Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 May 19;16(5):e0250151. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250151

Sex differences in sexual attraction for aesthetics, resources and personality across age

Stephen Whyte 1,2,*,#, Robert C Brooks 3,#, Ho Fai Chan 1,#, Benno Torgler 1,4,#
Editor: Alex Jones5
PMCID: PMC8133465  PMID: 34010298

Abstract

Because sexual attraction is a key driver of human mate choice and reproduction, we descriptively assess relative sex differences in the level of attraction individuals expect in the aesthetic, resource, and personality characteristics of potential mates. As a novelty we explore how male and female sexual attractiveness preference changes across age, using a dataset comprising online survey data for over 7,000 respondents across a broad age distribution of individuals between 18 and 65 years. In general, we find that both males and females show similar distribution patterns in their preference responses, with statistically significant sex differences within most of the traits. On average, females rate age, education, intelligence, income, trust, and emotional connection around 9 to 14 points higher than males on our 0–100 scale range. Our relative importance analysis shows greater male priority for attractiveness and physical build, compared to females, relative to all other traits. Using multiple regression analysis, we find a consistent statistical sex difference (males relative to females) that decreases linearly with age for aesthetics, while the opposite is true for resources and personality, with females exhibiting a stronger relative preference, particularly in the younger aged cohort. Exploring non-linearity in sex difference with contour plots for intelligence and attractiveness across age (mediated by age) indicates that sex differences in attractiveness preferences are driven by the male cohort (particularly age 30 to 40) for those who care about the importance of age, while intelligence is driven by females caring relatively more about intelligence for those who see age as very important (age cohort 40 to 55). Overall, many of our results indicate distinct variations within sex at key life stages, which is consistent with theories of selection pressure. Moreover, results also align with theories of parental investment, the gender similarities hypothesis, and mutual mate choice–which speaks to the fact that the broader discipline of evolutionary mate choice research in humans still contains considerable scope for further inquiry towards a unified theory, particularly when exploring sex-difference across age.

Introduction

Sexual attraction is a primary driver of mate selection and reproductive decisions, and those decisions impact not only the individuals involved but the broader market in which the decision occurs [1]. Micro level decision making on sex, reproduction, and relationship formation thus influences a wide variety of macro trends and social norms, including, inter alia, gender roles and equity, labor market dynamics, fertility rates, wider sexual liberalism, religion, politics, and the broader institution of marriage [24]. However, there is ongoing debate inside the field of evolutionary mate choice regarding exactly how similar or different male and female preferences are across age [58]. To investigate competing evolutionary theories, we analyze responses from more than 7000 Australian online dating participants to generate a descriptive overview of both relative and absolute sex differences in individuals’ stated preferences for aesthetics, resources, and personality characteristics in a potential mate across age.

As argued in numerous scientific disciplines, the human preference for attractive mates and the ability to quickly identify such attractiveness in others [9] reflect an evolved adaptive preference to reproduce “good genes” [10] And while both sexes prefer mates who are physically attractive and state clear preferences for the level of attractiveness sought in a potential partner, males have been shown to report stronger preferences for attractiveness relative to females [11]. Trivers’ 1972 [12] seminal work argued that these different preferences between the sexes for particular characteristics in a mate should clearly reflect sexual selection pressures. That is, females are more selective, not only because their maximum fecundity is time limited but because choosing poorly increases the long-term opportunity costs of reproduction (internal gestation, ongoing lactation, and disproportionate maternal investment) and reduces the probability of offspring [12]. Their mate preferences should thus reflect characteristics or traits that can compensate for disproportionate maternal investment and ensure offspring survival and reproductive success, especially with respect to economic proxies for resources and/or increased paternal investment such as educational level, intelligence, and income. In fact, research has shown females demonstrate far more stringent preferences than males for mates with good earning potential or higher education [11], particularly during the years of peak fertility [13, 14]. Males, in contrast, need only invest the time taken to copulate, which paucity of paternal investment implies the favoring of mates whose genetic fitness guarantees a maximum chance of offspring survival and reproduction.

Accordingly, based on an assumption that aesthetics such as age, attractiveness, and symmetry of physical build or features imply a lower likelihood of disease or pathogen prone ancestry, humans use these instantly recognizable characteristics as proxies for both genetic and phenotypic condition. Not only are males more likely than females to state a preference for physically attractive characteristics in a mate [15], but their prioritizing of female facial cues over body shape is dependent on the planned mating duration [16]. That is, whereas females remain unaffected by mating temporality, males prioritize facial cues in a long-term mating context but bodily cues in a short-term one. This difference may stem from female faces and bodies simultaneously showcasing traits that are pronounced correlates of health and fertility, such as estrogen-dependent facial features (lips, cheeks, jaw line) and body features (waist-to-hip ratio and accentuated gait) p. 490 [17].

What attracts an individual to a mate, therefore–what the average human finds “sexy”–is an integral part of short- or long-term mate choice strategies and inherently a key point of similarity or difference between the sexes. More recently, a new body of literature has begun to emerge that is more critical of the good genes hypothesis [1820], and psychology’s possible overstatement of sex differences in human mate choice [6]. While there is indeed a large sex difference in obligate parental investment in humans, sex differences in “typical” parental investment (particularly in modern developed economies) are much smaller, which ultimately leads to similar levels of choosiness in long-term mating domains. This new body of literature points out that, even cross-culturally, certain favored mate choice traits or characteristics are still important for both sexes [21] and that the dynamics of mutual mate choice (MMC) reveal sex differences that are more appropriately characterized as relative, rather than absolute.

This study therefore descriptively examines the responses of a large sample of Australian online dating participants (n = 7325) to assess level of sexual attraction in a mate expressed as stated preferences by both sexes. Our key focus is an exploration of how relative sex preferences change across age as limited empirical evidence and theoretical understandings are available regarding such potential preference changes. Most studies on sexual attractiveness rely on a limited age distribution skewed towards the younger population. We therefore utilize a large age distribution (18–65 years of age) of a cross-sectional data set to better understand relative sex differences across age for different individuals’ sexual attraction towards aesthetic, resource, and personality traits in a potential mate. Thus, our main goal is empirical rather than theoretical, as the literature behind sex preference differences makes no or very limited predictions for how this pattern might change with age.

Aesthetics, resources and personality in mate choice

Aesthetics

Attractive individuals derive a broad social range of utility, enjoying everything from greater choice in the mating market to greater human capital investment during one’s education, and even increased returns from their labor market productivity [2224]. This may be because judgements on attractiveness potentially reflect evaluation of apparent physical health [25] or phenotypic condition. In fact, applying the biological “good genes” hypothesis specifically to sexual selection implies that the mate preference for healthy looking (i.e., attractive) individuals who promise the associated direct and indirect benefits is an adaptive preference for physical traits that increase both parental and offspring fitness [10]. Of course, it is reasonable to expect differences in the use of aesthetic indicators of fertility and reproductive capacity such as age, attractiveness, and physical build and features. For example, whereas the human female has a relatively short reproductive phase and declining fecundity with age [26], human males often maintain their reproductive function (with only minimal decline) until old age [27]. And while older males are more prone to the rare spontaneous de novo mutations that can increase the risk of conditions like autism, there is no critical threshold for sperm production, and men can realize offspring far beyond their 40s [28]. Therefore, evolutionary science has theorized and demonstrated that males are more likely to state a preference for females at peak fertility [11] even when they themselves are beyond this stage [29], whereas females are more likely to refine their specific mate preferences across their years of peak fertility [11]. Yet, the size of the relative difference in both sexes’ preference for aesthetics in a mate–at different life stages–remains unclear.

Resources

Because of the human female’s disproportionate opportunity costs of gestation and lactation, males have historically been rewarded (or at least not disadvantaged like females) in both higher education and the labor market for their ability to work continuously without reproduction-related career interruptions [2]. The fact that this freedom to continue attracting income translates into increased human capital, higher wage rates, and greater lifetime earnings may partly explain some of the current and historical large intersex variance in the ability to access, possess, and accrue resources [11]. For example, according to the Australian Bureau of Statistics [30], average hourly earnings peak for males in the mid-40s but for females, earnings peak in their 30s [31]. Such wage gaps are common and consistent across developed economies [32], accentuating male access to earnings and resources, and forcing women who seek higher education or equivalent earnings in their late 20s through their 30s to postpone or delay pregnancy [27]. As mate choice research across a myriad of disciplines repeatedly demonstrates, this combination of disproportionate physiological investment and constraints in accessing resources places evolutionary selection pressure on females to secure or favor mates who can compensate for this cost constraint [1, 11, 14, 33]. With such disproportionate opportunity costs for both labor market and reproductive participation, sex differentiated preferences for accrued resources or proxies for access should also be visible in any mating market. Furthermore, because income is positively correlated with age (on average), but shows diminishing returns beyond 50 years of age [31], relative and absolute preference for resources potentially also change as a function of age for both sexes.

