Abstract
An adaptive memory system should prioritize information surrounding a powerful learning event that may prove useful for predicting future meaningful events. The behavioral tagging hypothesis provides a mechanistic framework to interpret how weak experiences persist as durable memories through temporal association with a strong experience. Memories are composed of multiple elements, and different mnemonic aspects of the same experience may be uniquely affected by mechanisms that retroactively modulate a weakly encoded memory. Here, we investigated how emotional learning affects item and source memory for related events encoded close in time. Participants encoded trial-unique category exemplars before, during, and after Pavlovian fear conditioning. Selective retroactive enhancements in 24-h item memory were accompanied by a bias to misattribute items to the temporal context of fear conditioning. The strength of this source memory bias correlated with participants’ retroactive item memory enhancement, and source misattribution to the emotional context predicted whether items were remembered overall. In the framework of behavioral tagging: Memory attribution was biased to the temporal context of the stronger event that provided the putative source of memory stabilization for the weaker event. We additionally found that fear conditioning selectively and retroactively enhanced stimulus typicality ratings for related items, and that stimulus typicality also predicted overall item memory. Collectively, these results provide new evidence that items related to emotional learning are misattributed to the temporal context of the emotional event and judged to be more representative of their semantic category. Both processes may facilitate memory retrieval for related events encoded close in time.
Emotional experiences gain privileged access to the neurobehavioral mechanisms of long-term memory (LaBar and Cabeza 2006; Kensinger 2009; McGaugh 2015; Yonelinas and Ritchey 2015; Mather et al. 2016). Importantly, this emotional enhancement of memory can spread to seemingly mundane details encoded close in time to the emotional experience. Through this temporal association, affectively neutral information retroactively acquires the capacity to predict emotional events, allowing us to better avoid or seek out those outcomes. However, what aspects of memory are modulated via temporal proximity to an emotional event? Episodic memories, for instance, are composed of stimulus information (e.g., item memory) embedded with contextual details (e.g., source memory) (Johnson et al. 1993; Tulving 2002). Emotion enhances item memory (Sharot and Phelps 2004; LaBar and Cabeza 2006; Mather 2007) but has inconsistent effects on contextual details associated with emotional stimuli (Yonelinas and Ritchey 2015). While there is evidence that episodic memory is selectively prioritized for related information encoded before (Dunsmoor et al. 2015) and after (Dunsmoor et al. 2015, 2018; Tambini et al. 2017; Keller and Dunsmoor 2020) an emotional experience, retroactive and proactive effects of emotional learning on source memory for this same information is unknown. Here, we investigated how temporal proximity to an emotional learning event influences both item memory and contextual details for related information.
Enhancement in memory via a temporal association between mundane and salient events is consistent with neurobiological models of long-term memory. For example, the behavioral tagging hypothesis (derived from the synaptic tagging hypothesis; Frey and Morris 1997) proposes that weak learning is strengthened in memory if it is encoded within a critical time window of a more salient event and if the two events share overlapping neural ensembles (Moncada and Viola 2007; Ballarini et al. 2009; Wang et al. 2010; Takeuchi et al. 2016). As emotion is a powerful learning event, information encoded within temporal proximity may be strengthened in memory via a mechanism of behavioral tagging. The behavioral tagging hypothesis has been translated to humans using novelty (Fenker et al. 2008; Ballarini et al. 2013; Ramirez Butavand et al. 2020), threat (Dunsmoor et al. 2015), and reward (Patil et al. 2017) to induce memory enhancements for weakly encoded information encoded close in time. However, it is unclear whether and how this mechanism may affect memory for the contextual details associated with the weak event.
Emotion can sometimes improve memory accuracy for information directly associated with an emotional stimulus, such as the spatial and temporal context in which the emotional stimulus was encoded (Kensinger and Schacter 2006; Schmidt et al. 2011; Rimmele et al. 2012; Talmi et al. 2019). Therefore, one possibility is that emotional learning improves both item and source memory for information in temporal proximity to an emotional event. In this case, we might expect item memory to be accompanied by source memory accuracy, such that items are appropriately organized in time relative to the emotional event. Alternatively, emotional learning might have no effect (Sharot and Yonelinas 2008; Wang and Fu 2010) or even impair memory for contextual information. Indeed, it may be simpler to remember a trivial item by virtue of associating the item with the salient context (Takashima et al. 2016). It is therefore plausible that linking weak and strong events by temporal proximity might improve item memory at the cost of source memory. Consequently, we may expect a relationship between the strength of item memory for related events encoded close in time and the strength of a misallocation bias to source the item to the more salient temporal context. This might suggest that retroactive memory enhancement for weakly encoded items relies in part on misattribution to the more salient emotional context.
In addition to our exploration of how emotional learning impacts item and source memory for proximal events, we tested a parallel hypothesis that emotional learning alters abstract stimulus properties that may indirectly facilitate memory retrieval for those items as well. Previous work demonstrates that emotional learning is sensitive to how well an item represents its broader category; that is, typicality (Dunsmoor and Murphy 2014, 2015; Dunsmoor et al. 2014; Struyf et al. 2018; Lei et al. 2019). Whether emotional learning has the power to alter an abstract stimulus property such as subjective typicality is unknown, but stimulus memorability may be an underappreciated factor contributing to item memory (Bainbridge 2019). For instance, the hippocampus plays a role in both episodic memory and concept representations (Quiroga 2012; Davis and Poldrack 2014; Mack et al. 2016). Moreover, typical category members used as conditioned stimuli in Pavlovian fear conditioning preferentially engage the hippocampus and hippocampal-amygdala functional connectivity (Dunsmoor et al. 2014), which is a substrate for emotional memory enhancement (Murty et al. 2010).
