Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 May 21;16(5):e0252028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252028

Gender differences in suicide-related communication of young suicide victims

Elias Balt 1,*, Saskia Mérelle 1, Diana van Bergen 1,2, Renske Gilissen 1, Pommeline van der Post 1, Milou Looijmans 1, Daan Creemers 3, Sanne Rasing 3,4, Wico Mulder 5, Lieke van Domburgh 6,7, Arne Popma 7
Editor: Vincenzo De Luca8
PMCID: PMC8139476  PMID: 34019584

Abstract

Objectives

There is limited insight into gender differences in suicide-related communication (SRC) in youths. SRC is defined as “the act of conveying one’s own suicide ideation, intent or behaviours to another person”. Increasing our understanding of SRC in youths will enable us to recognize and specify needs of female versus male youths. The current study explores SRC in a sample of Dutch suicide victims aged under 20 and examines gender differences.

Methods

Interview data from a psychological autopsy study of 35 youths who died by suicide in the Netherlands in 2017 were analysed. Qualitative analyses were performed to examine explicit SRC throughout the youths’ lives and implicit SRC during the last months prior to suicide. We employed the Constant Comparative Method to explore patterns in the debut, form, frequency, medium, content, type of recipient, and SRC in the last months prior to suicide death.

Results

We identified commonalities in the SRC of youths, including the content of suicide notes and an emphasis on suicide method and preparation in the last months. Girls, however, had an earlier debut of SRC, a higher frequency of explicit SRC, and more often directed SRC towards varied types of recipients compared to boys. Moreover, SRC of girls seemed focused on coping and achieving support from others more than SRC of boys. The SRC of boys in comparison to girls was often ambiguous or diluted by “humorous” connotations.

Conclusion

Unique patterns in SRC of boys and girls posed corresponding challenges for next of kin to interpret communications and respond adequately to SRC. The early debut of girls’ SRC highlights the importance of early screening and prevention efforts in girls, while the late debut and ambiguity in boys’ SRC implores professionals and next of kin to encourage young males to be unequivocal about suicide ideation or intent.

Introduction

Suicide is a leading cause of death in young individuals in the Netherlands [1] and the third leading cause of death in young people aged 15–29 worldwide [2]. Suicidal behaviours (i.e. suicide ideation, deliberate self-harm, and suicide attempts) often debut between the ages of 12–17 [3, 4]. The suicidal process is the timeframe in which an individual moves from first ideation towards the act of an attempted or completed suicide. The timeframe from ideation to lethal suicide shows large individual variation. Previous research showed that it can be as short as two months in young individuals (aged 15–29) and in 28% of youths spanned three months or less [3]. Approximately one third of adolescents with suicide ideation attempt suicide [5]. Sometimes, the suicidal process can go unnoticed until death by suicide occurs [6].

In what is often referred to as the gender paradox [7], males tend to have 2–4 times higher suicide rates, whereas suicide attempts are 3–9 times more common in females [4, 8]. In the Netherlands, 1.85 per 100,000 boys versus 1.23 per 100,000 girls aged under 20 died by suicide between 2015 and 2019 [9]. Gender differences in the suicidal process manifest during early adolescence [10, 11]. Kaess and colleagues [11] determined lifetime suicide ideation in 19.8% of girls in a school-based sample in Germany, and 10.8% had engaged in a suicide attempt. This was 9.3% and 4.9%, respectively, in males. The suicidal process may flow differently between boys and girls. Ideation peaks in mid adolescence in girls, whereas in boys a slow increase through late adolescence is seen [12]. Voss and colleagues [13] studied the onset of suicide attempts in a youth sample aged 21 and below. They reported an earlier onset of nonfatal attempts among boys but found that the cumulative incidence of nonfatal attempts was higher in girls from the age of 14 on.

While gender differences in the prevalence rates of suicide ideation, suicidal behaviour, and suicide death have been clearly established, the current literature offers limited insight into gender differences in the communication of suicidal thoughts throughout the lives of young suicide victims. This is a missed opportunity as enhancing our understanding of gender patterns in suicide-related communication can improve our recognition of the needs of suicidal boys and girls, which in turn is valuable for optimizing suicide prevention efforts. Recent findings from the reviews of Morken and colleagues [14] and Cha and colleagues [4] indicate that early intervention, such as school-based interventions, may prevent suicidal ideation and attempts in the short-term, and suicide attempts in the long term in youths and adolescents. The recognition of the youths’ needs through their communication can guide strategies for early intervention. Therefore, this paper has suicide-related communication in relation to gender as its focal point.

In this article, we define personal suicide-related communication (SRC) as “the act of conveying one’s own suicide ideation, intent, or behaviours to another person” [15], in which we include both online and offline communication. SRC carries a unique signalling function. Crucially, it reflects a capacity to ask for help [16, 17]. Evidently, SRC is not always a form of help-seeking behaviour. For example, Frey and colleagues [15] suggest that a suicide-related notification aims to address practical considerations, such as planning a date or acquiring means, and suggests no clear intention to seek help. Frey also suggests that it is important to differentiate among different forms of SRC, such as disclosure, in which a subject intentionally communicates current or past suicidal thoughts and behaviour with the goal of seeking help or sharing their own personal history. Whether aimed at help-seeking or not, SRC appears to consistently indicate a desire to change a status quo unfavourable to one’s wellbeing.

There is a scarcity of research into SRC of youths. Talking constructively about suicide can avert the risk of suicide [18]. Obviously, this requires an in-depth understanding of the way young people talk about suicide in the context of their social environment. Previous research on SRC has predominantly addressed the proportion of communicators or frequency of communications in a sample, which does not fully appreciate its complexity [19]. Moreover, these studies have presented various proportions of communicators, i.e. individuals who engaged in SRC, in samples of deceased youths, ranging from 5 to 85 percent [2026]. Pompili and colleagues [19] suggest that the disagreement in findings may be largely explained by former studies’ definition of SRC and methodological quality.

According to Rhodes and colleagues [27], males may be less predisposed to help-seeking behaviours. Alternatively, suicide ideation in young males may not be signalled properly due to ineffective or limited SRC. Gender differences can be observed in the use of health services by young suicidal persons. A Quebecois study [28], where health records of almost 1250 adolescents aged under 25 who died by suicide were examined, showed that significantly more female decedents [82.5%] had utilized healthcare services in the year prior to death by suicide, compared to male decedents [74.9%]. Females who died by suicide were also significantly more likely to have a mental health diagnosis, regardless of the care setting.

Rytterström and colleagues [29] emphasize that to better estimate the risk of suicide in youths, we should explore “the emotions behind the façade” and try to understand the “internal experience of life”. Here lies a clear direction for rigorous qualitative research in order to obtain a deeper understanding of the what (content), how (form, medium) when (debut, frequency, last perceived event) and to whom (recipient) of SRC of young suicide victims. In the current study we conduct an in-depth exploration of SRC in the lives of young suicide victims in the Netherlands. This study aims to elucidate gender differences by comparing patterns in SRC of young males and females who died by suicide.

Materials and methods

Study design

Proxy informants of youths aged under 20 who lived in the Netherlands and died by suicide in 2017 were interviewed in a psychological autopsy study in 2019 [30]. Parents were the principal informants. Additional interviews were conducted with peers, teachers, and health care professionals. Respondents were contacted via their general practitioner. We obtained the contact data of the general practitioner from coroners who operate regionally and keep records of all suicide cases in the Netherlands. Secondary informants were recruited through the parents. The sampling strategy is further detailed by Mérelle and colleagues [30]. Interviews lasted approximately 2–3 hours and were conducted by an interviewer and a researcher (EB, ML). The interview team received a three-day training from a psychologist and an actor, aimed to get acquainted with the instrument and to master adaptive techniques to interview a vulnerable study population. A qualitative, explorative design was used to analyse interviews with next of kin (NOK). These were the parents and peers.

Materials

Interviews were semi-structured. The instrument was largely based on instruments from international psychological autopsies in Belgium, Ireland, Norway, and the United Kingdom [20, 3134] and consisted of two parts. NOK were invited in the first part to provide a narrative account of the youth’s life, their relationship, and their own perceptions about key factors contributing to the suicide. The second part consisted of sections covering five pre-identified topics: [1] adolescence, [2] healthcare, [3] social media use and contagion effects, [4] sexual orientation and gender identity, and [5] religion and ethnicity. The aforementioned topics were based on an extensive body of evidence on risk factors for suicide and obtained by consensus from the research group and an advisory committee for the current study. Seven questions and corresponding follow-up questions were dedicated to SRC (e.g. did the youth ever talk about dying, to you or anyone else?). The interview was piloted in four test cases to assess its application in practice and make final changes. The final instrument, including the questions regarding SRC, has been added as a supplement.

