0.01 | 0.12 | −0.15 | 0.41 |
Ind9: | XM2Y | 0.17 | 1.62 | −2.14 | 4.88 |
Ind10: | XM2M3Y | −0.01 | 0.13 | −0.31 | 0.21 |
Ind11: | XM2M4Y | −0.07 | 0.60 | −1.87 | 0.83 |
Ind12: | XM2M3M4Y | −0.01 | 0.06 | −0.14 | 0.09 |
Ind13: | XM3Y | 0.05 | 0.91 | −1.29 | 2.67 |
Ind14: | XM3M4Y | 0.04 | 0.42 | −0.54 | 1.34 |
Ind15: | XM4Y | 2.22 | 2.48 | −0.63 | 9.70 |
Abbreviations: actrIFGtri, aggression‐related activation in the right IFGtri; actrMFG, aggression‐associated activation in the MFG; volrIFGtri, right IFGtri volume; volrMFG, right MFG volume.
no of bootstrap samples: 10,000; level of confidence for all confidence intervals: 95%; FDR correction for 15 comparisons revealed a q*=0.003; bold: significant indirect effect as CI does not include 0.
2.9. Statistical analysis
In a first step, group comparisons were performed on phenotype scores using two‐sample tests with the independent factor TPH2 G‐703T (GG versus. T+) and phenotype‐related trait dimensions (impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety) as dependent variables.
To address the interaction between TPH2 G‐703T genotype, the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype, and the waiting impulsivity network, mediation analyses were defined. To do so, waiting impulsivity parameters which were influenced by the reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype had to be identified in a pre‐analysis using multiple regressions (2a) with phenotype scores as independent factors, (a) behavioral performance (# premature responses, accuracy, rt), (b) regional brain volumes, and (c) neural activation (contrast images for cue, target, and reward) as dependent variables. Age and diagnostic group (ADHD versus. TDC) were included as nuisance variables. (2b) Finally, mediation analyses were performed We used PROCESS model 4 (assuming mediators operate in parallel) and PROCESS model 6 (assuming that mediators operate in serial) as suggested by Hayes (Hayes, 2017) (Figure S1).
3. RESULTS
There were no significant a priori differences between TPH2 G‐703T variants in behavioral performance and regional brain volume (Table 1) (for differences between diagnostic groups Table S1). Across all subjects, task accuracy and rt varied with age: accuracy increased and rt decreased with age. Developmental changes were comparable between TPH2 G‐703T variants (accuracy*age: RGG = 0.461, p = .007, RT+ = 0.446, p = .025; Z GGvs.T+ = 0.1, p = .464; rt*age: RGG = −0.437, p = .029, RT+ = −0.687, p = .000; ZGGvs.T+ = 1.7, p = .075).
TABLE 1.
A priori group differences between TPH2 G‐703T polymorphisms using two‐sample t tests (GG versus T+)
GG | T+ | Statistics | |
---|---|---|---|
Sample characteristics | |||
Diagnostic group (ADHD/TDC) | 14/18 | 17/8 | Χ 2 = 2.9, p=.07 |
Sex(male/female) | 30/3 | 22/3 | Χ 2 = 0.1, p=.52 |
Age | 13.9 ± 2.2 | 13.1 ± 2.3 | T(56,2)=1.3, p=.18 |
IQ | 104.4 ± 16.7 | 105.9 ± 10.9 | T(56,2)=0.4, p=.69 |
Tanner stages | 3.2.±1.1 | 2.8 ± 1.3 | T(56,2)=1.2, p=.22 |
Reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype | |||
Impulsivity (FBB_Imp) | 0.5 ± 0.6 | 0.9 ± 0.9 | T(56,2)=2.0, p=.05 |
Aggression (FAVK) | 33.4 ± 23.5 | 45.2 ± 29.7 | T(56,2)=1.7, p=.10 |
Anxiety (STAIC‐T) | 32.6 ± 7.2 | 32.7 ± 9.2 | T(56,2)=0.11, p=.97 |
A priori group differences | |||
Behavioral performance | |||
Premature responses | 2.8 ± 1.7 | 2.6 ± 1.8 | T(56,2)=0.3, p=.80 |
Accuracy [%] | 79.8 ± 15.6 | 81.1 ± 14.4 | T(56,2)=0.3, p=.75 |
Reaction times [ms] | 439 ± 93 | 464 ± 101 | T(56,2)=1.0, p=.32 |
Regional volumes | |||
Left HC | 0.27 ± 0.08 | 0.28 ± 0.07 | T(56,2)=0.7, p=.51 |
Left AMY | 0.12 ± 0.01 | 0.13 ± 0.02 | T(56,2)=1.5, p=.14 |
Left NAcc | 0.05 ± 0.01 | 0.05 ± 0.01 | T(56,2)=0.4, p=.69 |
Right HC | 0.29 ± 0.03 | 0.30 ± 0.03 | T(56,2)=0.5, p=.65 |
Right AMY | 0.12 ± 0.01 | 0.12 ± 0.02 | T(56,2)=0.9, p=.38 |
Right NAcc | 0.05 ± 0.01 | 0.05 ± 0.01 | T(56,2)=1.3, p=.20 |
Right ACC | 0.39 ± 0.05 | 0.39 ± 0.05 | T(56,2)=0.6, p=.53 |
Right IFGop | 0.25 ± 0.04 | 0.24 ± 0.03 | T(56,2)=1.0, p=.34 |
Right IFGorb | 0.07 ± 0.01 | 0.07 ± 0.02 | T(56,2)=0.3, p=.73 |
Right IFGtri | 0.19 ± 0.04 | 0.19 ± 0.04 | T(56,2)=0.1, p=.94 |
Right MFG | 0.72 ± 0.14 | 0.66 ± 0.14 | T(56,2)=1.6, p=.11 |
Left ACC | 0.36 ± 0.04 | 0.35 ± 0.05 | T(56,2)=0.7, p=.50 |
Left IFGop | 0.27 ± 0.05 | 0.26 ± 0.05 | T(56,2)=0.6, p=.54 |
Left IFGorb | 0.07 ± 0.02 | 0.07 ± 0.02 | T(56,2)=0.3, p=.80 |
Left IFGtri | 0.22 ± 0.05 | 0.21 ± 0.05 | T(56,2)=0.8, p=.44 |
Left MFG | 0.81 ± 0.14 | 0.76 ± 0.15 | T(56,2)=1.5, p=.13 |
Abbreviations: ADHD, attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder, TDC, typically developing children, HC, hippocampus, AMY, amygdala, NAcc, nucleus accumbens, ACC, anterior cingulate cortex, IFG, inferior frontal gyrus, IFGop, opercular part of the IFG, IFGorb, orbital part of the IFG, IFGtri, triangular part of the IFG, MFG, middle frontal gyrus; FDR correction for 27 comparisons revealed a q* = 0.002.
3.1. Reverse Tph2‐/‐ phenotype
Impulsivity positively correlated with aggression, but not anxiety. Aggression and anxiety were negatively related; all correlations were significant across all subjects and genotype‐specifically (impulsivity*aggression: R = .541, p = .000; RGG = 0.420, p = .015, RT+ = 0.582, p = .002; Z ZGGvs.T+ = 1.1, p = .149; aggression*anxiety: R = −.378, p = .003; RGG = −0.341, p = .052, RT+ = −0.408, p = .043, ZGGvs.T+ = 0.4, p = .356). Neither impulsivity, nor aggression, or anxiety differed significantly between sexes, and only anxiety correlated with age (R impulsivity = −0.181, p = .086; R aggression = −0.238, p = .065; R anxiety = 0.356, p = .001).
3.1.1. Analysis step 1
Group comparisons did not reveal any significant difference between TPH2 G‐703T variants; however, in absolute values T+ were more impulsive compared with GG (uncorrected p < .05), and aggression scores were higher (Table 1).
3.1.2. Analysis step 2a
Multiple regressions revealed (a) on the behavioral level, a positive influence of impulsivity on rt (Table S2). (b) On brain structure, no significant correlations were found (Table S3). (c) In brain activation, the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype correlated predominantly with activation in the reward condition: Whereas impulsivity and aggression correlated with overlapping regions in the triangular part of the right IFG and the right MFG, anxiety correlated with an isolated region in right MFG. In addition, correlation with aggression was negative, and correlations with impulsivity and anxiety were positive (Table 2, Figure 1). During cue processing, impulsivity and aggression positively correlated with activation in the opercular part of the IFG (impulsivity: left IFG, aggression: right IFG), the same was found for anxiety during target processing (right IFG) (Table 2).
FIGURE 1.
represents brain regions, associated with the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype. On the left, brain activation is plotted on a representative brain surface. On the right, scatterplot represents correlations
3.1.3. Analysis step 2b
Mediation analyses using (a) behavioral, (b) structural, and (c) functional parameters alone did not reveal a significant interaction with TPH2 G‐703T and the impulsive/aggressive/anxious phenotype.
However, when combining structural and functional parameters, significant serial mediations were found: The relation between the TPH2 G‐703T genotype and the aggressive phenotype was mediated by brain activation in the right IFG (triangular part) and right MFG volume (Table 3, ind 4), and right IFG volume (Table 3, ind 6). Furthermore, indirect effects differed between genotypes: Whereas the mediation effect of IFG (triangular part) activation and MFG volume (ind 4) was stronger in T+, mediation effect of IFG (triangular part) activation, MFG activation, and MFG volume (ind 6) was stronger in GG (Figure 2).
FIGURE 2.
represents the significant mediation model for aggression as part of the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype with the independent factor X = TPH2 G‐703T, the dependent variable Y = aggression, and mediating variables M1 = aggression‐related activation in the right IFGtri, M2 = aggression‐associated activation in the MFG, M3 = right IFGtri volume, and M4 = right MFG volume. On the left side, significant T+‐specific indirect effect is presented, that is, in T‐allele carriers, the TPH2 G‐703T genotype is related to trait aggression not directly but via right IFG activation and right MFG volume. On the right side, GG‐specific indirect effect is depicted, that is, in GG homozygotes, and the TPH2 G‐703T genotype is related to trait aggression not directly but via right IFG activation, MFG activation and right MFG volume
4. DISCUSSION
In this study, we addressed the mechanisms underlying the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype in children and adolescents with and without ADHD. We showed that the influence of TPH2 G‐703T on aggression (as part of the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype) was mediated by brain activation and regional volume of the triangular part of the right IFG and the right MFG. We did not find similar associations for impulsivity and anxiety. Thus, for aggression we were able to demonstrate a similar genotype/phenotype relation in mice and humans.