Personality

Given the inheritability of personality traits [34], if males exhibit particular personality traits that signal (increased) paternal investment, it seems likely that females have also evolved specialized mechanisms that suggest corresponding maternal characteristics. In addition to increasing pair bond strength through parental investment, such positive externalities in mate choice may also reinforce reciprocally altruistic behavior between mates [12], increase complementary production in the household [2], promote kin selection towards genetic relatives [3], and increase the chances of long-term mate retention. Their importance in mate selection may also be increasing in developed countries where sex-discrimination legislation and wider efforts towards gender equity have narrowed the gap in the ability of males and females to acquire the income and wealth resources that benefit child rearing and welfare. Hence, modern female preferences may not only more acutely favor personality traits or “good father attributes” that increase reproductive success [35], but as household, gender, and labor market roles evolve and even converge, personality traits may become a greater point of differentiation [36] in a potential mate than resources or the ability to acquire them. The current study provides a unique opportunity to explore both relative and absolute sex difference stated preference for key personality factors such as trust, openness, and emotional connection in a large sample (n = 7325) of online dating participants and how those preferences change with age. The way in which sex preference differences for personality change with age is theoretically as well as empirically underexplored. Thus, due to the limited existing theoretical understanding, we try to contribute to the area by providing novel empirical insights that may guide future theoretical insights–as science can be seen as a constant interaction between speaking to theorists and searching for facts.

Method

Research design

Our analysis is based on participant responses to nine different versions of the same question format, covering nine characteristics associated with sexual attraction–age, attractiveness, physical build/features, intelligence, education, income, trust, openness, emotional connection:

To what extent do you find a person’s [SPECIFIC TRAIT] influences how sexually attractive you find them:

0 = Not important at all ……………………………..…. Extremely important = 100.

Each question thus asks respondents to rate the level of importance they assign to each characteristic in relation to sexual attraction on a sliding scale from 0 to 100. These characteristics are then grouped into three key categories commonly associated with sexual attraction: aesthetics (age, attractiveness, and physical build/features), resources (intelligence, education, and income), and personality (trust, openness, and emotional connection). The aesthetic factors are easily recognizable and assessable in even minute interpersonal interactions or exposure [37]; the three resource factors are all commonly used in mate choice research, as they aid parental investment [1, 11, 38]; and finally, the three personality factors matter for interpersonal relationships, pair bonding, and parental investment [3941].

Data collection

These data were collected as part of the national online Australian Sex Survey, administered to the Australian general public between July 25 and September 19, 2016, and resulting in a very broad Australian sample. Some data from the survey has already been published in unrelated research [36, 4245]. Participation was incentivized by three random draws for approximately $1,500 worth of prizes donated by the industry partners Adultmatchmaker.com and its affiliated dating web sites, Eros Association, the Australian Sex Party, Max Black, and Giga Pty Ltd. All research was conducted in accordance with Queensland University of Technology (QUT) human research ethics on clearance approval number 1600000221. All participants were 18 years of age or older at the time of the survey, and provided written informed consent to participate (see A1 Table in S1 Appendix for the summary statistics of the sample, by sex).

Results

Descriptive results

We first explore the distribution of the responses to the nine characteristics, differentiated by sex (see A1 Fig in S1 Appendix). Overall, the distribution between the nine traits follows a similar pattern for both sexes; for example, the three personality traits, physical build, and attractiveness, are rated quite high on the importance scale for both sexes, while age, intelligence, and education are more evenly rated, and income is rated quite low on the importance scale. Table 1 shows significant within-trait sex differences for 8 out of 9 traits. In particular, we find that, on average, females rate age (Cliff’s delta δ = 0.255, p<0.001), education (δ = 0.253, p<0.001), intelligence (δ = 0.309, p<0.001), income (δ = 0.25, p<0.001), trust (δ = 0.222, p<0.001), and emotional connection (δ = 0.309, p<0.001) between 9 and 14 points higher than males do (on a scale ranging from 0 to 100). On the other hand, there is no statistical sex difference in terms of importance rating in the attractiveness attribute (δ = 0.013, p = 0.730), and the difference in physical build (δ = 0.039, p = 0.121) is minimal. One should also note that the overlapping coefficients for the (male and female) distributions for attractiveness, physical build, and openness are among the highest. The overlapping coefficient indicates the degree of overlap between the kernel density estimates of the respective distribution (male and female). For example, a value of 1 would indicate a perfect overlap between the two distributions [46, 47].

Table 1. Sex preference differences in characteristics.

Males Females
Absolute importance Mean Diff. Cliff’s delta z-stat. (two tailed) Mean SE 95% CI Lower 95% CI Upper Mean SE 95%CI Lower 95%CI Upper Overlap. Coeff.
Aesthetics
    Age 11.927 0.255 18.01*** 48.1 0.41 47.3 48.9 60.0 0.48 59.1 61.0 0.700
    Attractiveness -0.187 0.013 -0.89 65.9 0.34 65.3 66.6 65.7 0.42 64.9 66.6 0.928
    Physical build -0.838 0.039 -2.77* 65.3 0.35 64.7 66.0 64.5 0.42 63.7 65.3 0.898
Resources
    Education 12.172 0.253 17.88*** 41.2 0.41 40.4 42.1 53.4 0.52 52.4 54.4 0.689
    Intelligence 13.894 0.309 21.86*** 55.8 0.40 55.0 56.6 69.7 0.43 68.9 70.6 0.640
    Income 9.695 0.25 17.69*** 19.6 0.31 19.0 20.2 29.3 0.45 28.4 30.2 0.675
Personality
    Openness 4.445 0.132 9.35*** 69.2 0.31 68.6 69.9 73.7 0.37 73.0 74.4 0.817
    Trust 9.143 0.222 15.66*** 68.9 0.38 68.2 69.7 78.1 0.40 77.3 78.9 0.717
    Emotional connection 12.397 0.309 21.86*** 65.1 0.39 64.3 65.9 77.5 0.42 76.7 78.3 0.623
Males Females
Relative importance Mean Diff. Cohen’s d t-stat. Mean SE 95% CI 95% CI Mean SE 95%CI 95%CI Overlap.
(two tailed) Lower Upper Lower Upper Coeff.
Aesthetics
    Age 0.145 0.179 7.26*** -0.280 0.012 -0.304 -0.256 -0.135 0.016 -0.166 -0.104 0.823
    Attractiveness -0.292 0.433 -17.30*** 0.400 0.010 0.380 0.419 0.107 0.014 0.081 0.134 0.701
    Physical build -0.326 0.478 -19.32*** 0.375 0.010 0.355 0.395 0.049 0.013 0.022 0.075 0.673
Resources
    Education 0.130 0.17 6.78*** -0.514 0.011 -0.536 -0.492 -0.384 0.016 -0.415 -0.354 0.804
    Intelligence 0.227 0.317 13.09*** 0.039 0.011 0.017 0.061 0.266 0.013 0.240 0.293 0.833
    Income -0.030 0.047 -1.86 -1.411 0.009 -1.429 -1.393 -1.441 0.013 -1.467 -1.415 0.804
Personality
    Openness -0.131 0.189 -7.65*** 0.509 0.010 0.489 0.529 0.378 0.014 0.351 0.405 0.867
    Trust 0.083 0.108 4.49*** 0.508 0.012 0.484 0.532 0.591 0.014 0.564 0.619 0.866
    Emotional connection 0.195 0.252 10.33*** 0.374 0.012 0.351 0.397 0.569 0.015 0.540 0.598 0.756

Notes: Nmales = 4,375. Nfemales = 2,685.

†p < 0.10

*p < 0.05

**p < 0.01

***p < 0.001. Wilcoxon rank-sum (Mann-Whitney) test were used for the sex difference in absolute importance and t-test for relative importance. We account for multiple comparisons of characteristics within the same group with Bonferroni adjustment (sets the significance cut-off at α/3). Setting the significance cut-off at α/9 (nine characteristics) returns the same conclusion. Effect size measures (Cliff’s delta (non-parametric) and Cohen’s d) are absolute value.

To gauge the relative importance of the nine characteristics for each individual participant, we standardized the responses to the nine traits ‘within’ subject. Specifically, for each individual, we first calculate the average value (level) of the nine responses as well as the standard deviation (spread), then we subtract the value of each trait to this average and divide by the standard deviation. Essentially, one can interpret the standardized values as the importance of one trait relative to the average importance of all nine traits. This approach safeguards the results from comparison with omitted factors (e.g., common interests), which might cause respondents to have a lower/higher level (all factors are not as important as the omitted factor). In addition, we also find a sex difference effect on absolute rating, i.e., female respondents gave, on average, 8.1 points higher for each of the nine ratings (total 73 points) than male respondents (p<0.001; t = 24.8). It is also evident that for 7 out of 9 characteristics, females gave a higher rating than males. Likewise, we find large variation in the average and variance of the ratings given by respondents; such variations also seem to differ across sex and age (see F2 Fig in S1 Appendix). Additionally, our results do not change if we use the rank ordering of traits instead of the standard deviation from the average rating. Pair-wise correlations for all nine characteristics with standardized values are also provided in A2 Table in S1 Appendix.