Analysis

For the qualitative analyses, interviews were recorded, transcribed verbatim and coded in several cycles using an ATLAS.ti (version 8.3) software package. In the first cycle, the main researcher (EB) coded the interviews. Initial coding was based on predetermined elements of SRC, as guided by the study’s first research question: “What are the debut, form, medium, frequency, content, recipient, and the last perceived events of suicide-related communications by youths who died by suicide? The variables as formulated in the research question were decided on a priori as relevant, operationalized into a coding sheet and integrated into ATLAS.TI. The second cycle was iterative. Additional codes were created, and existing codes were refined (e.g. “SRC medium: texting and messenger apps”). In the third cycle, a second researcher (PP) assessed the coded data, suggested changes, and marked conflicts. Lastly, proposed changes were presented to a third researcher (SM), and consensus was sought. The final coding sheet has been added as a supplement to this article and includes the current study’s working definitions of SRC.

While implicit communications, both verbal and nonverbal, can be crucial signals, the extent to which they reflect suicidal intent can be uncertain using psychological autopsy data. Therefore, we have included only explicit communications throughout the course of life. The disclosure of suicidal feelings, or acquiring means to end one’s life, for example, are an unequivocal reflection of suicide ideation. In contrast, only in the last months of the youths’ life, we have also analysed implicit SRC, which were verbal communications and notable (changes in) behaviours that NOK perceived, in retrospect or at the time, as signals of suicide ideation or intent, or impending suicide. This was guided by the assumption that suicide ideation would have manifested itself by that time in a majority of cases.

We adapted the Constant Comparative Method [35] to examine patterns in SRC of young males and females and identify gender differences. We first explored individual SRC and thereafter compared differences and similarities in SRC within and between gender groups. To explore individual SRC, the first author created a matrix of communication events (i.e. interview data describing the occurrence of SRC). This comprised a brief account of the content, form, and recipient(s) of individual SRC events. Additionally, the occurrence of SRC events throughout the youth’s life (e.g. debut, frequency, last recorded event) was determined. By detailing the collection of individual SRC events and creating an exhaustive account of the data, we aimed to reduce researcher bias [36]. Descriptive statistics were used to analyse the demographic characteristics of the sample and the number of recorded SRC events. Two-tailed t-tests were performed to examine gender differences in the occurrence of SRC events.

To subsequently compare SRC within gender groups, the first author amalgamated data into case-level data clusters. This entailed a summary of all recorded events of a case, which facilitated axial comparison. An example of a converged data cluster is illustrated below (Fig 1). Finally, we compared patterns in SRC between the male and female groups to answer the research question: What gender differences can be observed in the suicide-related communications of youths who died by suicide in the Netherlands?

Fig 1. Example case-level data cluster.

Fig 1

Ethical approval

The Medical Ethical Committee of Amsterdam UMC approved the study [registration number: 2018.651—NL68348.029.18]. All participants in this manuscript gave written informed consent.

Results

Proxy informants of a sample of 35 deceased young people, 18 females and 17 males, were interviewed. We conducted 37 interviews with 54 parents and 18 interviews with 19 peers. The number of conducted interviews per case varied from one to four. We interviewed two NOK per case on average, which always included at least one parent.

Table 1 presents an overview of the SRC events as derived from analyses. SRC events were identified in 100 percent of youths in our sample. The mean number of SRC events reported per case throughout life was 22.80 (min. 3, max. 61), with a relatively high standard deviation (13.70). While the mean number of SRC events (explicit and implicit) was notably higher in female cases, this was not statistically significant. However, nearly twice as many explicit SRC events were recorded in girls, which was significant (p = 0.02).

Table 1. SRC of sample cases.

All (n = 35) Females (n = 18) Males (n = 17)
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Age 17.0 (1.5) 17.3 (1.3) 16.7 (1.7)
Interviews 1.5 1.5 1.6
Respondents 2.1 1.9 2.3
SRC events 22.8 (13.7) 26.1 (15.5) 19.3 (10.9)
Verbal SRC events 6.6 6.9 6.2
Nonverbal SRC events 12.5 14.9 10.0
Written SRC events 3.7 4.3 3.0
Explicit SRC events 13.7 (10.3) 17.6 (11.6) 9.6 (6.8)

The following sections address our findings from pattern analyses. Where applicable, the sections start by presenting general patterns identified in the SRC of youths. Thereafter, patterns in SRC of boys and girls will be specifically highlighted.

Section 1: The occurrence and dispersion of SRC

The debut (i.e. the first recorded event) of SRC appeared to occur later in male cases than in female cases, usually between 14–16 years old, and was often more proximal to the suicide-act in male cases. A single exception was a boy who first said he wanted to die when he was five years old. In four out of 17 male cases, there were no recorded explicit SRC events (verbal, nonverbal, or written) until the last months before the suicide-act. In five other male cases, there were singular events in their lives prior to the last months. Common debuts of SRC in young males consisted of verbal SRC events. To a lesser extent we recorded debuts of nonverbal SRC in males, such as deliberate self-harm, nonfatal suicide attempts, or suicide information seeking. For several male cases, the debut of SRC concerned an implicit communication event in the last months alive.

Most female cases in the study sample, on the other hand, were characterized by various SRC events over a longer period preceding the suicide-act. This encompassed verbal expressions of suicide ideation or intent, as well as long-term deliberate self-harm, former nonfatal suicide attempts, suicide-related online behaviours, and written SRC. Half of the girls had a debut of SRC around the transition to high school, at 11–13 years old. The most common debut of SRC in girls was deliberate self-harm, followed by disclosure and nonfatal suicide attempts.

Verbal SRC

Verbal SRC events in boys rarely included straightforward disclosure of suicidal thoughts or clearly recognizable suicidal behaviour. Instead, boys frequently asked questions about death or reincarnation, spoke about death or suicide in a general sense, voiced their opinion about suicide, vaguely hinted at not being alive anymore (e.g. “I am never going to be old anyway”), or made jokes involving death or suicide that seemed random at the time. When the parents of one boy said they would go shopping and asked him if he needed anything, he replied: “all I need is a rope”, which at the time his parents believed to be a joke. Only six of the 17 boys unequivocally spoke about suicide ideation or suicide plans in the years preceding their death.

Girls addressed suicidal feelings more directly in verbal SRC and made few cynical remarks or macabre jokes. Fifteen out of 18 girls had unambiguously disclosed their own suicide ideation or suicide plans to at least one other person. Notably, three girls did not verbally express their suicide ideation or suicide plans, of whom one engaged in deliberate self-harm and one had made a nonfatal suicide attempt. Eleven girls had had multiple conversations with their NOK about wanting to take their own life. Notably, these were mostly girls with complex psychopathology, such as clinical depression with comorbid eating disorder, who had been admitted to a mental health institution at least once in their life.

Nonverbal SRC

Deliberate self-harm was described in five out of 17 male cases. Two boys cut themselves, a method also reported in females, but there were also methods of deliberate self-harm unique in the male subjects, such as hitting objects with their hands until they bled or slamming their head against a wall. One boy deliberately jeopardized his physical integrity by ignoring treatment prescriptions for somatic symptoms, neglecting the concerns of NOK. Deliberate self-harm or nonfatal suicide attempts prior to the last months of their life were described in eight male cases, but many nonverbal communications were ambiguous, despite an inextricable link to death, dying, or suicide. One boy had carried around a miniature noose, which was found in his pocket when he died. His parents had reason to believe he had carried it for a long time and that he kept it around as “a symbol [to remind or comfort him] of how he always could [end it]”. One boy went out with his bicycle in the middle of the night to visit the location where he took his own life one year later. When his parents confronted him with his behaviour at the time, he would not say why he went there.