4.1. Aggression and prefrontal structures
Aggression in humans is associated with an imbalance within a cortico‐limbic network (Rosell & Siever, 2015), that is, a deficient regulation of the amygdala via prefrontal areas (Coccaro et al., 2011). Fronto‐amygdala pathways have been involved in the control of aggressive impulses (Bufkin & Luttrell, 2005); reduced volumes in the prefrontal cortex and amygdala were associated with antisocial traits (Gregory et al., 2012), MFG activation counteracted with aggressive reactions (Achterberg et al., 2016), and IFG–amygdala connectivity was reduced in aggressive individuals (Bogerts et al., 2018). Specifically, findings suggested right‐lateralized alterations for both volume reductions in the prefrontal cortex (Cha et al., 2015) and IFG–amygdala connectivity (Gilam et al., 2017). In line with these findings, we found that prefrontal regions were lateralized in the right hemisphere and that predominantly the IFG and MFG were involved.
However, we did not find any correlation between aggression and amygdala. This finding could not be explained by diagnostic group, as (a) all analyses were corrected for group and (b) there were no a priori group differences between ADHD and TDC in our sample. In addition, post hoc group‐specific multiple regressions did not reveal significant relations between aggression and amygdala volume neither for ADHD nor for TDC (ADHD: betaaggression = 0.06, p = .792; TDC: betaaggression = 0.04, p = .858). Likewise, the lack of relation between aggression and amygdala volume was not confounded by group‐specific brain development, as multiple regressions were corrected for age, and post hoc group‐specific age correlations did not reveal a significant correlation (ADHD: RrAMY_vol*age = −0.07, p = .722; TDC: RrAMY_vol*age = −0.02, p = .921). Thus, especially the role of the amygdala should be further investigated in future studies including greater sample sizes.
4.2. Aggression and TPH2 G‐703T
Aggression has been shown to vary between TPH2 G‐703T variants in numerous studies. For example, Yoon et al. (2012) linked TPH2 G‐703T to anger‐related personality traits, finding that GG had a higher anger control scores compared with T+ and that the correlation between aggression and orbitofrontal cortex volume differed between genotypes, suggesting that the orbitofrontal cortex is “an intermediate phenotype that bridges serotonin synthesis and anger‐related traits. The mechanism underlying the effect of the TPH2 gene on OFC abnormality, however, may be complex and may involve several processes related to anger expression.” (Yoon et al., 2012) More generally, Wolf et al. (2018) showed serotonergic modulation of aggression in a pharmacological fMRI study. Participants were playing a video game during fMRI scanning after a single dose of selective 5‐HT reuptake inhibitor (escitalopram). They found that medication reduced neural response in the right IFG and ACC to violent but not nonviolent actions and underlined the validity of serotonin in the modeling of aggressive behavior (Rosell & Siever, 2015; Wolf et al., 2018). Similar effects have also been found in mice: After injections of 5‐HT receptor agonists in the prefrontal cortex, animals showed reduced aggressive behavior in terms of attack bites and lateral threats (Centenaro et al., 2008). Likewise, we found that the mediation effect differed between TPH2 G‐703T variants: In contrast to GG, in T+ IFG volume was not a significant mediator. Thus, our results expand on these findings, (a) suggesting IFG as further region increasing susceptibility to serotonergic imbalance and (b) showing that brain function in addition to structure plays a role in serotonergic modulation.
4.3. Relation between impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety
At first sight, examining impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety in humans may seem counter‐intuitive (Cohn et al., 2016). However, they represent a cohesive behavioral program for reacting in stressful and dangerous situations: the fight or flight response, with fight representing the “impulsive/aggressive” way to react and flight the rather “anxious” manner to respond. Thus, impulsivity, aggression, and anxiety represent internal emotional states and are natural adaptive consequences of stress to cope with the stressor (Lesch, 2005). Additionally, in the context of ADHD symptoms aggression and anxiety have been shown to closely interact (Schatz & Rostain, 2006), even though the direction of relations is still being discussed. The exacerbation hypothesis proposes that the presence of anxiety increases the risk of aggression arguing that anxiety increases the emotional response to a stressful situation, thus, lowers the threshold for the need to react, and inhibits the fine‐tuned behavioral reaction. The attenuation hypothesis assumes that the presence of anxiety protects against aggression as anxiety inhibits process inducing rather freezing behavior than aggressive one. A very recent study, in return, showed that the relation between aggression and anxiety was direct (neither mediated nor moderated by further variables), that is, aggression was negatively related to anxiety and did not interact with ADHD symptoms at all (Murray et al., 2018). Overlapping function between impulsivity and aggression and inverse relation between aggression and anxiety could be found in our fMRI findings as well: a priori correlations revealed a positive relation between impulsivity and aggression, whereas it was negative between aggression and anxiety. Likewise, impulsivity‐ and aggression‐related regions were in overlapping areas, whereas anxiety was associated with an isolated region in the MFG. Finally, while the more anxious subjects were, the stronger they activated the MFG, it was reverse in aggression (i.e., the more aggressive subjects were, the weaker MFG and IFG activation).
4.4. TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) polymorphism and ADHD
Even though numerous papers showed that this polymorphism plays a significant role in the context of ADHD via reduced serotonin in T‐allele carriers, these findings are not undisputed as they were not replicated in recent studies. For example, the TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) polymorphism did not show up as a risk gene in large GWAS study (Demontis et al., 2019) and a recent meta‐analysis (Ottenhof et al., 2018) did not show an association with ADHD. In addition, the common noncoding G‐703T polymorphisms have not been conclusively shown to alter expression of TPH2, and thus, serotonin function: Scheuch et al. (2007) reported that it is more likely the rs11178997 of the human TPH2 that significantly reduces promoter activity, not rs4570625 (Scheuch et al., 2007). Similar, Heinrich et al. (2017) showed that methylation in the TPH2, probe cg14791008 was associated with reward‐based reaction times and ADHD symptoms in ADHD patients (Heinrich et al., 2017). However, a genome‐wide link between genes, cortical surface area, and ADHD has been shown in a recent paper by Gutiérrez et al. (2002). They found for ADHD significant negative genetic correlations between surface area of the MFG and IFG and numerous chromosomes (see Grasby et al., Table S1), for example, between the triangular part of the IFG and chromosome 11q23.1. Chromosome 11q23.1 has been linked to serotonergic 5‐hydroxytryptamine A‐receptor (Gutiérrez et al., 2002; Weiss et al., 1995), which also have both been reported to be relevant in ADHD (Hou et al., 2018; Li et al., 2014; Oades et al., 2008) undermining the role of serotonin in the context of ADHD in general and manifestation of serotonergic modulation in the triangular part of the IFG in particular. Similar genetic correlation analyses with aggression, in return, did not reveal any significant results (Fernàndez‐Castillo & Cormand, 2016; Grasby et al., 2020). Thus, it would be crucial in future studies (a) to examine multiple (TPH2) gene variants and/or perform genome‐wide analyses, (b) to quantify their influence on promotor activity in human brain, for example, via the determination of peripheral serotonin levels, and (c) to address multiple behavioral/cognitive dimensions like in the reversed phenotyping approach, when studying TPH2 effects on brain structure and function of the normally and pathologically developing brain.
5. LIMITATIONS
In summary, we examined the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype was present in humans. We were able to detect a mechanism for aggression but not for impulsivity and anxiety. One argument is that the reversed Tph2‐/‐ phenotype is a phenotype of a complete knockout mouse, which is not reflected by T+. Analogies between heterozygote knockout mice would have been more realistic, and therefore, it is plausible that we did not find the full phenotype. Another explanation might be that the waiting impulsivity network was too complex. Impulsivity tasks stimulating, for example, only the fronto‐striatal loop such as the GoNogo task might have shown similar PFC results, however, leaving more statistical variance to only one further region instead of multiple, enhancing the probability of significant results also for the second region (Cohen, 2013). However, in our study, we chose the 4‐CSRTT as the current study is part of a translational project and the 4‐CSRTT is a task of which versions for both humans and animals have been designed and published by the same laboratory (Bari et al., 2008; Voon et al., 2014), offering a unique chance to compare behavioral performance between two species.
Finally, the approach of mediation analyses has statistical limitations. One major aspect is that mediations promise to predict causal relations. However, this is only the case, if one pre‐assumption is fulfilled, the no confounding or ignorability assumption (Lee et al., 2019). This means that all major variables are implemented in the model. Thus, potential confounders might also have influenced our findings, for example, the bivariate differentiation between genotypes. We know that epigenetic markers modulate the impact of genotypes (Schuebel et al., 2016); thus, further genetic markers such as methylation (Gottschalk & Domschke, 2016) of TPH2 G‐703T (rs4570625) would have been highly advantageous. Furthermore, we examine the same variables for three different phenotype dimensions. Thus, insignificant results on anxiety and impulsivity may reflect that additional variables were lacking, which was not the case regarding aggression. Finally, we cannot exclude that “unmeasured confounding may still introduce bias even if known confounders have been adjusted for” page 698, (Lee et al., 2019).
6. CONCLUSION
In this study, we used the “reverse phenotyping” approach as a potential methodological way to combine findings from animal and human literature. At the example of aggression, we were able to show that the animal phenotype in humans was based on a similar gene X (brain X)–phenotype interaction as described in animal findings. In our view, this is a promising approach for a direct way to examine experimental models in psychiatric populations. Future studies need to confirm how the found relationships may account for ADHD‐associated symptoms and covarying comorbid disorders in the context of ongoing development throughout early and later adulthood.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None of the authors declare any conflict of interest.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTION
KPL, MR, AS‐B, and SN were involved in designing the concept of project; KPL and AS‐B involved in the animal part of the study and genotyping; and MR and SN involved in the human part of the study and funding acquisition. SN and KP were responsible for recruitment and examination of participants (phenotyping and scanning). SN, AA, and AR performed statistical analyses and manuscript drafting. All participants finalized the submitted manuscript.
PEER REVIEW
The peer review history for this article is available at https://publons.com/publon/10.1002/brb3.2054.
Supporting information
Fig S1
Table S1
Table S2
Table S3
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
MR and SN were funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation)—Project number 44541416‐TRR 58 and Project Number 404502177. MR was funded by the Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung (BMBF, German Federal Ministry of Education and Research Grant 01EE1408). KPL is supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG: CRC‐TRR 58 A1/A5), the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007‐2013) under Grant No. 602805 (Aggressotype), and the Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme under Grant No. 728018 (Eat2BeNICE), 5‐100 Russian Academic Excellence Project, ERA‐Net NEURON/RESPOND, No. 01EW1602B, and ERA‐Net NEURON/DECODE, No. 01EW1902. DFG and BMBF had no further role in the interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; and in the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Akhrif A, Roy A, Peters K, et al. REVERSE phenotyping—Can the phenotype following constitutive Tph2 gene inactivation in mice be transferred to children and adolescents with and without adhd? Brain Behav. 2021;11:e02054. 10.1002/brb3.2054
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.
REFERENCES
- Achterberg, M. , van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. , Bakermans‐Kranenburg, M. J. , & Crone, E. A. (2016). Control your anger! The neural basis of aggression regulation in response to negative social feedback. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience., 11(5), 712–720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alenina, N. , Kikic, D. , Todiras, M. (2009). Growth retardation and altered autonomic control in mice lacking brain serotonin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(25), 10332–10337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baehne, C. G. , Ehlis, A. C. , Plichta, M. M. (2009). Tph2 gene variants modulate response control processes in adult ADHD patients and healthy individuals. Molecular Psychiatry, 14(11), 1032–1039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bari, A. , Dalley, J. W. , & Robbins, T. W. (2008). The application of the 5‐choice serial reaction time task for the assessment of visual attentional processes and impulse control in rats. Nature Protocols, 3(5), 759–767. 10.1038/nprot.2008.41 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bi, X. , Yang, L. , Li, T. , Wang, B. , Zhu, H. , & Zhang, H. (2017). Genome‐wide mediation analysis of psychiatric and cognitive traits through imaging phenotypes. Human Brain Mapping, 38(8), 4088–4097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bogerts, B. , Schone, M. , & Breitschuh, S. (2018). Brain alterations potentially associated with aggression and terrorism. CNS Spectrums., 23(2), 129–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brivio, P. , Sbrini, G. , Peeva, P. , Todiras, M. , Bader, M. , Alenina, N. , & Calabresea, F. (2018). TPH2 deficiency influences neuroplastic mechanisms and alters the response to an acute stress in a sex specific manner. Frontiers Molecular Neuroscience, 11, 389–389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S. M. , Peet, E. , Manuck, S. B. (2005) A regulatory variant of the human tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 gene biases amygdala reactivity. Molecular Psychiatry, 10(9):884–888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bufkin, J. L. , & Luttrell, V. R. (2005). Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: Current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 6(2), 176–191. 10.1177/1524838005275089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Canli, T. , Congdon, E. , Todd Constable, R. , & Lesch, K. P. (2008). Additive effects of serotonin transporter and tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 gene variation on neural correlates of affective processing. Biological Psychology, 79(1), 118–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centenaro, L. A. , Vieira, K. , Zimmermann, N. , Miczek, K. A. , Lucion, A. B. , & de Almeida, R. M. (2008). Social instigation and aggressive behavior in mice: Role of 5‐HT1A and 5‐HT1B receptors in the prefrontal cortex. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 201(2), 237–248. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cha, J. , Fekete, T. , Siciliano, F. , Biezonski, D. , Greenhill, L. , & Pliszka, S. R. (2015). Neural correlates of aggression in medication‐naive children with adhd: Multivariate analysis of morphometry and tractography. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(7), 1717–1725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coccaro, E. F. , Sripada, C. S. , Yanowitch, R. N. , & Phan, K. L. (2011). Corticolimbic function in impulsive aggressive behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), 1153–1159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Cohn, M. D. , van Lith, K. , Kindt, M. , Pape, L. E. , Doreleijers, T. A. H. , van den Brink, W. , Veltman, D. J. , & Popma, A. (2016). Fear extinction, persistent disruptive behavior and psychopathic traits: fMRI in late adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(7), 1027–1035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalley, J. W. , Everitt, B. J. , & Robbins, T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top‐down cognitive control. Neuron, 69(4), 680–694. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demontis, D. , Walters, R. K. , Martin, J. , Mattheisen, M. , Als, T. D. , & Agerbo, E. (2019). Discovery of the first genome‐wide significant risk loci for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nature Genetics, 51(1), 63–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Desikan, R. S. , Ségonne, F. , Fischl, B. , Quinn, B. T. , Dickerson, B. C. , Blacker, D. , Buckner, R. L. , Dale, A. M. , Maguire, R. P. , Hyman, B. T. , Albert, M. S. , & Killiany, R. J. (2006). An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage, 31(3), 968–980. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Döpfner, M. , & Görtz‐Dorten, A. (2017). Diagnostik‐system für psychische störungen nach icd‐10 und dsm‐5 für kinder‐und jugendliche (DISYPS‐III). Hogrefe. [Google Scholar]
- Duke, A. A. , Begue, L. , Bell, R. , & Eisenlohr‐Moul, T. (2013). Revisiting the serotonin‐aggression relation in humans: A meta‐analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1148–1172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fernàndez‐Castillo, N. , & Cormand, B. (2016) Aggressive behavior in humans: Genes and pathways identified through association studies. American Journal of Medical Genetics Neuropsychiatric Genetics Part B, 171(5), 676–696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischl, B. , Salat, D. H. , Busa, E. , Albert, M. , Dieterich, M. , Haselgrove, C. , van der Kouwe, A. , Killiany, R. , Kennedy, D. , Klaveness, S. , Montillo, A. , Makris, N. , Rosen, B. , & Dale, A. M. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: Automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron, 33(3), 341–355. 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Furmark, T. , Marteinsdottir, I. , Frick, A. (2016). Serotonin synthesis rate and the tryptophan hydroxylase‐2: G‐703T polymorphism in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Psychopharmacology (Oxford, England), 30(10), 1028–1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilam, G. , Maron‐Katz, A. , Kliper, E. , Lin, T. , Fruchter, E. , Shamir, R. , & Hendler, T. (2017). Tracing the neural carryover effects of interpersonal anger on resting‐state fmri in men and their relation to traumatic stress symptoms in a subsample of soldiers. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 252. 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gizer, I. R. , Ficks, C. , & Waldman, I. D. (2009). Candidate gene studies of ADHD: A meta‐analytic review. Human Genetics, 126(1), 51–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goertz‐Dorten, A. , & Döpfner, M. (2010). Fragebogen zum aggressiven Verhalten von Kindern: Hogrefe. Goettingen. [Google Scholar]
- Gottschalk, M. G. , & Domschke, K. (2016). Novel developments in genetic and epigenetic mechanisms of anxiety. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 29(1), 32–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grasby, K. L. , Jahanshad, N. , Painter, J. N. , Colodro‐Conde, L. , Bralten, J. , Hibar, D. P. , Lind, P. A. , & Pizzagalli, F. (2020). The genetic architecture of the human cerebral cortex. Science (NY), 367(6484), 6690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory, S. , Ffytche, D. , & Simmons, A. The antisocial brain: Psychopathy matters. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69(9):962–972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutiérrez, B. , Arranz, M. J. , Huezo‐Diaz, P. , Dempster, D. , Matthiasson, P. , Travis, M. , Munro, J. , Osborne, S. , & Kerwin, R. W. (2002). Novel mutations in 5‐HT3A and 5‐HT3B receptor genes not associated with clozapine response. Schizophrenia Research, 58(1), 93–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gutknecht, L. , Waider, J. , Kraft, S. , (2008). Deficiency of brain 5‐HT synthesis but serotonergic neuron formation in Tph2 knockout mice. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna), 115(8), 1127–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haghighi, F. , Bach‐Mizrachi, H. , Huang, Y. Y. . (2008). Genetic architecture of the human tryptophan hydroxylase 2 Gene: Existence of neural isoforms and relevance for major depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 13(8), 813–820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn, T. , Heinzel, S. , Notebaert, K. (2013). The tricks of the trait: Neural implementation of personality varies with genotype‐dependent serotonin levels. NeuroImage, 81, 393–399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression‐based approach. Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Heinrich, H. , Grunitz, J. , Stonawski, V. , Frey, S. , Wahl, S. , Albrecht, B. , Goecke, T. W. , Beckmann, M. W. , Kornhuber, J. , Fasching, P. A. , Moll, G. H. , & Eichler, A. Attention, cognitive control and motivation in ADHD: Linking event‐related brain potentials and DNA methylation patterns in boys at early school age. Scientific Reports, 2017, 7(1), 3823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hou, Y. W. , Xiong, P. , Gu, X. , Huang, X. , Wang, M. , & Wu, J. (2018). Association of serotonin receptors with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Current Medical Science, 38(3), 538–551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kneer, K. , Reinhard, J. , Ziegler, C. , Slyschak, A. , Schiele, M. , Vietz, M. , Peters, K. , & Meisenzahl, E. M. (2020). Serotonergic influence on depressive symptoms and trait anxiety is mediated by negative life events and frontal activation in children and adolescents. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(5), 691–706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ko, M. , Choi‐Kwon, S. , Jun, S.‐E. , Kim, J. H. , Cho, K.‐H. , Nah, H.‐W. , Song, H. , & Kim, J. S. (2018). Poststroke emotional disturbances and a tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene polymorphism. Brain Behav., 8(2), e00892–e00892. 10.1002/brb3.892 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kotting, W. F. , Bubenzer, S. , Helmbold, K. , Eisert, A. , Gaber, T. J. , & Zepf, F. D. (2013). Effects of tryptophan depletion on reactive aggression and aggressive decision‐making in young people with ADHD. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 128(2), 114–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee, H. , Herbert, R. D. , & McAuley, J. H. (2019). Mediation analysis. JAMA, 321(7), 697–698. 10.1001/jama.2018.21973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesch, K. P. (2005). Serotonergic gene inactivation in mice: Models for anxiety and aggression? Novartis Foundation Symposium, 268, 167–170. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesch, K. P. , Araragi, N. , Waider, J. , van den Hove, D. , & Gutknecht, L. (2012). Targeting brain serotonin synthesis: Insights into neurodevelopmental disorders with long‐term outcomes related to negative emotionality, aggression and antisocial behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 367(1601), 2426–2443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lesch, K. P. , & Merschdorf, U. (2000). Impulsivity, aggression, and serotonin: A molecular psychobiological perspective. Behavioral Sciences & the Law, 18(5), 581–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li, Z. , Chang, S. H. , Zhang, L. Y. , Gao, L. , & Wang, J. (2014). Molecular genetic studies of ADHD and its candidate genes: A review. Psychiatry Research, 219(1), 10–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, Y. M. , Chao, S. C. , Chen, T. M. , Lai, T. J. , Chen, J. S. , & Sun, H. S. (2007). Association of functional polymorphisms of the human tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene with risk for bipolar disorder in Han Chinese. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(9), 1015–1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lowry, C. A. , Hale, M. W. , Evans, A. K. (2008). Serotonergic systems, anxiety, and affective disorder: Focus on the dorsomedial part of the dorsal raphe nucleus. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1148, 86–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manor, I. , Laiba, E. , Eisenberg, J. , Meidad, S. , Lerer, E. , Israel, S. , Gritsenko, I. , Tyano, S. , Faraone, S. V. , & Ebstein, R. P. (2008) Association between tryptophan hydroxylase 2, performance on a continuance performance test and response to methylphenidate in ADHD participants. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 147b(8), 1501–1508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mark, M. D. , Wollenweber, P. , Gesk, A. , Kösters, K. , Batzke, K. , Janoschka, C. , Maejima, T. , Han, J. , Deneris, E. S. , & Herlitze, S. (2019). RGS2 drives male aggression in mice via the serotonergic system. Communications Biology, 2, 373–373. 10.1038/s42003-019-0622-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall, W. A. , & Tanner, J. M. (1969). Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 44(235), 291. 10.1136/adc.44.235.291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marshall, W. A. , & Tanner, J. M. (1970). Variations in the pattern of pubertal changes in boys. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 45(239), 13–23. 10.1136/adc.45.239.13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKinney, J. , Johansson, S. , Halmoy, A. (2008). A loss‐of‐function mutation in tryptophan hydroxylase 2 segregating with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry., 13(4), 365–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mechelmans, D. J. , Strelchuk, D. , Donamayor, N. (2017). Reward sensitivity and waiting impulsivity: Shift towards reward valuation away from action control. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 20(12), 971–978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mertens, G. , Braem, S. , Kuhn, M. , Lonsdorf, T. B. , van den Hout, M. A. , & Engelhard, I. M. (2018). Does US expectancy mediate the additive effects of CS‐US pairings on contingency instructions? Results from subjective, psychophysiological and neural measures. Behavior Research and Therapy, 110, 41–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Migliarini, S. , Pacini, G. , Pelosi, B. , Lunardi, G. , & Pasqualetti, M. (2013). Lack of brain serotonin affects postnatal development and serotonergic neuronal circuitry formation. Molecular Psychiatry, 18(10), 1106–1118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray, A. L. , Booth, T. , Obsuth, I. , Zirk‐Sadowski, J. , Eisner, M. , & Ribeaud, D. (2018). Testing the exacerbation and attenuation hypotheses of the role of anxiety in the relation between ADHD and reactive/proactive aggression: A 10‐year longitudinal study. Psychiatry Research, 269, 585–592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neufang, S. , Akhrif, A. , Herrmann, C. G. , Drepper, C. , Homola, G. A. , Nowak, J. , Waider, J. , Schmitt, A. G. , Lesch, K.‐P. , & Romanos, M. ( 2016). Serotonergic modulation of 'waiting impulsivity' is mediated by the impulsivity phenotype in humans. Translational Psychiatry, 6(11), e00940 10.1038/tp.2016.210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oades, R. D. , Lasky‐Su, J. , Christiansen, H. , Faraone, S. V. , Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. S. , Banaschewski, T. , Chen, W. , Anney, R. J. L. , Buitelaar, J. K. , Ebstein, R. P. , Franke, B. , Gill, M. , Miranda, A. , Roeyers, H. , Rothenberger, A. , Sergeant, J. A. , Steinhausen, H.‐C. , Taylor, E. A. , Thompson, M. , & Asherson, P. (2008). The influence of serotonin‐ and other genes on impulsive behavioral aggression and cognitive impulsivity in children with attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Findings from a family‐based association test (FBAT) analysis. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 4, 48. 10.1186/1744-9081-4-48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ottenhof, K. W. , Sild, M. , Levesque, M. L. , Ruhe, H. G. , & Booij, L. (2018). TPH2 polymorphisms across the spectrum of psychiatric morbidity: A systematic review and meta‐analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 29–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robinson, E. S. , Eagle, D. M. , Economidou, D. (2009). Behavioural characterisation of high impulsivity on the 5‐choice serial reaction time task: Specific deficits in 'waiting' versus 'stopping'. Behavioural Brain Research, 196(2), 310–316. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosell, D. R. , & Siever, L. J. (2015). The neurobiology of aggression and violence. CNS Spectrums, 20(3), 254–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruf, B. M. , Bessette, K. L. , Pearlson, G. D. , & Stevens, M. C. (2016) Effect of trait anxiety on cognitive test performance in adolescents with and without attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 3, 1–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachs, B. D. , Rodriguiz, R. M. , Siesser, W. B. (2013). The effects of brain serotonin deficiency on behavioural disinhibition and anxiety‐like behaviour following mild early life stress. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 16(9), 2081–2094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savelieva, K. V. , Zhao, S. , Pogorelov, V. M. , Rajan, I. , Yang, Q. , Cullinan, E. , & Lanthorn, T. H. (2008). Genetic disruption of both tryptophan hydroxylase genes dramatically reduces serotonin and affects behavior in models sensitive to antidepressants. PLoS One, 3(10), e3301. 10.1371/journal.pone.0003301 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schatz, D. B. , & Rostain, A. L. (2006). ADHD with comorbid anxiety: A review of the current literature. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10(2), 141–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheuch, K. , Lautenschlager, M. , Grohmann, M. , Stahlberg, S. , Kirchheiner, J. , Zill, P. , Heinz, A. , Walther, D. J. , & Priller, J. (2007). Characterization of a functional promoter polymorphism of the human tryptophan hydroxylase 2 gene in serotonergic raphe neurons. Biological Psychiatry, 62(11), 1288–1294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schuebel, K. , Gitik, M. , Domschke, K. , & Goldman, D. Making sense of epigenetics. (2016) International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 19(11), pyw058. 10.1093/ijnp/pyw058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schulze, T. G. , & McMahon, F. J. (2004). Defining the phenotype in human genetic studies: Forward genetics and reverse phenotyping. Human Heredity, 58(3–4), 131–138. 10.1159/000083539 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheehan, K. , Lowe, N. , Kirley, A. (2005). Tryptophan hydroxylase 2 (TPH2) gene variants associated with ADHD. Molecular Psychiatry, 10(10), 944–949. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinhausen, H. C. , Novik, T. S. , Baldursson, G. , Curatolo, P. , Lorenzo, M. J. , Rodrigues Pereira, R. , Ralston, S. J. , & Rothenberger, A. (2006). Co‐existing psychiatric problems in ADHD in the ADORE cohort. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(Suppl 1), I25–29. 10.1007/s00787-006-1004-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoltenberg, S. F. , Christ, C. C. , & Highland, K. B. (2012). Serotonin system gene polymorphisms are associated with impulsivity in a context dependent manner. Progress in neuro‐psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 39(1), 182–191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Unnewehr, S. , Joormann, S. , Schneider, S. , & Margraf, J. (1992). Deutsche Übersetzung des state‐trait anxiety inventory for children. Manuscript Drafting Process.
- von Polier, G. G. , Biskup, C. S. , Kotting, W. F. , Bubenzer, S. , Helmbold, K. , Eisert, A. , Gaber, T. J. , & Zepf, F. D. (2013). Change in electrodermal activity after acute tryptophan depletion associated with aggression in young people with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Neural Transmission, 121(4), 451–455. 10.1007/s00702-013-1119-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon, V. (2014). Models of impulsivity with a focus on waiting impulsivity: Translational potential for neuropsychiatric disorders. Current Addiction Reports, 1(4), 281–288. 10.1007/s40429-014-0036-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voon, V. , Irvine, M. A. , Derbyshire, K. , Worbe, Y. , Lange, I. , Abbott, S. , Morein‐Zamir, S. , Dudley, R. , & Caprioli, D. (2014). Measuring "waiting" impulsivity in substance addictions and binge eating disorder in a novel analogue of rodent serial reaction time task. Biological Psychiatry, 75(2), 148–155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waider, J. , Popp, S. , Mlinar, B. , Montalbano, A. , Bonfiglio, F. , Aboagye, B. , Thuy, E. , Kern, R. , Thiel, C. , Araragi, N. , Svirin, E. , Schmitt‐Böhrer, A. G. , Corradetti, R. , Lowry, C. A. , & Lesch, K.‐P. (2019). Serotonin deficiency increases context‐dependent fear learning through modulation of hippocampal activity. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 245–245. 10.3389/fnins.2019.00245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walitza, S. , Renner, T. J. , Dempfle, A. , Wewetzer, C. , Halbach, A. , Herpertz‐Dahlmann, B. , Remschmidt, H. , Smidt, J. , Linder, M. , Flierl, L. , Knölker, U. , Friedel, S. , Schäfer, H. , Gross, C. , Hebebrand, J. , Warnke, A. , & Lesch, K. P. (2005). Transmission disequilibrium of polymorphic variants in the tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 gene in attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 10(12), 1126–1132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss, B. , Mertz, A. , Schröck, E. , Koenen, M. , & Rappold, G. (1995). Assignment of a human homolog of the mouse Htr3 receptor gene to chromosome 11q23.1‐q23.2. Genomics, 29(1), 304–305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiß, R. H. (2006) Grundintelligenztest,Skala 2 (CFT 20‐R) mit Wortschatztest (WS) und Zahlenfolgentest (ZF). Intellektuelle Hochbegabung. 80.
- Wolf, D. , Klasen, M. , Eisner, P. , Zepf, F. D. , Zvyagintsev, M. , Palomero‐Gallagher, N. , Weber, R. , Eisert, A. , & Mathiak, K. (2018). Central serotonin modulates neural responses to virtual violent actions in emotion regulation networks. Brain Structure & Function, 223(7), 3327–3345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoon, H. K. , Lee, H. J. , Kim, L. , Lee, M. S. , & Ham, B. J. (2012). Impact of tryptophan hydroxylase 2 G‐703T polymorphism on anger‐related personality traits and orbitofrontal cortex. Behavioural Brain Research, 231(1), 105–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang, X. , Beaulieu, J. M. , Sotnikova, T. D. , Gainetdinov, R. R. , & Caron, M. G. (2004) Tryptophan hydroxylase‐2 controls brain serotonin synthesis. Science, 305(5681), 217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zill, P. , Baghai, T. C. , Zwanzger, P. (2004). SNP and haplotype analysis of a novel tryptophan hydroxylase isoform (TPH2) gene provide evidence for association with major depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 9(11), 1030–1036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zill, P. , Büttner, A. , Eisenmenger, W. , Bondy, B. , & Ackenheil, M. (2004). Regional mRNA expression of a second tryptophan hydroxylase isoform in postmortem tissue samples of two human brains. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 14(4), 282–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Fig S1
Table S1
Table S2
Table S3
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.