Compared to the distributions of the raw importance rating, we identify subtle differences in terms of sex difference of the relative importance of traits to sexual attraction (Fig 1). First, we find that, in terms of aesthetic factors, males regard both attractiveness (Cohen’s d = -0.433, p<0.001) and physical build (d = -0.478, p<0.001) as more important characteristics for sexual attraction, relative to all other traits, compared to females, while the latter regard age (d = 0.179, p<0.001) as relatively more important. The sex differences in attractiveness and physical build are substantial; for example, males rate the two factors .29 and .33 SD higher than the mean ratings given for all nine traits, whereas females rate them .11 and .05 SD higher than their average rating, respectively (see t-test results in Table 1). These two factors also have the lowest overlapping coefficient, signifying the magnitude of the sex difference. Second, while both sexes regard income as the least important factor for sexual attraction, after adjusting for the variance in individual ratings, we do not find a significant difference between males and females (d = -0.047, p = 0.190). Compared to males, females place the other two resource factors, namely education (d = 0.130, p<0.001) and intelligence (d = 0.227, p<0.001), as relatively more important. Third, we find that despite females giving a higher absolute rating to openness compared to males, if compared with the importance of other traits, males actually regard openness as a slightly more important factor than do females (d = -0.131, p<0.001).

Fig 1. Relative importance of aesthetics, resources, and personality factors for sexual attraction, by sex.

Fig 1

Distribution density for each factor is estimated within sex.

Next, we examine whether the expressed preferences for the nine characteristics, as well as their respective sex differences, covary with age. We first present graphical evidence in Fig 2, which indicates the average relative importance of the nine characteristics (standardized within respondents) across sex and age. To show potential non-linear (e.g., curvilinear) relationships with age, we use a local cubic polynomial smoothing on the average importance. For transparency, a linear fit and the raw difference between sexes are also plotted. We also show the sex differences across age in A3 Fig in S1 Appendix. Similar to the previous findings, we find that males exhibit stronger preferences for attractiveness and physical build (relative to other traits) across all ages but weaker preference for age, compared to females. We find that, while the relative importance for age and attractiveness decreases over age for both sexes, the preference for physical build increases over age for females and remains flat for males over age. There is also a tendency that sex difference in preferences for attractiveness and physical build decreases over age (A3 Fig in S1 Appendix). With respect to resource factors, we find that both sexes seem to regard education as relatively less important on average (females’ preference is slightly stronger than male except at late 20s and early 30s), and exhibits a decreasing trend over age. It should be noted that (on average) there is an increase in female preference for education in the age 60+ group, despite the small number of observations. We also see a decrease in importance of intelligence for both sexes. However, females’ preference for intelligence is stronger than that of males, and this difference seems particularly strong in the mid-20s and late 40s. Again, we do not see any significant sex difference in terms of income as both males and females regard it as the least important factor; however, one should note that (on average) younger people regard income as less important than older people. Lastly, we find that the preference for openness and trust increases over age for both sexes. Across all ages, females deem trust as relatively more important compared to males. This sex difference in trust appears to decrease with age, while older males regard openness as a relatively more important factor than do older females. We find that the relative importance of emotional connection for both sexes remain at the same level across age, while noting a small positive deviation for females in the early 30s and late 50s.

Fig 2. Level of sexual attraction for aesthetic, resource, and personality characteristics over age, by sex.

Fig 2

Markers represent the average relative importance for each characteristic within sex (males = blue, females = red), calculated at each year of age. Smoothed lines represent the local cubic polynomial with Gaussian kernel function and bandwidth of 5, with shaded areas representing the 95% confidence intervals. We also show the linear fit of the observations across age, represented in grey dashed lines. The green reference line (relative importance = 0) indicates a factor is of the same importance to the average of all nine characteristics. Each characteristic is graphed for participants aged 18–64 years.

Multiple regression analysis

As our study seeks to descriptively explore sex differences in perceived importance of general aesthetic, resource, and personality factors in relation to sexual attraction, we first perform a principal components analysis on the nine characteristics. Our results (A3 Table in S1 Appendix) show that the nine characteristics fit well into the three principal factors with eigenvalue larger than 1 (cumulative proportion of variance explained = .64). In particular, each characteristic is shown to have high (at least 0.5) and positive factor loadings on the principal factors identified. Utilizing these three factors (aesthetic, resource, and personality), in conjunction with the original nine characteristics, we conduct a series of regression analyses to explore factors influencing the level of importance that males and females place on each characteristic. We perform the same within-subject standardization on the three factors as implemented on the nine characteristics. For the majority of characteristics (with the exception being Resources), we find that sex differences are at most quartic with respect to age. As the results still capture the lower-order effect (taking into account the higher-order age effect), we present the results of the cubic.

We first estimate the following model to see how sex difference in mating preferences change across age, by controlling for other factors:

Yik=β0+β1malei+β2agei+β3agei2+β4agei3+β5malei*agei+β6malei*agei2+β6malei*agei3+γXi+ϵi, (1)

where Yi is the relative importance of characteristic j to respondent i, and X is the vector of control variables such as physique, education, income, marital status, sexual orientation, and self-rated happiness, health, and attractiveness. Because of the inherent sex difference bias in the distribution of variables such as height, income, or self-rated health, we standardize the controls within sex. We capture both the linear and non-linear age effects (i.e., if the sex difference changes across life span) by interacting the male dummy variable with age and its squared and cubed term. For each regression conducted, we employ an ordinary least squares (OLS) regression model with heteroscedasticity-consistent standard errors.

By holding other factors constant, we visualize the sex difference on the relative importance of each characteristic (compared to how they perceived the importance of the other factors) across age in Fig 3. The full regression results are provided in A5 Table in S1 Appendix. For most characteristics (the exception being Resources), we find that sex differences are at most quartic with respect to age. Since the results would still capture the lower-order effect (while taking into account the higher-order age effect), we present the results of the cubic relationship in the main text. For transparency and to assist interpretation of the results, whe also report the regression results using quadratic and linear age effect in A5 and A6 Tables in S1 Appendix. In terms of aesthetic (Fig 3A), we find that the consistent and significant positive sex difference decreases relatively linearly across age. We see a small negative sex difference in the preference for age, but this does not appear to differ across age. In terms of physical build, we also observe that the positive sex difference is strongest in younger people (under 30s) while relative preference for attractiveness does not seem to change across age (Fig 3B). None of the coefficients of the interaction of sex and linear, quartic, and cubic age terms are significant.

Fig 3. Sex differences in importance of aesthetic, resource, and personality factors for sexual attraction, across lifespan.

Fig 3

Positive (negative) sex difference shows males (females) place higher relative importance in terms of sexual attraction to a characteristic; sex difference not significantly differ from 0 means the characteristics have equal importance to both males and females, relative to other factors. Estimates are obtained from Eq (1) using OLS. Shaded areas represent 95% confidence intervals.

Next, we observe that females place resource as a more important factor than do males overall (compared to aesthetic and personality), but such difference appears to increase with age till age 30 and decrease beyond (Fig 3C). It seems that sex difference again increases after age 50, however, there is not enough evidence to provide a definite conclusion (large confidence intervals). There are no sex differences across all ages in terms of preference for income, confirming our earlier observations (Fig 3D). Females in their late 20s and early 30s regard education as more important (relative to how they perceive other characteristics) compared to their male counterparts, while females of other ages place a higher relative importance on education. However, we observe a strong U-shape relationship with age for sex difference in the preference for intelligence, showing that females in their early 40s regard intelligence as far more important than other factors compared to males of the same age bracket.

Lastly, we observe that sex difference in preference for personality (where females care relatively more than males) is largest for younger respondents. This difference appears to decrease with age before 30 and remain stable until around 55, where the sex difference is then no longer significant. The two slopes on openness and trust exhibit a positive gradient with respect to age. This aligns with our earlier observations: for males, the increase in importance of these two factors across age is higher than that of females, leading to larger positive and smaller negative sex difference for openness and trust, respectively. With respect to importance in emotional connection, the difference in sex is significant across age, but we also note that the gap increases from age 18 to 30 and decreases thereafter.

We further explore the relative differences observed across age in Fig 3 by considering possible non-linearity of sex differences in attractiveness across participants’ age, mediated by the relative importance of age as an attractiveness characteristic (see Fig 4A (without controls), and 4b (with controls)). The sex difference is more dominant (darker coloration) among those who care about age (positive value on the y-axis). We also see that sex differences in the relative importance of attractiveness are largest for the age group around 30 to 40. Those sex differences are driven by the male population aged 30 to 40 who care the most about age, as evidenced in A4 Fig in S1 Appendix.

Fig 4.

Fig 4

Sex differences in importance of attractiveness for sexual attraction across participants’ age, mediated by relative importance of age for sexual attraction (panel a and b) and sex differences in importance of intelligence across age, mediated by relative importance of attractiveness (panel c) and age (panel d). Estimates of sex differences (represented by color) were obtained from OLS regressions. In each regression, we include the interaction term between sex, importance of age or attractiveness (y-axis) and age and age square (x-axis). Positive sex differences (blue) indicate males place relatively higher importance on the explained trait (z-axis) compared to females, relative to other factors. In panel a, no control variables were added to the regression, in panel b, c, and d, control variables were included in the regression model.

In Fig 4C we show that sex differences in relative importance of intelligence are greatest for the age group 40 to 55 years and those who have a higher preference for the importance of attractiveness. A5 Fig in S1 Appendix indicates that for low values of importance of attractiveness there are almost no sex differences across age. Among the higher values, on the other hand, the sex difference was driven by males caring relatively less about intelligence (compared to females), in particular around the age range 40 to 55. When looking at the importance of age rather than attractiveness as a mediator of age (Fig 4D) we see an age shift of sex differences to the left, which means that the strongest differences are found in the cohort between 35 and 45 among those who place high relative importance of age. Those differences are driven by (younger) females caring relatively more about intelligence than do the male cohort of the same age (see A6 Fig in S1 Appendix). Comparing A5d and A6d Figs in S1 Appendix indicates that older females who care more about age as an attractiveness factor care less about intelligence, while the male pattern seems to be quite similar for both the importance of attractiveness and age. Thus, the shift of the sex differences between Fig 4C and 4D are driven by females.