In the present study participants underwent a 2-d Pavlovian fear conditioning task that included trial-unique (i.e., nonrepeating) pictures of animals and tools as conditioned stimuli (CSs), based on the protocol from Dunsmoor et al. (2015). Items were encoded before, during, and after Pavlovian fear conditioning. We predicted that emotional learning would have divergent effects on 24-h episodic memory accuracy for CS items and the encoding temporal context. Specifically, we predicted that CSs semantically related to the fear conditioned category would be selectively remembered regardless of their temporal context (Dunsmoor et al. 2015), but that participants would have a bias to attribute these related CSs to the temporal context of fear conditioning. We also investigated whether emotional learning enhances subjective typicality for CS category members related to the fear conditioning category, and whether long-term memory would be influenced by how strongly an item was deemed to represent its superordinate category. Such findings might indicate that weak memories formed in temporal proximity to an emotional experience our bound to the more salient temporal context, and that emotional learning can alter abstract stimulus properties of weakly encoded information to make this information more memorable.
Results
Encoding on day 1 included three phases: preconditioning, fear conditioning, and extinction. Each trial was a trial-unique basic-level exemplar of a different animal or a tool. Prior to fear conditioning (preconditioning), subjects simply categorized the object as an animal or tool. During fear conditioning, participants learned through experience that exemplars from one category (CS+, animals or tools, counterbalanced between participants) were paired with an aversive electrical shock unconditioned stimulus (US) to fingers of the left hand (50% CS–US pairing rate). Exemplars from the other category (CS−, tools or animals, respectively) were never paired with shock. Following fear conditioning, subjects viewed additional pictures of animals and tools, but no more shocks were delivered; that is, extinction. Each encoding phase was separated by a short (<5-min) break. The following day (∼24 h later), participants underwent a recognition memory test that included each “old” exemplar plus “new” pictures of animals and tools. The recognition memory test was then followed by a separate source memory test that asked participants to place each item encoded the previous day into the context in which it was presented (three-alternative forced choice [AFC]; before, during, or after fear conditioning). Participants also rated the typicality for each basic-level exemplar in relation to its superordinate category (animal or tool) (see the Materials and Methods for further details on the experimental design).
Twenty-four-hour recognition memory
In order to determine the effect of an emotional event on memory for temporally related items, we collected 24-h delayed recognition memory for items encoded before, during and after fear conditioning. Analysis of recognition memory performance revealed better memory for items from the CS+ compared with the CS− category for all temporal contexts (Fig. 1). Memory performance was calculated as corrected recognition (high confidence hits minus high confidence false alarms). The false alarm rate did not differ between CS+ and CS− (two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test P = 0.13, CLES = 0.55) (see Supplemental Tables 1, 2 for full recognition memory results). A repeated measures ANOVA with CS Type (CS+ or CS−) and Temporal Context (preconditioning, conditioning, or postconditioning) revealed a main effect of CS (F(1,44) = 28.66, Pperm = 0.0001, η2 G = 0.073) and Context (F(2,88) = 19.72, Pperm = 0.0001, η2G = 0.092), as well as a CS by Context interaction (F(2,88) = 9.04, Pperm = 0.0002, η2 G = 0.020). Follow-up planned comparisons showed enhanced memory for CS+ items encoded during fear conditioning (two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test P = 5 × 10−6, CLES = 0.72). This selective enhancement in memory was retroactive for CS+ compared with CS− items encoded during preconditioning (P = 0.0055, CLES = 0.59), and proactive for CS+ compared with CS− items encoded postconditioning (P = 0.0053, CLES = 0.61). Thus, recognition memory results replicate previous findings that Pavlovian conditioning enhances memory for CS+ trials encoded before, during, and after fear conditioning (Dunsmoor et al. 2015).
Temporal context memory
In order to compare the effects of an emotional event on item versus context memory, we collected source temporal context judgements for each item. Source memory judgements for the temporal context (preconditioning, fear conditioning, and postconditioning) associated with each CS from encoding were calculated as the proportion of items per CS type attributed to each context as a function of when the item was encoded (Fig. 2). A three-way repeated measures ANOVA with CS Type, Temporal Context, and Source Memory Response revealed a significant main effect of Source Memory Response (F(2,66) = 10.24, Pperm = 0.0004, η2 G = 0.12), an interaction of CS Type and Source Memory Response (F(2,66) = 17.87, Pperm = 0.0001, η2 G = 0.13), an interaction of Temporal Context and Source Memory Response (F(4,132) = 6.09, Pperm = 0.0005, η2 G = 0.027), and a significant three-way interaction (F(4,132) = 3.15, Pperm = 0.0154, η2 G = 0.013). Only CS+ items encoded during fear conditioning and CS− items encoded during postconditioning (i.e., extinction) were, on average, attributed to their appropriate temporal context. Significance was determined by testing the mean response proportion against chance (33%) using a bootstrap resampling procedure (Davison and Hinkley 1999). There was a bias to misattribute CS+ items from each encoding phase to the fear conditioning context regardless of veracity (all mean proportions greater than chance; see Supplemental Tables 3 and 6 for full temporal context memory results and recognition memory by temporal context memory response). This bias was selective to the CS+ versus CS− for preconditioning (two-sided Wilcoxon signed-rank test P = 7.07 × 10−4, CLES = 0.68), fear conditioning (P = 1.21 × 10−4, CLES = 0.80) and postconditioning (P = 0.0017, CLES = 0.74).
Association between selective memory enhancements and source memory bias
For each phase of encoding, there was a bias to recognize more CS+ than CS− items as well as a bias to report CS+ items as having been encoded during fear conditioning regardless of when they were encountered. One possibility is that there is an association between these biases in item and source memory. Specifically, participants who exhibit a strong memory selectivity for CS+ items encoded before, during, and after fear conditioning may also be biased in attributing these items to the fear conditioning temporal context. To explore this possibility, we examined the correlation between each participant's recognition memory bias (i.e., CS+ minus CS− corrected recognition from each phase) (data shown in Fig. 1) against the fear conditioning temporal context bias (i.e., the bias to attribute CS+ minus CS− items to the temporal context of fear conditioning) (data shown in Fig. 2). We observed a significant robust Pearson's correlation for preconditioning (Fig. 3) (rskipped = 0.34, 95% CI = [0.07, 0.59], P = 0.0188), fear conditioning (rskipped = 0.36, 95% CI = [0.06, 0.61], P = 0.0194), and postconditioning (rskipped = 0.37, 95% CI = [0.03, 0.66], P = 0.0304). This suggests that selective recognition memory for CS+ items at each stage of encoding, including before and after fear conditioning, was related to the tendency to selectively misattribute CS+ items to the more salient temporal context.
Item recognition is influenced by temporal context judgements
To further test our hypothesis that neutral items are remembered better if they are misattributed to the emotional context, we conducted a multiple logistic regression to predict recognition memory as a function of the item's source memory judgement (Fig. 4). Each regression was run as a fixed-effect analysis across all participants, and a bootstrap resampling procedure was used to test for generalizability. The significance of the logistic regression coefficients was determined by testing the obtained bootstrap distribution against zero. Results showed that for all CS+ and CS− items encoded before, during, and after fear conditioning, attributing items to the fear conditioning context was positively linked to accurate recognition memory (all “Conditioning” coefficients >0) (see Supplemental Table 4 for full results). In contrast, attributing CS+ or CS− items to the postconditioning (extinction) context was negatively linked to recognition memory, indicating items were more likely to be forgotten. This pattern of attributing forgotten items to the extinction context was observed for items encoded in each phase. These results show that remembered items were more likely to be judged as having been encoded during fear conditioning, regardless of CS type and regardless of when they were encoded.
Emotional learning alters perceived typicality
At the conclusion of the experiment on day 2, participants rated the typicality of each item encoded the previous day to determine how representative each exemplar was to its category, animal or tool (Fig. 5A). A CS Type by Temporal Context repeated measures ANOVA revealed a main effect of CS (F(1,33) = 6.25, P = 0.018, η2 G = 0.043). There was no main effect of Temporal Context, nor was there a CS by Context interaction (Ps > 0.72). Follow up comparison revealed that CS+s were rated as more typical than CS−s for preconditioning (two-sided paired t(33) = 2.12, P = 0.041, d = 0.36), fear conditioning (t(33) = 2.22, P = 0.033, d = 0.43), and postconditioning (t(33) = 2.40, P = 0.022, d = 0.47). These data suggest that fear conditioning alters the perceived typicality of basic-level category exemplars to their superordinate category (see Supplemental Figure 1 for a stimulus-wise comparison of the difference in typicality between CS+ and CS−). The category (animal or tool) that was used as the CS+ during fear conditioning was counterbalanced across participants. One possibility is that the typicality effect was influenced by which category was paired with shock. In order to rule out an effect of which category was used as the CS+, a repeated measures ANOVA was run with CS type as a within-subjects factor, and shocked category as a between-subjects factor. There was no significant main effect or interaction of shocked category (Ps > 0.15); thus, the effect of emotional learning on perceived typicality was not different in animals versus tools. These data provide new evidence that fear conditioning alters the perceived typicality of category exemplars associated with an aversive outcome.
Typical is memorable
Given our secondary hypothesis regarding a potential role of typicality on selective episodic memory enhancements, we assessed whether perceived stimulus typicality was related to item recognition memory. For each CS type in each temporal context, a logistic regression was run using typicality ratings (1–7) to predict high confidence recognition memory hits (Fig. 5B). Typicality was predictive of recognition memory for both CS+ and CS− in all temporal contexts (all Ps < 0.057) (see Supplemental Table 5 for full results). There was no difference in the strength of this relationship between CS+ and CS− in any phase (Ps > 0.30). These results indicate that items that were perceived as more typical were also more likely to be remembered, but this relationship was not affected by CS type nor encoding temporal context.
Typicality varies with temporal context source judgements
As an exploratory analysis, we investigated whether there were differences in perceived typicality based on responses during the temporal context memory test. Since some participants lacked certain response types, we used a linear mixed effects model, and focused our analysis on main effects and interactions of Source Memory Response (see the Materials and Methods for details). Interestingly, there was a main effect of Source Memory Response (χ2 = 49.3, P = 1.97 × 10−11). There were no significant interactions of Source Memory Response with CS Type or Temporal Context (Ps > 0.21). Post-hoc pairwise contrasts revealed a pattern of decreasing typicality ratings with temporal context source judgements (Fig. 5C). Specifically, items that were sourced to preconditioning were judged to be more typical than items sourced to conditioning (typicality difference = 0.16, z = 2.40, PFDR = 0.017) and postconditioning (typicality difference = 0.49, z = 6.80, PFDR = 3.09 × 10−11). Additionally, items that were sourced to conditioning were also judged to be more typical than those sourced to postconditioning (typicality difference = 0.33, z = 5.02, PFDR = 7.70 × 10−7). These results indicate that an item's perceived source context also influences how typical participants perceive it to be.
Relative contributions of source memory misattributions and typicality on memory
Our results indicate that fear conditioning enhances recognition memory for related items (Fig. 1). This emotional learning also results in an increase in source memory misattribution to the conditioning context for CS + s (Fig. 2), and an increase in perceived typicality for CS + s (Fig. 5A). These source memory misattributions and typicality ratings are in turn also predictive of recognition memory (Figs. 4, 5B). At the same time, source memory misattributions to postconditioning predict worse recognition memory (Fig. 4). We assessed the relative strength of all four factors (CS type, perceived typicality, conditioning source context misattributions, and postconditioning source context misattributions) on recognition memory. This is of particular interest for items encoded during preconditioning, where these effects emerge retroactively after emotional learning. Multiple logistic regressions were run for each participant that included CS type, conditioning source memory misattributions, and postconditioning source memory misattributions as binary predictors, and typicality ratings as a continuous predictor of recognition memory for all items encoded during preconditioning. Unsurprisingly given our previous results, all four factors were significantly linked to recognition memory (two-tailed t-test against 0, CS type: t(33) = 3.28, P = 0.002, d = 0.56; typicality: t(33) = 2.82, P = 0.008, d = 0.48; conditioning source misattributions: t(33) = 4.04, P = 0.0003, d = 0.69; preconditioning source misattributions t(33) = −3.78, P = 0.0006, d = 0.65). Source memory misattribution to conditioning was a more powerful predictor of recognition memory than typicality (two-tailed paired t-test: t(33) = 2.32, P = 0.027, d = 0.51), but was equally strong as CS type (t(33) = 1.37, P = 0.18, d = 0.29). There was no difference between CS type and typicality (t(33) = 0.95, P = 0.35, d = 0.23). In contrast, source memory misattribution to postconditioning was a significantly stronger predictor of forgetting (i.e., more negative) than misattributions to conditioning (t(33) = −5.80, P = 2 × 10−6, d = 1.34), CS type (t(33) = −4.58, P = 6.30 × 10−5, d = 1.21), and typicality (t(33) = −5.05, P = 1.6 × 10−5, d = 1.14). These results indicate that source memory misattributions to conditioning and postconditioning, perceived typicality, and the effects of conditioning itself (CS type) all have separable contributions to the retroactive enhancement in recognition memory observed in this study.
Discussion
While emotional events are often prioritized in memory, it is far less clear how emotional events affect memory for other information encoded close in time. Here, we provide support for a behavioral tagging mechanism in human episodic memory, whereby emotional learning selectively and retroactively enhanced item memory conceptually related to the emotional event, replicating prior findings (Dunsmoor et al. 2015; Patil et al. 2017). Building on these prior findings, we found that related information encoded before or after fear conditioning is consistently misattributed to the more salient fear conditioning temporal context. Put in terms of a tag-and-capture mechanism (Redondo and Morris 2011): Memory attribution was biased to the temporal context of the strong event that provided the source of memory stabilization for the weak event. We also found new evidence that emotional learning selectively enhances subjective typicality for related exemplars, and that typical items were better remembered overall. Collectively, these findings advance knowledge on how emotional learning affects mnemonic and abstract stimulus representations for information encoded before and after an emotional experience.
While effects of emotional arousal on item memory are fairly consistent (LaBar and Cabeza 2006; Murty et al. 2010; McGaugh 2015), the effect of emotion on memory for contextual details is less straightforward. Contextual details sometimes receive a boost in memory (Schmidt et al. 2011; Rimmele et al. 2012), but other studies find no effect or worse memory for contextual details associated with an emotional item (Sharot and Yonelinas 2008; Wang and Fu 2010; Takashima et al. 2016). The divergent effects of emotion on item and source memory may be related to different temporal dynamics in forgetting between item-emotion binding, supported by the amygdala, and item-context binding, supported by the hippocampus (Yonelinas and Ritchey 2015). Specifically, amygdala-dependent emotional item memory is resistant to forgetting, whereas hippocampus-dependent item-context memory decays at a faster rate. Notably, much of the work on the role of emotional arousal on item versus source memory uses memoranda that have an intrinsically emotionally arousing feature (e.g., evocative images with a contextual detail; but see Wang and Fu 2010). Extending this model to neutral memoranda encoded around the time of emotional learning has interesting implications for interpreting emotional enhancements of episodic memory through the lens of a putative behavioral tagging mechanism. Specifically, it suggests that emotional events act predominately on the amygdala to support retroactive and proactive item memory enhancements for related information. An amygdala-based item memory enhancement would accord with the use of the Pavlovian conditioning design used here. In this way, amygdala-dependent fear conditioning may up-regulate hippocampal processing to support selective consolidation of otherwise mundane items encoded close in time. In an emotional-binding framework (Yonelinas and Ritchey 2015), this retroactive item-emotion memory benefit would not necessarily be accompanied by a retroactive hippocampus-dependent item-context memory benefit for those same weakly encoded items.
One important question is the relative contribution of consolidation versus retrieval processes on the bias to misattribute CS+ items to the temporal context of fear conditioning. That is, is item memory enhanced because the item is bound to the emotional context, or is the item attributed to the emotional context because it is remembered? One possibility is that emotional learning boosted memory for weakly encoded items by linking these memories to the temporal context that provided the source of memory stabilization (Clewett et al. 2020). This explanation is in keeping with a tag-and-capture model, which is primarily a mechanistic account for the consolidation of newly formed memories (Redondo and Morris 2011). This account may afford some explanatory power for the strong correlation between selective item memory enhancements (CS+ > CS−), which our prior work shows is consolidation dependent (Dunsmoor et al. 2015, 2018; Patil et al. 2017), and the bias to misattribute CS+ items to the fear conditioning context.
Alternatively, source memory errors may be a factor of retrieval processes, per se. One possibility is that memory retrieval of a CS+ exemplar may be accompanied by memory retrieval of the salient fear conditioning context. As a consequence, participants may have a bias to attribute more CS+ items to the emotional context because those items are accompanied by a stronger feeling of remembering (Sharot et al. 2004) induced by reactivation of the emotional context. This explanation is consistent with the general properties of source monitoring that involve retrospectively attributing the origin of a memory to a particular source (Johnson et al. 1993), and with mental context reinstatement models of memory retrieval (Tulving and Thomson 1973; Smith and Vela 2001; Howard and Kahana 2002a; Gershman et al. 2013). It is also consistent with a retrieval focused account of emotional memory enhancements (Talmi et al. 2019), wherein emotional item memory is bound to changing representations of mental contexts. Interestingly, there is evidence that distinct phases of a fear conditioning experiment can be understood as separable mental contexts that uniquely organize memory (Dunsmoor et al. 2018; Hennings et al. 2020) in a manner consistent with how shifts in mental context segment experience for distinct episodes (DuBrow et al. 2017; Clewett et al. 2019). That source memory misattributions and CS type had a separable contributions to the retroactive enhancement in item memory (Fig. 6) may also support the importance of a retrieval mechanism. Put together, it is possible that remembering a CS+ item reactivates the mental context associated with the fear conditioning phase, thereby promoting retrieval for related items, including those encoded in temporal proximity before or after the fear conditioning phase.
Interestingly, while attributing items to the conditioning context was associated with better item memory, attributing items to the extinction context was associated with worse item memory. This finding is in keeping with the tenuous nature of extinction as a mnemonic process, such that extinction memories tend to be weaker and fade over time (Rescorla 2004). We have previously found that 24-h recognition memory for CSs encoded during extinction is also relatively weaker than for CSs encoded during fear conditioning (Dunsmoor et al. 2018). Similar to the enhancement in memory for items attributed to the conditioning context, this finding could also be interpreted through the lens of consolidation or retrieval. During consolidation, associating an item with the extinction context (falsely or correctly) following encoding could reduce the chance the item will be stored in long-term memory. Alternatively, subjects could reason that the item was encoded during extinction because they do not recognize it. The forced-choice source memory test used here did not include confidence judgements, which could shed some light on this matter. That is, these source memory response biases for remembered and forgotten items could be affected by how confident subjects are in their judgement. Disentangling a consolidation from retrieval based mechanism, and getting closer to a causal explanation of these effects, motivates further work. We also observed that items sourced to extinction were judged to be less typical. Although this analysis was exploratory, an interesting possibility is that these items are viewed as “exceptions.” Extinction is a secondary inhibitory association that provides an exception to the rule established during conditioning. The decreased perceived typicality of items sourced to the extinction context could be a result of neural processes separating extinction from previous experiences, but again more work is needed.
We also found that items from the CS+ category were rated as more typical than CS− items, regardless of when the item was encoded. One constraint to this experimental design is the inability to gather typicality ratings from each participant prior to the encoding session, as pre-exposing items prior to encoding would necessarily interfere with interpretation of the episodic memory results. Importantly, we showed that there was no inherent difference in typicality as a function of superordinate category (animal or tool), suggesting that fear conditioning itself modulated typicality judgements. These results are new evidence that conditioning can alter an abstract stimulus property like how well an item represents its category. This enhancement in representativeness may enable CS+ items to be better remembered over time, as we also found that typical items were more memorable overall. There is surprisingly little research on the link between stimulus typicality and memorability. However, there is evidence that semantic similarity promotes free recall (Howard and Kahana 2002b), and abundant evidence that the hippocampus plays a role in concept representation and categorization (Quiroga 2012; Davis and Poldrack 2014; Mack et al. 2016). Thus, one mechanistic possibility is that fear conditioning augmented semantic organization by raising the overall typicality of distinct category members from the CS+ category, which in turn facilitated memory for related items encoded close in time. More research is warranted to help elucidate the relationship between typicality and memorability more broadly.
Recent work has made progress on translating neurobiological models of long-term memory to explain memory strengthening for weak memories encoded in temporal proximity to a more salient event. According to the synaptic tagging hypothesis (Frey and Morris 1997; Redondo and Morris 2011), and its behavioral counterpart (Moncada and Viola 2007), weak activation that is only sufficient to produce a transient short-term memory can be transformed into a durable long-term memory if accompanied by a more salient event that relies on the same neural system within a critical time window. The weak event sets a putative learning tag that is stabilized by release of plasticity related proteins induced by the strong event, broadening the window of time by which salient events can modulate long-term memory. However, memories are multifaceted, and how a putative behavioral tagging mechanism impacts separate mnemonic aspects of the same experience, or other abstract stimulus properties, is not well understood. Matching the task demands of episodic memory and Pavlovian conditioning allowed us to show that an emotional event has divergent effects on item and source memory and enhances subjective typicality of related events encoded close in time. Mechanisms that link seemingly inconsequential information to a future or past emotional event can serve an adaptive function. For instance, we do not always know the significance of numerous details we encounter throughout the day. By integrating memories encoded close in time to a meaningful event, we can remember information that may be relevant to seeking out or avoiding similar outcomes in the future. Enhancing the representativeness of the experience may serve a similar mnemonic function. However, linking memories by temporal proximity may come at the cost of misattributing nonemotional memories to an emotional context and over interpreting the representativeness of information related to an emotional event. Such an organization proves maladaptive when innocuous cues experienced around the time of highly negative events trigger retrieval of unwanted emotional memories. Thus, as a consequence of organizing memories based on temporal proximity to a salient event, a host of potentially irrelevant and innocuous information acquires the capacity to reactivate emotional memories in the future.
Materials and Methods
Participants
This experiment was approved by the University of Texas at Austin Institutional Review Board (IRB; 2017-02-0094). A total of 48 adult volunteers (mean age 24 yr old, SD 4.5 yr; 32 female) were recruited and completed a 2-d functional MRI investigation. An additional three participants were recruited but did not complete the study. A primary aim of the neuroimaging component of the investigation was to examine the neural correlates of fear extinction memory encoding and retrieval. As such, fMRI and behavioral data pertaining to fear conditioning and extinction are reported in Hennings et al. (2020). We recruited participants who were psychologically healthy (N = 24) as well as participants who reported PTSD symptoms (N = 24). For the purposes of the memory analyses, and given no a priori hypotheses regarding episodic memory results between these groups, we combined participants for this report. We confirmed highly similar results in both groups; specifically, in the analysis of 24-h recognition memory (Fig. 1) a mixed ANOVA revealed no main effect or interaction with Group (Healthy vs. PTSD symptoms) (see the Supplemental Material). Three additional participants were removed from analysis due to extremely poor memory performance that was near chance (N = 1), or a failure to make a response during the recognition memory test on >10% of trials (N = 2). Finally, 11 participants were unable to complete the source memory test due to time constraints at the MRI facility, and hence data from these participants were not included in the temporal context memory and typicality results. After exclusions, our sample was thus N = 45 participants for the analysis of recognition memory (23 PTSS), and N = 34 participants for the analyses of temporal context memory and typicality (17 PTSS).
Conditioned and unconditioned stimulus
Memoranda (i.e., CSs) included color photographs of animals and tools presented on a white background, obtained from http://www.lifeonwhite.com or publicly available resources on the internet. Each CS was a distinct basic-level exemplar with a unique name; in other words, there were not two different pictures of a bear throughout the entire experiment. The order of stimuli was counterbalanced and pseudorandomized such that no more than three pictures from the same category occurred in a row. Stimulus presentation was controlled using E-Prime 3 (https://pstnet.com/products/e-prime). The US was a 50-msec electrical shock delivered to the index and middle fingers of the left hand. The US was calibrated prior to the start of the experiment to reach a level deemed “highly annoying but not painful.” The shock was controlled using the STMEPM-MRI stimulation system from BIOPAC Systems.
Psychophysiology
Task design
The paradigm was based in large part on Dunsmoor et al. (2015). Day 1 included preconditioning, fear conditioning, and postconditioning (i.e., extinction). Each phase included 24 CS+ and 24 CS− trials, for a total of 144 trials on day 1. A short (<5-min) break was interspersed between each encoding phase. During preconditioning, participants viewed pictures of animals and tools and made a 2-AFC category judgement (animal, tool) using a button box in their right hand. During fear conditioning, 50% of the images from one category (CS+) trials coterminated with the US, and participants rated shock expectancy (yes/no). CS+ category was counterbalanced across participants. During postconditioning, participants continued to rate shock expectancy but no shocks were delivered (i.e., extinction). CS duration in all phases on day 1 was 4.5 sec ± 0.5 sec (jittered) followed by an intertrial interval of 6 sec ± 1 sec. During preconditioning and fear conditioning, the intertrial interval was a blank background with a fixation cross. For reasons detailed in Hennings et al. (2020), postconditioning (i.e., extinction) included a rapidly presented series of scene pictures (five, six, or seven pictures presented for 1 sec each) during the intertrial interval. The rationale for presenting scene pictures during the ITI was for multivariate pattern analysis of the extinction context, but is not related to the purposes of the episodic memory data reported here. The same collection of pictures were used for each subject during encoding, but the trial order was counterbalanced between subjects. Participants returned ∼24 h later and underwent a test of fear renewal (data reported in Hennings et al. 2020), and a surprise recognition memory test inside the scanner. The recognition memory test included 144 old pictures from the previous day and 96 new pictures of animals and tools. Participants had 3 sec to rate each item as old or new and their level of confidence (definitely old, maybe old, maybe new, and definitely new). Trial order was pseudorandomized to ensure a balance of old and new stimuli from each category from each encoding phase.
Finally, participants completed the source memory test outside the scanner in a different room. The source memory test included only old images from day 1, and participants were informed that each item they saw had been seen the previous day. Participants were reminded that the experiment had been divided into three different phases (labeled phases 1, 2, and 3), and were given a brief reminder of each phase (i.e., “phase 1 was at the beginning when you were classifying each image as an animal or a tool”). Participants were instructed to respond to their best of the ability when they had seen each picture.
After the source memory instructions, participants were instructed on how to rate the typicality of each image on a seven-point scale (1 = Not at all typical; 7 = Very typical). An example of typicality was given using an unrelated category: “For example, an apple is a typical fruit, it is representative of the entire fruit category. On the other hand a dragonfruit is not a typical fruit, at least in American grocery stores.” The task was self-paced, with a brief 500-msec or 800-msec, randomly chosen, ITI after participants had answered both questions for each picture.
Statistical analyses
Behavioral data was analyzed using a combination of Python and R. Corrected recognition and temporal context memory were determined to be nonnormally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk test Ps < 0.05), and thus were analyzed with nonparametric statistics (Shapiro and Wilk 1965). When mentioned, the number of permutations or bootstrap iterations was set to 10,000, which allows for a minimum empirical P-value of 0.0001. All data were first submitted to an ANOVA using the R package ez (http://github.com/mike-lawrence/ez), and the package permuco was used to permute ANOVAs for nonnormal data (https://github.com/cran/permuco). ANOVA effect sizes are reported as generalized η2 (Bakeman 2005), the parametric effect size is reported for nonnormal data for clarity. Student's t-tests or Wilcoxon sign-ranked tests were used for follow up comparisons using the Python package pingouin (Vallat 2018). Effect size for t-test is reported as Cohen's d, and the effect size for Wilcoxon sign-ranked tests is the common language effect size (CLES) (Vargha and Delaney 2000).
For the analysis correlating the emotional bias in temporal context memory to the emotional bias in recognition memory, a robust skipped-Pearson's correlation was used for nonnormal data (Pernet et al. 2013). After the removal of any outliers, the data were resampled with replacement and significance was determined by testing the obtained distribution of Pearson's r values against 0. Logistic regressions analyses were performed using the Python package scikit-learn (Pedregosa et al. 2012) with the lbfgs solver and L2 regularization. For the regression using source memory to predict recognition memory hits, 3-AFC temporal context memory responses for each CS type in each temporal context were transformed into a one-hot encoding scheme (binary coding across three columns, one for each possible 3-AFC response) and used to predict high-confidence recognition memory hits. In order to overcome a limited number of certain trial types in some participants (e.g., a participant with few or no preconditioning CS+ items accompanied by a source memory attribution to the “postconditioning” phase), each logistic regression was run as a fixed-effect analysis across all participants. To test for generalizability, we implemented a bootstrapping procedure in which whole participants were sampled with replacement on each iteration. Significance of regression coefficients was determined by testing the obtained distribution against 0. The same fixed-effect bootstrap procedure was used in the regression using typicality to predict recognition memory. Several experimental design features, such as counterbalancing CS category and encoding context, serve to normalize observations across trials and participants for these fixed-effects analyses. We also note that the same underlying assumption made in traditional hypothesis testing (i.e., that the sample is a good representation of the population) is made here as well.
For the analysis of typicality by temporal context memory responses, a linear mixed effects model was implemented using the afex package in R (http://cran.r-project.org/package=afex). Experimental variables were entered as fixed effects, and a random intercept of participant was included. The full model specification was as follows: Typicality ∼ CS Type * Temporal Context * Source Memory Response + (1|subject). Note that main effects and interactions are implicitly coded in the model. The significance of main effects and interactions was evaluated using χ2 tests, comparing the log likelihood of models with and without the term of interest. Post-hoc tests of the marginal means in the model were completed using the emmeans package in R (https://cran.r-project.org/package=emmeans). Asymptotic degrees of freedom were used as the number of total observations in the model was quite large (4896 total trials), and false discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied to the P-values.
Data Deposition
All deidentified behavioral data are available in an Open Science Framework repository (doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/QEG83).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We thank Mason McClay for support with data collection and participant scheduling. This work was supported by National Science Foundation Career 1844792 and National Institutes of Health R00MH106719 to J.E.D.
Footnotes
[Supplemental material is available for this article.]
Article is online at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.053371.120.
References
- Bainbridge WA. 2019. Memorability: how what we see influences what we remember. In Psychology of learning and motivation (ed. Federmeier KD, Beck DM), pp. 1–27. Academic Press. 10.1016/bs.plm.2019.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bakeman R. 2005. Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs. Behav Res Methods 37: 379–384. 10.3758/BF03192707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballarini F, Moncada D, Martinez MC, Alen N, Viola H. 2009. Behavioral tagging is a general mechanism of long-term memory formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106: 14599–14604. 10.1073/pnas.0907078106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballarini F, Martínez MC, Díaz Perez M, Moncada D, Viola H. 2013. Memory in elementary school children is improved by an unrelated novel experience. PLoS One 8: 66875. 10.1371/journal.pone.0066875 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clewett D, DuBrow S, Davachi L. 2019. Transcending time in the brain: how event memories are constructed from experience. Hippocampus 29: 162–183. 10.1002/hipo.23074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clewett D, Dunsmoor JE, Bachman S, Phelps E, Davachi L. 2020. Survival of the salient: emotion rescues otherwise forgettable memories via neural reactivation and post-encoding hippocampal connectivity. bioRxiv 10.1101/2020.07.07.192252 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis T, Poldrack RA. 2014. Quantifying the internal structure of categories using a neural typicality measure. Cereb Cortex 24: 1720–1737. 10.1093/cercor/bht014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davison A, Hinkley D. 1999. Bootstrap methods and their application. Cambridge University Press, New York. [Google Scholar]
- DuBrow S, Rouhani N, Niv Y, Norman KA. 2017. Does mental context drift or shift? Curr Opin Behav Sci 17: 141–146. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.08.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Murphy GL. 2014. Stimulus typicality determines how broadly fear is generalized. Psychol Sci 25: 1816–1821. 10.1177/0956797614535401 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Murphy GL. 2015. Categories, concepts, and conditioning: how humans generalize fear. Trends Cogn Sci 19: 73–77. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Kragel PA, Martin A, La Bar KS. 2014. Aversive learning modulates cortical representations of object categories. Cereb Cortex 24: 2859–2872. 10.1093/cercor/bht138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Murty VP, Davachi L, Phelps EA. 2015. Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events. Nature 520: 345–348. 10.1038/nature14106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Kroes MCW, Moscatelli CM, Evans MD, Davachi L, Phelps EA. 2018. Event segmentation protects emotional memories from competing experiences encoded close in time. Nat Hum Behav 2: 291–299. 10.1038/s41562-018-0317-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fenker DB, Frey JU, Schuetze H, Heipertz D, Heinze HJ, Duzel E. 2008. Novel scenes improve recollection and recall of words. J Cogn Neurosci 20: 1250–1265. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frey U, Morris RGM. 1997. Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature 385: 533–536. 10.1038/385533a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gershman SJ, Schapiro AC, Hupbach A, Norman KA. 2013. Neural context reinstatement predicts memory misattribution. J Neurosci 33: 8590–8595. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0096-13.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hennings AC, McClay M, Lewis-Peacock JA, Dunsmoor JE. 2020. Contextual reinstatement promotes extinction generalization in healthy adults but not PTSD. Neuropsychologia 147: 107573. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Howard MW, Kahana MJ. 2002a. A distributed representation of temporal context. J Math Psychol 46: 269–299. 10.1006/jmps.2001.1388 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Howard MW, Kahana MJ. 2002b. When does semantic similarity help episodic retrieval? J Mem Lang 46: 85–98. 10.1006/jmla.2001.2798 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S, Lindsay DS. 1993. Source monitoring. Psychol Bull 114: 3–28. 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller NE, Dunsmoor JE. 2020. The effects of aversive-to-appetitive counterconditioning on implicit and explicit fear memory. Learn Mem 27: 12–19. 10.1101/lm.050740.119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger EA. 2009. Remembering the details: effects of emotion. Emot Rev 1: 99–113. 10.1177/1754073908100432 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kensinger EA, Schacter DL. 2006. Reality monitoring and memory distortion: effects of negative, arousing content. Mem Cogn 34: 251–260. 10.3758/BF03193403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- LaBar KS, Cabeza R. 2006. Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 7: 54–64. 10.1038/nrn1825 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lei Y, Wang J, Dou H, Qiu Y, Li H. 2019. Influence of typicality in category-based fear generalization: diverging evidence from the P2 and N400 effect. Int J Psychophysiol 135: 12–20. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2018.11.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mack ML, Love BC, Preston AR. 2016. Dynamic updating of hippocampal object representations reflects new conceptual knowledge. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113: 13203–13208. 10.1073/pnas.1614048113 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mather M. 2007. Emotional arousal and memory binding: an object-based framework. Perspect Psychol Sci 2: 33–52. 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00028.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mather M, Clewett D, Sakaki M, Harley CW. 2016. Norepinephrine ignites local hotspots of neuronal excitation: how arousal amplifies selectivity in perception and memory. Behav Brain Sci 39: e200. 10.1017/S0140525X15000667 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGaugh JL. 2015. Consolidating memories. Annu Rev Psychol 66: 1–24. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-014954 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moncada D, Viola H. 2007. Induction of long-term memory by exposure to novelty requires protein synthesis: evidence for a behavioral tagging. J Neurosci 27: 7476–7481. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1083-07.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murty VP, Ritchey M, Adcock RA, LaBar KS. 2010. FMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 48: 3459–3469. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.030 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patil A, Murty VP, Dunsmoor JE, Phelps EA, Davachi L. 2017. Reward retroactively enhances memory consolidation for related items. Learn Mem 24: 65–69. 10.1101/lm.042978.116 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Michel V, Thirion B, Grisel O, Blondel M, Müller A, Nothman J, Louppe G, et al. 2012. Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J Mach Learn Res 12: 2825–2830. [Google Scholar]
- Pernet CR, Wilcox R, Rousselet GA. 2013. Robust correlation analyses: false positive and power validation using a new open source matlab toolbox. Front Psychol 3: 606. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00606 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quiroga RQ. 2012. Concept cells: the building blocks of declarative memory functions. Nat Rev Neurosci 13: 587–597. 10.1038/nrn3251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez Butavand D, Hirsch I, Tomaiuolo M, Moncada D, Viola H, Ballarini F. 2020. Novelty improves the formation and persistence of memory in a naturalistic school scenario. Front Psychol 11: 48. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redondo RL, Morris RGM. 2011. Making memories last: the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Nat Rev Neurosci 12: 17–30. 10.1038/nrn2963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 2004. Spontaneous recovery. Learn Mem 11: 501–509. 10.1101/lm.77504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rimmele U, Davachi L, Phelps EA. 2012. Memory for time and place contributes to enhanced confidence in memories for emotional events. Emotion 12: 834–846. 10.1037/a0028003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt K, Patnaik P, Kensinger EA. 2011. Emotion's influence on memory for spatial and temporal context. Cogn Emot 25: 229–243. 10.1080/02699931.2010.483123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro SS, Wilk MB. 1965. An analysis of variance test for normality (Complete Samples). Biometrika 52: 591. 10.1093/biomet/52.3-4.591 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sharot T, Phelps EA. 2004. How arousal modulates memory: disentangling the effects of attention and retention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 4: 294–306. 10.3758/CABN.4.3.294 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharot T, Yonelinas AP. 2008. Differential time-dependent effects of emotion on recollective experience and memory for contextual information. Cognition 106: 538–547. 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharot T, Delgado MR, Phelps EA. 2004. How emotion enhances the feeling of remembering. Nat Neurosci 7: 1376–1380. 10.1038/nn1353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SM, Vela E. 2001. Environmental context-dependent memory: a review and meta-analysis. Psychon Bull Rev 8: 203–220. 10.3758/BF03196157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Struyf D, Hermans D, Vervliet B. 2018. Maximizing the generalization of fear extinction: exposures to a peak generalization stimulus. Behav Res Ther 111: 1–8. 10.1016/j.brat.2018.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takashima A, van der Ven F, Kroes MCW, Fernández G. 2016. Retrieved emotional context influences hippocampal involvement during recognition of neutral memories. Neuroimage 143: 280–292. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi T, Duszkiewicz AJ, Sonneborn A, Spooner PA, Yamasaki M, Watanabe M, Smith CC, Fernández G, Deisseroth K, Greene RW, et al. 2016. Locus coeruleus and dopaminergic consolidation of everyday memory. Nature 537: 357–362. 10.1038/nature19325 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talmi D, Lohnas LJ, Daw ND. 2019. A retrieved context model of the emotional modulation of memory. Psychol Rev 126: 455–485. 10.1037/rev0000132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tambini A, Rimmele U, Phelps EA, Davachi L. 2017. Emotional brain states carry over and enhance future memory formation. Nat Neurosci 20: 271–278. 10.1038/nn.4468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E. 2002. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev Psychol 53: 1–25. 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tulving E, Thomson DM. 1973. Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychol Rev 80: 352–373. 10.1037/h0020071 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vallat R. 2018. Pingouin: statistics in Python. J Open Source Softw 3: 1026. 10.21105/joss.01026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vargha A, Delaney HD. 2000. A critique and improvement of the ‘CL’ common language effect size statistics of McGraw and Wong. J Educ Behav Stat 25: 101. 10.3102/10769986025002101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wang B, Fu X. 2010. Gender differences in the effects of post-learning emotion on consolidation of item memory and source memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem 93: 572–580. 10.1016/j.nlm.2010.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang SH, Redondo RL, Morris RGM. 2010. Relevance of synaptic tagging and capture to the persistence of long-term potentiation and everyday spatial memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107: 19537–19542. 10.1073/pnas.1008638107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yonelinas AP, Ritchey M. 2015. The slow forgetting of emotional episodic memories: an emotional binding account. Trends Cogn Sci 19: 259–267. 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.