In total, 13 out of 18 girls had engaged in some form of deliberate self-harm, including cutting, ingestion of objects, and self-starvation. The latter was exclusively found in girls. First events of deliberate self-harm occurred as early as 11 or 12 years old in our sample. Nearly all girls who initially self-harmed in early adolescence continued these behaviours until the moment of their death. In 11 girls, deliberate self-harm was followed by one or multiple nonfatal attempts. Ingesting an excessive amount of medication was frequently described by respondents as a first attempt. Over time, most girls proceeded to use more lethal methods for attempting suicide, such as auto-suffocation and hanging. This was clearly illustrated in one of the female cases.

Parent: on December 12th, she made a first suicide attempt […] she took 20 paracetamols. On February 15th, she attempted again, with 30 paracetamols. She started cutting more, and closer to her wrists. […] In June, she made another attempt using 40 paracetamols.

Respondents of a small number of female cases recalled other forms of explicit nonverbal SRC. One girl made a bucket list of things to do before her death, which she shared with a friend. Another wrote and sang multiple songs about dying with one of her friends and posted these on YouTube. Several girls, and one boy, developed an interest in emotional texts, images, songs, and/or videos relating to death or suicide. Girls selected online communities in which youths with mental health problems and suicide ideation aired their thoughts. Here they lurked at dark and depressing quotes and images (memes) from online contacts or created their own collection.

Written SRC

Written explicit communications about suicide or suicide ideation or intent were rare among males in the years preceding death by suicide. Particularly posting about mental health or suicidal thoughts in online communities was seldom mentioned. This was described in only two male cases. Three boys communicated suicide plans in WhatsApp messages to their mother or girlfriend. Notes of two boys detailing their struggle with suicidal thoughts were only found after their deaths.

By contrast, at least 13 of the girls described their suicide ideation or intent explicitly in diary entries, online posts, fictional stories involving dying or suicide, or in drawings related to suicide. According to respondents, most of the conversations in the online communities that the girls were part of were not about suicide. Nevertheless, multiple girls had reportedly written about suicide ideation to friends or strangers online and included pictures of their deliberate self-harm and selfies after nonfatal attempts. The latter behaviour was exclusively reported for girls who had been residents of a healthcare institution.

SRC in the last months

In our next step, we compared patterns of explicit SRC in the last months to those during life. Specific nonverbal SRC became more pronounced in the last months. Whereas six youths seemed to have prepared for their suicide in the year before suicide, this number had increased to 18 in the last months. These 18 youths enquired about methods on forums or social media, gathered information through search engines, scouted locations for a suicide, looked up train schedules, or acquired means for suicide, such as a rope or helium tanks. One girl set up her social media to keep posting after her death. A boy started a bucket list, like the aforementioned girl, but briefly before death. Crucially, preparations for suicide were discovered after death in eight of the 18 cases. In three cases it was unclear from the interview when preparations were discovered by NOK. Suicide notes were written by six boys and seven girls. Five other youths wrote a farewell message through a computer or in a text message. The other youths had not left a farewell message according to NOK.

Even proximal to the suicide-act, explicit SRC remained scarce in most boys. In two male cases with no history of SRC, there was a sudden onset and rapid escalation of both verbal and nonverbal events in the last months, which happened parallel to sudden development of psychotic symptoms. One of these boys, who had never disclosed any suicidal feelings or reported suicidal behaviours, rapidly proceeded from communicating distress, stating: “my thinking is driving me crazy”, to openly discussing suicide methods with his mother, saying: “I want to talk to you tonight. I want to tell you how I’ll do it.

Implicit communications

Respondents elucidated various implicit communication events in the last months which they associated with the suicide. Most youths verbalized their emotional distress and sadness, were increasingly tired, changed their sleeping patterns, or isolated themselves from their loved ones. Furthermore, multiple youths deleted social media accounts or exited WhatsApp groups. Respondents of a small number of cases recalled notable changes in appearance, academic performance, and religious behaviours in the last months. A distinctive phenomenon in our interview data was a perceived upswing of youths. Varying from one day to several weeks before death, 13 youths (seven boys, six girls) seemed more “relaxed” or “happy” to our respondents than before. Some youths were said to have invested more time in social contacts in the period just before their death. This perceived upswing was most common in the days briefly before the suicide-act.

Parent: “That Sunday, I came back from the theatre. She was sitting on the couch; she had colour in her cheeks. She hadn’t looked that good in years. She was lively, had taken a shower, everything was shaved, she had washed her hair. […] we had diner together, it was a very relaxing day.

Gender differences in the occurrence of implicit communications were limited. In hindsight, it became clear to NOK that six girls had been actively, yet implicitly, saying goodbye. They repeatedly spoke out their love and affection for NOK, ordered excessive numbers of gifts for NOK, met up with friends they had not seen for a long time, or hosted longer visits of their NOK in the clinic. One girl had rearranged her entire bedroom to represent memories from her life, using pictures and other objects. These behaviours were not recorded in boys.

Section 2: Content of communications

Recorded verbal and written SRC events provided insight into the youths’ own vocabularies. In 25 out of 35 cases, NOK described the content of verbal and written SRC events, by paraphrasing or quoting the youth. Central themes in the content of explicit SRC throughout life were universal for boys and girls. Explicit SRC events reflected the youths’ distress, depressive feelings, and hopelessness and revealed how suicide ideation started and became more pressing over time. The young people additionally spoke out their fears and, crucially, many asked for help and support.

Young boys were less inclined than girls to disclose suicide ideation or ask for help in the content of SRC during life. They seldom expressed a need for professional healthcare. Several boys who had received care described negative feelings or an opposing attitude towards care. A parent illustrates: “He [son] said: so, I’m supposed to go here and just do a bit of clay modelling with more of those people, and that’s supposed to make me feel better? He [son] said: forget it. I am not going.” Another mother argued that sitting face-to-face with a young psychologist to talk was not effective for her son, although it did not keep him from going: “He would mope every time, but he went every week. He was always compliant. Although he did walk out a few times. [] They should have connected better with him [] but these were not the right people for that, these were young female therapists; he needed something else. NOK’s own attempts to talk to boys about emotional wellbeing were strained when trying to get to the bottom of their distress. As one mother illustrated: “he always said: ‘I am really losing my mind.’ Then I would sit next to him, ask: what does that mean? [] He would not say anything. […] When I tried to delve deeper, he said: ‘Stop it! I don’t want to talk about it anymore.’” Something another boy wrote in a note found after he died suggested an avoidant coping strategy: “nobody notices, not even my dear mother”.

By contrast, the content of explicit SRC events in girls in our sample seemed more purposeful compared to males in that they more often clearly addressed suicide ideation or intent, expressed their will to seek professional help, or focused on coping or the difficulty thereof. Several girls had asked for support from friends and family, or actively addressed unmet health needs.

Parent: “She was 15 and had just started treatment. It didn’t work out, and she frequently entered emergency services. She opened up about it, too. She said that she wasn’t as strong as me and that she couldn’t do it alone.

Content of SRC in the last months

In the content of SRC events in the last months, there was increasing emphasis on emotional detachment from friends and family, hopelessness, negative perceptions about the future, and readiness to die. Several youths stated that they were only alive for others.

Friend: “And then she told me: ‘I really cannot go on living for you.’ That got to me. I asked her: ‘Can’t we try to do anything fun together, or make it work, or try something that will make it better?. ‘No, no’ [she said]”

The topics of verbal communications shifted. Eight youths started discussing suicide methods. Some shared intimate details about previous nonfatal attempts, asked for permission to die, or made threats to take their own life, which put a great strain on NOK. The topic of euthanasia or assisted suicide, which had formerly been reported in three girls, was addressed by four more youths (two males, two females) in the last months. Youths would discuss how they valued dying in a “proper” or “clean” way. Lastly, it stood out that five girls and two boys made various claims in the last months which seemed to suggest that they believed suicide was not a definite ending to their existence.

Parent: “If you believe that dying is the end of all things, that everything stops and that there is nothing, then the step to take your own life is much more frightening. That was not at all how she saw it. She felt like: ‘I’m going to my grandfather [in the afterlife]’.

Notably, negative feelings about healthcare emerged in SRC of girls in the last months. Whereas several boys seemed opposed to reaching out to professional help throughout their lives, a group of five girls who had received various forms of therapy stated in the last months that they had lost faith in the effect of therapy. As a result, they believed themselves to be “beyond help”. Parents of a girl illustrate: “We were invited to the therapist’s office, who again focused on healing or at least dealing with the problems and looking forward. But she [daughter] formally stated in that conversation that she wanted to cease living. [] that she wanted to stop treatment.