In addition to respondent’s age, we also examine how other variables such as physique, education, income, marital status, sexual orientation, and self-rated happiness, health, and attractiveness influence the perception of sexual attraction. To model whether the effects of these variables are sex-specific (e.g., present in one sex but not the other) and whether the effects differ by sex we include interaction terms between sex and these variables. For transparency, we also present regression results with male and female subsamples in A7-A9 Tables in S1 Appendix (with quartic age effects), as one can assess the raw effects of each variable on the sexual preferences for the two sexes. Since we focus on each subsample, we use the original (non-standardized) variables and present the beta coefficients (in italics) to assess the effect size in terms of unit and standard deviation change in the independent variables. For comparison of the effect size between each variable across sexes, we again employ the variables standardized within the two sexes.

We summarize the regression results on the three principal components (aesthetic, resource, and personality) in Fig 5 and present the results on the nine individual characteristics in A7-A9 Figs in S1 Appendix. Sex-specific age effects are included in the model, but we omit the results on age as they do not differ from the earlier regression results. We observe that those with higher education place less emphasis on aesthetics, with males even less so compared to females. Those who are single and heterosexual place higher emphasis on resources, and again, males show a greater preference when compared to females. Interestingly, the opposite is true for personality, with more attractive males and single males caring less about personality than their female counterparts. Further, males with offspring care less about personality, but females with offspring care more about personality.

Fig 5. Sex-specific factors on sexual attraction importance of aesthetic, resource, and personality factors.

Fig 5

We show the effect of each independent variable on the relative importance of characteristics of interest for both sexes (effects on male and female sexual preferences are indicated by blue and red markers, respectively). P-values of the sex-specific effects are shown above the corresponding markers, with error bars indicating 90%, 95%, 99%, and 99.5% confidence intervals. The statistical significance of the sex difference of each independent variable (i.e., interaction terms with sex) is shown to the right of the coefficient estimates. † p < .10; * p < .05; ** p < .01; *** p < .001.

Discussion

Mating market preferences and decisions regarding attractiveness are arguably based on three core areas: appearances (aesthetics), personal characteristics and qualities (personality), and the ability to provide (resource) access and security to potential suitors. As our study shows, individual differences between preferences for each of these characteristics differ between women and men, as well as with age. Despite significant sex differences, however, men and women gave broadly similar priority to the measured preferences, consistent with a model of mutual mate choice [6] or the broader gender similarities hypothesis [5].

At its simplest, our study’s descriptive findings demonstrate that for all nine characteristics of interests, both males and females show similar distribution patterns in their preference responses. That said, there are statistically significant sex differences within traits for eight out of the nine traits explored; on average, females rated age, education, intelligence, income, trust, and emotional connection around 9 to 14 points higher than males on our 0–100 scale range. On the surface, one may make the observation that for the population sampled, and compared with males, females care more about a greater number of characteristics when considering attractiveness in a potential mate. Such findings lend confirmatory weight to previous research findings and broader historical evolutionary theory that predicts that females tend to be choosier than men [11, 12]

By standardizing the responses to the nine traits within subject, our relative importance analysis forced an effective ranking of the nine measured preferences. Interestingly, our findings indicate greater male priority for attractiveness and physical build, compared to females, relative to all other traits. For example, males rated attractiveness .29 SD and physical build .33 SD higher than the mean ratings (to all nine traits) given; whereas females rate attractiveness and physical build .11 SD and .05 SD higher than their average rating, respectively. Conversely, compared to males, females place relatively more importance on the two resource factors, namely education and intelligence. Such results are in line with previous research findings supporting sex differences according to the predictions from parental investment theory [1, 12]. Forced ranking of preferences exposes small but detectable differences in relative emphasis on preferences that are consistent with male resource-holding and female fecundity-nubility being important considerations in mate choice [4850].

Our study also explored variation in perceived importance for sexual attraction of the nine characteristics, as well as their respective sex differences at different life stages. Our most novel findings again center on attractiveness and physical build (relative to other traits), with males exhibiting stronger preferences (than females) for both, across all ages. Interestingly, for both sexes, preference for attractiveness appears negatively correlated with age, but preference for openness and trust is positively associated with age. In many mating preference studies, the focus is on young adults, which means that we know relatively little about older cohorts’ preferences. The consonant changes shown by women and men with age suggest one possible source of age-dependent assortative mating, consistent with predictions that mutual mate choice may be worth consideration in addition to sex-dependent preferences [6]. Age-assortative preferences warrant further research.

The study also explored non-linearity in sex-difference preferences for intelligence and attractiveness across age, mediated by the importance of age: when exploring intelligence, we checked attractiveness as a mediator. Sex differences across age are the smallest for those who reported the lowest preferences for aesthetics (age and attractiveness); however, for those who care more about aesthetics, there is a larger sex difference and such differences depend on participants’ age. The sex differences in the preference for attractiveness were driven by the male cohort who cared more about age aesthetics, and were largest for the age group 30 to 40. Sex differences in the importance of intelligence were also positively affected by the importance of attractiveness and age, but sex differences for those with high aesthetic preferences were driven by females caring relatively more about intelligence, particularly for females age 40 to 55. Such findings indicating distinct variation within sex at key life stages may again speak to theories of sexual selection pressures resulting in biologically specific adaptions [11, 12].

Our multiple regression analysis explores factors impacting preferences for all nine characteristics individually, as well as their three groupings. Here, we find a consistent statistical sex difference (males relative to females) that decreases linearly with age for aesthetics. The opposite is true for resources and personality, with females exhibiting a stronger relative preference, particularly in the younger cohort of our sample.

Finally, our principal component regression results demonstrate interesting associations between individual differences in personality traits and our measures of preference, indicating a clear relative sex differences for single males’ preferences for resources compared to females. More highly educated females express a higher relative preference for aesthetics, and more attractive females exhibit a higher relative preference for personality. We also find absolute differences for females with offspring, who place more emphasis on personality, whereas males with offspring report this trait as less important.

Overall, our study provides descriptive findings concerning sex and individual differences in self-reported mating preferences, most of which are consistent with predictions made by existing theories about attraction to aesthetic, resource, and personality traits. That so many of our findings align with theories of both parental investment and mutual mate choice speaks to the fact that the broader discipline of evolutionary mate choice research in humans still contains considerable scope for further inquiry before reaching any unified theory. The fact that such rapid advances in modern technology (such as the internet, and big data more broadly) now allows behavioral science a gamut of new avenues for analysis suggests a growing opportunity for more rigorous analysis and continued scientific debate on the topic of human mating behavior [43].

The authors acknowledge several limitations to the current study. Firstly, our sample population is the result of self-selection; naturally, any online open access national survey generates an unavoidable selection bias. While our sample population is extremely large compared to previous mate choice studies (n = 7325), it is important to acknowledge limitations due to representativeness of the Australian general population. The second problem lies with the subjectivity of the participants’ ratings and self-ratings; for example, the term “sexual attractiveness” may not be homogenous in meaning or interpretation for all participants in our sample, a methodological issue that is, however, present across all fields of behavioral science research. Likewise, surveying such a large number of individuals may induce “noise” around individual decisions and responses compared to the results from a more controlled laboratory experiment setting. Nevertheless, not only were the survey questions standardized for all participants in terms of both the dependent variables and their relation to the respondent’s own sexual attraction, but the study delineated nine different characteristics for which the participants made their own independent assessments. Further, the large sample (n = 7325) and age distribution (18–65 years) of real-world online dating participants provides a unique robustness check for comparative mate choice research that has traditionally sampled more homogenous undergraduate student samples. Admittedly, however, in 21st century cyber mating markets (just as all historical mate choice settings) stated preferences are not always definitive indicators of actual behavior [51]. Future revealed preference research would do well to collect longitudinal data that explored individuals’ stated preference and actual mate choice decisions across time. Further, it is important to note that linear high/low scales may not necessarily be the most efficient way to capture data on preference, mainly due to participant indifference. Positive-negative scales do not necessarily allow an individual to respond with indifference, and rather only permit choice of a middle 50-point marker on a 0–100 scale. Such methodological constraints are an important and ongoing consideration for future work in this space. Finally, while the current study analyses and reports the sexual attraction preference for an extremely large population of Australian online dating participants (n = 7325), the authors caution over-emphasis of statistically significant results stemming from such a large sample size. Any and all descriptive analysis in the current study were reported so as to provide scientific transparency, and in accordance with the current standards across the evolutionary behavioral sciences.

At different life stages both sexes prioritize (or favor) different (or similar) characteristics in a mate. For example, given that peak female fertility is essentially restricted to the (late) second and third decades of life, it seems logical that preferences will differ between males and females across these years. But this is not to say that these differences are absolute, with parental investment being a good example; not least because modern developed societies exhibit probably the most homogenous gender roles in human history. Traits and proxies for parental care and investment are thus highly valued in both sexes–although, as our research repeatedly shows–they can differ relatively at different life stages. As such, future mate choice research would do well to take into account both relative and absolute perspectives when conducting sex difference research. Given the importance of sexual attraction in reproductive decision making, ongoing research is warranted into this large-scale decision process. That the broader field of evolutionary mate choice is yet to reach a unified theory of sex differentiated stated preference across the life span speaks to the need for greater descriptive analysis of large-scale real-world mating market participants such as those included in the current study.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix

(DOCX)

Data Availability

Data and codes used in this study can be found on the Open Science Framework (DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/DSJ9W).

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Buss D. M. (2016). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. Basic books. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Becker G. S. (1985). Human capital, effort, and the sexual division of labor. Journal of labor economics, 3(1, Part 2), S33–S58. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Binmore K. (2005). Natural justice. Oxford university press. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Miller G. (2001). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human. Nature. London: Vintage, Random House. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hyde J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypothesis. American psychologist, 60(6), 581. 10.1037/0003-066X.60.6.581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Stewart-Williams S., & Thomas A. G. (2013). The ape that thought it was a peacock: Does evolutionary psychology exaggerate human sex differences? Psychological Inquiry, 24(3), 137–168. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Zell E., Krizan Z., & Teeter S. R. (2015). Evaluating gender similarities and differences using metasynthesis. American Psychologist, 70(1), 10. 10.1037/a0038208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Boothroyd L. G., & Vukovic J. (2018). Mate preferences across the lifespan. The Oxford Handbook on Evolutionary Psychology and Behavioral Endocrinology. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Olson I. R., & Marshuetz C. (2005). Facial attractiveness is appraised in a glance. Emotion, 5(4), 498. 10.1037/1528-3542.5.4.498 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Scheib J. E., Gangestad S. W., & Thornhill R. (1999). Facial attractiveness, symmetry and cues of good genes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 266(1431), 1913–1917. 10.1098/rspb.1999.0866 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Buss D. M., & Barnes M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(3), 559. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Trivers. R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. Sexual Selection & the Descent of Man, Aldine de Gruyter, New York, 136–179.
  • 13.Whyte S., Torgler B., & Harrison K. L. (2016). What women want in their sperm donor: A study of more than 1000 women’s sperm donor selections. Economics & Human Biology, 23, 1–9. 10.1016/j.ehb.2016.06.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Whyte S., Chan H. F., & Torgler B. (2018). Do men and women know what they want? Sex differences in online daters’ educational preferences. Psychological science, 29(8), 1370–1375. 10.1177/0956797618771081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Buss D. M. (1985). Human mate selection: Opposites are sometimes said to attract, but in fact we are likely to marry someone who is similar to us in almost every variable. American scientist, 73(1), 47–51. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Confer J. C., Perilloux C., & Buss D. M. (2010). More than just a pretty face: Men’s priority shifts toward bodily attractiveness in short-term versus long-term mating contexts. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 348–353. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Perilloux C., Cloud J. M., & Buss D. M. (2013). Women’s physical attractiveness and short-term mating strategies. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(4), 490–495. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Thornhill R., & Gangestad S. W. (2006). Facial sexual dimorphism, developmental stability, and susceptibility to disease in men and women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(2), 131–144. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Foo Y. Z., Simmons L. W., & Rhodes G. (2017). Predictors of facial attractiveness and health in humans. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–12. 10.1038/s41598-016-0028-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cai Z., Hahn A. C., Zhang W., Holzleitner I. J., Lee A. J., DeBruine L. M., et al. (2019). No evidence that facial attractiveness, femininity, averageness, or coloration are cues to susceptibility to infectious illnesses in a university sample of young adult women. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(2), 156–159. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Thomas A. G., Jonason P. K., Blackburn J. D., Kennair L. E. O., Lowe R., Malouff J., et al. (2020). Mate preference priorities in the East and West: A cross‐cultural test of the mate preference priority model. Journal of Personality, 88(3), 606–620. 10.1111/jopy.12514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hamermesh D. S., & Biddle J. E. (1993). Beauty and the labor market (No. w4518). National Bureau of Economic Research. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hamermesh D. S., & Parker A. (2005). Beauty in the classroom: Instructors’ pulchritude and putative pedagogical productivity. Economics of Education Review, 24(4), 369–376. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pfann G. A., Biddle J. E., Hamermesh D. S., & Bosman C. M. (2000). Business success and businesses’ beauty capital. Economics Letters, 67(2), 201–207. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Jones B. C., Little A. C., Penton-Voak I. S., Tiddeman B. P., Burt D. M., & Perrett D. I. (2001). Facial symmetry and judgements of apparent health: Support for a “good genes” explanation of the attractiveness–symmetry relationship. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(6), 417–429. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cano F., Simón C., Remohí J., & Pellicer A. (1995). Effect of aging on the female reproductive system: evidence for a role of uterine senescence in the decline in female fecundity. Fertility and Sterility, 64(3), 584–589. 10.1016/s0015-0282(16)57797-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Virtala A., Vilska S., Huttunen T., & Kunttu K. (2011). Childbearing, the desire to have children, and awareness about the impact of age on female fertility among Finnish university students. The European Journal of Contraception & Reproductive Health Care, 16(2), 108–115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kidd S. A., Eskenazi B., & Wyrobek A. J. (2001). Effects of male age on semen quality and fertility: a review of the literature. Fertility and Sterility, 75(2), 237–248. 10.1016/s0015-0282(00)01679-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kenrick D. T., & Keefe R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(1), 75–91. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Australian Bureau of Statistics (2014). Australian Social Trends; [cited 24 November 2019]; [about 5 screens] available from: http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/abs@.nsf/2f762f95845417aeca25706c00834efa/c5d8171a65f09e5aca2570ec000e4b09!OpenDocument
  • 31.Australian Bureau of Statistics 2019. Average Weekly Earnings, Australia, May 2019; [cited 24 November 2019]; [about 2 screens] available from: https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@.nsf/mf/6302.0
  • 32.Blau F. D., & Kahn L. M. (1992). The gender earnings gap: learning from international comparisons. The American Economic Review, 82(2), 533–538. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Whyte S., & Torgler B. (2017). Things change with age: Educational assortment in online dating. Personality and Individual Differences, 109, 5–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rushton J. P., Russell R. J., & Wells P. A. (1985). Personality and genetic similarity theory. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 8(1), 63–86. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lu H. J., Zhu X. Q., & Chang L. (2015). Good genes, good providers, and good fathers: Economic development involved in how women select a mate. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 9(4), 215. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Whyte S., Brooks R. C., Chan H. F., & Torgler B. (2019). Do certain personality traits provide a mating market competitive advantage? Sex, offspring & the big 5. Personality and Individual Differences, 139, 158–169. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kenrick D. T. (1989). Bridging social psychology and sociobiology: The case of sexual attraction. In Bell R. W. & Bell N. J. (Eds.), Sociobiology and the social sciences (p. 5–23). Texas Tech University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Shackelford T. K., Schmitt D. P., & Buss D. M. (2005). Universal dimensions of human mate preferences. Personality and individual differences, 39(2), 447–458. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Botwin M. D., Buss D. M., & Shackelford T. K. (1997). Personality and mate preferences: Five factors in mate selection and marital satisfaction. Journal of personality, 65(1), 107–136. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00531.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fishbane M. D. (2007). Wired to connect: Neuroscience, relationships, and therapy. Family Process, 46(3), 395–412. 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2007.00219.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Alford J. R., Hatemi P. K., Hibbing J. R., Martin N. G., & Eaves L. J. (2011). The politics of mate choice. Journal of Politics, 73(2), 362–379. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Blake K., Godwin M., & Whyte S. (2020). “I sexually identify as an Attack Helicopter”: Incels, trolls, and non-binary gender politics online. First Monday. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Chan HF, Torgler B. & Whyte S. (2021) Exploring sexual orientation beyond genital arousal: Using large-scale online dating contact behavior to study male & female bisexuality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (PNAS), 118(12), e2026320118. 10.1073/pnas.2026320118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Whyte S., Brooks R. C., & Torgler B. (2018). Man, woman,“other”: Factors associated with nonbinary gender identification. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(8), 2397–2406. 10.1007/s10508-018-1307-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Whyte S., Brooks R. C., & Torgler B. (2019). Sexual economic theory & the human mating market. Applied Economics, 51(57), 6100–6112. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Inman H. F., & Bradley E. L. Jr (1989). The overlapping coefficient as a measure of agreement between probability distributions and point estimation of the overlap of two normal densities. Communications in Statistics-Theory and Methods, 18(10), 3851–3874. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Pastore M. (2018). Overlapping: a R package for estimating overlapping in empirical distributions. Journal of Open Source Software, 3(32), 1023. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lassek W. D. & Gaulin S. J. C. (2019). Evidence Supporting Nubility and Reproductive Value as the Key to Human Female Physical Attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40, 408–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Hopcroft R. L. (2006). Sex, Status, and Reproductive Success in the Contemporary United States. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27, 104–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Nettle D. & Pollet T. V. (2008). Natural Selection on Male Wealth in Humans. American Naturalist, 172, 658–66. 10.1086/591690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Whyte S., & Torgler B. (2017). Preference versus choice in online dating. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20(3), 150–156. 10.1089/cyber.2016.0528 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Alex Jones

21 Jan 2020

PONE-D-19-32550

The importance of sexiness: The impact of biological and socio-economic characteristics on human sexual attraction

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr Whyte,

Thank you for submitting your research to PLOS ONE. I have now received the opinion of two expert reviewers in the field who have considered the manuscript. Given the differences in their judgments I had hoped to secure another reviewer, but given that others had not responded I opted to provide a review for the manuscript myself. After doing this, I felt a major revision is needed, which is likely to involve significant edits and re-analysis.

As you will see, the reviewers felt that while the manuscript could offer a lot to the field - particularly because of its large and diverse sample size - there were two issues that stood out and prevented them from recommending minor revisions. One of these issues concerned the scope of the background literature and how the hypotheses were framed and the data interpreted. The second issue was with the statistical analysis of data, and the the claims that were made about these analyses that were not supported. On these issues I agree with the reviewers.

Reviewer 1 points out that the claims of an interaction analysis are not supported, as at no point is an interaction analysis clearly tested. Reviewer 2 points out the exaggeration of certain aspects of theory and glossing over of other aspects (such as strong evidence humans exhibit mutual mate choice, as opposed to great asymmetries). Addressing these kinds of points are vital for a successful revision.

I have provided my own review below this letter and hope you will find it useful. I am sorry to bear this news, and realise these edits are asking for a very different manuscript. But without significant and major revisions to the analysis and presentation of the work, the utility of a diverse and large sample of data is lost.

We would appreciate receiving your revised manuscript by Mar 05 2020 11:59PM. When you are ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter.

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). This letter should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. This file should be uploaded as separate file and labeled 'Manuscript'.

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alex Jones

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

My own concerns are as follows:

The claim that females remain unaffected by mating temporality is overstated. Females tend to prefer more masculine men facially for short term relationships (Jones et al, 2019; Psychological Science), and women seem to choose more muscular men for short term mating (Frederick & Haselton, 2007; Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin). Reviewer 2 points out more of these kinds of overstatements, and this needs to be drastically changed.

There seem to be a lot of confusion around statistical terms and practice. For example, on page 12, line 7, you describe a set of multivariate analyses. Multivariate analysis involves a set of multiple Y and single-to-multiple X (in the context of regression) variables; whereas the data described here seems to simply reflect a large number of models, split by sex of participant, for age, attractiveness, build, etc (and the other kinds of ‘grouped’ variables). This not a multivariate analysis - though you certainly have scope to do so - and so should not be described as such. The notes on the tables also claim these regressions are ‘robust OLS regressions’. It is not mentioned anywhere that these regressions are robust regression in the statistical sense of the word - indeed, robust regression penalises outliers through various methods and is therefore not ordinary least squares. Reviewer 2 also asks whether any kind of multiple correction was done, and this should be reported if done - or carried out if so. Finally, the use of the Epanechnikov kernel function as a way to test relative preferences of a given trait seems too complex for what can be approximated with linear models - if there is a specific reason for using this method it should be stated and expanded upon in the methods section before it is introduced. The difference between female and male groupings at different age categories was also confusing.

In addition to addressing the queries of both reviewers, here is an analytical strategy that I think would make sense. The authors are free to reject this idea, but it is important to have a clear and concise analysis that allows readers to interpret what is going on in the data entirely, and without the overwhelming set of numbers in the manuscript as-is.

First, I suggest computing a principal components analysis or factor analysis of the ratings of the nine traits for the entire dataset (grouped here by Aesthetic, Resource and Personality). The labels assigned to these are fair groupings, but it is very unlikely that they will not be correlated in some fashion with one another - a simple idea would be age and income, or openness and intelligence, for example). A rigorous PCA or FA would boil this multidimensional data-space into something more coherent. You can then in a more data driven way provide a set of groupings by checking how each of the individual traits correlate with the new factors. An ideal setting perhaps would be finding 3 factors, wherein age, attractiveness, and physical build correlate strongly with the first factor, education, income, and intelligence with the second but not the first and so on - this may or may not occur, but the correlations are important.

Second, it is then possible to take the characteristics of your participants as you did in the later analyses (reported in the tables) and, in three models if you prefer, regress these demographics against the factors.

If you find, say, a factor that captures attractiveness and build, perhaps you will find that the sex of the participant (now entered as a predictor) will associate more strongly with this factor - i.e. men care more about this constellation of traits represented by the factor than women do. You can also test your interactions properly here in a correctly specified linear model, by allowing say age and sex to interact - do older men care less about this factor than women? This can be properly tested as at the moment the number of models is too much and they are isolated.

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and http://www.journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please modify the title to ensure that it is meeting PLOS’ guidelines (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title). In particular, the title should be "specific, descriptive, concise, and comprehensible to readers outside the field" and in this case it is not informative and specific about your study's scope and methodology.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for inviting me to review this paper. The study is a large sample's report on the perceptions of attractive features in others. The topic is interesting and it is good to see large sample science.

My comments by paper section

Abstract

I found the wording of the results in the introduction a little confusing. In particular, it is not made clear how an interaction analysis can reveal a sex difference effect (“our interaction analysis across age pinpoints a stronger male preference for attractiveness and physical build but a stronger female preference for intelligence, trust, and emotional connection.” This latter part appears to be the interaction: “Though of these sex differences show large variation or change at different life stages for both sexes”

Introduction

It would be good to cite more work critical of the good genes hypothesis. Research on health and behavioural outcomes of attractive physiology since Trivers (1972) and Scheib et al (1999) suggest that this is not as simple as is suggested in the opening background paragraph. Some of the key findings in good genes do not replicate (Thornhill & Gangestad, 2006; Foo, 2017). Most recently, Cai et al (https://psyarxiv.com/hnbv7/) show this with health outcomes related to attractiveness, averageness, femininity and coloration. Whilst I would not expect a comprehensive review to be added (I am aware of space limitations), it would be good to see some caveats in the largely uncritical background.

It would be useful and transparent to end the introduction with operationalised hypotheses. This would also add clarification as to why the selected ‘personality traits’ are used. (I appreciate some of this appears in the method section but given the theory-informed nature of this study hypotheses and predictions would be expected).

It would useful to have a summary of the evidence of the relationship between self-reported attractiveness preferences and tested influence of attractive features. I am not familiar with evidence that the relationship between self-reported importance of, say intelligence, for attractiveness and the contribution that intelligence has in overall perceptions of attractiveness on meeting new people. This content would strengthen the justification for the paper’s core methodology.

Method

“Each question thus asks respondents to rank the level of importance they assign to each characteristic in relation to sexual attraction on a sliding scale from 1 to 100.” -> does this mean that the concrete activity was to produce a hierarchy (ordinal) data or was each attributed presented and intended to be evaluated in isolation?

Results

Table 1 should include some effect size metrics, especially given the sample size. Perhaps even include metrics such as the overlap coefficient (see Inman and Bradley, 1989). For example, the largest difference between sexes on Physical build/features t=19.32 has an 81% overlap in distribution of responses which is informative for a reader for understanding what a difference between the sexes mean in a concrete sense. The transparency of the table could be improved by clarifying ‘upper’ and ‘lower’ headings.

The explanation of figure 2 invites the reader to draw inference from their perceived difference in the difference in effect of linear prediction across age to understand sex differences in the sample. I would strongly advise against this. This would encourage the testing of an age*sex interaction to demonstrate this effect empirically.

As far as I can tell, other than the mentions in the abstract and discussion there is no empirical evaluation of the interaction between sex and age in this study. Tables 2, 3 and 4 test the effects of age within the sexes but not across (using an interaction, Fisher’s z’ or Steiger’s T/z tests) the sexes. i.e. Tables 2, 3 and 4 column 1 conducts analysis on only the 4106 male participants and then column 2 conducts analysis on only the 2519 female participants. Therefore, there are concerns about making comments on the observed difference in the relationships displayed by the sexes. The authors refer to interaction analysis

I strongly recommend that the authors consider including sex*age interaction effects on the traits or they report tests of the size of different effects (i.e. Steiger’s T).

The authors do refer to interactions throughout but no interaction tests appear in this version of the manuscript.

Discussion

The authors conclude “More specifically, our interaction analysis of sex differences in preferences across age…” however I am not able to find any interaction tests in the current paper.

The authors note at the end of their discussion “Admittedly, however, in cyber mating market settings, stated preferences are not always a good indicator of actual behaviour”. This should have much more prominence in the discussion (and perhaps introduction) as it caveats the implications of the research significantly.

Overall this paper adds a general description of sex differences in self-reported points of attraction to the literature. There needs to be inferential tests of the sex*age interaction and the paper’s final caveat should be much more prominent as the study is reliant on the honesty of those engaging with “cyber mating markets”

Reviewer #2: This review uses a large dataset from Australia to examine sex differences in mating preferences for traits related to physical attractiveness, the ability to provide resources, and three different facets of personality important for pair bonding.

There are numerous merits to the article. It uses models which account for linear and curvilinear relationships with age, which gives an indication of lifetime trajectories of mating preferences (and their associated sex differences). This is particularly relevant given that a considerable amount of mate preference research is conducted on young adults. Should sex differences in preferences changes across the life span, then conclusions drawn from young adults alone may exaggerate or understate sex differences.

The methodology of the paper and the chosen forms of analysis are sound. And the results could make a decent theoretical contribution to the area. However, the paper has some weaknesses that make it unsuitable for publication in its current form.

The main weakness is that it presents many data, but does very little in the way of actually interpreting the results. That is, it is too descriptive, and the precise merits of looking at the changes in mate preferences across age is missing from the narrative. A second weakness is the lack of specific details surrounding the choice of the statistics and presentation methods used, which makes the benefits of this approach relative to others hard to decipher. I recommend that the article be revised and resubmitted, with substantially expanded results and discussion sections, and a focus on the novel contributions to knowledge this analysis provides.

Below, I have given some more specific recommendations that I hope may improve the paper. Those which I consider to be major issues, I have marked with an asterisks.

Abstract: I find it odd that t-tests are mentioned here rather than the OLS regression and kernel-smoothing plots, which I think give a much more nuanced interpretation of the data.

Page 3, line 102. It is worth considering that the good genes hypothesis isn’t the only explanation for the preference for physical attractiveness. In fact the sexy sons hypothesis (or should we say, sexy offspring, in paid bonded species) appears to be a stronger force for selection. See a recent meta-analysis of 55 species by Prokop et al. (2012).

*Page 4, line 117. The preceding paragraph polarises the sex differences between the sexes by applying the logic of MCFC (Males compete, females choose) species to humans which have mutual mate choice. There is a large sex differences in obligate parental investment in humans, but sex differences in *typical* parental investment is much smaller, leading to similar choosiness in the long-term mating domain of humans. Humans are a MMC (mutual mate choice) species, and sex differences are relative, rather than absolute. Furthermore, polarising sex differences can be detrimental to scientific communication and pedagogy (see Stewart-Williams and Thomas, 2013). I urge the authors to acknowledge the reduced sex differences in pair-bonded species like humans by tempering their narrative here.

Page 4, line 120-3. As above, physical attractiveness is *relatively* more important for men, but is still absolutely important for both sexes. And this effect appears to be present in different cultures, even when mate preferences are constrained: see Thomas et al., (2019) for a recent study looking at samples from the UK, Norway, Australia, Singapore, and Malaysia.

Page 4, line 133-4. What does this study add that is not already known? This question has been examined countless times since the Second World War, with an additional focus on cross-cultural differences since the late 1980’s. There is a unique contribution here that I think is important, but the authors are expecting the reader to go out of their way to figure out what it is.

Page 5, line 140. The physical health = attractiveness connection is not as clear as initially thought (for a meta-analysis see Weeden & Sabini, 2005). More likely, men’s increase preference for physical attractiveness relates to its association with youth and, therefore, fecundity.

Page 6, lines 169-170. Are these economic factors the only driving force behind these preferences? It seems odd that the previous section relies on evolutionary arguments, but this one considers the driver for resource provision as the consequence of modern day market forces – especially seeing as provision for neonatal offspring is one of the most clear-cut selection pressures faced by humans throughout our history.

Page 6, lines 179-181. Is this really the case? Thomas et al., (2019), for example, found that culturally disparate groups, with different views on sex discrimination and egalitarianism, showed remarkable consistency in their preference for one of the personality traits key in successful pair bonding: kindness.

*Page 7, line 195. The fact that relationship context is not given, and that relationship preferences vary across short- and long-term domains (see Stewart-Williams et al., 2017; Thomas and Stewart-Williams, 2018; and Thomas, 2018) is problematic. One assumes that the participants are answering in line with their sociosexuality so that those who want short-term mates answer in one context, and those who want long-term mates answer in the others. This needs to be addressed in the discussion.

Page 8, line 220. What is the reason for men to outnumber women 1.7 to 1? Seems unusual for a national survey.

Table 1. Were any corrections made for multiple comparisons?

Page 9, lines 242-7. More clarity is needed regarding the selection of the Epanechinikov kernel function.

Figure 2. The notes should include which group the red reference line refers to.

Page 12, lines 7+. The multivariate analysis section. This whole section talks about which betas are significant and in what direction, but has very little in the way of interpretation of the results. It may be that the authors prefer to do this in the discussion section, but it is absent from there as well. The three tables present many data, and a more nuanced discussion of directions of findings, strengths of findings, and within category consistency and deviation is appropriate. There is also very little discussion of the age effects, and what implications a curvilinear pattern has for how we view sex differences.

References

Prokop, Z. M., Michalczyk, Ł., Drobniak, S. M., Herdegen, M., & Radwan, J. (2012). Meta‐analysis suggests choosy females get sexy sons more than “good genes”. Evolution: International Journal of Organic Evolution, 66(9), 2665-2673.

Stewart-Williams, S., & Thomas, A. G. (2013). The ape that thought it was a peacock: Does evolutionary psychology exaggerate human sex differences?. Psychological Inquiry, 24(3), 137-168.

Stewart-Williams, S., Butler, C. A., & Thomas, A. G. (2017). Sexual history and present attractiveness: People want a mate with a bit of a past, but not too much. The Journal of Sex Research, 54(9), 1097-1105.

Thomas, A. G., & Stewart-Williams, S. (2018). Mating strategy flexibility in the laboratory: Preferences for long-and short-term mating change in response to evolutionarily relevant variables. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 82-93.

Thomas, A. G. (2018). Lowering partner standards in a short‐term mating context. In T. K. Shackelford, & V. A. Weekes‐Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science (pp. 1– 3). Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing.

Thomas, A. G., Jonason, P. K., Blackburn, J. D., Kennair, L. E. O., Lowe, R., Malouff, J., ... & Li, N. P. (2019). Mate preference priorities in the East and West: A cross‐cultural test of the mate preference priority model. Journal of personality.

Weeden, J., & Sabini, J. (2005). Physical attractiveness and health in Western societies: a review. Psychological bulletin, 131(5), 635.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files to be viewed.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email us at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 May 19;16(5):e0250151. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250151.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


6 Oct 2020

PONE-D-19-32550

The importance of sexiness: The impact of biological and socio-economic characteristics on human sexual attraction

Dear Dr Jones,

We would like to sincerely thank yourself as editor and both reviewers for such a comprehensive and informative review. As you note in your review the large and diverse sample size offers the opportunity for a unique and significant contribution to the mate choice literature. That said we also acknowledge your comments requesting major revisions.

We have addressed all of the comments and incorporated all of the requested changes and edits, by completing a major re-write of the study. We have incorporated the majority of the new literature and theory suggested by the reviewers into the re-write. We have also conducted more and completely new analysis based on the reviewers and your feedback.

In line with both of the reviewers comments we have completely revised the entire analysis section (Reviewer 1), and slightly re-aligned the scope of the manuscript to provide a more nuanced paper on both absolute and relative sex differences across age (Reviewer 2), with rewrites to the abstract, introduction, background and discussion.

We appreciate that because of such significant changes to the manuscript the review process will again be substantial by yourself and the reviewers, and we would like to express our gratitude for the opportunity to resubmit to PLoS One.

Thank you,

Dr Stephen Whyte

Attachment

Submitted filename: Respons_to_reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Alex Jones

8 Dec 2020

PONE-D-19-32550R1

The importance of sexiness: Relative and absolute sex differences in human sexual attraction at different life stages

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Whyte,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 22 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Alex Jones

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (if provided):

Dear Dr Whyte,

Thank you for submitting your revised manuscript, and for the care you have taken in your responses to the reviewers comments as well as my own suggestions. I have now had a chance to review your manuscript and obtain further commentary from the original reviewers.

While one reviewer recommended acceptance, the other suggested another round of major revisions. My own leaning is between these two perspectives. I find the manuscript much improved, but I agree with Reviewer 1 that the theoretical contribution of the work has not been clearly highlighted, and that there are some inconsistencies throughout the paper in terms of analyses missing from the supplementary information, as well as some grammatical errors throughout. I also agree with Reviewer 2 that the interpretation of the effects, especially with such a large sample size, are often speculative. I would urge the authors to express strong caution in their interpretation of these data, where statistical significance may well be meaningless.

Finally, I would also agree with Reviewer 1 that the authors should make every effort possible to make these data openly available, as well as responding to their comments in general.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear authors,

I am pleased to re-review this paper. The revised text is a comprehensive, nuanced and complete take on the dataset. I particularly appreciate that the authors have welcomed the use of overlap coefficients and formalised tests of the interactions. I think the paper tells a very interesting story and have spent quite some time reading the results with interest. The clarity of the figures and the comprehensiveness of the tables should be applauded. This is large sample research at it's best.

Perhaps there is an overemphasis on statistical significance values. With N>7000 inference from p values by traditional liberal criteria leads to some low thresholds of notable effects (I did note the identification of significance of p<.10 at some points - which in N>7000 is a very small effect). However, the discussion and interpretation of these results are clear and I consider this report to be in line with current standards.

The authors should be proud of this work and I wish them good luck in future similar research.

Reviewer #2: I'm afraid that despite a substantial rewrite, the authors have done very little to address the concerns I raised in my original review. I still do not believe this manuscript is suitable for publication in its current form for the reasons outlined below. I still see merit in this manuscript, and think it has the potential to make a theoretical contribution but only after a substantial revision. My recommendation to the editor is that the authors revise and resubmit this article.

Major points:

#1 The theoretical contribution of this paper is still unclear. Sex differences in the variables under consideration have been studied for decades and this research is now becoming more and more nuanced. Where does this paper fit in with the current state of the literature? A large sample isn't enough. My observation is that the change in preferences across age is the novel aspect, but this is not expressed or highlighted in the manuscript at all. For example, the literature behind sex differences in personality makes no prediction for how this pattern might change with age. The narrative needs dramatic reframing and focus. This is still an overwhelmingly descriptive manuscript with very little theoretical focus or integration.

#2 The opening paragraph makes several claims about decision making in the mating domain yet is barren of literature.

#3 There are no formal predictions made in the article (or its exploratory nature is not made clear).

#4 There is no formal start to the results section

#5 The descriptives section is incredibly long. It's broken down into fine detail for no theoretical reason. What does all of this "add" from a theoretical perspective? What is the advantage of this approach over a simple table with averages, measures of spread, and effect sizes?

#6 The table (A4) which contains the coefficients for the key regression model - the model which examines changes with age, and is arguably the most important part of the article - is missing. There are other cases of lack of attention to detail throughout the article.

#7 The discussion makes no attempt to integrate the findings with the literature, it simply provides a summary of the key patterns observed. Often times, the article reads as if the examination of sex differences among these traits in and of itself is a novel contribution to the evolutionary literature. That is not the case. I suspect that this assumption may have something to do with the literature covered which is, for the most part, quite dated.

Minor points:

#8 I recommend standardized the use of effect sizes throughout (Cohen's d or r) for easier comparison between works on sex differences.

#9 The authors indicate that the data for this article is not readily accessible as per the Plos ONE guidelines. They infer that if someone wishes to access the data they need to seek permission from a third party (their institution). The authors should seek this permission and then upload the data to a repository such as OSF, rather than ask potentially dozens of researchers to embark on this battle themselves.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes: Dr Andrew G. Thomas

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 May 19;16(5):e0250151. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0250151.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


30 Mar 2021

31 March 2021

2 George St,

Brisbane, Queensland

Australia

RE: PONE-D-19-32550-R2 - The importance of sexiness: Relative and absolute sex differences in sexual attraction at different life stages

Dear Dr Jones,

We again thank you and both reviewers for helpful feedback on our revised manuscript. We have now revised the manuscript in line with recommendations from the reviewers and the editor’s comments, all of which helped clarifying the important perspectives of both reviewers. Below you will find our point-by-point response to the comments. Beyond that we have decided to change the title of the manuscript to: Sex Differences in Sexual Attraction for Aesthetics, Resources and Personality Across Age. We believe that the new title is a better reflection of the content of the paper. Moreover, we would like to note that the data are now openly available on OSF (see OSF | Sex Differences in Sexual Attraction for Aesthetics, Resources and Personality Across Age).

We hope the revision will meet your expectations and facilitate the decision to publish our paper in PLoS One.

Reviewer #1: Dear authors,

I am pleased to re-review this paper. The revised text is a comprehensive, nuanced and complete take on the dataset. I particularly appreciate that the authors have welcomed the use of overlap coefficients and formalised tests of the interactions. I think the paper tells a very interesting story and have spent quite some time reading the results with interest. The clarity of the figures and the comprehensiveness of the tables should be applauded. This is large sample research at it's best.

Perhaps there is an overemphasis on statistical significance values. With N>7000 inference from p values by traditional liberal criteria leads to some low thresholds of notable effects (I did note the identification of significance of p<.10 at some points - which in N>7000 is a very small effect). However, the discussion and interpretation of these results are clear and I consider this report to be in line with current standards.

The authors should be proud of this work and I wish them good luck in future similar research.

We thank Reviewer 1 for their helpful comments and feedback throughout the review process. While we note the reviewer’s comments that “interpretation of these results are clear and I consider this report to be in line with current standards” we also provide extensive clarification regarding any possible over-emphasis on notable effects:

While the current study analyses and reports the sexual attraction preference for an extremely large population of Australian online dating participants (n=7325), the authors caution over-emphasis on statistically significant results stemming from such a large sample size. Any and all descriptive analysis in the current study was reported wtihin the study so as to provide scientific transparency, and in accordance with the current standards across the evolutionary behavioural sciences.

Reviewer #2: I'm afraid that despite a substantial rewrite, the authors have done very little to address the concerns I raised in my original review. I still do not believe this manuscript is suitable for publication in its current form for the reasons outlined below. I still see merit in this manuscript, and think it has the potential to make a theoretical contribution but only after a substantial revision. My recommendation to the editor is that the authors revise and resubmit this article.

Major points:

#1 The theoretical contribution of this paper is still unclear. Sex differences in the variables under consideration have been studied for decades and this research is now becoming more and more nuanced. Where does this paper fit in with the current state of the literature? A large sample isn't enough. My observation is that the change in preferences across age is the novel aspect, but this is not expressed or highlighted in the manuscript at all. For example, the literature behind sex differences in personality makes no prediction for how this pattern might change with age. The narrative needs dramatic reframing and focus. This is still an overwhelmingly descriptive manuscript with very little theoretical focus or integration.

We agree with the reviewer that the novel aspect of the studies is the exploration of how preferences change across age. We have now restructured the paper accordingly (narrative, reframing, and focus). This means that we have placed some previous results in the main text in the Appendix and we have extended the empirical approach by adding a new figure with various contour plots showing interaction effects between the importance of age for sexual attraction and age to understand sex differences in attractiveness as well as the importance of age and attractiveness across age in regards to sex differences for the importance of intelligence. Those visualizations provide additional insights on preference changes across age – beyond what was previously achieved – and offers a better understanding of non-linear relationships. We agree with the reviewer’s statement that the literature behind sex differences in personality makes no prediction for how preferences change with age. Our comparative empirical advantage is the analysis of a dataset with a larger age distribution as (most) previous studies often have an age distribution skewed towards the younger population. We therefore believe that an empirically oriented study like the one conducted here can hopefully guide future theoretical and empirical studies; taking into account that science can be seen as a constant interaction between speaking to theorists and searching for facts and moving between phases of interpretation and the more descriptive and explorative summarization.

#2 The opening paragraph makes several claims about decision making in the mating domain yet is barren of literature.

We have re-written the introduction, re-formatted it, and cited extra literature to provide greater clarity for the reader.

#3 There are no formal predictions made in the article (or its exploratory nature is not made clear).

We have now made the contribution clearer (see previous response) and refer in more detail to the explorative and empirical orientation of the study.

#4 There is no formal start to the results section

We have re-formatted and titled the “Results” section.

#5 The descriptives section is incredibly long. It's broken down into fine detail for no theoretical reason. What does all of this "add" from a theoretical perspective? What is the advantage of this approach over a simple table with averages, measures of spread, and effect sizes?

The descriptive statistics section has been removed and replaced instead with a simple table of descriptive statistics as per the reviewer’s suggestion. We now also place the table in the Appendix.

#6 The table (A4) which contains the coefficients for the key regression model - the model which examines changes with age, and is arguably the most important part of the article - is missing. There are other cases of lack of attention to detail throughout the article.

We agree with this evaluation, but we believe that Figure 3 is more appealing to the readers for understanding what is happening in the regression results. Thus, we retain the full regression results in the Appendix. We have also checked the entire manuscript for lack of attention to details including also a careful checking and correcting of grammatical, labelling, or formatting errors.

#7 The discussion makes no attempt to integrate the findings with the literature, it simply provides a summary of the key patterns observed. Often times, the article reads as if the examination of sex differences among these traits in and of itself is a novel contribution to the evolutionary literature. That is not the case. I suspect that this assumption may have something to do with the literature covered which is, for the most part, quite dated.

We believe that the reframing towards preferences change over age allows to emphasize the innovative nature of the contribution. The discussion section not only summarises what we can learn from the empirical results but also tries to link the results back to the previous evolutionary mate choice literature and sex difference literature (e.g., mutual mate choice (see Stewart-Williams & Thomas 2013), or the gender similarities hypothesis (see Hyde 2005), and also referencing more recent studies (see, e.g., Lassek and Gaulin 2019). The revised discussion now provides more clarity on where the key findings sit within the literature and how it informs the different views in the broader evolutionary mate choice literature.

Minor points:

#8 I recommend standardized the use of effect sizes throughout (Cohen's d or r) for easier comparison between works on sex differences.

We have now reported the Cohen’s d (relative importance) and Cliff’s delta (absolute importance; non-parametric) for the sex differences in Table 2.

#9 The authors indicate that the data for this article is not readily accessible as per the Plos ONE guidelines. They infer that if someone wishes to access the data they need to seek permission from a third party (their institution). The authors should seek this permission and then upload the data to a repository such as OSF, rather than ask potentially dozens of researchers to embark on this battle themselves.

This is a very good point. We now provide full access to the data via OSF | Sex Differences in Sexual Attraction for Aesthetics, Resources and Personality Across Age

Decision Letter 2

Alex Jones

1 Apr 2021

Sex Differences in Sexual Attraction for Aesthetics, Resources and Personality Across Age.

PONE-D-19-32550R2

Dear Dr. Whyte,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Alex Jones

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Alex Jones

16 Apr 2021

PONE-D-19-32550R2

Sex Differences in Sexual Attraction for Aesthetics, Resources and Personality Across Age

Dear Dr. Whyte:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Alex Jones

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Appendix

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Respons_to_reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    Data and codes used in this study can be found on the Open Science Framework (DOI: 10.17605/OSF.IO/DSJ9W).


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES