Abstract
HIV‐infection has been associated with widespread alterations in brain structure and function, although few studies have examined whether such aberrations are co‐localized and the degree to which clinical and cognitive metrics are related. We examine this question in the somatosensory system using high‐resolution structural MRI (sMRI) and magnetoencephalographic (MEG) imaging of neural oscillatory activity. Forty‐four participants with HIV (PWH) and 55 demographically‐matched uninfected controls completed a paired‐pulse somatosensory stimulation paradigm during MEG and underwent 3T sMRI. MEG data were transformed into the time‐frequency domain; significant sensor level responses were imaged using a beamformer. Virtual sensor time series were derived from the peak responses. These data were used to compute response amplitude, sensory gating metrics, and spontaneous cortical activity power. The T1‐weighted sMRI data were processed using morphological methods to derive cortical thickness values across the brain. From these, the cortical thickness of the tissue coinciding with the peak response was estimated. Our findings indicated both PWH and control exhibit somatosensory gating, and that spontaneous cortical activity was significantly stronger in PWH within the left postcentral gyrus. Interestingly, within the same tissue, PWH also had significantly reduced cortical thickness relative to controls. Follow‐up analyses indicated that the reduction in cortical thickness was significantly correlated with CD4 nadir and mediated the relationship between HIV and spontaneous cortical activity within the left postcentral gyrus. These data indicate that PWH have abnormally strong spontaneous cortical activity in the left postcentral gyrus and such elevated activity is driven by locally reduced cortical gray matter thickness.
Keywords: gamma oscillations, magnetoencephalography, MRI, somatosensory gating, voxel‐based morphometry
Forty‐four participants with HIV (PWH) and 55 demographically‐matched uninfected controls completed a paired‐pulse somatosensory stimulation paradigm during MEG and underwent 3T sMRI. These data were used to compute response amplitude, sensory gating metrics, and spontaneous cortical activity power. Our findings indicated both PWH and controls exhibit somatosensory gating, and that spontaneous cortical activity was significantly stronger in PWH within the left postcentral gyrus. Interestingly, within the same tissue, PWH also had significantly reduced cortical thickness relative to controls.
1. INTRODUCTION
Over the past 20 years, HIV disease has evolved from a terminal illness to a chronic yet manageable condition with a life expectancy that now approaches that of seronegative persons. However, despite these advances, participants with HIV (PWH) remain at a significantly elevated risk of developing cognitive impairment due to their HIV‐infection, with current data indicating that 35–70% of all PWH develop at least a mild form of HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorder (HAND; Heaton et al., 2010; Heaton et al., 2011; Gannon, Khan, & Kolson, 2011; Simioni et al., 2010; Grant et al., 2014; Sacktor et al., 2002). This persistence is partially driven by the emergence of age‐related cognitive decline in PWH due to the increased longevity of this population, although many other factors also contribute, and the precise mechanisms remain poorly understood.
A growing body of studies have employed neuroimaging techniques to understand the effects of HIV‐infection on the brain (Ances et al., 2009; Ances et al., 2010; Ances, Vaida, Ellis, & Buxton, 2011; Chang et al., 2004; Chang, Yakupov, Nakama, Stokes, & Ernst, 2008; Ernst, Chang, Jovicich, Ames, & Arnold, 2002; Spooner et al., 2018; Wiesman et al., 2018; Wilson, Fox, et al., 2013; Wilson, Heinrichs‐Graham, et al., 2013, 2016; Wilson, et al., 2017, 2019). These studies have shown that HIV‐infection has an extensive effect on both brain structure (Klunder et al., 2008; Sanford et al., 2017; Sanford, Fellows, Ances, & Collins, 2018; Wilson et al., 2015) and function (Simioni et al., 2009; Spooner et al., 2018; Spooner et al., 2020), although the degree to which such structural abnormalities directly translate into functional brain aberrations is unclear. For example, Sanford et al. found reduced volumes in the thalamus and brainstem and cortical thinning in areas such as the primary motor cortex, primary somatosensory cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, cingulate cortex, as well as other frontal and temporal areas in PWH (Sanford et al., 2017, 2018). However, like many neuroimaging studies of HIV, Sanford and colleagues utilized a unimodal neuroimaging approach that focused on structural outcomes, without any direct measure of brain function. Of the few neuroHIV studies that have measured both brain structure and function, Wilson et al. (2015) found reduced gray matter volumes in PWH relative to their uninfected peers in the postcentral gyrus, parahippocampal gyrus, bilateral lingual gyri, and reduced cerebellar volume in crus I, consistent with other findings (Klunder et al., 2008; Wilson et al., 2015). Critically, they found that at least some of the structural findings were accompanied by functional deficits in the same tissue. However, importantly, the sample size in Wilson et al. (2015) was rather limited (17 participants per group), the participants were older (>55 years‐old on average), and the authors did not find any direct link between the structural and functional deficits besides the fact that they occurred in the same tissue. Thus, the relationship between such deficits has yet to be elucidated.
Previous studies have shown that PWH exhibit functional alterations in somatosensory processing, sensory gating, and inhibitory functioning (Spooner et al., 2018; Spooner, Wiesman, et al., 2020), but the degree to which these alterations are attributable to aberrations in brain structure is unknown. Sensory gating is a neurophysiological phenomenon whereby the neural response to the second stimulus in an identical pair is dramatically reduced relative to the first stimulus. This attenuation is thought to reflect the brain's capacity to filter out less relevant sensory information to preserve cognitive resources for more behaviorally relevant stimuli (Cromwell, Mears, Wan, & Boutros, 2008). Sensory gating has been generally understood as a bottom‐up, pre‐attentive process, and numerous studies of somatosensory gating have shown robust oscillations stretching into the high gamma range (i.e., 75 Hz) (Cheng et al., 2016; Kurz, Wiesman, Coolidge, & Wilson, 2018; Spooner et al., 2018; Proskovec, Spooner, Wiesman, & Wilson, 2020; Wiesman et al., 2017). However, of note, a recent study has suggested that attention may modulate somatosensory gating, at least in healthy populations (Wiesman & Wilson, 2020).
In the current study, we utilized magnetoencephalography (MEG) and a paired‐pulse electrical stimulation paradigm to derive functional measures of somatosensory processing, gating, and local spontaneous neural activity, and combined these with computational neuroanatomical analysis of structural MRI data. Our primary goals were to determine whether functional brain aberrations observed in PWH were accompanied by structural deviations in the same tissue, and to identify the nature of these structural/functional links. In addition, we examined the relationship between key HIV clinical metrics such as disease duration and CD4 nadir and structural and functional brain measures. Our primary hypotheses were that both groups would exhibit sensory gating, but that PWH would have stronger spontaneous cortical activity relative to controls in the left postcentral gyrus, in agreement with prior work (Spooner et al., 2018), and that this increase in spontaneous activity would be mediated by decreased cortical thickness in the same tissue.
2. METHODS
2.1. Participants
Adult participants (age range: 27–60 years) were drawn from a large ongoing MEG study of healthy and pathological aging (R01‐MH103220). Participants were selected based on their completion of a paired‐pulse MEG paradigm, 3 T structural MRI, and demographics. The exclusionary criteria for this study included any medical illness that affects CNS function (other than HIV), neurological disorders (other than HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorder), psychiatric disease, a history of head trauma, and current substance abuse. All PWH were currently receiving effective combination antiretroviral therapy (cART) and had undetectable viremia at the time of the study. Viral suppression was determined as <50 copies/ml. The group included 109 participants, including 63 uninfected control participants and 46 PWH who were demographically matched based upon ethnicity, age, sex, handedness, and educational levels. All participants provided informed consent consistent with the requirements of the Institutional Review Board of the University of Nebraska Medical Center.
2.2. Neuropsychological testing
A neuropsychological battery that assessed multiple domains including attention, fine motor, working memory, language, speed of processing, verbal learning and memory, and executive functioning was completed by each participant. The raw scores from each assessment were then converted into demographically‐adjusted z‐scores (Heaton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004) and, along with activities of daily living, were used to assess PWH for the presence of HAND. Of note, this battery was consistent with the Frascati consensus (Antinori et al., 2007). None of the uninfected adults were impaired on the neuropsychological assessments or activities of daily living.
2.3. Experimental paradigm
Participants were seated with their head within the MEG helmet, as 80 paired‐pulse trials of cutaneous electrical stimulation were applied to their right median nerve using an inter‐stimulation interval (ISI) of 500 ms. An ISI of 500 ms was chosen based on extensive data from previous studies (Arpin, Davies, & Kurz, 2016; Kurz et al., 2018; Spooner, Eastman, Wiesman, & Wilson, 2020; Wiesman et al., 2016). Each pulse consisted of a 0.2 ms square wave, with an amplitude of 10% above the motor threshold. The inter‐pair interval (IPI) varied between 4,500 and 4,800 ms. This paradigm is described in detail and extensively tested in Spooner et al. (2020a).
2.4. MEG data acquisition and structural MRI coregistration
Our MEG data acquisition methods are described in detail elsewhere (Wiesman & Wilson, 2020). Briefly, using an acquisition passband of 0.1–330 Hz, neuromagnetic responses were continuously sampled at 1 kHz using a MEG system equipped with 204 planar gradiometers and 102 magnetometers (306 total magnetic sensors; MEGIN, Helsinki, Finland). The resulting MEG data were corrected for head motion, noise reduced (Taulu & Simola, 2006), and coregistered with individual T1‐weighted sMRI data before source space analyses. Following beamformer analysis, each participant's structural and functional images were transformed into standardized space and spatially resampled.
2.5. MEG preprocessing, time‐frequency transformation, and sensor‐level statistics
Eye‐blink and cardiac artifacts were removed from the MEG time series using signal‐space projection (SSP; Uusitalo & Ilmoniemi, 1997). The time series was divided into epochs of 3,700 ms duration (−800 to 2,900 ms). This epoch included 400 ms baseline window shifted away from stimulus onset (stimulation 1 onset = 0 ms) from −700 to −300 ms.
Of note, we shifted the baseline window away from the time period immediately before stimulus onset to circumvent any possible contamination by anticipatory responses; however, no such anticipatory responses were detected in our final analyses. For artifact rejection, a fixed threshold method supplemented with visual inspection was used, resulting in an average of 74 trials per participant being retained for source analysis. The mean number of accepted trials per group did not differ (p = .30).
Complex demodulation was used to transform the artifact‐free epochs into the time‐frequency domain (Hoechstetter et al., 2004; Kovach & Gander, 2016; Papp & Ktonas, 1977). The resulting spectral power estimations were averaged over trials (per sensor) and normalized per frequency bin using the respective bin's mean power during the −700 to −300 ms baseline period. To derive the time‐frequency windows used for source reconstruction, a two‐stage, data‐driven statistical analysis was conducted on the sensor‐level spectrograms across all participants and gradiometers. This statistical approach is extensively described in our prior work (Spooner, Eastman, et al., 2020; Wiesman & Wilson, 2020). Briefly, paired‐sample t‐tests against baseline were conducted on each pixel in the spectrogram (per sensor), with the output being thresholded at p < .05. These potentially significant time‐frequency bins were then evaluated using clustered‐based nonparametric permutation testing (10,000 permutations per comparison; Ernst, 2004; Maris & Oostenveld, 2007), and those bins remaining significant in the permutation analysis were selected for beamforming (see below).
2.6. MEG Beamformer imaging and source space analyses
The dynamic imaging of coherent sources (DICS) beamformer was used to image the time‐frequency bins identified in the sensor‐level analysis (Van Veen, Drongelen, Yuchtman, & Suzuki, 1997; Gross et al., 2001). Essentially, using the DICS approach, we computed normalized source power for the selected time‐frequency periods (see Results) per participant at 4.0 × 4.0 × 4.0 mm resolution. The resulting images were averaged across both stimulations and all participants to assess the anatomical basis of the oscillatory activity identified through the sensor‐level analysis. Using the peak voxel coordinates derived from this grand‐averaged image, virtual sensors (i.e., voxel time series) were extracted from each participant's data by applying the sensor weighting matrix to the preprocessed signal vector. Note that we extracted virtual sensor data from each participant using the peak coordinate from the grand‐averaged image, and then estimated the envelope of the time series for the spectral band identified in the sensor level analysis. Using these envelopes, we computed the relative (baseline‐corrected) and absolute time series for each participant. This data processing pipeline is extensively described elsewhere (Spooner, Eastman, et al., 2020).
2.7. Structural MRI morphometry
A Philips Achieva 3 T scanner equipped with an eight‐channel head coil was used to acquire structural T1‐weighted MRI data. We used a 3D fast field echo sequence with these parameters: TR: 8.09 ms; TE: 3.7 ms; the field of view: 24 cm; matrix: 256 × 256; slice thickness: 1 mm with no gap; in‐plane resolution: 0.9375 × 0.9375 mm; sense factor: 1.5. The details of our MRI data processing pipeline are available (Proskovec et al., 2020). Briefly, we used the Computational Anatomy Toolbox (CAT12 v12.6; Gaser & Dahnke, 2016) within SPM12 to conduct standard surface‐based morphometry (SBM) on the high‐resolution structural data. Fundamentally, the SBM pipeline within the CAT12 toolbox is an automated approach that employs a projection‐based thickness (PBT) approach to estimate local cortical thickness and reconstruct the central surface (Dahnke, Yotter, & Gaser, 2013). The PBT approach accounts for partial volume correction, sulcal blurring, and sulcal asymmetries without sulcal reconstruction. To rectify defects in topology, correction based upon spherical harmonics was used (Yotter, Dahnke, Thompson, & Gaser, 2011), and the resulting cortical surface mesh was reparametrized into a common coordinate system. This was accomplished by an algorithm that reduces area distortion (Yotter, Thompson, & Gaser, 2011). The resulting maps were finally resampled and smoothed using a 15 mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.
Our CAT12 pipeline included a noise reduction step (Manjón, Coupé, Martí‐Bonmatí, Collins, & Robles, 2010), the Markov Random Field approach (Rajapakse, Giedd, & Rapoport, 1997), and affine registration and local intensity transformation to the bias‐corrected images. An adaptive maximum a posteriori technique (Ashburner & Friston, 2005) was then used to segment the images, with partial volume estimation based on a simplified mixed model (Tohka, Zijdenbos, & Evans, 2004). Once the MRI data were fully processed and in MNI space, a mask was constructed for the cortical surface mesh using the same peak voxel coordinates as the MEG virtual sensor extraction. Briefly, using the WFU Pick atlas (version 3.0; Maldjian, Laurienti, Kraft, & Burdette, 2003; Maldjian, Laurienti, & Burdette, 2004), an 8 mm sphere centered on the MEG peak voxel was generated, and this was spatially resampled to 1 mm to align with the structural data. Using the transform provided in CAT12, this normalized volume mask was then transformed into the surface template space, which allowed cortical thickness values within the mask to be extracted per participant. For clarity, the final values reflect the mean cortical thickness across the mask. This masking procedure is also described in Proskovec, Spooner, et al., 2020. Finally, to enhance rigor, we derived mean cortical thickness using spheres of varying sizes (8, 9, and 10 mm) centered on the same peak MEG coordinates, and across all sizes, our statistically significant findings remained.
2.8. Statistics
We used mixed‐model ANOVAs to evaluate group differences in somatosensory processing and gating and t‐tests to evaluate group differences in spontaneous activity and cortical thickness. To identify relationships between brain structure and function and determine how these are modulated by HIV‐infection, we used structural equation modeling. Statistical significance of indirect effects was determined using bias‐corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals (Efron & Tibshirani, 1986; Fritz & MacKinnon, 2007) around all indirect effects using 1,000 bootstrapped samples. Note that because of the use of confidence intervals to determine significance, exact p‐values are not available for indirect effects. As a follow‐up to these analyses, we examined whether any clinical measures of HIV (i.e., CD4 nadir) could explain the noted effects of HIV group membership on brain structure and function. These models were tested only in PWH. We utilized full information maximum likelihood (FIML) to estimate any missing data in all models; analyses were conducted using Mplus version 8.1.
3. RESULTS
Of the 109 participants 10 participants were excluded due to artifacts in their MEG and/or MRI data. The remaining 99 participants, 55 controls and 44 PWH, did not statistically differ in age, sex, education, ethnicity, or other demographic variables (see Table 1). Fifteen of the 44 PWH met the criteria for HAND according to the Frascati guidelines. For the PWH, time since diagnosis, current CD4, and CD4 nadir were recorded at the time of enrollment.
TABLE 1.
Control (n = 55) | PWH (n = 44) | Significance | |
---|---|---|---|
Age (years) | 42.5 ± 10.6 | 45.9 ± 10.0 | p = .358 |
Sex | 32 M, 23 F | 26 M, 18 F | p = .991 |
Handedness | 51 R, 4 L | 41 R, 3 L | p = .931 |
Weight (kg) | 91.9 ± 23.8 | 85.1 ± 23.5 | p = .212 |
Education (years) | 16.8 ± 2.34 | 14.5 ± 2.22 | p = .500 |
Time on cART (years) | – | 9.12 ± 6.01 | |
Time since HIV diagnosis (years) | – | 10.8 ± 6.48 | |
CD4 nadir (cells/μl) | – | 209 ± 159 | |
Current CD4 (cells/μl) | – | 792 ± 460 |
Note: Values displayed are mean ± SD.
Abbreviations: cART, combination antiretroviral therapy; F, female; L, left; M, male; R, right.
3.1. MEG sensor‐level analysis
Robust broadband synchronizations spanning 10–90 Hz were observed in several MEG sensors near the sensorimotor and parietal regions during the 100 ms directly following the onset of electrical stimulation (p < .001, corrected). These responses, especially those in gamma frequencies, were considerably stronger in the 50 ms immediately following each electrical stimulation (Figure 1a). Thus, we focused our beamformer analyses on these 50 ms time windows (i.e., 0–50 ms and 500–550 ms) following each stimulation and the 20–75 Hz range. Our main analyses started at 20 Hz as this was the lowest frequency that we could precisely resolve using a 50 ms time interval. We stopped at 75 Hz on the high end as the relative power of the neuronal responses decreased sharply thereafter, particularly in response to the second stimulus.
3.2. HIV‐related alterations in somatosensory cortical structure and function
Beamformer output images indicated robust responses in the contralateral somatosensory hand region of the postcentral gyrus following each stimulation (Figure 1b). The peak locations of the responses to the first and second stimulations were virtually identical in the contralateral postcentral gyrus, and these locations were highly similar across the two groups (Figure 1c). As described in the Methods section, the beamformer images were grand averaged across all participants and stimulations, and then virtual sensor data were extracted in each participant from the peak voxel in the grand‐averaged image. The envelope of the resulting time series was then computed for the 20–75 Hz frequency range.
To examine group differences in sensory processing, we computed two mixed‐model 2 × 2 analysis of variances (ANOVAs) (stimulation‐by‐group). Participants with responses outside of ±2.5 SD were excluded from these analyses (n = 2). The first examined response amplitude and the second tested peak frequency. The analysis of response amplitude indicated a main effect of stimulation, such that the response to stimulation 2 was weaker than that to stimulation 1 across all participants (F[1,96] = 45.65, p < .001). In other words, there was a significant somatosensory gating effect across all participants (Figure 2b). In contrast, neither the main effect of the group (p = .159) nor the interaction were significant (p = .776). As per peak frequency, the main effect of stimulation was significant (F[1,96] = 4.19, p < .05), indicating that the peak frequency of the response to the second stimulation (mean = 49.7, SD = 14.9) was higher than that to the first stimulation (mean = 45.0, SD = 14.9). Similar to the amplitude findings, neither the main effect of the group (p = .385) nor the interaction were significant (p = .386).
Next, we examined the strength of spontaneous neural activity during the baseline period by computing the mean amplitude from −700 to −300 ms using the absolute power time series (i.e., not baseline corrected). Independent sample t‐tests indicated significantly stronger spontaneous power in the PWH relative to controls (p = .001; Figure 2c,d). We further explored the extent to which these group‐level differences in neural activity may be explained by group differences in cortical structure, namely cortical thickness within the left postcentral gyrus.
3.3. Local cortical thickness mediates the effect of HIV on spontaneous neural activity
To evaluate differences in cortical thickness, we computed the mean cortical thickness in each participant within an 8 mm sphere centered on the peak somatosensory response in the left postcentral gyrus, which was derived from the grand‐averaged MEG beamformer images. These mean values were examined using independent sample t tests which revealed significant group differences (Figure 3a) such that PWH had significantly reduced cortical thickness relative to controls. Then, to evaluate the extent to which gray matter thickness within the left postcentral gyrus mediated the effects between HIV and spontaneous neural activity, we tested a mediation model in which diagnostic group (i.e., PWH or control) predicted gray matter thickness within the left postcentral gyrus which subsequently predicted spontaneous neural activity within the same brain tissue. As identified in our primary analysis, there was a significant total effect of HIV on spontaneous activity (β = .31, b = 4.44, 95% CI [1.51, 7.30]),
such that PWH tended to have higher resting power relative to controls. Importantly, there was also a significant mediation whereby cortical thickness in the left postcentral gyrus partially explained the relationship between HIV and spontaneous neural activity during the baseline (indirect: β = .080, b = 1.15, 95% CI [.087, 2.65]; Figure 4). Specifically, PWH had significantly reduced postcentral gyrus thickness relative to healthy controls (β = −.39, b = −.062, p < .001), and this reduced thickness subsequently predicted stronger spontaneous power during the baseline period (β = −.21, b = −18.59, p = .040). Note that the direct effect of HIV on spontaneous activity remained significant (β = .23, b = 3.29, 95% CI [.33, 5.94]). There were no other statistically significant relationships between group membership and brain structure or function.
To follow up these analyses, we tested an exploratory model exclusively in PWH to determine whether indices of disease severity could explain the noted effects of HIV‐infection on cortical thickness and spontaneous power in the left postcentral gyrus. The model was structured such that current CD4 counts, CD4 nadir, and time since HIV diagnosis all served as predictors of spontaneous activity before somatosensory stimulation with gray matter thickness in the left postcentral gyrus modeled as a mediator. Indices of HIV disease were allowed to freely correlate. Postcentral gyrus thickness was significantly predicted by CD4 nadir (β = .31, b = .004, p = .04) indicating that individuals with lower CD4 nadir tended to have thinner gray matter in the left postcentral gyrus (Figure 3b). However, there were no other significant associations between markers of HIV disease and brain structure or function, nor were there any significant indirect effects.
4. DISCUSSION
The primary goal of this study was to examine the relationship between brain structure and function in the postcentral gyrus of PWH with virologic suppression. To this end, participants underwent paired‐pulse electrical stimulation of the right median nerve during MEG, as well as a 3 T structural MRI session. We hypothesized that PWH would exhibit normal sensory gating but robustly stronger spontaneous baseline power relative to controls in the postcentral gyrus, consistent with findings from Spooner et al. (2018), and that this increased spontaneous neural activity would be mediated by locally reduced cortical thickness in the left postcentral gyrus. Our results indicated that PWH had relatively preserved somatosensory gating, as well as reduced cortical thickness and increased spontaneous neural activity in the brain tissue generating the strongest somatosensory response. Below, we discuss the implications of these findings, with an emphasis on the spontaneous cortical activity, the novelty of the structure/function relationships identified in this study, and the relationships between CD4 nadir and cortical thickness.
One of our principal findings was the increase in spontaneous neural activity in the left postcentral gyrus of PWH compared with controls. Such increased spontaneous neural activity has previously been reported in healthy older relative to younger adults in the primary motor (Heinrichs‐Graham et al., 2018; Heinrichs‐Graham & Wilson, 2016) and somatosensory cortices (Spooner, Wiesman, Proskovec, Heinrichs‐Graham, & Wilson, 2019), and has been linked to altered neural oscillations and impaired behavioral performance in older adults (Heinrichs‐Graham & Wilson, 2016). Such spontaneous activity is also known to increase throughout the day and be strongest in the late afternoon (Wilson, Heinrichs‐Graham, & Becker, 2014) when motor performance is known to be less precise (Carrier & Monk, 2000; Drust, Waterhouse, Atkinson, Edwards, & Reilly, 2005). Thus, given the evidence for accelerated aging in HIV (Gross et al., 2016; Holt, Kraft‐Terry, & Chang, 2012; Horvath & Levine, 2015; Lew et al., 2021; Pathai, Bajillan, Landay, & High, 2013), our finding of robustly increased spontaneous neural activity in PWH is not surprising. In fact, previous studies have shown increased spontaneous alpha and gamma in the occipital cortices of PWH (Wiesman et al., 2018), as well as increases in spontaneous theta and alpha in frontal, parietal, and occipital cortices serving selective attention in PWH relative to demographically‐matched controls (Lew et al., 2018). Of note, the Lew et al. study found a step‐wise increase in spontaneous power insofar as control participants exhibited the lowest spontaneous power in theta and alpha activity, PWH had slightly elevated spontaneous power in these bands, and impaired PWH (persons with HAND) exhibited the highest spontaneous power in the theta and alpha canonical bands. This pattern was observed in several regions of interest providing evidence for the stability of this pattern. The ability to distinguish those with HAND from unimpaired PWH and controls in multiple brain regions was also observed by Wiesman and colleagues (Wiesman, Mills, et al., 2018). Similar findings of elevated spontaneous activity distinguishing those with HAND from unimpaired PWH and controls was also recently reported in the somatosensory cortices (Spooner, Wiesman, et al., 2020). Given these data and the findings from healthy aging, such increases in spontaneous activity almost certainly reflect pathological changes in local circuit function, and studies that have collected behavioral performance metrics in parallel with MEG have indeed supported this (Lew et al., 2018; Wiesman, O'Neill, et al., 2018). Unfortunately, our MEG task did not require behavioral responses and thus we cannot directly comment on whether the increased spontaneous activity would have impacted cognition. Nonetheless, the converging evidence reveals elevated spontaneous activity impairs local circuit function and may lead to a decline in cognitive performance. Given recent evidence that noninvasive electrical brain stimulation can modulate local spontaneous activity in healthy adults (Heinrichs‐Graham, McDermott, Mills, Coolidge, & Wilson, 2017; McDermott et al., 2019; Wiesman, O'Neill, et al., 2018; Wilson, Mcdermott, Mills, Coolidge, & Heinrichs‐Graham, 2018), studies examining the efficacy of such stimulation in PWH could be fruitful.
Our structural findings of reduced cortical thickness in the somatosensory cortex in PWH is consistent with previous structural work (Cardenas et al., 2009; Lew et al., 2021; Wilson et al., 2015). Evidence suggests that even in the cART era, PWH experience progressive gray matter volume loss, as well as abnormally diminished white matter volumes and increased ventricular volumes (Cardenas et al., 2009; Cohen et al., 2010), although cART has unquestionably reduced the severity of volume and cortical thickness reductions in PWH (Cohen et al., 2010; Heindel et al., 1994; Stout et al., 1998; Thompson et al., 2005). An interesting finding in our study was the relationship between CD4 nadir and brain structure in PWH, such that lower CD4 nadir values were associated with greater cortical thinning in the left postcentral gyrus. This finding could indicate that the severity of these neural deficits is at least partially attributable to legacy effects related to the early stages of the infection. Similar findings linking CD4 nadir and neuropsychological function have been widely reported (Cysique & Brew, 2009; Ellis et al., 2011; Jernigan et al., 2011; Robertson et al., 2007; Tozzi et al., 2007).
A key novel finding of this study was the critical link between aberrant thinning in the left postcentral gyrus and the stronger spontaneous cortical activity within the same tissue in PWH. Essentially, our mediation model revealed a fully‐mediated effect whereby HIV‐infection predicted decreased cortical thickness in the left postcentral gyrus, which subsequently predicted increased baseline power in the same brain tissue. This novel structure/function relationship suggests that the loss of neural tissue is directly related to the HIV‐associated alterations in spontaneous power, which is critical for understanding the neuropathology of HIV. Further, these data fill a key missing piece of the puzzle, as the net impact of structural degeneration on neural function in PWH has remained unclear. Essentially, prior studies have generally examined either brain structure or function, and those that have examined both have either not tested a direct relationship between the findings and/or had too small of a sample for such analyses (i.e., see Wilson et al., 2015). While these findings are compelling, it should be noted that we restricted our analyses to the left postcentral gyrus and cannot comment on wider associations. Future studies should directly test other brain regions for such structure/function relationships in PWH.
Before closing, it is important to acknowledge the limitations of this study. First, our study focused on virally‐suppressed PWH who did not have neurologic or psychiatric illnesses (except HAND), acute concurrent illnesses, or major substance abuse or depression symptoms. Thus, the sample was relatively healthy, and our findings may not generalize to samples that include individuals with high viral loads, depression, or other neurologic or psychiatric conditions. Second, our PWH group included patients with and without cognitive impairment (i.e., HAND) and future studies should examine the impact of HAND directly. We would speculate that the structure/function relationships identified here would not be affected by HAND, but that should be confirmed. Third, as stated previously, our study focused on the left postcentral gyrus, and future studies should use higher‐level tasks to activate widespread brain networks which could then be probed for similar structure/function relationships as observed here. Again, we would speculate that the relationship would hold in other brain regions, but directly testing this would be a strong next step. Fourth, the cross‐sectional nature of the study design limits our understanding of the trajectory of these deficits, and thus future longitudinal studies would be very informative. Finally, we only stimulated the right hand and did not investigate hemispheric asymmetries in brain structure or function. Future studies should extend these findings by stimulating the left hand and evaluating possible hemispheric differences in both brain function (i.e., somatosensory function) and cortical thickness. Despite these limitations, the current study provides critical new data linking aberrant thinning in the somatosensory cortices of PWH to altered spontaneous cortical activity in the same brain tissue.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors have no competing financial or other interests.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Chloe Casagrande: data curation, formal analysis, writing‐ original draft. Brandon Lew: data curation, formal analysis, writing‐ review, and editing. Brittany Taylor: formal analysis, writing‐ review, and editing. Mikki Schantell: data curation, project administration, writing‐ review, and editing. Jennifer O'Neill: data curation, project administration. Pamela May formal analysis, writing‐ review, and editing. Susan Swindells: conceptualization, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, writing‐ review, and editing. Tony Wilson: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, writing‐ review, and editing.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH; R01‐MH116782, R01‐MH118013, P30‐MH062261, R03‐DA041917, R01‐DA047828, and F30‐DA048713) and the National Science Foundation (NSF: #1539067). The funders had no role in the study design, data acquisition, and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Casagrande CC, Lew BJ, Taylor BK, et al. Impact of HIV‐infection on human somatosensory processing, spontaneous cortical activity, and cortical thickness: A multimodal neuroimaging approach. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021;42:2851–2861. 10.1002/hbm.25408
Funding information National Institutes of Health, Grant/Award Numbers: F30‐DA048713, P30‐MH062261, R01‐DA047828, R01‐MH116782, R01‐MH118013, R03‐DA041917; National Science Foundation, Grant/Award Number: 1539067
DATA AVAILABILITY STATEMENT
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
REFERENCES
- Ances, B. , Vaida, F. , Ellis, R. , & Buxton, R. (2011). Test‐retest stability of calibrated BOLD‐fMRI in HIV‐ and HIV+ subjects. NeuroImage, 54(3), 2156–2162. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ances, B. M. , Sisti, D. , Vaida, F. , Liang, C. L. , Leontiev, O. , Perthen, J. E. , … HNRC group . (2009). Resting cerebral blood flow: A potential biomarker of the effects of HIV in the brain. Neurology, 73(9), 702–708. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b59a97 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ances, B. M. , Vaida, F. , Yeh, M. J. , Liang, C. L. , Buxton, R. B. , Letendre, S. , … Ellis, R. J. (2010). HIV infection and aging independently affect brain function as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Infectious Diseases, 201(3), 336–340. 10.1086/649899 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antinori, A. , Arendt, G. , Becker, J. T. , Brew, B. J. , Byrd, D. A. , Cherner, M. , … Wojna, V. E. (2007). Updated research nosology for HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology, 69(18), 1789–1799. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arpin, D. J. , Davies, B. L. , & Kurz, M. J. (2016). Multiple sclerosis influences the precision of the ankle plantar flexion muscular force production. Gait & Posture, 45, 170–174. 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashburner, J. , & Friston, K. J. (2005). Unified segmentation. NeuroImage, 26(3), 839–851. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardenas, V. A. , Meyerhoff, D. J. , Studholme, C. , Kornak, J. , Rothlind, J. , Lampiris, H. , … Weiner, M. W. (2009). Evidence for ongoing brain injury in human immunodeficiency virus‐positive patients treated with antiretroviral therapy. Journal of Neurovirology, 15(4), 324–333. 10.1080/13550280902973960 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carrier, J. , & Monk, T. H. (2000). Circadian rhythms of performance: New trends. Chronobiology International, 17(6), 719–732. 10.1081/cbi-100102108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang, L. , Lee, P. , Yiannoutsos, C. , Ernst, T. , Marra, C. , Richards, T. , & Navia, B. (2004). A multicenter in vivo proton‐MRS study of HIV‐associated dementia and its relationship to age. NeuroImage, 23(4), 1336–1347. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang, L. , Yakupov, R. , Nakama, H. , Stokes, B. , & Ernst, T. (2008). Antiretroviral treatment is associated with increased attentional load‐dependent brain activation in HIV patients. Journal of Neuroimmune Pharmacology, 3(2), 95–104. 10.1007/s11481-007-9092-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, C.‐H. , Chan, P.‐Y. S. , Niddam, D. M. , Tsai, S.‐Y. , Hsu, S.‐C. , & Liu, C.‐Y. (2016). Sensory gating, inhibition control and gamma oscillations in the human somatosensory cortex. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 20437. 10.1038/srep20437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, R. A. , Harezlak, J. , Gongvatana, A. , Buchthal, S. , Schifitto, G. , Clark, U. , … HIV Neuroimaging Consortium . (2010). Cerebral metabolite abnormalities in human immunodeficiency virus are associated with cortical and subcortical volumes. Journal of Neurovirology, 16(6), 435–444. 10.3109/13550284.2010.520817 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cromwell, H. C. , Mears, R. P. , Wan, L. , & Boutros, N. N. (2008). Sensory gating: A translational effort from basic to clinical science. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 39(2), 69–72. 10.1177/155005940803900209 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cysique, L. A. , & Brew, B. J. (2009). Neuropsychological functioning and antiretroviral treatment in HIV/AIDS: A review. Neuropsychology Review, 19(2), 169–185. 10.1007/s11065-009-9092-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahnke, R. , Yotter, R. A. , & Gaser, C. (2013). Cortical thickness and central surface estimation. NeuroImage, 65, 336–348. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drust, B. , Waterhouse, J. , Atkinson, G. , Edwards, B. , & Reilly, T. (2005). Circadian rhythms in sports performance‐ ‐an update. Chronobiology International, 22(1), 21–44. 10.1081/cbi-200041039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Efron, B. , & Tibshirani, R. (1986). Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals, and other measures of statistical accuracy. Statistical Science, 1, 54–75. 10.1016/j.cct.2010.11.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, R. J. , Badiee, J. , Vaida, F. , Letendre, S. , Heaton, R. K. , Clifford, D. , … CHARTER Group . (2011). CD4 nadir is a predictor of HIV neurocognitive impairment in the era of combination antiretroviral therapy. AIDS (London, England), 25(14), 1747–1751. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32834a40cd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst, M. D. (2004). Permutation methods: A basis for exact inference. Statistical Science, 19(4), 676–685. 10.1214/088342304000000396 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ernst, T. , Chang, L. , Jovicich, J. , Ames, N. , & Arnold, S. (2002). Abnormal brain activation on functional MRI in cognitively asymptomatic HIV patients. Neurology, 59(9), 1343–1349. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000031811.45569.b0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fritz, M. S. , & MacKinnon, D. P. (2007). Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychological Science, 18, 233–239. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gannon, P. , Khan, M. Z. , & Kolson, D. L. (2011). Current understanding of HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorders pathogenesis. Current Opinion in Neurology, 24(3), 275–283. 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834695fb [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaser, C. , & Dahnke, R. (2016). CAT‐a computational anatomy toolbox for the analysis of structural MRI data. Human Brain Mapping, 336–348. https://www.neuro.uni-jena.de/hbm2016/GaserHBM2016.pdf [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant, I. , Franklin, D. R. , Deutsch, R. , Woods, S. P. , Vaida, F. , Ellis, R. J. , … Heaton, R. K. (2014). Asymptomatic HIV‐associated neurocognitive impairment increases risk for symptomatic decline. Neurology, 82(23), 2055–2062. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000492 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross, A. M. , Jaeger, P. A. , Kreisberg, J. F. , Licon, K. , Jepsen, K. L. , Khosroheidari, M. , … Ideker, T. (2016). Methylome‐wide analysis of chronic HIV infection reveals five‐year increase in biological age and epigenetic targeting of HLA. Molecular Cell, 62(2), 157–168. 10.1016/j.molcel.2016.03.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gross, J. , Kujala, J. , Hamalainen, M. , Timmermann, L. , Schnitzler, A. , & Salmelin, R. (2001). Dynamic imaging of coherent sources: Studying neural interactions in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 694–699. 10.1073/pnas.98.2.694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heaton, R. K. , Clifford, D. B. , Franklin, D. R. , Woods, S. P. , Ake, C. , Vaida, F. , … Grant, I. (2010). HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER study. Neurology, 75(23), 2087–2096. 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d727 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heaton, R. K. , Franklin, D. R. , Ellis, R. J. , McCutchan, J. A. , Letendre, S. L. , Leblanc, S. , … HNRC Group . (2011). HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: Differences in rates, nature, and predictors. Journal of Neurovirology, 17(1), 3–16. 10.1007/s13365-010-0006-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heaton, R. K. , Miller, S. W. , Taylor, M. J. , Grant, I . (2004). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead‐Reitan battery: Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Psychological Assessment Resources .
- Heindel, W. C. , Jernigan, T. L. , Archibald, S. L. , Achim, C. L. , Masliah, E. , & Wiley, C. A. (1994). The relationship of quantitative brain magnetic resonance imaging measures to neuropathologic indexes of human immunodeficiency virus infection. Archives of Neurology, 51(11), 1129–1135. 10.1001/archneur.1994.00540230067015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , McDermott, T. J. , Mills, M. S. , Coolidge, N. M. , & Wilson, T. W. (2017). Transcranial direct‐current stimulation modulates offline visual oscillatory activity: A magnetoencephalography study. Cortex, 88, 19–31. 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , McDermott, T. J. , Mills, M. S. , Wiesman, A. I. , Wang, Y.‐P. , Stephen, J. M. , … Wilson, T. W. (2018). The lifespan trajectory of neural oscillatory activity in the motor system. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 30, 159–168. 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , & Wilson, T. W. (2016). Is an absolute level of cortical beta suppression required for proper movement? Magnetoencephalographic evidence from healthy aging. NeuroImage, 134, 514–521. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hoechstetter, K. , Bornfleth, H. , Weckesser, D. , Ille, N. , Berg, P. , & Scherg, M. (2004). BESA source coherence: A new method to study cortical oscillatory coupling. Brain Topography, 16(4), 233–238. 10.1023/b:brat.0000032857.55223.5d [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt, J. L. , Kraft‐Terry, S. D. , & Chang, L. (2012). Neuroimaging studies of the aging HIV‐1‐infected brain. Journal of Neurovirology, 18(4), 291–302. 10.1007/s13365-012-0114-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Horvath, S. , & Levine, A. J. (2015). HIV‐1 infection accelerates age according to the epigenetic clock. Journal of Infectious Diseases, 212(10), 1563–1573. 10.1093/infdis/jiv277 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jernigan, T. L. , Archibald, S. L. , Fennema‐Notestine, C. , Taylor, M. J. , Theilmann, R. J. , Julaton, M. D. , … CHARTER Group . (2011). Clinical factors related to brain structure in HIV: The CHARTER study. Journal of Neurovirology, 17(3), 248–257. 10.1007/s13365-011-0032-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klunder, A. D. , Chiang, M. , Dutton, R. A. , Lee, S. E. , Toga, A. W. , Lopez, O. L. , … Thompson, P. M. (2008). Mapping cerebellar degeneration in HIV/AIDS. Neuroreport, 19(17), 1655–1659. 10.1097/WNR.0b013e328311d374 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kovach, C. K. , & Gander, P. E. (2016). The demodulated band transform. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 261, 135–154. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurz, M. J. , Wiesman, A. I. , Coolidge, N. M. , & Wilson, T. W. (2018). Children with cerebral palsy hyper‐gate somatosensory stimulations of the foot. Cerebral Cortex, 28, 1–8. 10.1093/cercor/bhx144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lew, B. J. , McDermott, T. J. , Wiesman, A. I. , O'Neill, J. , Mills, M. S. , Robertson, K. R. , … Wilson, T. W. (2018). Neural dynamics of selective attention deficits in HIV‐associated neurocognitive disorder. Neurology, 91(20), e1860–e1869. 10.1212/wnl.0000000000006504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lew, B. J. , Schantell, M. , O'Neill, J. , Morsey, B. , Wang, T. , Ideker, T. , … Wilson, T. W. (2021). Reductions in grey matter linked to epigenetic HIV‐associated accelerated aging. Cerebral Cortex (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maldjian, J. A. , Laurienti, P. J. , & Burdette, J. H. (2004). Precentral gyrus discrepancy in electronic versions of the Talairach atlas. NeuroImage, 21(1), 450–455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maldjian, J. A. , Laurienti, P. J. , Kraft, R. A. , & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas‐based interrogation of fMRI data sets. NeuroImage, 19(3), 1233–1239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manjón, J. V. , Coupé, P. , Martí‐Bonmatí, L. , Collins, D. L. , & Robles, M. (2010). Adaptive non‐local means denoising of MR images with spatially varying noise levels: Spatially adaptive nonlocal denoising. Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 31(1), 192–203. 10.1002/jmri.22003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maris, E. , & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG‐ and MEG‐data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDermott, T. J. , Wiesman, A. I. , Mills, M. S. , Spooner, R. K. , Coolidge, N. M. , Proskovec, A. L. , … Wilson, T. W. (2019). tDCS modulates behavioral performance and the neural oscillatory dynamics serving visual selective attention. Human Brain Mapping, 40(3), 729–740. 10.1002/hbm.24405 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papp, N. , & Ktonas, P. (1977). Critical evaluation of complex demodulation techniques for the quantification of bioelectrical activity. Biomedical Sciences Instrumentation, 13, 135–145. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pathai, S. , Bajillan, H. , Landay, A. L. , & High, K. P. (2013). Is HIV a model of accelerated or accentuated aging? The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 69(7), 833–842. 10.1093/gerona/glt168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proskovec, A. L. , Rezich, M. T. , O'Neill, J. , Morsey, B. , Wang, T. , Ideker, T. , … Wilson, T. W. (2020). Association of Epigenetic Metrics of biological age with cortical thickness. JAMA Network Open, 3(9), e2015428. 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.15428 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Proskovec, A. L. , Spooner, R. K. , Wiesman, A. I. , & Wilson, T. W. (2020). Local cortical thickness predicts somatosensory gamma oscillations and sensory gating: A multimodal approach. NeuroImage, 214, 116749. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116749 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rajapakse, J. C. , Giedd, J. N. , & Rapoport, J. L. (1997). Statistical approach to segmentation of single‐channel cerebral MR images. IEEE Transactions on Medical Imaging, 16(2), 176–186. 10.1109/42.563663 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robertson, K. R. , Smurzynski, M. , Parsons, T. D. , Wu, K. , Bosch, R. J. , Wu, J. , … Ellis, R. J. (2007). The prevalence and incidence of neurocognitive impairment in the HAART era. AIDS, 21(14), 1915–1921. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e32828e4e27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sacktor, N. , Mcdermott, M. P. , Marder, K. , Schifitto, G. , Selnes, O. A. , Mcarthur, J. C. , … Epstein, L. (2002). HIV‐associated cognitive impairment before and after the advent of combination therapy. Journal of Neurovirology, 8(2), 136–142. 10.1080/13550280290049615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanford, R. , Fellows, L. K. , Ances, B. M. , & Collins, D. L. (2018). Association of brain structure changes and cognitive function with combination antiretroviral therapy in HIV‐positive individuals. JAMA Neurology, 75(1), 72–79. 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.3036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanford, R. , Fernandez Cruz, A. L. , Scott, S. C. , Mayo, N. E. , Fellows, L. K. , Ances, B. M. , & Collins, D. L. (2017). Regionally specific brain volumetric and cortical thickness changes in HIV‐infected patients in the HAART era. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 74(5), 563–570. 10.1097/QAI.0000000000001294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simioni, S. , Cavassini, M. , Annoni, J.‐M. , Abraham, A. R. , Bourquin, I. , Schiffer, V. , … Pasquier, R. A. D. (2009). Cognitive dysfunction in HIV patients despite long‐standing suppression of Viremia. AIDS, 24(9), 1243–1250. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e3283354a7b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simioni, S. , Cavassini, M. , Annoni, J. M. , Rimbault Abraham, A. , Bourquin, I. , Schiffer, V. , … Du Pasquier, R. A. (2010). Cognitive dysfunction in HIV patients despite long‐standing suppression of viremia. AIDS (London, England), 24(9), 1243–1250. 10.1097/QAD.0b013e3283354a7b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spooner, R. K. , Eastman, J. A. , Wiesman, A. I. , & Wilson, T. W. (2020). Methodological considerations for a better somatosensory gating paradigm: The impact of the inter‐stimulus interval. NeuroImage, 220, 117048. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spooner, R. K. , Wiesman, A. I. , Mills, M. S. , O'Neill, J. , Robertson, K. R. , Fox, H. S. , … Wilson, T. W. (2018). Aberrant oscillatory dynamics during somatosensory processing in HIV‐infected adults. NeuroImage: Clinical, 20, 85–91. 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.07.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spooner, R. K. , Wiesman, A. I. , O'Neill, J. , Schantell, M. D. , Fox, H. S. , Swindells, S. , & Wilson, T. W. (2020). Prefrontal gating of sensory input differentiates cognitively impaired and unimpaired aging adults with HIV. Brain Communications, 2(2), fcaa080. 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spooner, R. K. , Wiesman, A. I. , Proskovec, A. L. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , & Wilson, T. W. (2019). Rhythmic spontaneous activity mediates the age‐related decline in somatosensory function. Cerebral Cortex, 29(2), 680–688. 10.1093/cercor/bhx349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stout, J. C. , Ellis, R. J. , Jernigan, T. L. , Archibald, S. L. , Abramson, I. , Wolfson, T. , … Grant, I. (1998). Progressive cerebral volume loss in human immunodeficiency virus infection: A longitudinal volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. HIV neurobehavioral research center group. Archives of Neurology, 55(2), 161–168. 10.1001/archneur.55.2.161 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taulu, S. , & Simola, J. (2006). Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Physics in Medicine and Biology, 51(7), 1759–1768. 10.1088/0031-9155/51/7/008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, P. M. , Dutton, R. A. , Hayashi, K. M. , Toga, A. W. , Lopez, O. L. , Aizenstein, H. J. , & Becker, J. T. (2005). Thinning of the cerebral cortex visualized in HIV/AIDS reflects CD4+ T lymphocyte decline. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(43), 15647–15652. 10.1073/pnas.0502548102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tohka, J. , Zijdenbos, A. , & Evans, A. (2004). Fast and robust parameter estimation for statistical partial volume models in brain MRI. NeuroImage, 23(1), 84–97. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tozzi, V. , Balestra, P. , Bellagamba, R. , Corpolongo, A. , Salvatori, M. F. , Visco‐Comandini, U. , … Narciso, P. (2007). Persistence of neuropsychologic deficits despite long‐term highly active antiretroviral therapy in patients with HIV‐related neurocognitive impairment: Prevalence and risk factors. Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes, 45(2), 174–182. 10.1097/QAI.0b013e318042e1ee [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uusitalo, M. A. , & Ilmoniemi, R. J. (1997). Signal‐space projection method for separating MEG or EEG into components. Medical & Biological Engineering & Computing, 35(2), 135–140. 10.1007/BF02534144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veen, B. V. , Drongelen, W. V. , Yuchtman, M. , & Suzuki, A. (1997). Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 44(9), 867–880. 10.1109/10.623056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesman, A. I. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , Coolidge, N. M. , Gehringer, J. E. , Kurz, M. J. , & Wilson, T. W. (2016). Oscillatory dynamics and functional connectivity during gating of primary somatosensory responses. The Journal of Physiology, 595(4), 1365–1375. 10.1113/JP273192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesman, A. I. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , Coolidge, N. M. , Gehringer, J. E. , Kurz, M. J. , & Wilson, T. W. (2017). Oscillatory dynamics and functional connectivity during gating of primary somatosensory responses. The Journal of Physiology, 595(4), 1365–1375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesman, A. I. , Mills, M. S. , McDermott, T. J. , Spooner, R. K. , Coolidge, N. M. , & Wilson, T. W. (2018). Polarity‐dependent modulation of multi‐spectral neuronal activity by transcranial direct current stimulation. Cortex, 108, 222–233. 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.08.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesman, A. I. , O'Neill, J. , Mills, M. S. , Robertson, K. R. , Fox, H. S. , Swindells, S. , & Wilson, T. W. (2018). Aberrant occipital dynamics differentiate HIV‐infected patients with and without cognitive impairment. Brain, 141(6), 1678–1690. 10.1093/brain/awy097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiesman, A. I. , & Wilson, T. W. (2020). Attention modulates the gating of primary somatosensory oscillations. NeuroImage, 211, 116610. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116610 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Fox, H. S. , Robertson, K. R. , Sandkovsky, U. , O'Neill, J. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , & Swindells, S. (2013). Abnormal MEG oscillatory activity during visual processing in the prefrontal cortices and frontal eye‐fields of the aging HIV brain. PLoS One, 8(6). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , & Becker, K. M. (2014). Circadian modulation of motor‐related beta oscillatory responses. NeuroImage, 102, 531–539. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , Becker, K. M. , Aloi, J. , Robertson, K. R. , Sandkovsky, U. , & Swindells, S. (2015). Multimodal neuroimaging evidence of alterations in cortical structure and function in HIV‐infected older adults. Human Brain Mapping, 36(3), 897–910. 10.1002/hbm.22674 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , Proskovec, A. L. , McDermott, T. J. (2016). Neuroimaging with magnetoencephalography: A dynamic view of brain pathophysiology. Translational Research, 175, 17–36. 10.1016/j.trsl.2016.01.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , Robertson, K. R. , Sandkovsky, U. , O'Neill, J. , Knott, N. L. , & Swindells, S. (2013). Functional brain abnormalities during finger‐tapping in HIV‐infected older adults: A magnetoencephalography study. Journal of Neuroimmune Pharmacology, 8(4), 965–974. 10.1007/s11481-013-9477-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Lew, B. J. , Spooner, R. K. , Rezich, M. T. , & Wiesman, A. I. (2019). Aberrant brain dynamics in neuroHIV: Evidence from magnetoencephalographic (MEG) imaging. Progress in Molecular Biology and Translational Science, 165, 285–320. 10.1016/bs.pmbts.2019.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Mcdermott, T. J. , Mills, M. S. , Coolidge, N. M. , & Heinrichs‐Graham, E. (2018). tDCS modulates visual gamma oscillations and basal alpha activity in occipital cortices: Evidence from MEG. Cerebral Cortex, 28(5), 1597–1609. 10.1093/cercor/bhx055 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, T. W. , Proskovec, A. L. , Heinrichs‐Graham, E. , O'Neill, J. , Robertson, K. R. , Fox, H. S. , & Swindells, S. (2017). Aberrant neuronal dynamics during working memory operations in the aging HIV‐infected brain. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 41568. 10.1038/srep41568 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yotter, R. A. , Dahnke, R. , Thompson, P. M. , & Gaser, C. (2011). Topological correction of brain surface meshes using spherical harmonics. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 1109–1124. 10.1002/hbm.21095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yotter, R. A. , Thompson, P. M. , & Gaser, C. (2011). Algorithms to improve the reparameterization of spherical mappings of brain surface meshes. Journal of Neuroimaging, 21, e134–e147. 10.1111/j.1552-6569.2010.00484.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.