The content of suicide notes or other farewell messages were provided by NOK of 14 youths. Youths mostly expressed their love for NOK and tried to minimize potential feelings of guilt about the suicide. As a parent recalled: “[he wrote] that he had a good life. That he was happy. That this was the only option for him. But with a message to everybody to not grieve, to be happy. Youths further described how they could no longer deal with the pain, provided justification for their death, or made practical arrangements. In contrast with the content of preceding verbal and written SRC, we identified no notable gender differences in the content of suicide notes.

Content of implicit communications

The content of implicit communications, where suicide was not directly mentioned, was diverse. Most youths, regardless of gender, highlighted emotional distress, predominantly sadness, anxiety, and insecurity. According to NOK, however, boys more frequently ventilated their frustrations and felt that they did not belong or that they were “stuck”. A few respondents recalled increased verbal aggression in male youths. In contrast, NOK mentioned that girls verbalized perpetual guilt and thought of themselves as a burden. As a mother illustrated: “She [daughter] did not know what the matter was. She constantly felt guilty. That feeling of guilt and those crazy jumps [in behaviour] and saying things that she did not understand why she said. […] She [daughter] said: mostly that feeling of guilt towards everyone”.

Section 3: Recipients of SRC events

Recipients can play a crucial role in early signalling and suicide prevention. In a group of five boys, parents were the only recipients of SRC events, and in six other cases it was only the parents and a close friend or a romantic partner. In contrast, there were more varied recipients of SRC events for girls. These included parents, friends, classmates, healthcare personnel, peers with similar problems online, and fellow residents of mental care institutions. The latter two were unique recipients of female SRC events.

NOK’s interpretation of SRC events and NOK’s response to SRC appeared to parallel the identified gender patterns in SRC. Additionally, these themes reflected the challenges in talking about suicide with young boys and girls from the perspective of NOK.

NOK’s interpretation of SRC events

SRC was predominantly interpreted by NOK as a signal of distress or a cry for help and prompted a response such as emotional support or seeking professional help together. Both explicit and implicit communications affected NOK’s perceptions about suicide risk. In several cases, the seemingly overwhelming number of SRC events led to a fatalistic view among NOK, which one parent summarized thus: “It wasn’t a question of ‘if’ but ‘when’ it would happen. In other cases, NOK explained how they normalized SRC events because they were afraid to acknowledge the reality of suicide. As a mother illustrated: “There is no way you can live with the idea that she’d do it because that would drive you insane. So, you just have to live by: ‘I am doing the best I can’, and ‘she won’t do it’. Several youths denied suicide ideation and/or suicide plans when asked by NOK, which lowered NOK’s perceived risk of suicide.

Some gender differences were observed across interpretations of SRC events by NOK. Due to their isolated occurrence and sometimes vague or macabrely humorous presentation, NOK found the seriousness of SRC events of young males difficult to determine. This difficulty was not mentioned by NOK of females. Furthermore, NOK of nine boys explained that while they did notice that the youth seemed to struggle, appeared unhappy, or behaved differently than before, they never expected suicide to be a realistic outcome of these dynamics. Crucially, in the absence of unequivocal signals of suicide ideation or intent in boys, NOK perceived no mental health needs to respond to at the time.

Parent: “In hindsight I sometimes think maybe his ‘not feeling happy’ was stronger than we noticed at the time. But you just don’t knowOnly when looking back. At that moment there was no reason to worry.

Sometimes SRC events, in particular deliberate self-harm and nonfatal suicide attempts, were perceived by NOK as attention seeking, a dramatic display of emotions, or a way to achieve a desired outcome. Brother: “Many of her attempts had been half-hearted. […] Sometimes it seemed she was doing it mostly for the attention. This was observed in SRC events of at least seven girls who showed frequent suicidal behaviour. It must be noted that NOK of these girls generally still sought help despite their own beliefs.

NOK’s response to SRC events

In response to most SRC events of both boys and girls, NOK tried to engage in open conversation, recommended the youth to seek professional care, or sought help together. However, parents and peers mentioned that it was challenging to talk about suicide with the young person. They attributed this to stigma, which was reflected in feeling uncomfortable or inadequate to address such a difficult subject, and some NOK were afraid to aggravate or normalize the problems if they were to discuss them with the young person.

Parent: “How are you supposed to react [to the expression of suicidal feelings]? It scared the hell out of me to hear him say things like that. I said nothing. You become immobilized.

Parent: “Whenever I communicated about it [suicide], it was through WhatsApp. […] I asked her some things through WhatsApp that I did not dare ask face-to-face.

While considering that support and help-seeking were most common, the interview data did suggest that NOK of boys would more commonly respond with “tough love”. In seven male cases of whom multiple SRC events were noted, NOK did not respond, hardly asked questions, avoided the conversation, stressed the youth’s own responsibility, or (in hindsight) downplayed the seriousness of SRC. One parent told us: “I said: [NAME], you called this out on yourself, you alone are the reason you are here [in a psychiatric hospital]. […] Now you’ve done it. Because they are going to lock you up now, is that what you want? Another parent remembered: “that conversation we had at the dinner table; he was crying. He had frequent headaches. […] later it turned out he could not sleep for nights, or only a few hours, because of those headaches. I remember that we used to say: ‘keep your head up, take an aspirin.’ Looking back, we would never have done that, of course.

In three male cases, NOK responded with physical aggression. When one of the boys hit his head against a wall, his father would pick him up and shake him until he stopped. One brother described how he tried to help his depressed, suicidal brother in various ways but ended up physically assaulting him when words failed.

Brother: “He was in his bedroom, and I beat him up quite badly, just because I felt powerless. I tried to help; I tried all kinds of things. One time, a colleague and I made a whole list of positive things about him [victim] because he thought he wasn’t good enough. […] you feel so powerless and there’s nothing you can do, so I barged into his room and we had a bad fight.

Instead, SRC events of girls more often triggered conversations. NOK of 14 girls had asked follow-up questions to elucidate details, and seeking help together was common. Notably, data suggested that deliberate self-harm and nonfatal suicide attempts were strong cues for NOK to discuss suicide ideation or seek professional help. Adversely, frequent occurrence of deliberate self-harm and attempts had NOK constantly worry, which could evoke negative feelings about their relationship with the youth. One mother well remembers her struggle: “has she been on WhatsApp? Oh, she’s been offline for 45 minutes… Who has seen her since? Has someone seen her on Instagram? You know… that’s no way to live. It’s impossible. Consequently, some NOK restrained contact.

Peer: “[I received] a weekly phone call, in which she said: ‘I tried to take my life again’. I thought to myself: I have a life too. I am in school, I go to work, I cannot handle all this. We grew apart. […] Honestly, I was not looking forward to visiting her [in a psychiatric hospital]. You sit in front of each otherWhat do you say? My life is moving forward, yours is at a halt. That felt awkward.

Discussion

This study aimed to explore patterns in SRC of youths and to examine gender differences in these patterns. Almost 800 SRC events of 35 youths who died by suicide in the Netherlands in 2017 were explored by means of qualitative analyses.

Frequency and debut

We have identified SRC events in all youths in our sample. Results show that females had an earlier debut of SRC and significantly more explicit SRC events. Most debuts of SRC in girls occurred at the start of puberty, whereas the first SRC events of boys more frequently occurred in the last months of their lives. Marttunen and colleagues [26] found that adolescent males without a diagnosis for mental health problems communicated suicidal thoughts shortly before suicide. Aligning this, fewer boys than girls in our sample had diagnosed mental health problems [37]. Finally, our findings concerning SRC debut show parallels with the onset of suicide ideation in boys versus girls [4, 12], which may suggest that boys and girls engage in SRC briefly after first ideation.

There may be several reasons for the gender discrepancy in the frequency of explicit SRC events. Logically, the fact that the SRC debut in boys was often proximal to suicide narrows the timeframe in which SRC events could have occurred. Second, the difference in the frequency of nonverbal events may be explained by the propensity of girls to engage in deliberate self-harm and nonfatal suicide attempts more than boys [4], which could also be observed in our sample. De Beurs [38] suggests that the relatively higher prevalence of psychopathology such as depression and ideation in young girls, as well as trauma from sexual abuse at a young age, may contribute to this propensity. Crucially, there is limited evidence showing which prevention and treatment strategies work for youths with chronic suicidal behaviours [18]. Lastly, respondents explained that boys refrained from conversations about emotional distress overall and believed this probably has hampered verbal SRC. Husky and colleagues [39] report that being male is indeed associated with non-disclosure of suicide ideation, and De Luca and Wyman [40] confirmed this in a study among Latino adolescents. Nolen-Hoeksema and Girgus [41] propose that the emotion-focused coping of girls may be more focused on communication and rumination, whereas the coping of boys may be more distraction-oriented. Corroborating this, girls are more likely to disclose emotions to parents and friends than boys [42]. Alternatively, male beliefs about masculine norms may influence non-disclosure of suicide ideation and affect help-seeking in males [38, 43], but it is unclear how this may affect youths.

The last months prior to suicide

We identified notable changes in explicit SRC during the last months, irrespective of gender. The emphasis of communications shifted towards practical considerations of suicide, such as discussing the methods or talking about a planned date for the suicide. NOK furthermore associated a plethora of implicit SRC events in the last months of the young people’s lives with the suicide. What stood out specifically was the perceived upswing of 13 youths briefly before suicide, which was universal for boys and girls. Our study cannot fully explain the underlying mechanisms of this upswing but provides evidence of its existence. Parents in our sample suggested that the upswing might indicate that their child had made a final decision. The phenomenon has been proposed by mental healthcare professionals as a signal of impending suicide in youths [18]. There is, however, a lack of conclusive evidence in the literature and no firm conceptual basis. Antecedents to youth suicide as observed in SRC can be subtle but may provide implications for signalling suicide ideation and arguably represent a closing window of opportunity for intervention. Academic consensus on an operational definition would facilitate research of antecedents such as the perceived mood upswing.

Content of SRC

The content of explicit SRC events in boys was often ambiguous. Additionally, several events in young males reflected an opposing attitude towards care. Contrastingly, the content of SRC events of girls seemed more purposeful, showing emphasis on coping and help-seeking, with a negative attitude towards care developing only in the last months, after trying extensive therapy. This corroborates cross-national research which shows that young males suffering suicide ideation are less predisposed to seek professional mental health for their mental distress [44, 45]. It is possible that boys’ use of more macabre jokes and vague terminology regarding suicide ideation reflect negative perceptions about help-seeking. These in turn may result from self-imposed gender-role restrictions among males, in which help-seeking is a sign of weakness and at odds with masculinity. This relates back to previous research into disclosure of depression by adult males [27, 46, 47]. In our study, vague communications seemed less likely to result in support or proactive help-seeking by NOK. This stresses the importance of encouraging young males to be unequivocal in their expression of suicide ideation, which may go hand in hand with help for NOK and professionals in learning how to ask boys more upfront questions about their wellbeing, particularly when boys seem ambiguous about their mental health.

Recipients of SRC

Girls in our sample directed SRC towards relatively more varied recipients than boys. They spoke to friends and companions with similar feelings, both online and offline. Additionally, as more girls were admitted into a mental health institution, healthcare professionals were common recipients of SRC events of girls. Boys who verbalized their suicidal thoughts generally limited any communications to parents, romantic partners, and a few close friends. There is evidence that young people are more likely to express suicide ideation or intent to peers than to adults [6, 20, 48]. Since we are using the psychological autopsy method with parents as primary proxy informants, we cannot confirm this. However, peer interviews provided additional insights into communication events in specific settings, such as in mental health institutions and online, which is also concluded by research of Looijmans and colleagues [49].

Additional insights emerged concerning NOK’s interpretation of SRC and their response to specific events. NOK acknowledged their inability to assess the seriousness of SRC events for various reasons, such as the ambiguity of SRC events in males and the overwhelmingness of SRC events in girls. Both appeared to lead to desensitization for future SRC events. Furthermore, it was difficult for NOK to differentiate between behavioural changes due to psychosocial development in adolescence and potential nonverbal SRC events. NOK’s responses to SRC varied. Open conversation, support, and help-seeking were the most common. In the male cases, we found that NOK more often did not respond to SRC events, emphasized the youths’ own responsibility, or responded with physical aggression.

Strengths and limitations

Interviews with NOK provided a rich, detailed account of the SRC events of the deceased youths. The sample had a demographic profile that was similar to the total group of 81 youths who died by suicide in 2017 in the Netherlands [50, 51]. Furthermore, the male to female ratio was near 1:1, which, although not reflecting actual figures of suicide [50], was suitable for an in-depth exploration of SRC and to highlight gender differences.

However, some limitations of this study must be considered. First, this psychological autopsy was conducted without a control group due to the nature of the original research. Therefore, we do not know how the identified patterns of SRC in our sample would relate to patterns of SRC found in young suicide survivors. Second, the small sample size limits the generalizability of conclusions based on statistical comparisons. Third, parents were asked to recruit secondary informants, which could have led to selection bias of NOK with a shared narrative. Lastly, interviewing proxy informants 1.5–2.5 years after the suicide may result in recall bias as well as socially desirable responses concerning SRC events. More specifically, NOK may have forgotten SRC events or feel conflicted to talk about specific events that they think might reflect negatively on them. Pouliot and De Leo [52] suggest that “Emotion-related response bias may pose as well a challenge to the reliability of life events assessment based on proxies’ report” in psychological autopsy studies, which is likely to affect recollection of SRC events in a similar manner. However, Assink [53] postulates that negative life events are more consistently recollected by people compared to neutral or positive events, which would include NOK’s memories about precipitating factors and antecedents for the suicide of their loved ones. Including multiple respondents can furthermore reduce such forms of bias, but secondary informants were not interviewed in all cases. Perhaps consequentially, we have obtained limited insight into online SRC events, particularly in boys. Several boys in our sample used videogame chat servers, such as Discord or Steam Chat, about which parents had little knowledge. Dutch research suggests that parents are not fully aware of their children’s online risk behaviours, such as cyber-bullying [54]. Furthermore, children are considered digital natives and may be well equipped to erase tracks of their online SRC.

Conclusions

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to conduct a qualitative analysis of suicide-related communication events of young people who died by suicide and to explore gender differences using psychological autopsy data. Gender-specific patterns in suicide-related communications were identified. The early debut of suicide-related communication in girls in our sample highlights the importance of early screening and prevention efforts in young girls. Ideally, specific attention must be drawn to establishing their needs earlier in the suicidal process to facilitate improved personalized care. The late debut and ambiguous presentation of suicide-related communication in boys may guide NOK and professionals to adapt communication strategies, for example by obtaining unequivocal confirmation or disconfirmation of suicide ideation. The overall findings underline the importance for NOK and professionals who work with potentially suicidal youths to be aware of gender differences in suicide-related communication and coherent challenges in communication. The potential causality between the frequency, form, or content of suicide-related communication and a young person’s likelihood of receiving care warrants further inquiry. Lastly, we encourage future research to expand on the topic of online suicide-related communication and the potential for suicide prevention on digital platforms.

Supporting information

S1 File. Interview instrument parents.

(DOCX)

S2 File. Code list and coding approach.

(DOCX)

S3 File. COREQ checklist.

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to all next of kin who have participated in this study. They have suffered a great tragedy and were willing to share their intimate, insightful stories with us. We believe these stories are of great value in informing suicide prevention strategies.

Data Availability

The interview data cannot be shared publicly because of ethical restrictions: the dataset contains potentially identifying and sensitive information. The Medical Research Ethics Committee (MREC) of Amsterdam UMC has imposed this restriction (registration number: 2018.651 – NL68348.029.18). Data are available upon request from the Ethics Committee for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. A request may be submitted by mail to metc@vumc.nl.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.CBS. Overledenen; belangrijke doodsoorzaken [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2020 Jul 24]. Available from: https://opendata.cbs.nl/#/CBS/nl/dataset/80202ned/table?ts=1595575707035
  • 2.WHO. Suicide—Key facts [Internet]. 2019 [cited 2020 Jul 24]. Available from: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/suicide
  • 3.Runeson BS, Beskow J, Waern M. The suicidal process in suicides among young people. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1996;93(1):35–42. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1996.tb10616.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Cha CB, Franz PJ, M. Guzmán E, Glenn CR, Kleiman EM, Nock MK. Annual Research Review: Suicide among youth–epidemiology, (potential) etiology, and treatment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2018;59(4):460–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Nock MK, Green JG, Hwang I, McLaughlin KA, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, et al. Prevalence, correlates, and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior among adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement. JAMA psychiatry. 2013. March;70(3):300–10. 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.55 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.McCauley J, Berkowitz L (Larry). Adolescent suicide. In: Violence and trauma in the lives of children: Overview of exposure, Vol 1. Santa Barbara, CA, US: Praeger/ABC-CLIO; 2018. p. 145–67.
  • 7.Canetto SS, Sakinofsky I. The Gender Paradox in Suicide. Suicide Life-Threatening Behav [Internet]. 1998. March 1;28(1):1–23. Available from: 10.1111/j.1943-278X.1998.tb00622.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kann L, McManus T, Harris WA, Shanklin SL, Flint KH, Queen B, et al. Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance—United States, 2017. MMWR Surveill Summ [Internet]. 2018. June 15;67(8):1–114. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/29902162 10.15585/mmwr.ss6708a1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.CBS. Overledenen; zelfdoding (inwoners), diverse kenmerken [Internet]. 14-10-2020. 2020 [cited 2020 Oct 16]. Available from: https://opendata.cbs.nl/#/CBS/nl/dataset/7022gza/table?searchKeywords=zelfdoding
  • 10.Nock MK, Kazdin AE. Examination of affective, cognitive, and behavioral factors and suicide-related outcomes in children and young adolescents. J Clin child Adolesc Psychol Off J Soc Clin Child Adolesc Psychol Am Psychol Assoc Div 53. 2002. March;31(1):48–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kaess M, Parzer P, Haffner J, Steen R, Roos J, Klett M, et al. Explaining gender differences in non-fatal suicidal behaviour among adolescents: a population-based study. BMC Public Health [Internet]. 2011;11(1):597. Available from: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Boeninger D, Masyn K, Feldman B, Conger R. Sex Differences in Developmental Trends of Suicide Ideation, Plans, and Attempts among European American Adolescents. Suicide Life Threat Behav. 2010. October 1;40:451–64. 10.1521/suli.2010.40.5.451 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Voss C, Ollmann TM, Miché M, Venz J, Hoyer J, Pieper L, et al. Prevalence, Onset, and Course of Suicidal Behavior Among Adolescents and Young Adults in Germany. JAMA Netw Open [Internet]. 2019. October 30;2(10):e1914386–e1914386. Available from: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.14386 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Morken IS, Dahlgren A, Lunde I, Toven S. The effects of interventions preventing self-harm and suicide in children and adolescents: an overview of systematic reviews [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research [Internet]. 2020;8(890). Available from: http://openr.es/j2l [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Frey LM, Fulginiti A, Sheehan L, Oexle N, Stage DL, Stohlmann-Rainey J. What’s in a word? Clarifying terminology on suicide-related communication. Death Stud. 2020;44(12):808–18. 10.1080/07481187.2019.1614111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Wasserman D, Thanh HTT, Minh DPT, Goldstein M, Nordenskiold A, Wasserman C. Suicidal process, suicidal communication and psychosocial situation. World Psychiatry. 2008;7:47–53. 10.1002/j.2051-5545.2008.tb00152.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Zalsman G, Hawton K, Wasserman D, van Heeringen K, Arensman E, Sarchiapone M, et al. Suicide prevention strategies revisited: 10-year systematic review. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(7). 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30030-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Meerdinkveldboom J, Kerkhof AJMF, Rood I. Handboek Suicidaal Gedrag bij Jongeren. 3rd ed. Meerdinkveldboom J, Kerkhof AJMF, Rood I, editors. Boom Uitgevers Amsterdam; 2019. 320 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pompili M, Belvederi Murri M, Patti S, Innamorati M, Lester D, Girardi P, et al. The communication of suicidal intentions: A meta-analysis. Vol. 46, Psychological Medicine. Cambridge University Press; 2016. p. 2239–53. 10.1017/S0033291716000696 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Portzky G, Audenaert K, van Heeringen K. Psychosocial and psychiatric factors associated with adolescent suicide: A case-control psychological autopsy study. J Adolesc. 2009. August;32(4):849–62. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Brent DA, Perper JA, Moritz G, Allman C, Friend A, Roth C, et al. Psychiatric risk factors for adolescent suicide: a case-control study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993. May;32(3):521–9. 10.1097/00004583-199305000-00006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Shaffi M, Carrigan S, Whittinghill J, Derrick A. Psychological autopsy of completed suicide in children and adolescents. Am J Psychiatry [Internet]. 1985. September 1;142(9):1061–4. Available from: 10.1176/ajp.142.9.1061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Orbach I, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Ofek H, Lubin G, Mark M, Bodner E, et al. A Chronological Perspective on Suicide—The Last Days of Life. Death Stud [Internet]. 2007. October 16;31(10):909–32. Available from: 10.1080/07481180701603394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Portzky G, Audenaert K, van Heeringen K. Suicide among adolescents. A psychological autopsy study of psychiatric, psychosocial and personality-related risk factors. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005. November;40(11):922–30. 10.1007/s00127-005-0977-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Brent DA, Perper JA, Goldstein CE, Kolko DJ, Allan MJ, Allman CJ, et al. Risk factors for adolescent suicide. A comparison of adolescent suicide victims with suicidal inpatients. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988. June;45(6):581–8. 10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800300079011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Marttunen MJ, Henriksson MM, Isometsä ET, Heikkinen ME, Aro HM, Lönnqvist JK. Completed suicide among adolescents with no diagnosable psychiatric disorder. Adolescence. 1998;33(131):669–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rhodes AE. Antecedents and sex/gender differences in youth suicidal behavior. World J Psychiatry. 2014;4(4):120. 10.5498/wjp.v4.i4.120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gontijo Guerra S, Vasiliadis H-M. Gender Differences in Youth Suicide and Healthcare Service Use. Crisis [Internet]. 2016. June 1;37(4):290–8. Available from: 10.1027/0227-5910/a000387 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Rytterström P, Lindeborg M, Korhonen S, Sellin T. Finding the Silent Message: Nurses’ Experiences of Non-Verbal Communication Preceding a Suicide. Psychology. 2019;10(01):1–18. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mérelle S, Van Bergen D, Looijmans M, Balt E, Rasing S, van Domburgh L, et al. A multi-method psychological autopsy study on youth suicides in the Netherlands in 2017: Feasibility, main outcomes, and recommendations. PLoS One [Internet]. 2020. August 27;15(8):e0238031. Available from: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Arensman E, Bennardi M, Larkin C, Wall A, McAuliffe C, McCarthy J, et al. Suicide among Young People and Adults in Ireland: Method Characteristics, Toxicological Analysis and Substance Abuse Histories Compared. PLoS One [Internet]. 2016. November 29;11(11):e0166881. Available from: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166881 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Rasmussen ML, Haavind H, Dieserud G, Dyregrov K. Exploring vulnerability to suicide in the developmental history of young men: a psychological autopsy study. Death Stud. 2014;38(6–10):549–56. 10.1080/07481187.2013.780113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Freuchen A, Kjelsberg E, Lundervold AJ, Grøholt B. Differences between children and adolescents who commit suicide and their peers: A psychological autopsy of suicide victims compared to accident victims and a community sample. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2012. January;6:1. 10.1186/1753-2000-6-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Houston K, Hawton K, Shepperd R. Suicide in young people aged 15–24: a psychological autopsy study. J Affect Disord [Internet]. 2001;63(1):159–70. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0165032700001750 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00175-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Glaser BG, Strauss AL, Strutzel E. The Discovery of Grounded Theory; Strategies for Qualitative Research. Nurs Res [Internet]. 1968;17(4). Available from: https://journals.lww.com/nursingresearchonline/Fulltext/1968/07000/The_Discovery_of_Grounded_Theory__Strategies_for.14.aspx [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Onwuegbuzie AJ, Leech NL. Validity and qualitative research: An oxymoron? Qual Quant. 2007;41(2):233–49. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Mérelle, Van Bergen, Popma, Balt E, Looijmans M, Sanne Rasing, et al. Suïcide onder 10- tot 20-jarigen in 2017: een verdiepend onderzoek. 2019;(december).
  • 38.Beurs D de. Mythen over zelfmoord. Amsterdam: Boom; 2020.
  • 39.Husky MM, Zablith I, Alvarez Fernandez V, Kovess-Masfety V. Factors associated with suicidal ideation disclosure: Results from a large population-based study. J Affect Disord [Internet]. 2016;205:36–43. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0165032716308394 10.1016/j.jad.2016.06.054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.De Luca SM, Wyman PA. Association between school engagement and disclosure of suicidal ideation to adults among Latino adolescents. J Prim Prev [Internet]. 2012. June;33(2–3):99–110. Available from: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/22538874 10.1007/s10935-012-0269-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Vol. 115, Psychological Bulletin. US: American Psychological Association; 1994. p. 424–43. 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Papini D, Farmer F, Clark S, Micka J, Barnett J. Early adolescent age and gender differences in patterns of emotional disclosure to parents and friends. Adolescence. 1990. February 1;25:959–76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Murphy GE. Why women are less likely than men to commit suicide. Compr Psychiatry [Internet]. 1998;39(4):165–75. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010440X98900578 10.1016/s0010-440x(98)90057-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.GOULD MS, VELTING D, KLEINMAN M, LUCAS C, THOMAS JG, CHUNG M. Teenagers’ Attitudes About Coping Strategies and Help-Seeking Behavior for Suicidality. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry [Internet]. 2004;43(9):1124–33. Available from: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0890856709614477 10.1097/01.chi.0000132811.06547.31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Biddle L, Gunnell D, Sharp D, Donovan JL. Factors influencing help seeking in mentally distressed young adults: a cross-sectional survey. Br J Gen Pract [Internet]. 2004. April 1;54(501):248 LP– 253. Available from: http://bjgp.org/content/54/501/248.abstract [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Good GE, Wood PK. Male Gender Role Conflict, Depression, and Help Seeking: Do College Men Face Double Jeopardy? J Couns Dev [Internet]. 1995. September 10;74(1):70–5. Available from: 10.1002/j.1556-6676.1995.tb01825.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Kilmartin C. Depression in men: Communication, diagnosis and therapy. J Men’s Heal Gend. 2005. March 1;2:95–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Gibson K, Wilson J, Grice J Le, Seymour F. Resisting the Silence: The Impact of Digital Communication on Young People’s Talk About Suicide. Youth Soc. 2019;51(8):1011–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Looijmans M, Merelle S, Van Bergen DD, Gilissen R (113 zelfmoordpreventie), Popma A. The additional value of peer informants in youth psychological autopsy studies. 2020; [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 50.Berkelmans G, Gilissen R. Zelfdoding bij jeugd tot 23 jaar. [Internet]. 2018. Available from: https://www.rijksoverheid.nl/documenten/rapporten/2018/12/21/zelfdoding-bij-jeugd
  • 51.Mérelle S, Van Bergen D, Popma A, et al. 113_Suicide onder 10- tot 20-jarigen in 2017—Een verdiepend onderzoek. 2020;(december).
  • 52.Pouliot L, De Leo D. Critical Issues in Psychological Autopsy Studies. Suicide Life-Threatening Behav [Internet]. 2006. October 1;36(5):491–510. Available from: https://guilfordjournals.com/doi/abs/10.1521/suli.2006.36.5.491 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Assink MHJ. Autobiographical Memory in Longitudinal Perspective: Stability and change of reported life-events over a five-year period. VU University Amsterdam; 2008.
  • 54.Dehue F, Bolman C, Völlink T. Cyberbullying: Youngsters’ Experiences and Parental Perception. Cyberpsychol Behav. 2008. May 1;11:217–23. 10.1089/cpb.2007.0008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Vincenzo De Luca

1 Mar 2021

PONE-D-20-36446

Gender differences in suicide-related communication of young suicide victims

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Balt,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please be advised that submitting a review does not guarantee acceptance.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 15 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Vincenzo De Luca

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

3. Please amend your list of authors on the manuscript to ensure that each author is linked to an affiliation. Authors’ affiliations should reflect the institution where the work was done (if authors moved subsequently, you can also list the new affiliation stating “current affiliation:….” as necessary).

4. We note you have included a table to which you do not refer in the text of your manuscript. Please ensure that you refer to Table 1 in your text; if accepted, production will need this reference to link the reader to the Table.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: General comments:

This is a well written paper that addresses an important topic, re: prevention strategies that can stem the rising trend of youth suicide worldwide. Specifically, it is an exploratory study of gender differences in adolescents communications about suicide leading up to their suicide. Research on suicide related communication is area of early intervention about which little is known currently. Evidence from a recent systematic review show that early intervention can prevent suicidal ideation and attempts in the short term and suicide attempts in the long term (Morken et al, 2020). This is the larger background that this current study has to be situated. A major shortcoming of the paper is the authors decision to not report interview data of the 4 "non-native" suicides in their study, as immigration status and ethnicity are often risk factors for youth suicide, and given the fact that the Netherlands has a sizeable immigrant population (10%).

Introduction: The research problem, study rationale, and study aims are clearly articulated. The background literature reviewed is relevant, and includes recent work on suicide related communication in adolescents.

Methods: The psychological autopsy methodology is well established in completed suicides research to investigate putative risk factors. Given the study aims, use of qualitative explorative semi-structured interviews is appropriate, as is use of convenience sampling as documented in an earlier report (Mérelle et al, 2017). It would be helpful to readers if the authors include a brief summary of their sampling strategy and mode of recruitment of study participants. Data analysis methods (thematic content analysis, constant comparative approach) are appropriate and and adequately described.

Results: results are clearly presented

Discussion: the discussion is relevant to the study findings, the authors suggest future research directions, and they adequately address study limitations

References

1. Morken, I. S., Dahlgren, A., Lunde, I., & Toven, S. (2019). The effects of interventions preventing self-harm and suicide in children and adolescents: an overview of systematic reviews. F1000Research, 8.

2. Lipsicas, C. B., & Mäkinen, I. H. (2010). Immigration and suicidality in the young. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 55(5), 274-281.

3. Mérelle, S., Van Bergen, D., Looijmans, M., Balt, E., Rasing, S., van Domburgh, L., ... & Popma, A. (2020). A multi-method psychological autopsy study on youth suicides in the Netherlands in 2017: Feasibility, main outcomes, and recommendations. PLoS one, 15(8), e0238031.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Ademola Adeponle

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 May 21;16(5):e0252028. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252028.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


2 Apr 2021

Authors’ response to the academic editor’s comments

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at:

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Reply: We have reviewed the manuscript to ensure it meets the PLOS ONE style requirements. Firstly, we have ensured that the files for our resubmission have been named in accordance with the indicated requirements of PLOS ONE.

- ‘Manuscript’

- ‘Revision with Track Changes’

- ‘Response to reviewers’

Secondly, changes to the style in the manuscript body text have been marked accordingly and can be viewed in the document titled ‘Revision with Track Changes’. We hope that these changes concur with the academic editor’s intended sections.

- Added reference to table 1 in manuscript body (page 8, line 190)

- Replaced dash with dot in the Table 1 title (page 8, line 195)

- Changed font of table and figure titles to normal body text instead of header level 2 (page 8, line 180, line 195)

- The PACE tool was used to ensure that the image file meets PLOS ONE image requirements

1. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

a. If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b. If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

Reply: In the Editorial Manager, we have indicated that the data are restricted.

a. We would like to specifically note that the interview data cannot be shared publicly because of ethical restrictions: the dataset contains potentially identifying and sensitive information. The Medical Research Ethics Committee (MREC) of Amsterdam UMC has imposed this restriction (registration number: 2018.651 – NL68348.029.18). For more information, the committee may be contacted through: metc@vumc.nl.

b. As the data is restricted, this comment does not apply.

2. Please amend your list of authors on the manuscript to ensure that each author is linked to an affiliation. Authors’ affiliations should reflect the institution where the work was done (if authors moved subsequently, you can also list the new affiliation stating “current affiliation: ____” as necessary).

Reply: The co-authors have been asked to review their affiliations. The list of authors has been amended, to ensure that each author has been linked to an affiliation which reflects the institution where the work was done. We noted that one affiliation was missing for author Pommeline van der Post. This err has been corrected in the manuscript (page 1, line 5). We corrected the affiliation of two other researchers (page 1, line 8-9 and line 11).

3. We note you have included a table to which you do not refer in the text of your manuscript. Please ensure that you refer to Table 1 in your text; if accepted, production will need this reference to link the reader to the Table.

Reply: We thank the academic editor for notifying us that we did not refer to Table 1 in the manuscript. We have included a reference to Table 1 in the manuscript body text (page 8, line 190).

Authors’ response to the reviewer’s comments

Reviewer #1

“This is a well written paper that addresses an important topic, re: prevention strategies that can stem the rising trend of youth suicide worldwide. Specifically, it is an exploratory study of gender differences in adolescents’ communications about suicide leading up to their suicide. Research on suicide related communication is area of early intervention about which little is known currently.”

Response: We thank the reviewer for complimenting our work.

“Evidence from a recent systematic review shows that early intervention can prevent suicidal ideation and attempts in the short term and suicide attempts in the long term (Morken et al, 2020). This is the larger background that this current study has to be situated.”

Response: We thank the reviewer for this important contribution from recent literature. We find that these recent findings provide a significant addition and help us to better delineate the context, relevance and aims of our research. We therefore included a reference to the work of Morken and colleagues in the introduction of our revised manuscript (page 4, lines 74-78):

‘Recent findings from reviews of Morken and colleagues [2020] and Cha and colleagues [2018] indicate that early intervention, such as school-based interventions, may prevent suicidal ideation and attempts in the short term, and suicide attempts in the long term in youths and adolescents. The recognition of youths’ needs as reflected in their communication can guide strategies for early intervention.’

“A major shortcoming of the paper is the authors decision to not report interview data of the 4 "non-native" suicides in their study, as immigration status and ethnicity are often risk factors for youth suicide and given the fact that the Netherlands has a sizeable immigrant population (10%).”

Response: We agree with the reviewer that it is crucial to better understand the suicidal process and coherent risk factors in youths with a migration background. In total, 25 of the 81 youths (31%) who died by suicide in the Netherlands in 2017 had an immigration background (Gilissen et al, 2018). Indeed, by having been able to only include four youths with a migration background into the sample, we are limited in our understanding of the suicide-related communication of these youths, as well as in our overall findings concerning the role of cultural and migration factors in suicidality of these youths (Mérelle et al, 2020).

The authors would, however, like to express that the four youths with a migration background have in fact been included in the analyses of the current research. Our findings are based on all 35 youths in the sample, including non-Dutch youths as well as Dutch native youths. Several fragments of interview data as presented in the manuscript refer to a youth with a migration background.

We included four cases with a migration background, which we defined as having at least one parent who was born outside of the Netherlands. They were three girls and one boy. Two youths had a Western migration background, and two youths had a non-Western migration background. Only one youth had two parents with a migration background. Notably, of the three youths with a Dutch and a non-Dutch parent, the Dutch parent participated in the interview, which may have limited the cultural sensitivity of the findings. The four youths had diverse life stories, and we found no evidence of cultural factors specifically related to the suicide-related communication of these youths. Deriu and colleagues (2018) state that intergenerational conflicts and the family environment, such as poor communication and lack of parental support, can be important risk factors for suicide ideation and attempts in adolescents with a migration background. Conceivably due to our limited inclusion of youths with a migration background, we found no evidence of such or similar problems reflected in the youths’ suicide-related communication.

The main lesson we took from our research is how crucial it is to adapt recruitment strategies effectively to reach individuals bereaved by the suicide of a victim with a migration background in the Netherlands. Research from Germany (Reiss et al, 2014) shows benefits and drawbacks of a community-based approach to improve inclusion of participants with a migration background. The strategy leads to higher inclusion and a heterogeneous sample, but it requires more staff and is costly. Others (Fête et al, 2019) suggest that a communication strategy and a snowball sampling method was key to recruit participants with a migrant background. In future psychological autopsies, we will consider such strategies to improve recruitment of participants with a migration background.

“Introduction: The research problem, study rationale, and study aims are clearly articulated. The background literature reviewed is relevant and includes recent work on suicide related communication in adolescents.”

Response: We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of the introduction section.

“Methods: The psychological autopsy methodology is well established in completed suicides research to investigate putative risk factors. Given the study aims, use of qualitative explorative semi-structured interviews is appropriate, as is use of convenience sampling as documented in an earlier report (Mérelle et al, 2017). It would be helpful to readers if the authors include a brief summary of their sampling strategy and mode of recruitment of study participants. Data analysis methods (thematic content analysis, constant comparative approach) are appropriate and adequately described.”

Response: For conciseness, we opted to refer to the work of Mérelle and colleagues (2020) for the sampling procedures. However, we agree with the reviewer that including a summary of the sampling strategy and mode of recruitment would add to the comprehensiveness for the reader. As such, we have provided a summary of our approach in the revised manuscript (page 5-6, lines 120-123). The added section summarizes the following steps briefly:

- We contacted coroners of municipality health services in the Netherlands. They keep records of suicides

- Coroners provided us with the contact details of general practitioners of whom a patient aged <20 had died by suicide in 2017

- We contacted the respective general practitioners, and asked them to inform the parents of the deceased youth about the research

- Parents who showed interest received a patient information folder

- Secondary informants (peers, relatives, teachers, and healthcare professionals) were recruited with the help of the parents

“Results: results are clearly presented”

Response: We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of the results section.

“Discussion: the discussion is relevant to the study findings, the authors suggest future research directions, and they adequately address study limitations.”

Response: We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of the discussion section.

References

Morken, I. S., Dahlgren, A., Lunde, I., & Toven, S. (2019). The effects of interventions preventing self-harm and suicide in children and adolescents: an overview of systematic reviews. F1000Research, 8.

Cha CB, Franz PJ, M. Guzmán E, Glenn CR, Kleiman EM, Nock MK. Annual Research Review: Suicide among youth – epidemiology, (potential) etiology, and treatment. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2018;59(4):460–82.

Gilissen R, Merelle S, Franx G, Popma A. Zelfdoding bij jeugd tot 20 jaar: eerste duiding van de cijfers. 2018. Ministry Report.

Mérelle, S., Van Bergen, D., Looijmans, M., Balt, E., Rasing, S., van Domburgh, L., ... & Popma, A. (2020). A multi-method psychological autopsy study on youth suicides in the Netherlands in 2017: Feasibility, main outcomes, and recommendations. PLoS one, 15(8).

Deriu V, Benoit L, Moro MR, Lachal J. Idées suicidaires et tentatives de suicide à l’adolescence en contexte migratoire [Suicidal thoughts and suicide attempts in adolescence among migrants]. Soins Psychiatr. 2018 May-Jun;39(316):22-26.

Reiss K, Dragano N, Ellert U, Fricke J, Greiser KH, Keil T, Krist L, Moebus S, Pundt N, Schlaud M, Yesil-Jürgens R, Zeeb H, Zimmermann H, Razum O, Jöckel K, Becher H. Comparing sampling strategies to recruit migrants for an epidemiological study. Results from a German feasibility study, European Journal of Public Health, Volume 24, Issue 5, October 2014, Pages 721–726.

Fête, M., Aho, J., Benoit, M. et al. Barriers and recruitment strategies for precarious status migrants in Montreal, Canada. BMC Med Res Methodol 19, 41 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12874-019-0683-2.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Vincenzo De Luca

10 May 2021

Gender differences in suicide-related communication of young suicide victims

PONE-D-20-36446R1

Dear Dr. Balt,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Vincenzo De Luca

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I want to commend the authors for their openness to addressing the questions that I raised in my initial review and for addressing them in a comprehensive manner.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Ademola Adeponle

Acceptance letter

Vincenzo De Luca

12 May 2021

PONE-D-20-36446R1

Gender differences in suicide-related communication of young suicide victims

Dear Dr. Balt:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Vincenzo De Luca

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Interview instrument parents.

    (DOCX)

    S2 File. Code list and coding approach.

    (DOCX)

    S3 File. COREQ checklist.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    The interview data cannot be shared publicly because of ethical restrictions: the dataset contains potentially identifying and sensitive information. The Medical Research Ethics Committee (MREC) of Amsterdam UMC has imposed this restriction (registration number: 2018.651 – NL68348.029.18). Data are available upon request from the Ethics Committee for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data. A request may be submitted by mail to metc@vumc.nl.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES