Abstract
Objective
To determine the association between country-level structural ageism and prevalence of violence against older persons.
Design
Country-level ecological study.
Setting
Structural ageism data were drawn from the nationally representative World Values Survey 2010–2014 (WVS), global databases from the WHO, United Nations and the World Bank. Violence data were based on the Global Burden of Diseases (GBD) study 2017.
Participants
Analysis of 56 countries that represented 63.1% of the world’s ageing population aged 60 and over across all six of WHO regions.
Exposure
Structural ageism, following established structural stigma measures, consisted of two components: (1) discriminatory national policies related to older persons’ economic, social, civil and political rights, based on the four core components of human rights protection in Madrid International Plan of Action on Aging and (2) prejudicial social norms against older persons, measured by negative attitudes toward older persons in 56 national polls in WVS aggregated to country-level. These components were z scored and combined such that higher score indicated greater structural ageism.
Main outcomes and measures
Prevalence rates of violence per 100 000 persons aged 70 and over in each country was based on extensive epidemiological surveillance data, survey, clinical data and insurance claims in GBD and compiled by the Institute of Health Metrics and Evaluation, University of Washington.
Results
There was a wide variation in levels of structural ageism across countries. As predicted, structural ageism was significantly associated with the prevalence rates of violence in multivariate models (β=205.7, SE=96.3, p=0.03), after adjusting for relevant covariates. Sensitivity analyses supported the robustness of our findings. That is, structural ageism did not predict other types of violence and other types of prejudice did not predict violence against older persons.
Conclusions
This study provides the first evidence of the association between higher structural ageism and greater violence against older persons across countries.
Keywords: geriatric medicine, public health, social medicine
Strengths and limitations of this study.
The current study, which is based on 56 countries that represented 63.1% of the world’s ageing population aged 60 and over across all six of WHO regions, is one of the largest cross-national investigations on violence against older persons to date.
The ecological analysis examines a previously unexplored link between structural ageism and violence against older persons.
A strength of our investigation was it examined country-level structural ageism for the first time by combining social norms and laws; this methodologically validated approach was informed by extensive scholarship on other types of structural stigma including of women and sexual minorities.
The robustness of the structural ageism-violence linkage was supported by three additional sets of sensitivity analyses.
The ecological design with country-level information could be strengthened by adding individual-level data.
Introduction
Violence directed against older persons is a pervasive public health problem. Globally, prevalence of violence against older persons has increased significantly over the last two decades.1 One in six older persons experiences elder abuse in the past year.2 Older persons’ safety may be particularly compromised during the current COVID-19 pandemic as they experience increased exposures to wide-spread age-based discrimination, social isolation with perpetrators and reduced options for support.3 The associated social, psychological and financial stressors further present significant barriers for reporting and help-seeking.4 To this end, the United Nations (UN) has called for improved protection for older persons’ safety and well-being during this health crisis.5
Addressing violence against older persons require population-level solutions. The WHO and the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) have strongly recommended the integration of a socio-ecological framework in violence prevention research and practice.6 Based on this multi-level model, risk factors for violence against older persons operate across individual, relationship, community and societal levels of social ecology, that jointly place individuals at a higher risk of violence victimisation and perpetration. However, the majority of research on violence against older persons pertain to individual factors.7 Compounding this lack of evidence beyond individuals is the disproportionate focus on the deficits of victims that erroneously suggests victims are to be blamed.8
Societal-level risk factors in violence against older persons deserve more attention given that interventions could be most effective when context-changing strategies are in place.9 This knowledge void may be owing to a few conceptual, measurement and methodological challenges. While contexts shape interactions, existing theories have largely overlooked the ways in which macro-level factors, such as policies and cultural norms specific to ageing, may be linked to downstream individual behaviours. Additionally, cross-national comparisons are lacking in this line of research.7 With data typically collected in a single country, participants’ responses may be restricted by the particular cultural climate in that country, hence, limiting the understanding of the role of societal determinants on violence victimisation and perpetration across cultures.10
As the most widespread form of bias and prejudice,11 12 ageism harms older persons’ health simultaneously at both structural and individual levels. At the structural level, ageism is manifested in the forms of explicit and implicit policies, practices or social norms that impose bias and discrimination against older persons. At the individual level, ageism is manifested by negative age stereotypes and negative self-perceptions of ageing. As premised by the stereotype embodiment theory (SET),13 both levels are closely intertwined because individual-level ageism is assimilated by older persons from surrounding cultures that propagate structural ageism. A recent systematic review based on 7 million older participants across 25 years found evidence the injurious health effects of structural ageism existed across country borders, but none of the studies included examined violence against older persons as its health outcome.14
There are strong theoretical and empirical evidence from parallel tracks of stigma and violence research against women and sexual minorities to suggest the potential link between structural ageism and violence against older persons.15 As suggested by SET, a plausible psychological pathway may be that ageism operating at the structural level could trickle down to shape individuals’ negative age beliefs, which in turn affect behavioural outcomes.13 Additionally, one might postulate that in social contexts that denigrate a group, individuals tend to be more accepting of violence toward that group. For instance, research in family violence has found that cultures with greater sexism exhibited higher tolerance of intimate partner violence.16 17
Finally, considering structural ageism embodies a sociopolitical climate that disempower older persons, the embedded hierarchical power relations may also leave older persons with less resources to protect themselves from violence and its associated risk factors. However, until now, the assumed link between structural ageism and violence had not been tested.18 19
In this present study, we predicted that structural ageism would be associated with greater prevalence of violence against older persons, after adjusting for socioeconomic and health risk factors.
Methods
Data sources
Data for our predictor, structural ageism, were drawn from the latest available wave of World Values Survey (WVS) in 2010–2014 and global health databases including the WHO and UN. The WVS consists of nationally representative polls of individuals’ attitudes and behaviours since 1981.20 Outcome data on the prevalence estimates of violence were drawn from the Global Burden of Disease (GBD) study in 2017 compiled by the Institute of Health Metrics and Evaluation (IHME) at the University of Washington.21 Data sources for covariates included WHO and the World Bank. Countries that had data for structural ageism measures and prevalence estimates for violence formed our final analyses, which was consisted of 56 countries representing 82 249 respondents in WVS.20 Together, these countries accounted for 63.1% of the global older population aged 60 years and older, representing all six WHO regions.
Exposure: structural ageism
To operationalise structural ageism, we followed a methodologically validated approach informed by extensive scholarship on structural stigma, which includes discriminatory social policies and prejudicial social norms.22–24 Discriminatory social policies referred to macro-level policies and practices that discriminate against or restrict the resources and opportunities for older persons. In order to match with prejudicial social norms that were measured between 2010 and 2014 in the WVS, the presence of four policies between 2010 and 2014 selected for the present index reflected four core components of human rights protection in the Madrid International Plan of Action on Aging,25 including economic, social, civil and political rights.26 As the most comprehensive international policy framework to address population ageing,27 the Madrid Plan strives to eliminate all forms of violence and discrimination against older persons.26 In our index, the protection of economic rights was based on whether or not each country had enacted pension reform laws including raising retirement benefits of workers as initially reported by country, and then subsequently validated and reported by UN experts in UN’s World Population Policies database.28 The protection of social rights—or recognising older persons as a social group deserving of their own rights, was assessed by coding the presence of national policies that included healthy ageing as a priority policy. Each country self-reported whether or not they have developed laws to protect the well-being of their ageing populations. Data were collected and compiled by United Nations Population Fund (UNFPA).29 The protection of civil rights was based on the existence of employment non-discrimination policies for older workers, drawn from expert evaluation in the publicly available Employment Protection Legislation Database, UN’s International Labor Organizations.30 The protection of political rights was based on the existence of constitutional-level protection against age discrimination, reported by each country and compiled by UNFPA.29
The absence or presence of each of these four policies were summed into a continuous variable, ranging from 0 to 4. Higher score indicated that a country had greater structural ageism, as indicated by fewer policies protecting older persons. Overall, 17.9% had one discriminatory policy, 42.9% had two discriminatory policies, 16.1% had all four indicators of discriminatory policies. Only one had zero (Spain) (see online supplemental table 1).
bmjopen-2020-042580supp001.pdf (31.8KB, pdf)
The second domain in our index pertains to societal-level prejudicial attitudes toward older persons that reflects overarching public opinions.24 31 Participants in WVS were asked their level of agreement on ‘older persons are a burden on society’. Score ranged from 1 to 4, with high levels indicated higher level of burden. The mean value was aggregated at the country-level. Higher values indicate more prejudicial social norms against older persons. Mean (SD) was 1.8 (0.2) that ranged from 1.2 to 2.4. Overall, 45.0% of all countries reported above-average level of endorsement in this statement.
Following analytical procedures in creating structural stigma indices,32 33 we standardised the scores for each of the two domains separately, and then summed up both z-transformed score to create the structural ageism index. Given the values ranged from negative to positive in standardised scores, to ease interpretation, we added the positive value of the lowest negative value across all scores, so that the final scores would be equal or larger than 0. A separate factor analysis showed that both domains loaded on the same factor (eigenvalues>1.0; factor loading=0.74), suggesting one underlying latent factor supporting the composite structural ageism index.
Structural ageism was examined as a continuous variable in the bivariate and multivariable models. As a secondary analysis to quantify risk levels of structural ageism in relation to prevalence of violence, we operationalised structural ageism as a categorical variable based on tertiles of final scores (ie, low-level, medium-level and high-level of structural ageism).
Outcome: prevalence estimates of violence against older persons
We obtained prevalence estimates of violence from the GBD, one of the most comprehensive cross-national epidemiological studies on injuries, morbidities and mortality based on extensive survey, epidemiological surveillance and clinical data sources.21 Recent reiteration of GBD was conducted by the IHME at the University of Washington using Bayesian meta-regression model to estimate rates of prevalence for each health and injury domain. In 2017, GBD was based on 68 781 data sources used for the analysis of non-fatal causes of disease and injury for a total of 354 causes.21 Estimates were presented for those 70 and over which we used within each of the 56 countries. GBD defined interpersonal violence according to the International Classification of Diseases (ICD)-10 (X85-Y08.9, Y87.1)34 that covered three categories: (1) physical assault by any means, including firearm, bodily force, sharp or blunt objects, (2) sexual assault by bodily force and (3) mistreatment, neglect and abandonment, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, torture and cruelty. The prevalence of violence victimisation used in this study was prevalence rate of both fatal and non-fatal violence victimisation per 100 000 persons in the age group of 70 years and over that covered all three categories of interpersonal violence. Although not yet applied to violence against older persons, prevalence estimates of violence drawn from GBD have been applied in systematic cross-national analysis of other forms of violence including intimate partner violence and suicides.35
Country-level covariates
We considered a wide range of sociodemographic and health variables as potential covariates a priori, owing to their known relationships with violence.15 36–40 The pool of potential covariates, assessed in 2010 to match with the timing of the predictor, included (1) population ratio, measured by the proportion of the population 70 years and older relative to that of the younger-age population (20–69 years), (2) gross national income (GNI) per capita (in 1000 international dollar increments), (3) average years of schooling, (4) unemployment rate and (5) alcohol consumption per capita. These covariates were available for all of the 56 countries.
To maintain study power and create the most parsimonious model, final covariates were selected based on the backward elimination strategy with the significant level set at p<0.10. We performed a backward selection stepwise regression model in accordance with the Akaike information criterion (AIC), a measure of model deviance adjusted for the parameters in the model.41 This approach with stepwise regression models was commonly applied in previous country-level ecological studies of violence with relatively smaller sample sizes.42 43 Based on this variable selection procedure, three covariates were selected and thus retained in the final multivariable model: population ratio, GNI per capita and alcohol consumption per capita.
Statistical analysis
Pearson correlation coefficients were used to examine the association between structural ageism, violence prevalence estimates and covariates. Bivariate and multivariable linear regression models were used to estimate the relationship between structural ageism and prevalence rates of violence. Goodness of fit of the models to the data was evaluated using AIC. We used residual plots and multicollinearity diagnostics to examine issues of heteroscedasticity and multicollinearity. To evaluate potential outliers in the model, we examined whether any observations was more than one Cook’s distance.44
To examine the robustness of the findings, we conducted several additional sensitivity analyses. First, to examine the discriminant validity of the structural ageism index, analyses of were performed replacing the prevalence rates of violence in older age groups with the estimates of violence in children in the forms of violent disciplines and peer violence. We hypothesise that structural ageism would not be related to estimates of violence in children. Data were drawn from nationally representative surveys in the UNICEF global databases.45 Violent discipline by caregivers included psychological aggression, physical and corporal punishment, as measured by the Parent-Child Conflict Tactics Scale.46 Peer-violence was measured by the proportion of students aged 13–15 years who reported being bullied on one or more days in the past 30 days. The number of countries that had available estimates of violent discipline and peer violence during years of 2014–2017 that matched with participating countries in WVS were 19 and 21, respectively.
The second sensitivity analysis examined the predictive validity of the structural ageism index by assessing the relationship between anti-immigrant, racial prejudice and prevalence estimates interpersonal violence in older age. We hypothesise that anti-immigrant and racial prejudice attitudes would not be related to interpersonal violence in older age. Anti-immigrant and racial prejudice attitudes were drawn from the WVS. Participants were asked to state which groups they would not like to have as neighbours: ‘people of another race’ or ‘immigrants/foreign workers’. This measure has been used to assess negative attitudes toward minority group members.47 48
The third sensitivity analysis examined whether the effects of structural ageism on violence prevalence estimates were only specific to older age groups, but not younger age groups. Based on GBD study 2017, we obtained prevalence rates of violence in age 15–49 years old.
All analyses were conducted in SAS (V.9.4, SAS Institute).
Patient and public involvement
Neither patients nor the public were involved in this research.
Results
As predicted, structural ageism was significantly associated with higher prevalence of violence against older persons. This was found in both bivariate (β=261.0, SE=106.0, p=0.02) and multivariable models (β=205.7, SE=96.3, p=0.03). After controlling for covariates, a one SD increase in the structural ageism index was associated with a 205.7 per 100 000 persons increase in the prevalence of violence against older persons aged 70 years and older. Also as predicted, in a secondary analysis that included the categorisation of the low-structural, medium-structural and high-structural ageism predictor, there was a linear pattern between increasing levels of ageism and higher prevalence rates of violence, after adjusting for covariates (test for linear trend: p=0.02) (figure 1).
With respect to model diagnostics, collinearity tests indicated no evidence of multicollinearity.49 Residual plots confirmed the model assumptions (normality and homoscedasticity of residuals) were met. All but two cases (China and Qatar) had a larger than Cook’s distance cut-off of one for outliers. When we removed China and Qatar, respectively from the multivariable model estimates, the positive association between structural ageism and prevalence remained significant.
Our results showed wide variation in levels of structural ageism across countries, with higher value indicating greater structural ageism (total values ranged from 0 to 7.3) (online supplemental table 1). Nigeria, Lebanon and Belarus reported highest structural ageism. Uzbekistan, Cyprus and Spain had the lowest structural ageism. China, Russia and Zimbabwe had the highest prevalence rates of violence against older persons; whereas Singapore, Germany and Egypt had the lowest prevalence rates of violence against older persons. In support of this index, the scoring of structural ageism in this study significantly correlated with a recent parallel report that ranked country-level ageism based on social indices of five domains, including economic, health, employment, environment and social participation, across 15 Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development countries (R=0.59, p=0.02).50
Results from three sensitivity analyses suggested the robustness of the results. First, in support of the discriminant validity of structural ageism index, ageism was not correlated with violent discipline (R=0.31, p=0.21) or bullying (R=−0.13, p=0.59). Second, in support of the predictive validity of structural ageism index, anti-immigrant and racial prejudice were not correlated with violence estimates against older persons (R=−0.18, p=0.20; R=−0.17, p=0.20, respectively). Third, the relationship between structural ageism and violence was non-significant in the younger age group of 15–49 years old, suggesting the validity of our findings.
Discussion
This study investigated a previously unexplored relationship between structural ageism and violence against older persons. As predicted, we found that structural ageism is significantly associated with prevalence rates of violence against persons aged 70 years and over, after controlling for known risk factors. Our findings suggest a comprehensive strategy for preventing violence against older persons should include structural ageism.
Public health research on improving support structures and societal-based solutions is needed to effectively prevent violence against older persons at a large scale, especially in times of unrest and relative instability. A recent CDC report estimated that the rate of non-fatal assaults against persons 60 years and older has risen by 53% between 2008 and 2016.51 Additionally, recent reports have indicated increasing rates of interpersonal violence in family settings during the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic.52 As psychological stressors continue to intensify during the ongoing pandemic as the consequences of self-quarantine, expanding structural-level programme response to improve the safety of older persons would be essential.
A strength of our investigation was it examined country-level structural ageism combining social laws and norms. The wide variation in country-level attitudes toward older persons was in line with previous cross-cultural analyses.53 54 In the domain of policies, we also found variation in the level of each country’s protection toward older persons. This may be a reflection of the gap in existing legal provisions and international conventions specifically supporting the rights of older persons.55
Congruent with intersectional theories,56 our findings showed that countries reported higher structural ageism coincided with those that also reported greater inequality in other realms of stigma. For instance, Nigeria ranked among the highest in structural ageism in this study as well as highest in structural stigma against sexual minorities in a recent cross-cultural study of 197 countries.57 The opposite estimate was found in Spain where it ranked among the lowest in both structural stigma measures. Parallel scholarship in gender-based stigma also recently found that women residing in countries with greater structural-level gender-based stigma were more likely to experience violence.58 Indeed, both older age and female gender could be potential modifying factors in the association between ageism and violence. As structural systems of oppression are often mutually manifested to reinforce health inequalities, analyses that only focus on gender, race, ethnicity alone are insufficient to understand population-level health disparities.59 Future multi-level analyses that combine both population level and individual level data may assist in addressing the examination of these intersectional axes between older age, sex and structural stigma in predicting violence.
Integrating structural ageism as a societal-level risk factor in existing elder abuse research may also help inject a multi-systemic, sociocultural lens in developing much-needed interventions.60 Theories of elder abuse have been predominantly interpersonal in nature, where victims of abuse were typically described as uniformly dependent and powerless.61–63 The emphasis on older persons’ vulnerabilities is not only a form of victim-blaming, but also shifts our attention away from the broader status inequality that each individual is embedded in. Considering the ways in which structural ageism seep through social interactions and its downstream consequences in shaping the inherent power imbalance between victims, perpetrators and their environments can offer promising opportunities for primary prevention strategies.64
Our findings have a few limitations that point to future research directions. First, given that violence in general and elder abuse in particular is stigmatising and illegal in most of the countries, it is possible that the violence against older person was under-reported. Second, the current ecological study design did not allow us to produce casual inference between structural ageism and violence against older persons. However, there are two reasons that we consider it likely that greater structural ageism led to greater risk of violence. First, it does not seem likely that the reverse of a diagnostic health outcome would influence a structural-level variable. Second, to ascertain temporal association, our structural ageism variable was based on estimates in 2010–2014, that predated violence prevalence outcome in 2017. Finally, although we used global data with consistent definitions that allows for cross-national comparisons, the aggregate data structure would not permit individual-level interpretation. Future investigations should further assess whether the observed structural ageism-violence linkage extends to the individual level.
As one of the largest cross-country studies on violence against older persons to date, this study also has a number of methodological strengths including: mitigating data variability for cross-country violence prevalence estimates with various data processing and estimation techniques as employed by GBD study investigators21; following strict definition of interpersonal violence guided by ICD diagnostic codes; and developing and implementing the first structural ageism measure combining social norms and laws.
Our findings hold important implications for violence prevention programming. First, ageism that operates at both individual and structural level deserves more consideration in estimating the occurrence of violence and abuse in older persons. Second, social and legal policies are inherently public health policies.65 Social policies that protect the rights of older persons may reap significant public health benefits for population-level violence preventions. Third, as existing approaches for violence preventions are tailored toward individual ecology, positioning structural ageism as a societal risk factor of violence against older persons may help catalyse a paradigm shift in refining current primary preventions against violence and abuse. Such structural approach may focus on reducing ageism through improving political-legal, economic, as well as intergenerational support for older persons.66 Other ways that policy makers and public health officials can mitigate effects of structural ageism could be by establishing societal-wide campaigns that promote older persons’ rights and enhance diverse representation of ageing through social media.67
Conclusion
Our results suggest that structural ageism is a social determinant of elder abuse. Public health and population-based violence prevention policies may benefit from a targeted approach that tackles the harmful effects of structural ageism.
Supplementary Material
Footnotes
Twitter: @iggychang7
Contributors: E-SC was involved in study conceptualisation and design, data collection, analyses, interpretation of results and manuscript writing. JM and DZ contributed to study design, analyses and manuscript writing. BL contributed to study conceptualisation and design, supervised analyses, and contributed to interpretation of results and manuscript writing.
Funding: This study was supported by the Samuel and Liselotte Herman Fellowship, Yale School of Public Health to the first author, National Institute on Aging grants to the second author (R01AG058565, R21AG055861-01A1), National Institutes on Health grants to the third author (P50-CA196530, P30-CA16359, R01-CA177719, R01-ES005775, R41-A120546, U48-DP005023, R01-CA168733) and a National Institute on Aging grant (U01AG032284) to the senior author.
Competing interests: None declared.
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
Supplemental material: This content has been supplied by the author(s). It has not been vetted by BMJ Publishing Group Limited (BMJ) and may not have been peer-reviewed. Any opinions or recommendations discussed are solely those of the author(s) and are not endorsed by BMJ. BMJ disclaims all liability and responsibility arising from any reliance placed on the content. Where the content includes any translated material, BMJ does not warrant the accuracy and reliability of the translations (including but not limited to local regulations, clinical guidelines, terminology, drug names and drug dosages), and is not responsible for any error and/or omissions arising from translation and adaptation or otherwise.
Data availability statement
All data relevant to the study are included in the article or uploaded as supplementary information. All data are publicly available from the UN, WHO, World Bank, IHME Global Burden of Disease Study and World Values Survey websites.
Ethics statements
Patient consent for publication
Not required.
Ethics approval
This study was exempted by Yale University’s Institutional Review Board (IRB2000027318).
References
- 1.World Health Organization . Global status report on violence prevention. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Yon Y, Mikton CR, Gassoumis ZD, et al. Elder abuse prevalence in community settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Glob Health 2017;5:e147–56. 10.1016/S2214-109X(17)30006-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chang E-S, Levy BR. High prevalence of elder abuse during the COVID-19 pandemic: risk and resilience factors. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2021. 10.1016/j.jagp.2021.01.007. [Epub ahead of print: 19 Jan 2021]. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Elman A, Breckman R, Clark S, et al. Effects of the COVID-19 outbreak on elder mistreatment and response in New York City: initial lessons. J Appl Gerontol 2020;39:690-699. 10.1177/0733464820924853 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.United Nations . “Unacceptable” – UN expert urges better protection of older persons facing the highest risk of the COVID-19 pandemic. Geneva, Switzerland; 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dahlberg LK EG. Violence-a global public health problem. : Krug ED, Mercy JA, . World report on violence and health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization, 2002: 1–56. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Pillemer K, Burnes D, Riffin C, et al. Elder abuse: global situation, risk factors, and prevention strategies. Gerontologist 2016;56 Suppl 2:S194–205. 10.1093/geront/gnw004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Liu P-J, Conrad KJ, Beach SR, et al. The importance of investigating abuser characteristics in elder Emotional/Psychological abuse: results from adult protective services data. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2019;74:897–907. 10.1093/geronb/gbx064 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Frieden TR. A framework for public health action: the health impact pyramid. Am J Public Health 2010;100:590–5. 10.2105/AJPH.2009.185652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pachankis JE, Bränström R. Hidden from Happiness: structural stigma, sexual orientation concealment, and life satisfaction across 28 countries. J Consult Clin Psychol 2018;86:403–15. 10.1037/ccp0000299 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Levy BR, Slade MD, Chang E-S, et al. Ageism amplifies cost and prevalence of health conditions. Gerontologist 2020;60:174–81. 10.1093/geront/gny131 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Nelson TD. Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Levy B. Stereotype Embodiment: a psychosocial approach to aging. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 2009;18:332–6. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01662.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Chang E-S, Kannoth S, Levy S, et al. Global reach of ageism on older persons' health: a systematic review. PLoS One 2020;15:e0220857. 10.1371/journal.pone.0220857 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Wilkins N, Tsao B, Hertz M. Connecting the dots: an overview of the links among multiple forms of violence. National Center for Injury Prevention and Control: Oakland CA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Reichl AJ, Ali JI, Uyeda K. Latent Sexism in print ads increases acceptance of sexual assault. Sage Open 2018;8:215824401876975. 10.1177/2158244018769755 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ward LM. Media and Sexualization: state of empirical research, 1995-2015. J Sex Res 2016;53:560–77. 10.1080/00224499.2016.1142496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kosber J, Nahmiash D. Characteristics of Victims and Perpetrators and Milieus of Abuse and Neglect. : Baumhover L, Beall S, . Abuse, neglect, and exploitation of older persons: strategies for assessment and intervention. Baltimore: Health Professions Press, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 19.McDonald L. Ageism and elder mistreatment: Talking the talk but not walking the walk. : Nelson TD, . Ageism: Stereotyping and prejudice against older persons. Cambridge: MIT, 2017. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Inglehart R, Haerpfer C, Moreno C. World Values Survey: Round six - Country-pooled datafile version. Madrid: JD Systems Institute, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 21.James SL, Abate D, Abate KH, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 354 diseases and injuries for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2017. The Lancet 2018;392:1789–858. 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32279-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hatzenbuehler ML. Structural stigma: research evidence and implications for psychological science. Am Psychol 2016;71:742–51. 10.1037/amp0000068 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Hatzenbuehler ML, Phelan JC, Link BG. Stigma as a fundamental cause of population health inequalities. Am J Public Health 2013;103:813–21. 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Pachankis JE, Hatzenbuehler ML, Hickson F, et al. Hidden from health: structural stigma, sexual orientation concealment, and HIV across 38 countries in the European MSM Internet survey. AIDS 2015;29:1239–46. 10.1097/QAD.0000000000000724 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.United Nations Development Programme . Implementing the Madrid plan of action on ageing: what have we learned? and, where do we go from here? 2018.
- 26.United Nations . Political Declaration and Madrid international plan of action on ageing. New York; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sidorenko A. Implementation of the Madrid international plan of action on aging: research dimension. Geriatrics and Gerontology International 2004;4:S87–9. 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2004.00160.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.United Nations . World population policies 2013; 2013.
- 29.United Nations Population Fund, HelpAge International . Overview of available policies and legislation, data and research, and insitutional arrangements relating to older persons- progress since Madrid. New York; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 30.International Labor Organization . Employment protection legislation database ILO EPlex online database.
- 31.Evans-Lacko S, Brohan E, Mojtabai R, et al. Association between public views of mental illness and self-stigma among individuals with mental illness in 14 European countries. Psychol Med 2012;42:1741–52. 10.1017/S0033291711002558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Hatzenbuehler ML, Jun H-J, Corliss HL, et al. Structural stigma and cigarette smoking in a prospective cohort study of sexual minority and heterosexual youth. Ann Behav Med 2014;47:48–56. 10.1007/s12160-013-9548-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hatzenbuehler ML. The social environment and suicide attempts in lesbian, gay, and bisexual youth. Pediatrics 2011;127:896–903. 10.1542/peds.2010-3020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.World Health Organization . The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 35.Naghavi M, Global Burden of Disease Self-Harm Collaborators . Global, regional, and national burden of suicide mortality 1990 to 2016: systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2016. BMJ 2019;364:l94. 10.1136/bmj.l94 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.North MS, Fiske ST. Modern attitudes toward older adults in the aging world: a cross-cultural meta-analysis. Psychol Bull 2015;141:993–1021. 10.1037/a0039469 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Rennó Santos M, Testa A, Porter LC, et al. The contribution of age structure to the International homicide decline. PLoS One 2019;14:e0222996. 10.1371/journal.pone.0222996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Marques S, Marriano J, Mendonca J. Determinants of ageism against older adults: a systematic review of quantitative evidence. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2019;17:2560. 10.3390/ijerph17072560 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wolf A, Gray R, Fazel S. Violence as a public health problem: an ecological study of 169 countries. Soc Sci Med 2014;104:220–7. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.12.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Krug EG, Mercy JA, Dahlberg LL, et al. The world report on violence and health. Lancet 2002;360:1083–8. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)11133-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Aho K, Derryberry D, Peterson T. Model selection for ecologists: the worldviews of AIC and BIC. Ecology 2014;95:631–6. 10.1890/13-1452.1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Sardinha L, Nájera Catalán HE. Attitudes towards domestic violence in 49 low- and middle-income countries: a gendered analysis of prevalence and country-level correlates. PLoS One 2018;13:e0206101. 10.1371/journal.pone.0206101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Elgar FJ, Donnelly PD, Michaelson V, et al. Corporal punishment bans and physical fighting in adolescents: an ecological study of 88 countries. BMJ Open 2018;8:e021616. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-021616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cook D, Weiseberg S. Residuals and influence in regression. New York, NY: Chapman & Hall, 1982. [Google Scholar]
- 45.UNICEF . UNICEF databases. Violent discipline; 2020.
- 46.Straus MA, Hamby SL, Finkelhor D, et al. Identification of child maltreatment with the parent-child conflict tactics scales: development and psychometric data for a national sample of American parents. Child Abuse Negl 1998;22:249–70. 10.1016/s0145-2134(97)00174-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Haney JL. Predictors of Homonegativity in the United States and the Netherlands using the fifth wave of the world values survey. J Homosex 2016;63:1355–77. 10.1080/00918369.2016.1157997 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ceobanu AM, Escandell X. Comparative analyses of public attitudes toward immigrants and immigration using multinational survey data: a review of theories and research. Annu Rev Sociol 2010;36:309–28. 10.1146/annurev.soc.012809.102651 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Aiken L, West S. Multiple regression: testing and interpretating interactions. Newbury Park: Sage Publications, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kim J-H, Song A, Chung S, et al. The comparative Macro-Level Ageism index: an international comparison. J Aging Soc Policy 2020:1–14. 10.1080/08959420.2020.1750540 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Logan JE, Haileyesus T, Ertl A, et al. Nonfatal Assaults and Homicides Among Adults Aged ≥60 Years - United States, 2002-2016. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 2019;68:297–302. 10.15585/mmwr.mm6813a1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Peterman A, Potts A, O'Donnell M. Pandemics and violence against women and children. Washington, DC: Center for Global Development, 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Nash P, Officer A, de la Fuente-Núñez V, et al. Global trends in AGEIST disposition: comparing low, middle and high income countries using the WOLD values survey wave 6. Innov Aging 2018;2:34. 10.1093/geroni/igy023.125 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Nash P, Schneiders M, Officer A, et al. RESPECTED or a burden? global attitudes toward older people using the world values survey wave 6. Innov Aging 2017;1:116. 10.1093/geroni/igx004.478 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Doron I, Apter I. The debate around the need for an international convention on the rights of older persons. Gerontologist 2010;50:586–93. 10.1093/geront/gnq016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Crenshaw K. Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: a black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. University of Chicago Legal Forum 1989;1989. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Pachankis JE, Bränström R. How many sexual minorities are hidden? Projecting the size of the global closet with implications for policy and public health. PLoS One 2019;14:e0218084. 10.1371/journal.pone.0218084 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Scheer JR, Pachankis JE, Bränström R. Gender-Based structural stigma and intimate partner violence across 28 countries: a population-based study of women across sexual orientation, immigration status, and socioeconomic status. J Interpers Violence 2020;886260520976212:886260520976212. 10.1177/0886260520976212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Cole ER. Intersectionality and research in psychology. Am Psychol 2009;64:170–80. 10.1037/a0014564 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Roberto KA, Teaster PB. Theorizing elder abuse. : Dong X, . Elder abuse. Switzerland: Springer, 2017: 21–44. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Anetzberger GJ. An update on the nature and scope of elder abuse. Generations 2012;36:12–20. [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wolf RS. Elder abuse: an introduction. Generations 2000;20:301–5. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Burnight KM L. Theoretical model development in elder mistreatment, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Phelan A, Ayalon L. The intersection of ageism and elder abuse. : Advances in elder abuse research, international perpsectives. Switzerland: Springer, 2020: 11–22. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Philbin MM, Flake M, Hatzenbuehler ML, et al. State-level immigration and immigrant-focused policies as drivers of Latino health disparities in the United States. Soc Sci Med 2018;199:29–38. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.04.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ayalon L, Chasteen A, Diehl M, et al. Aging in times of the COVID-19 pandemic: avoiding Ageism and fostering intergenerational solidarity. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2021;76:e49–52. 10.1093/geronb/gbaa051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Levy BR. Age-Stereotype paradox: opportunity for social change. Gerontologist 2017;57:S118–26. 10.1093/geront/gnx059 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
bmjopen-2020-042580supp001.pdf (31.8KB, pdf)
Data Availability Statement
All data relevant to the study are included in the article or uploaded as supplementary information. All data are publicly available from the UN, WHO, World Bank, IHME Global Burden of Disease Study and World Values Survey websites.