Skip to main content
Frontiers in Endocrinology logoLink to Frontiers in Endocrinology
. 2021 May 5;12:608575. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2021.608575

A Researcher’s Guide to the Measurement and Modeling of Puberty in the ABCD Study® at Baseline

Theresa W Cheng 1,*, Lucía Magis-Weinberg 2, Victoria Guazzelli Williamson 1, Cecile D Ladouceur 3, Sarah L Whittle 4, Megan M Herting 5, Kristina A Uban 6, Michelle L Byrne 1,7, Marjolein E A Barendse 1, Elizabeth A Shirtcliff 8, Jennifer H Pfeifer 1
PMCID: PMC8131843  PMID: 34025573

Abstract

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development℠ (ABCD) Study is an ongoing, diverse, longitudinal, and multi-site study of 11,880 adolescents in the United States. The ABCD Study provides open access to data about pubertal development at a large scale, and this article is a researcher’s guide that both describes its pubertal variables and outlines recommendations for use. These considerations are contextualized with reference to cross-sectional empirical analyses of pubertal measures within the baseline ABCD dataset by Herting, Uban, and colleagues (2021). We discuss strategies to capitalize on strengths, mitigate weaknesses, and appropriately interpret study limitations for researchers using pubertal variables within the ABCD dataset, with the aim of building toward a robust science of adolescent development.

Keywords: adolescent brain cognitive development study, puberty, salivary hormones, testosterone, estradiol, DHEA

Introduction

Pubertal measures provide critical information about maturation beyond chronological age, and there is substantial variation in the timing of pubertal milestones (1, 2). The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study measures puberty across adolescence in a large and diverse sample (baseline ages = 9–10 years; annual sampling of pubertal measures for 10 years (ongoing); 21 research sites across the United States; N = 11,880; 48% female at baseline (3); see (4) for recruitment details). This represents an unprecedented opportunity to better understand relationships between puberty, sociodemographic variables, neurodevelopment, and health. The purpose of this article is to support scientists in planning and interpreting research about puberty from this dataset, particularly in light of what is known from recent analyses of the baseline data by Herting, Uban, and colleagues (5). We—a collaboration of puberty researchers including authors within and external to the ABCD consortium—seek to provide a balanced presentation of the study’s strengths, limitations, opportunities (areas of growth/innovation), and threats (issues endangering the validity of potential interpretations) as they pertain to puberty. We further explore practical considerations for planning investigations with these data, including a brief discussion of longitudinal analyses.

ABCD Study Design: Implications For Pubertal Research

This section addresses aspects of the study design that are relevant to studying puberty, including sample composition and assessment frequency [for information about specific measures in the ABCD Study, see the section on Pubertal measures in the ABCD dataset; for general recommendations regarding pubertal measurement/modeling, see (6, 7)]. Puberty is comprised of distinct (albeit temporally overlapping) hormonal processes that drive specific physical changes. Adrenarche involves a rise in adrenal hormones [testosterone (T); dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) and its sulfate], while gonadarche involves a rise in gonadal hormones [estradiol (E2) and progesterone from the ovaries; T from the testes; for a review, see (8)]. As the suffix -arche refers to first occurrences, we use the terms adrenal and gonadal processes to refer to the multi-year maturation of each endocrine axis (hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal and hypothalamic–pituitary–gonadal). The maturation of the growth (hypothalamic–pituitary–somatic) axis is associated with increases in growth hormone and regulation of overall growth and metabolism (9).

Strengths

The large sample size and narrow age range facilitate well-powered investigations related to pubertal development that can be contextualized using sociodemographic variables (5) and will likely provide a basis for normative pubertal development in the USA population at the beginning of the 21st century. By following participants over a decade, growth curves will describe average growth and typical variation across adolescence. Analyses of the baseline data suggest that there is sufficient variance to relate individual differences in pubertal development to other measures (5). Furthermore, the field of pubertal research has largely focused on girls of European descent, and examination of puberty in males and in racially/ethnically diverse participants fills in critical knowledge gaps (10, 11).

Limitations

Due to participant ages at recruitment, the study is unable to capture many adrenal processes for nearly all participants and early gonadal processes for many participants―especially females (5). This limitation is more pronounced for groups that start puberty earlier relative to the average, including Black and non-White Hispanic participants (12, 13), those from lower levels of socioeconomic status (14), and those with higher BMI (15) [trends replicated in the ABCD data at baseline (5)]. Puberty is classically considered complete following development of secondary sexual characteristics and capability for reproduction (roughly mid-adolescence; later for boys relative to girls). This prevailing definition is complicated by the fact that hormone levels (16) and body composition (17) mature into the late teens and 20s. The study plans to follow participants to 19–20 years of age, but this would fall short of addressing questions about how late hormonal changes may affect processes like risky decision-making at the ages when binge drinking (18) and other health risk behaviors peak (19). Additionally, pubertal development is non-linear with meaningful changes occurring at sub-annual time scales (e.g., growth spurts) that are not captured (20).

Pubertal Measures In The ABCD Dataset

Puberty-related variables in the ABCD Study are listed in Table 1 . For descriptive statistics of these variables in the baseline data, see (5).

Table 1.

Summary of puberty variables within the ABCD Study.

Content Description Sex* Informant/Source NDAR Element Name (Alias)**
Pubertal Development Scale and Menstrual Cycle Survey History (Youth: abcd_ypdms01, Caregiver: abcd_ppdms01)
Sex/gender (not disambiguated in the question wording)  Do you consider yourself male or female? All Youth pds_sex_y (pubertdev_sex)
Sex at birth What sex was your child assigned at birth? All Caregiver pubertal_sex_p (pubertdev_sex_p)
Height spurt Would you say that your/your child’s growth in height 1 = has not yet begun to spurt; 2 = has barely started; 3 = is definitely underway; 4 = seems complete; 999 = I don’t know; 777= refuse to answer All Both pds_ht2_y, pds_ht2_p (pubertdev_ht, pubertdev_1_p)
Skin changes Have you/your child noticed any skin changes, especially pimples? 1 = skin has not yet started changing; 2 = skin has barely started changing; 3 = skin changes are definitely underway; 4 = skin changes seem complete; 999 = I don’t know; 777 = refuse to answer All Both pds_skin2_y, pds_3_p*** (pubertdev_skin, pubertdev_3_p)
Body hair And how about the growth of your/your child’s body hair? 1 = has not yet begun to spurt; 2 = has barely started; 3 = is definitely underway; 4 = seems complete; 999 = I don’t know; 777 = refuse to answer All Both pds_bdy_hair_y, pds_2_p*** (pubertdev_bdyhair, pubertdev_2_p)
Breast development Have you noticed that your/your child’s breasts have begun to grow? 1 = have not yet started growing; 2 = have barely started growing; 3 = breast growth is definitely underway; 4 = breast growth seems complete; 999 = I don’t know; 777 = refuse to answer Female Both pds_f4_2_y, pds_f4_2_p (pubertdev_f4, pubertdev_f4_p)
Menstruation began? Have you/she begun to menstruate (started to have your period)? Female Both pds_f5_y, pds_f5_p (pubertdev_f5, pubertdev_f5_p)
Age of first period If yes, how old were you/she when you/she started to menstruate? Female Both pds_f6_y, pds_f6_p (pubertdev_f6, pubertdev_f6_p)
Deepening voice Have you noticed a deepening of your/your child’s voice? Male1 Both pds_m4_y, pds_m4_p (pubertdev_m4, pubertdev_m4_p)
Facial hair Have you/your child begun to grow hair on your/his face? Male 1 Both pds_m5_y, pds_m5_p (pubertdev_m5, pubertdev_m5_p)
First day of last period What was the date of the first day of your/your daughter’s last period? Female Both menstrualcycle1_y, menstrualcycle1_p (menstrual_1, menstrual_1_p)
Cycle length On average, how many days are there between the first day of your/her period and the first day of your/her next period? (e.g., 30 days)   Female Both menstrualcycle2_y, menstrualcycle2_p (menstrual_2, menstrual_2_p)
Cycle regularity Is your/her menstrual cycle regular? Female Both mentrualcycle3_y, menstrualcycle3_p (menstrual_3, menstrual_3_p)
Hormonal birth control Are you/is she currently using hormonal birth control (eg. the pill, hormone patch, hormone injection)? Female Both menstrualcycle4_y, menstrualcycle4_p (menstrual_4, menstrual_4_p)
Premenstrual irritability Do you/does she experience premenstrual symptoms, such as irritability, fatigue, etc., which start before a period and stop within a few days of bleeding?  Female Both menstrualcycle5_y, menstrualcycle5_p (menstrual_5, menstrual_5_p)
Does PMS interfere with activities Do your/her premenstrual symptoms interfere with your/her relationships with family and friends, productivity, and/or social life activities? Female Both menstrualcycle6_y, menstrualcycle6_p (menstrual_6, menstrual_6_p)
Derived pubertal development scores (ABCD Sum Scores Physical Health Youth and Parent; abcd_ssphp01)
Category score based on the Pubertal Development Scale (prepubertal/early/mid/late/post)3 All Both pds_y_ss_female_category (pubertdev_ss_female_category), pds_y_ss_male_category (pubertdev_ss_male_category), pds_p_ss_female_category (pubertdev_ss_female_category_p),
pds_p_ss_male_category (pubertdev_ss_male_category_p)
Salivary hormone measures (Hormone Saliva Salimetric Scores; hsss01)
Salimetrics hormone test mean (pg/ml) Hormone saliva variable recorded by Salimetrics dhea: all,
hse (E2), females only;
ert (T): all
Youth hormone_scr_dhea_mean, hormone_scr_hse_mean, hormone_scr_ert_mean
Salimetrics hormone test below lower limit of sensitivity 2 , replications 1 and 2**** (yes/no) hormone_scr_dhea_rep1, hormone_scr_hse_rep1, hormone_scr_ert_rep1
Salimetrics hormone quantity not sufficient, repetitions 1 and 2**** (yes/no) hormone_scr_dhea_rep1_qns, hormone_scr_hse_rep1_qns, hormone_scr_ert_rep1_qns
Salimetrics hormone test none detected, repetitions 1 and 2**** (yes/no) hormone_scr_dhea_rep1_nd, hormone_scr_hse_rep1_nd, hormone_scr_ert_rep1_nd
Other variables
Sex (male/female) All Caregiver sex (sex_at_birth, also aliased as “gender”)
Mean standing height (inches) All Youth anthroheightcalc (anthro_height_calc [mean of anthro_1_height_in, anthro_2_height_in, anthro_3_height_in])
Mean weight (lbs) All Youth anthroweightcalc (anthro_weight_calc [mean of anthro_weight1_lb, anthro_weight2_lb, anthro_weight3_lb])
Quality control measures for saliva samples (Pubertal Hormone Saliva information at time of fluid collection; sph01)
Wake-up time on test day What time did you wake up today? All Youth hormone_sal_wake_y (biospec_hormone_sal_wake)
Caffeine Have you had any caffeine in the last 12 hours?
How many drinks (mg) did you have
All Youth hormone_sal_caff_y (biospec_hormone_sal_caff)
hormone_sal_caff_mg_y (biospec_hormone_sal_caff_mg)
Exercise In the last 12 hours, did you exercise vigorously (sweating, breathing hard) for at least 20 minutes?
For how long did you exercise?
All Youth hormone_sal_active (biospec_hormone_sal_active)
hormone_sal_active_minutes_y (biospec_hormone_sal_active_minutes)
Sample Collection information Hormone saliva tube cap color, Sample time collection start, Sample time collection end, Sample barcode, Storage temperature, Time sample moved to freezer All Youth hormone_sal_sex, hormone_sal_start_y, (biospec_hormone_sal_start), hormone_sal_end_y, (biospec_hormone_sal_end) hormone_sal_bc_y, (biospec_hormone_sal_bc) hormone_sal_freeztemp_y, (biospec_hormons_sal_freeztemp) hormone_sal_freezer_y (biospec_hormone_sal_freezer)
Concerns about sample None, Contaminated, Discoloration, Excessive bubbles, Insufficient quantity, Other All Youth hormon_sal_notes_y:_1 through hormon_sal_notes_y:_6 biospec_hormon_sal_notes___1 through biospec_hormon_sal_notes___6)

Adapted from ABCD data release 2.0.1, last updated 05/06/2020 (doi: 10.15154/1506087; wave 01/baseline). The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The contents under NDAR Element Name (Alias) refer to abbreviations in the ABCD dataset codebook, and may be useful only to those who have successfully applied for (free) access to the data. *Sex-specific scores were calculated based on responses to the sex_at_birth variable, which only had binary response options; we note that there are various sex and gender related variables, and that a separate scale was administered to address gender identity (not described above); ** Throughout, “y”; denotes youth and “p”; denotes parent/caregiver versions; *** Number mismatch is because youth and caregiver measures were in a different order; **** Due to space constraints, only abbreviations for repetition 1 are shown.

1Only boys responded to these questions. This limits the documentation of girls with androgen excess who may also experience facial hair and deepening of the voice.

2The lower limit of sensitivity is reported for each hormone in Herting, Uban, and colleagues (2021).

3Converted Pubertal Development Scale values are not provided and must be calculated by researchers [see (5) for more information on possible calculations].

Measures of Physical Maturation

Strengths

The study assesses physical development using the Pubertal Development Scale (PDS), a minimally invasive, text-only measure designed for ease of administration (21). On the PDS, individuals rate their own/their child’s development on a four-point Likert scale from “had not begun” to “already complete” with respect to specific physical characteristics (e.g., skin changes, breast development; a subset of the items was administered based on sex). The PDS is versatile, as researchers might use the mean PDS score, converted scores made to be more comparable to Tanner staging (22, 23), derived scores intended to reflect adrenal and gonadal processes separately, and/or focus on a specific item (e.g., age at menarche). Collecting data from multiple informants allows researchers to use caregiver reports at earlier ages and adolescent reports at later ages [for examples, see (24, 25)]. Prioritizing caregiver reports may be useful at baseline considering the large number of “I don’t know” responses to several items (notably 34–42% for growth spurts; 10% for menstruation) (5); at these ages, caregivers may have greater knowledge of where adolescents are in the process of change. Adolescent reports may better reflect intimate experiences with body changes over time, particularly at later ages (26). Adolescent self-report may be an ideal measure for studies focused on the consequences of puberty for social- or self-related processes (21), and is sometimes as closely or more closely associated with hormone levels than Tanner staging via clinical examination (27, 28).

Limitations

When using the PDS and its derived scales, consider the following limitations: First, the PDS does not evaluate pubertal stage directly, and a description of the construct is better reported as “perceived pubertal stage,” although it is uncommon to do so (29). While there are conversions transforming the PDS to values more comparable to Tanner staging, some recommend that the PDS should not be used when precise Tanner staging is of interest (20). Second, the PDS does not cover the full range of puberty equally well (solicits less information about earlier changes) (29). Third, the PDS exhibits systematic discrepancies with clinician ratings. Consistent with desirability effects, relatively less advanced adolescents tend to overestimate their PDS score, while more advanced adolescents tend to underestimate (27, 30).

Pubertal Hormone Measures

Strengths

The study provides objective measures of the quantity of biologically available DHEA, T, and E2 [in girls only; for details on hormone methods see (5)] at remarkable scale via salivary measures. From gonadarche onwards, increases in DHEA and T in males reflect adrenal and gonadal processes, respectively. In females, DHEA and T largely reflect adrenal processes, while E2 levels reflect gonadal ones. Hormone levels are not redundant with information about physical maturation. In the ABCD data at baseline, PDS summary scores were modestly correlated with hormone levels [0.12–0.20 in males; 0.10–0.34 in females (5)]. In another study of 9–14 year-olds, PDS-derived scores accounted for 35–40% of the variance in DHEA and T levels in males and 15–27% of DHEA, T, and E2 levels in females (27). Associations between circulating hormone levels and physical changes vary by factors including sex, race/ethnicity, and body mass index (5, 15, 31). Examining hormone levels may be particularly useful during the earliest and latest pubertal stages: Adrenal hormone levels rise prior to physical changes associated with adrenarche (32), and circulating levels of DHEA, T, and E2 rise after the ages at which adolescents reach Tanner stage V (sometimes considered the last pubertal stage) (16).

Data for potential hormone quality confounds were recorded at the time of sampling, including time of day, caffeine consumption, and medication use (for a list, see Table 1 ); linear effects on hormone levels have been estimated at baseline (5). We encourage researchers to use transparent and reproducible processing procedures (i.e., following a pre-specified decision-tree as implemented in publicly available scripts by Herting, Uban, and colleagues (5); https://figshare.com/articles/software/R_scripts/12673754).

Limitations

Due to feasibility issues, saliva was sampled once per visit and time of day varied widely [7 am–7 pm (5);]. Hormone levels fluctuate dynamically and non-linearly over various time-scales, and reliance on a single biospecimen renders researchers unable to account for momentary, daily/diurnal, or monthly hormonal fluctuations. Time of day is a major source of variability; early in puberty, each of these hormones exhibit non-linear diurnal rhythms with peaks in the morning, and these diurnal patterns further vary across pubertal development (3335). Another major source of complex unmeasured variability is menstrual cyclicity in females: Even prior to menarche, cyclic changes in hormone levels can be detected, and variability in the menstrual cycle persists almost 2 years following menarche (7), with diurnal E2 rhythms attenuated approximately a year after menarche (35). Including time of day as a linear covariate may not sufficiently account for such effects. Sensitivity analyses within participants sampled during a restricted time window (and that reflect a random subsample across sites and demographics) may improve the validity of investigations employing these measures. Additionally, estimates from salivary assays are known to be less reliable at the extremes. We further note that other pubertal hormones were not assessed (notably progesterone, luteinizing hormone, and follicle stimulating hormone).

Discussion

Opportunities to Advance Our Understanding of Puberty

The ABCD Study presents the opportunity to parse the relative contributions of puberty and sociodemographic variables to adolescent development. Another opportunity, arising from the narrow age range and large sample size, is to disentangle effects of puberty and age (also see the section on Considerations for Building Models Incorporating Puberty). The study may also advance the development of multimethod pubertal measurement approaches. Herting, Uban, and colleagues (5) implemented group factor analyses with the ABCD baseline data to identify latent pubertal factors while accounting for method-related variance. They found a two-factor structure accounting for a combined 47.4% of the variability in pubertal measures in females and 38.6% in males. Researchers have typically focused on physical maturation or hormone levels separately, and novel multimethod approaches may approaches may contribute to longstanding questions in the field. (However, combining methods may not be necessary, and single-method approaches may be preferable for targeted research questions—for more on variable selection, see the section on Strategizing for open and reproducible analyses). For example, one question is the extent to which obesity itself is directly linked to early pubertal development rather than systematic measurement error, particularly overestimation of breast development in girls (15, 36). Group factor analyses with the ABCD baseline data found that greater body mass index was associated with higher-than-average hormone and physical maturation levels (higher Factor 1 scores), as well as more advanced physical maturation relative to hormone levels, compared to the sample average (higher Factor 2 scores). Advancement along both of these axes, relative to overall sample, is consistent with earlier pubertal development and suggests that measurement error of physical characteristics may not fully account for associations between obesity and more advanced puberty.

Threats Endangering the Validity of Potential Interpretations

Conclusions drawn from the ABCD Study will undoubtedly carry impact. However, we should be cautious when developing major conclusions regarding certain aspects of puberty. The lack of pre-pubertal female participants is more pronounced in groups found to start puberty earlier (5). These sociodemographic differences must be carefully considered in work addressing pubertal onset and/or the very earliest stages of puberty. Early pubertal onset and timing contribute to risk for health problems (37, 38), and this issue may bias estimates of pubertal timing-related effects. Its extent will remain unknown until scientists and funding agencies invest widely in puberty-related research at earlier ages, particularly for Black and non-White Hispanic girls.

To mitigate general design and measurement limitations and to guard against misinterpretation, we recommend the following minimum standards for puberty research using this dataset: (a) When using physical maturation data, language reflects that the PDS is a measure of self- or parent-perceived pubertal maturation, (b) when using salivary hormone data, data are processed using a standardized and/or publicly available analysis pipeline and sensitivity analyses consider the effect of time of day on hormone levels, and (c) exude caution when drawing conclusions regarding precise pubertal stages and hormone levels, in part by acknowledging measurement limitations (see Pubertal Measures in the ABCD Dataset for details on strengths and limitations).

Strategizing for Open and Reproducible Analyses

Like other complex constructs, puberty can be operationalized and modeled in ways that impact conclusions about its effects. For example, conclusions as to whether early pubertal maturation affects adult height in females differ when maturation is defined in terms of menarche versus breast development (39). This enhanced analytic flexibility (40) can have untoward effects in null-hypothesis significance testing, some of which can be mitigated by preregistration (41). While not comprehensive, potential a priori justifications for variable selection are presented in the section on PUBERTAL MEASURES in the ABCD DATASET. Decisions might also be informed by psychoneuroendocrinology, and neuroimaging analyses might consider what is known about hormone receptor types and density in brain tissue (42). Otherwise, Specification Curve Analysis, also known as multiverse analysis, can facilitate reporting of results from multiple model specifications that are consistent with the underlying theory; this approach can estimate the robustness of effects across numerous operationalizations of pubertal development (4345) [for an example using pubertal variables, see (46)]. Pre-specifying the smallest effect size of interest (47) and/or employing the use of discovery samples for model-fitting and replication or holdout samples for model testing might also be useful (48), especially because small effects are likely to be significant when sample sizes are large.

Considerations for Building Models Incorporating Puberty

There is no ideal or standard modeling approach, as decisions should reflect the research questions at hand. Drawing from our own work, we provide a few examples of possible approaches and highlight relevant considerations.

In one study, Ladouceur and colleagues (49) used measures of physical maturation (standardized composite scores) to separately consider effects of adrenal and gonadal processes. These scores, as well as hormone levels, were used as predictors in separate multiple linear regression models examining effects of puberty (controlling for age) on neural indices of reward processing in 10 to 13 year-olds (N = 79). This approach separately considered adrenal and gonadal processes, as well as physical and hormone measures, in order to disentangle various effects. Another study by Whittle and colleagues (50) showcased the use of exploratory factor analyses across multiple age-adjusted questionnaire items (including parent and child report PDS) to create a standardized pubertal timing measure (calculated at approximately age 12; N = 155). This measure was used in longitudinal analyses examining associations with pituitary volume and depressive symptoms. Longitudinal work by Vijayakumar et al. (51) compared nested mixed effects models (ages 9–18; N = 82) that predicted signal from a functional neuroimaging task from linear and quadratic effects of maturation (age and self-reported PDS scores converted into a Tanner-like scale). Analyses used both hormone and questionnaire measures by examining the impact of including T in best-fitting PDS models.

We recommend that researchers account for age when examining puberty (despite the narrow age window) because age was associated with each pubertal measure in the baseline data (6). Each of these studies highlights a different approach to disentangling puberty and age effects, specifically by including age as a covariate (49), creating age-adjusted pubertal indices (50), or by comparing age and puberty models (51).

Selecting and discretizing sex and gender variables requires careful theoretical and ethical consideration. Researchers should take care to use the terms sex and gender appropriately (52), to consider that they operate on developmental outcomes via different mechanisms (53), and to avoid essentializing either one as strictly binary (54). Youth and caregivers report on gender identity (55), but information about sex chromosomes or endocrine disorders is not formally acquired. In analyses highlighted above, researchers used composite or factor scores computed within sex, included sex as a covariate, and tested for interactions between sex and conditions of interest (49, 50). When evaluating hormones as putative sex-specific mechanisms, authors ran separate models by sex (49, 51).

Finally, physical maturation as measured by the PDS is ordinal, but many analyses (including those highlighted above) treats it as continuous; this specification is flawed because it implies that differences between all pubertal stages are equally meaningful or spaced with respect to some outcome. Some developmental shifts, e.g., in human face perception, are associated with transitions between certain pubertal stages only (56).

Opportunities and Challenges for Longitudinal Analyses

As more waves of ABCD data are released, there will be opportunities to describe sample norms and normed relative categorizations (e.g., describing timing as early, typical, or late) with accompanying hormone trajectories. Longitudinal data will also allow for new variables such as age at menarche or peak height velocity (7) and modeling techniques such as non-linear latent growth modeling (57). Challenges include measurement invariance, as pubertal measures may change in their accuracy (e.g., parent versus child reports) and/or substantive meaning (e.g., E2 reflecting diurnal versus monthly patterns). Another documented phenomenon is that some adolescents will regress on pubertal maturation as measured by the PDS (21). While commonly associated with measurement error and the coarseness of the measure (21), further investigation of regression within the ABCD Study is warranted, including consideration that adolescents’ self-perceptions may change over time. Finally, as the number of self-identified sexual and gender minority youth increase longitudinally (55) there will be opportunities to study puberty in gender diverse youth, and these groups should be consulted when conducting and interpreting this research.

Conclusion

This article outlines critical considerations for investigations using pubertal data from the ABCD Study. Major strengths include the size and demographic diversity of the sample and the use of both questionnaires and salivary hormones. Limitations include the inability to investigate earlier aspects of puberty and an inability to account for multiscale hormone fluctuations, such that smaller longitudinal studies are still needed to answer open questions about puberty. Overall, we recommended that researchers using these data describe the PDS as perceived rather than objective pubertal development, use sensitivity analyses to account for diurnal hormone cycles, prioritize open and reproducible science, and account for age in their models despite the narrow age band. We hope that facilitating a better understanding of the ABCD Study’s design and measures will ultimately support a stronger science of adolescent development.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: Researchers can apply for access to the data at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd/request-access.

Author Contributions

Authors TC, VGW, LM-W, and CL wrote different portions of this manuscript. All authors participated in extensive feedback, comments, and discussion, including TC, VGW, LM-W, CL, MEAB, SW, MLB, ES, MH, KU, and JP. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This project was conceptualized at the ABCD Workshop 2019, which was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R25MH120869. Author TC was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under award number TL1TR002371. Author CL was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number MH099007. Author MLB was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number K01MH111951. Author MH was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under Award Number: K01 MH10876. Author SW was supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council under award number 1125504. Author KU was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism under Award Number: K01 AA026889. Author JP was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under award number MH174108. To prepare this article, we examine and present details about measures administered in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multi-site, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children ages 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/scientists/workgroups/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in analysis or writing of this report. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. Author TC was supported by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under award number TL1TR002371 and by the National Institute of Mental Health under award number 1F31MH124353.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to express gratitude to the organizers (especially Dr. Kathryn Mills, Samantha Chavez, and Dr. Danielle Cosme) as well as attendees of the 2019 ABCD Workshop on Brain Development and Mental Health, where this project was first proposed. We also wish to express appreciation to members of the Developmental Social Neuroscience lab and to Jonny Saunders at the University of Oregon for providing feedback on this manuscript.

Footnotes

1

Only boys responded to these questions. This limits the documentation of girls with androgen excess who may also experience facial hair and deepening of the voice.

2

The lower limit of sensitivity is reported for each hormone in Herting, Uban, and colleagues (2021)

References

  • 1. Biro FM, Pajak A, Wolff MS, Pinney SM, Windham GC, Galvez MP, et al. Age of Menarche in a Longitudinal Us Cohort. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol (2018) 31(4):339–45. 10.1016/j.jpag.2018.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Tanner JM, Whitehouse RH. Clinical Longitudinal Standards for Height, Weight, Height Velocity, Weight Velocity, and Stages of Puberty. Arch Dis Childhood (1976) 51(3):170–9. 10.1136/adc.51.3.170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Jernigan TL, Brown SA, Dowling GJ. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. J Res Adolesc (2018) 28(1):154–6. 10.1111/jora.12374 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Garavan H, Bartsch H, Conway K, Decastro A, Goldstein RZ, Heeringa S, et al. Recruiting the ABCD Sample: Design Considerations and Procedures. Dev Cognit Neurosci (2018) 32:16–22. 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Herting MM, Uban KA, Gonzalez MR, Baker FC, Kan EC, Thompson WK, et al. Correspondence Between Perceived Pubertal Development and Hormone Levels in 9-10 Year-Olds From the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. Front Endocrinol (2021) 11. 10.3389/fendo.2020.549928 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Mendle J, Beltz AM, Carter R, Dorn LD. Understanding Puberty and Its Measurement: Ideas for Research in a New Generation. J Res Adolescence (2019) 29(1):82–95. 10.1111/jora.12371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Dorn LD, Biro FM. Puberty and Its Measurement: A Decade in Review. J Res Adolescence (2011) 21(1):180–95. 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00722.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Byrne ML, Whittle S, Vijayakumar N, Dennison M, Simmons JG, Allen NB. A Systematic Review of Adrenarche as a Sensitive Period in Neurobiological Development and Mental Health. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2017) 25:12–28. 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Olarescu NC, Gunawardane K, Hansen TK, Møller N, Jørgensen JOL. Normal Physiology of Growth Hormone in Adults. In: Feingold KR, Anawalt B, Boyce A, Chrousos G, de Herder WW, Dungan K, et al. editors. Endotext. South Dartmouth (MA: MDText.com, Inc; (2000). Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK279056/. [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Marceau K, Hottle S, Yatcilla JK. Puberty in the Last 25 Years: A Retrospective Bibliometric Analysis. J Res Adolescence (2019) 29(1):96–114. 10.1111/jora.12396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Deardorff J, Hoyt LT, Carter R, Shirtcliff EA. Next Steps in Puberty Research: Broadening the Lens Toward Understudied Populations. J Res Adolescence (2019) 29(1):133–54. 10.1111/jora.12402 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Chumlea WC, Schubert CM, Roche AF, Kulin HE, Lee PA, Himes JH, et al. Age At Menarche and Racial Comparisons in US Girls. Pediatrics (2003) 111(1):110–3. 10.1542/peds.111.1.110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Wu T, Mendola P, Buck GM. Ethnic Differences in the Presence of Secondary Sex Characteristics and Menarche Among US Girls: The Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 1988-1994. PEDIATRICS (2002) 110(4):752–7. 10.1542/peds.110.4.752 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Kelly Y, Zilanawala A, Sacker A, Hiatt R, Viner R. Early Puberty in 11-Year-Old Girls: Millennium Cohort Study Findings. Arch Dis Childhood (2017) 102(3):232–7. 10.1136/archdischild-2016-310475 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Rosenfield RL, Lipton RB, Drum ML. Thelarche, Pubarche, and Menarche Attainment in Children With Normal and Elevated Body Mass Index. Pediatrics (2009) 123(1):84–8. 10.1542/peds.2008-0146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Søeborg T, Frederiksen H, Mouritsen A, Johannsen TH, Main KM, Jørgensen N, et al. Sex, Age, Pubertal Development and Use of Oral Contraceptives in Relation to Serum Concentrations of DHEA, Dheas, 17α-Hydroxyprogesterone, Δ4-Androstenedione, Testosterone and Their Ratios in Children, Adolescents and Young Adults. Clin Chim Acta (2014) 437:6–13. 10.1016/j.cca.2014.06.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Loomba-Albrecht LA, Styne DM. Effect of Puberty on Body Composition. Curr Opin Endocrinol Diabetes Obes (2009) 16(1):10–5. 10.1097/MED.0b013e328320d54c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Patrick ME, Terry-McElrath YM, Lanza ST, Jager J, Schulenberg JE, O’Malley PM. Shifting Age of Peak Binge Drinking Prevalence: Historical Changes in Normative Trajectories Among Young Adults Aged 18 to 30. Alcohol: Clin Exp Res (2019) 43(2):287–98. 10.1111/acer.13933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Willoughby T, Good M, Adachi PJC, Hamza C, Tavernier R. Examining the Link Between Adolescent Brain Development and Risk Taking From a Social–Developmental Perspective. Brain Cogn (2013) 83(3):315–23. 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Dai J, Scherf KS. Puberty and Functional Brain Development in Humans: Convergence in Findings? Dev Cogn Neurosci (2019) 39:100690. 10.1016/j.dcn.2019.100690 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, Boxer A. A Self-Report Measure of Pubertal Status: Reliability, Validity, and Initial Norms. J Youth Adolescence (1988) 17(2):117–33. 10.1007/BF01537962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Marshall WA, Tanner JM. Variations in the Pattern of Pubertal Changes in Boys. Arch Dis Childhood (1970) 45(239):13–23. 10.1136/adc.45.239.13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Marshall WA, Tanner JM. Variations in Pattern of Pubertal Changes in Girls. Arch Dis Childhood (1969) 44(235):291–303. 10.1136/adc.44.235.291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA, Boyce WT, Deardorff J, Essex MJ. Quality of Early Family Relationships and the Timing and Tempo of Puberty: Effects Depend on Biological Sensitivity to Context. Dev Psychopathol (2011) 23(1):85–99. 10.1017/S0954579410000660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Ruttle PL, Javaras KN, Klein MH, Armstrong JM, Burk LR, Essex MJ. Concurrent and Longitudinal Associations Between Diurnal Cortisol and Body Mass Index Across Adolescence. J Adolesc Health (2013) 52(6):731–7. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.11.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Dorn LD, Susman EJ, Nottelmann ED, Inoff-Germain G, Chrousos GP. Perceptions of Puberty: Adolescent, Parent, and Health Care Personnel. Dev Psychol (1990) 26(2):322–9. 10.1037/0012-1649.26.2.322 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Shirtcliff EA, Dahl RE, Pollak SD. Pubertal Development: Correspondence Between Hormonal and Physical Development. Child Dev (2009) 80(2):327–37. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01263.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Chavarro JE, Watkins DJ, Afeiche MC, Zhang Z, Sánchez BN, Cantonwine D, et al. Validity of Self-Assessed Sexual Maturation Against Physician Assessments and Hormone Levels. J Pediatr (2017) 186:172–8.e3. 10.1016/j.jpeds.2017.03.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Dorn LD, Dahl RE, Woodward HR, Biro F. Defining the Boundaries of Early Adolescence: A User’s Guide to Assessing Pubertal Status and Pubertal Timing in Research With Adolescents. Appl Dev Sci (2006) 10(1):30–56. 10.1207/s1532480xads1001_3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Schlossberger NM, Turner RA, Irwin CE. Validity of Self-Report of Pubertal Maturation in Early Adolescents. J Adolesc Health (1992) 13(2):109–13. 10.1016/1054-139X(92)90075-M [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Mäntyselkä A, Jääskeläinen J, Lindi V, Viitasalo A, Tompuri T, Voutilainen R, et al. The Presentation of Adrenarche is Sexually Dimorphic and Modified by Body Adiposity. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (2014) 99(10):3889–94. 10.1210/jc.2014-2049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Ilondo MM, Vanderschueren-Lodeweyckz M, Vlietinck R, Pizarro M, Malvaux P, Eggermont E, et al. Plasma Androgens in Children and Adolescents. Part I: Control Subjects. Hormone Res (1982) 16(2):61–77. 10.1159/000179486 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Erosheva EA, Kroboth PD, Greenhouse JB. Characterizing the Diurnal Rhythm of DHEA. Am Statistician (2002) 56(4):273–83. 10.1198/000313002579 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Matchock RL, Dorn LD, Susman EJ. Diurnal and Seasonal Cortisol, Testosterone, and DHEA Rhythms in Boys and Girls During Puberty. Chronobiol Int (2007) 24(5):969–90. 10.1080/07420520701649471 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Norjavaara E, Ankarberg C, Albertsson-Wikland K. Diurnal Rhythm of 17 Beta-Estradiol Secretion Throughout Pubertal Development in Healthy Girls: Evaluation by a Sensitive Radioimmunoassay. J Clin Endocrinol Metab (1996) 81(11):4095–102. 10.1210/jcem.81.11.8923866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Bonat S, Pathomvanich A, Keil MF, Field AE, Yanovski JA. Self-Assessment of Pubertal Stage in Overweight Children. Pediatrics (2002) 110(4):743–7. 10.1542/peds.110.4.743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Patton GC, Viner R. Pubertal Transitions in Health. Lancet (2007) 369(9567):1130–9. 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60366-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Ullsperger JM, Nikolas MA. A Meta-Analytic Review of the Association Between Pubertal Timing and Psychopathology in Adolescence: Are There Sex Differences in Risk? Psychol Bull (2017) 143(9):903–38. 10.1037/bul0000106 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Huang B, Biro FM, Dorn LD. Determination of Relative Timing of Pubertal Maturation Through Ordinal Logistic Modeling: Evaluation of Growth and Timing Parameters. J Adolesc Health (2009) 45(4):383–8. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Gelman A, Loken E. The Garden of Forking Paths: Why Multiple Comparisons can be a Problem, Even When There is No “Fishing Expedition” or “P-Hacking” and the Research Hypothesis was Posited Ahead of Time. Columbia University: Department of Statistics; (2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Flournoy JC, Vijayakumar N, Cheng TW, Cosme D, Flannery JE, Pfeifer JH. Improving Practices and Inferences in Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2020) 45:100807. 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100807 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Goddings A-L, Beltz A, Peper JS, Crone EA, Braams BR. Understanding the Role of Puberty in Structural and Functional Development of the Adolescent Brain. J Res Adolescence (2019) 29(1):32–53. 10.1111/jora.12408 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Simonsohn U, Simmons JP, Nelson LD. Specification Curve: Descriptive and Inferential Statistics on All Reasonable Specifications. SSRN J (2015). 10.2139/ssrn.2694998 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Silberzahn R, Uhlmann EL, Martin DP, Anselmi P, Aust F, Awtrey E, et al. Many Analysts, One Data Set: Making Transparent How Variations in Analytic Choices Affect Results. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci (2018) 1(3):337–56. 10.1177/2515245917747646 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Steegen S, Tuerlinckx F, Gelman A, Vanpaemel W. Increasing Transparency Through a Multiverse Analysis. Perspect Psychol Sci (2016) 11(5):702–12. 10.1177/1745691616658637 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Barendse M, Byrne ML, Flournoy J, McNeilly EA, Williamson VG, Barrett A-M, et al. Multi-Method Assessment of Pubertal Timing and Associations With Internalizing Psychopathology in Early Adolescent Girls. PsyArXiv (2020). 10.31234/osf.io/p5vfb [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Anvari F, Lakens D. Using Anchor-Based Methods to Determine the Smallest Effect Size of Interest. PsyArxchiv (2019). 10.31234/osf.io/syp5a [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Feldstein Ewing SW, Bjork JM, Luciana M. Implications of the ABCD Study for Developmental Neuroscience. Dev Cogn Neurosci (2018) 32:161–4. 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Ladouceur CD, Kerestes R, Schlund MW, Shirtcliff EA, Lee Y, Dahl RE. Neural Systems Underlying Reward Cue Processing in Early Adolescence: The Role of Puberty and Pubertal Hormones. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2019) 102:281–91. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.12.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Whittle S, Yücel M, Lorenzetti V, Byrne ML, Simmons JG, Wood SJ, et al. Pituitary Volume Mediates the Relationship Between Pubertal Timing and Depressive Symptoms During Adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2012) 37(7):881–91. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Vijayakumar N, Pfeifer JH, Flournoy JC, Hernandez LM, Dapretto M. Affective Reactivity During Adolescence: Associations With Age, Puberty and Testosterone. Cortex (2019) 117:336–50. 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Consideration of Sex as a Biological Variable in NIH-funded Research (Not-Od-15-102). National Institutes of Health; (2015). Available at: http://orwh.od.nih.gov/sexinscience/overview/pdf/NOT-OD-15-102_Guidance.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Hartung CM, Lefler EK. Sex and Gender in Psychopathology: DSM–5 and Beyond. psychol Bull (2019) 145(4):390–409. 10.1037/bul0000183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Ainsworth C. Sex Redefined. Nat News (2015) 518(7539):288. 10.1038/518288a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Calzo JP, Blashill AJ. Child Sexual Orientation and Gender Identity in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Cohort Study. JAMA Pediatr (2018) 172(11):1090–2. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.2496 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Picci G, Scherf KS. From Caregivers to Peers: Puberty Shapes Human Face Perception. Psychol Sci (2016) 27(11):1461–73. 10.1177/0956797616663142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Marceau K, Ram N, Houts RM, Grimm KJ, Susman EJ. Individual Differences in Boys’ and Girls’ Timing and Tempo of Puberty: Modeling Development With Nonlinear Growth Models. Dev Psychol (2011) 47(5):1389–409. 10.1037/a0023838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: Researchers can apply for access to the data at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd/request-access.


Articles from Frontiers in Endocrinology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES