Abstract
We measured P300, an event-related potential, in subjects with at-risk mental states (ARMS) and aimed to determine whether P300 parameter can predict progression to overt schizophrenia. Thirty-three subjects with ARMS, 39 with schizophrenia, and 28 healthy controls participated in the study. All subjects were antipsychotic-free. Subjects with ARMS were followed-up for more than two years. Cognitive function was measured by the Brief assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) and Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale (SCoRS), while the modified Global Assessment of Functioning (mGAF) was used to assess global function. Patients with schizophrenia showed smaller P300 amplitudes and prolonged latency at Pz compared to those of healthy controls and subjects with ARMS. During the follow-up period, eight out of 33 subjects with ARMS developed overt psychosis (ARMS-P) while 25 did not (ARMS-NP). P300 latency of ARMS-P was significantly longer than that of ARMS-NP. At baseline, ARMS-P elicited worse cognitive functions, as measured by the BACS and SCoRS compared to ARMS-NP. We also detected a significant relationship between P300 amplitudes and mGAF scores in ARMS subjects. Our results suggest the usefulness of prolonged P300 latency and cognitive impairment as a predictive marker of later development of schizophrenia in vulnerable individuals.
Keywords: P300, event-related potentials, clinical high-risk, psychosis, at risk mental state, schizophrenia, cognition, latency
1. Introduction
Patients with schizophrenia suffer from impairments of several types of cognitive functions which are considered to affect quality of life (QOL) and social functions [1]. Therefore, early detection and intervention into cognitive disturbances of schizophrenia are needed to achieve a satisfactory outcome for patients. For the same reason, the im-portance of intervention into the prodromal stage of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders has also been recognized [2,3,4]. For this purpose, operational criteria to detect putative prodromal symptoms of psychosis have been used worldwide [5], and are designated as ultra-high risk, clinical high risk, or at-risk mental state (ARMS). These criteria allow for the identification of subjects with ~30% risk of developing psychosis over a two-year period [6], mostly schizophrenia-spectrum disorders [7]. For better prediction, the use of objective biomarkers, such as those based on brain morphology, neurophysiology, and neuropsychology, has been proposed [8,9,10,11,12]. For example, event-related potentials (ERPs), such as duration mismatch negativity (MMN) and P300, have been reported to provide sensitive and feasible electrophysiological tools [13,14]. These measures of ERPs have also been suggested to provide biological substrates for some aspects of cognitive disturbances in patients with schizophrenia and ARMS subjects [9,15,16].
P300 was discovered in 1960s and has been shown to reflect a putative electrophysiological basis of cognitive functions, such as attention-dependent information processing and immediate memory [17,18,19], which are impaired in schizophrenia [20,21]. Specifically, patients with schizophrenia have been reported to elicit smaller amplitudes and pro-longed latencies of P300 compared to those in healthy control subjects [22,23,24]. In fact, a recent meta-analysis reported that the effect sizes of amplitude reduction and latency prolongation are as large as 0.83 and 0.48, respectively [25]. As in the case for schizophrenia, abnormalities in P300 have been reported in other neuropsychiatric disorders, such as mood disorders (e.g., bipolar disorder and major depression), developmental disorders (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder), and Parkinson’s disease [26,27,28,29,30,31]. Therefore, P300 may be considered to provide a biomarker of schizophrenia, especially in patients in whom these diseases are excluded.
Potential P300 abnormalities have also been examined in subjects with ARMS [32,33,34,35,36,37,38]. To our knowledge, five studies to date have attempted to determine whether P300 parameters (e.g., amplitude and latency) at baseline can predict later onset of overt psychosis [33,34,35,36,37]. Two studies [34,36], but not others [33,35,37] reported differences in amplitude between ARMS subjects who later developed psychosis (ARMS-P) and those who did not (ARMS-NP). On the other hand, these previous ARMS studies did not find associations between baseline P300 latency and later psychosis onset [33,35,37]. These negative results may contradict the concept that prolonged P300 latency would reflect trait abnormality of schizophrenia irrespective of illness stages, including the prodromal stage [25]. The reasons for these inconsistent findings in ARMS remain unclear but may include the difference in clinical profiles of participants, e.g., medication status that has been reported to alter P300 parameters [9,39,40]. Thus, further investigations to examine the potential ability of P300 latency to predict later onset of psychosis in high-risk subjects would be desired.
In this study, we reported P300 amplitudes and latencies in subjects with antipsychotics-free ARMS or schizophrenia in comparison with healthy control subjects. On the basis of P300 abnormalities as a trait marker of schizophrenia [25], we hypothesized that baseline P300 latency in ARMS subjects would predict onset of psychosis. We also explored whether abnormal P300 parameters would be associated with cognitive and functional deficits in these subjects.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Thirty-three ARMS subjects (male/female, 23/10; mean age 19.2 ± 4.6 years), recruited from University of Toyama Hospital or Toyama Prefectural Mental Health Centre [41], as well as 39 schizophrenia patients (male/female, 16/23; mean age 24.4 ± 7.2 years) participated in this study. Diagnosis was made by experienced psychiatrists based on ICD-10 for schizophrenia and the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental State (CAARMS) for ARMS [42]. We also recruited 28 healthy volunteers (male/female, 16/12; mean age 21.7 ± 5.0 years) from our community. All subjects were physically healthy and had well hearing ability. A psychiatric and treatment history was collected from the subjects themselves, their families, and medical records. Exclusion criteria included the following: subjects with a history of substance abuse or dependence, seizure, and head injury. Eligible patients were confirmed to be physically healthy by physical examinations and standard laboratory tests. Healthy volunteers and their first-degree relatives did not have any psychiatric disorders. Subjects with an estimated premorbid IQ less than 70 were also excluded.
For the clinical assessments, experienced psychiatrists administered the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [43]. The Japanese adult reading test (JART) [44] was performed to estimate premorbid IQ. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) [45,46], Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale (SCoRS) [47,48,49] and modified Global Assessment of Functioning (mGAF) [50] were used to evaluate cognitive and social functions. BACS was standardized by z-scores, with the mean score of Japanese healthy controls set to zero and the standard deviation set to one [51]. Furthermore, the BACS composite score was calculated by averaging the z-scores of the six primary BACS measurements [45]. The demographic data at baseline evaluation for healthy control, ARMS and schizophrenia is shown in Table 1.
Table 1.
H | ARMS | Sch | Effect Size | Group Comparison | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(n = 28) | (n = 33) | (n = 39) | |||
Male/female | 16/12 | 23/10 | 16/23 | - | χ2 = 6.00, p = 0.05, n.s. |
Age (years) | 21.7 (5.0) | 19.2 (4.6) | 24.4 (7.2) | - | F(2,97) = 7.93, p = 0.001 **, ARMS < Sch |
Age of onset (years) | - | - | 24.4 (6.6) | - | - |
Duration of illness (years) | - | - | 2.7 (3.0) | - | - |
JART a | 102.5 (7.4) | 96.9 (10.0) | 99.3 (11.1) | - | F(2,85) = 1.91, p = 0.15, n.s. |
PANSS: positive symptom | - | 13.1 (3.7) | 16.7 (5.8) | - | p = 0.003 **, ARMS < Sch |
negative symptom | - | 19.5 (7.2) | 19.1 (7.4) | - | p = 0.81, n.s. |
general psychopathology | - | 34.9 (8.9) | 36.5 (9.1) | - | p = 0.45, n.s. |
mGAF | - | 46.5 (9.1) | 34.1 (8.6) | - | p < 0.001 **, ARMS > Sch |
SCoRS | - | 4.7 (2.2) | 5.8 (2.3) | - | p = 0.069, n.s. |
BACS a,b: verbal memory | −0.16 (1.1) | −0.54 (1.4) | −1.46 (1.6) | 0.12 | F(2,86) = 5.81, p = 0.004 **, H, ARMS > Sch |
working memory | 0.10 (0.8) | −0.9 (1.3) | −1.32 (1.3) | 0.15 | F(2,86) = 7.99, p = 0.001 **, H > ARMS, Sch |
motor function | −0.15 (1.0) | −1.31 (1.7) | −2.04 (1.5) | 0.18 | F(2,86) = 9.45, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS, Sch |
verbal fluency | 0.096 (1.0) | −0.96 (1.2) | −1.44 (1.3) | 0.19 | F(2,86) = 9.84, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS, Sch |
attention | 0.70 (0.8) | −0.22 (1.5) | −1.60 (1.3) | 0.32 | F(2,86) = 20.97, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS > Sch |
executive function | 0.29 (1.1) | −0.36 (1.3) | −1.47 (1.9) | 0.16 | F(2,86) = 8.64, p < 0.001 **, H, ARMS > Sch |
composite score c | 0.14 (0.5) | −0.72 (1.0) | −1.55 (1.2) | 0.28 | F(2,86) = 17.52, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS > Sch |
P300 Amplitude (µV):T3 | 9.26 (4.5) | 6.12 (3.1) | 5.14 (2.6) | 0.20 | F(2,97) = 11.76, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS, Sch |
T4 | 9.30 (5.5) | 6.50 (4.1) | 4.04 (3.0) | 0.20 | F(2.97) = 12.19, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS, Sch |
Fz | 13.59 (7.2) | 8.46 (6.3) | 4.80 (4.2) | 0.27 | F(2,97) = 17.89, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS > Sch |
Cz | 19.51 (8.6) | 13.08 (6.3) | 7.07 (3.6) | 0.40 | F(2,97) = 32.33, p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS > Sch |
Pz | 20.57 (8.9) | 15.66 (4.9) | 9.4 (3.9) | 0.37 | F(2,97) = 28.39 p < 0.001 **, H > ARMS > Sch |
P300 Latency (msec):T3 | 327.8 (35.9) | 325.5 (32.5) | 347.1 (52.9) | 0.07 | F(2,97) = 2.78, p = 0.067, n.s. |
T4 | 326.3 (29.5) | 320.1 (39.0) | 343.4 (52.6) | 0.06 | F(2,97) = 2.88, 0.061, n.s. |
Fz | 323.6 (30.9) | 322.9 (38.6) | 343.9 (52.7) | 0.05 | F(2,97) = 2.76, p = 0.068, n.s. |
Cz | 314.8 (29.1) | 315.3 (36.1) | 340.5 (52.2) | 0.08 | F(2,97) = 4.43, p = 0.014 *, H, ARMS < Sch |
Pz | 313.5 (29.5) | 314.9 (32.5) | 339.0 (45.7) | 0.10 | F(2,97) = 5.17, p = 0.007 **, H, ARMS < Sch |
Values represent mean (SD). ARMS; at-risk mental state, H; healthy controls, Sch; schizophrenia. JART; Japanese Adult Reading Test, PANSS; Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, mGAF; modified Global Assessment Functioning. P300 data represent peak amplitudes (µV) and latencies (msec) for each group [mean (SD)]. All participants were antipsychotic-free at the time of measurement P300. Demographic differences between groups were examined with one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA), Student’s t-test or the chi-square test (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01). Effect sizes are represented in η2. a BACS and JART data were missing for some healthy control patients. b BACS was standardized by creating z-scores, with the mean score of Japanese healthy controls set to zero and the standard deviation set to one [45]. c BACS composite score was calculated by averaging all z-scores of the six primary measures from the BACS [46].
Subjects with ARMS were clinically followed-up at our hospital. When the severity of psychotic symptoms exceeded the criteria of CAARMS, the subject was regarded as ARMS-P. In this study, eight out of the 33 (24.2%) subjects with ARMS developed schizophrenia during the observation period (1.0 ± 1.1 years). Twenty-five subjects who did not develop psychosis were defined as ARMS-NP. The observation period for ARMS-NP was more than two years, with average 3.5 ± 2.3 years. The demographic data at baseline evaluation for ARMS-NP and ARMS-P is shown in Table 2.
Table 2.
ARMS-NP (n = 25) | ARMS-P (n = 8) | Group Comparison | |
---|---|---|---|
Male/female | 17/8 | 6/2 | χ2 = 0.14, p = 0.70, n.s. |
Age (years) | 18.7 (3.8) | 20.5 (6.6) | p = 0.33, n.s. |
JART | 98.1 (9.6) | 93.3 (11.1) | p = 0.25, n.s. |
PANSS: positive symptom | 12.9 (4.1) | 13.8 (2.1) | p = 0.53, n.s. |
negative symptom | 19.6 (8.0) | 19.2 (4.3) | p = 0.85, n.s. |
general psychopathology | 34.1 (9.1) | 37.2 (8.4) | p = 0.40, n.s. |
mGAF | 48.6 (9.0) | 41.7 (7.9) | p = 0.096, n.s. |
SCoRS | 4.1 (1.9) | 6.6 (2.0) | p = 0.004 ** |
BACS: verbal memory | −0.36 (1.4) | −1.1 (1.3) | p = 0.21, n.s. |
working memory | −0.65 (1.1) | −1.70 (1.8) | p = 0.060, n.s. |
motor function | −1.09 (1.8) | −2.02 (1.4) | p = 0.20, n.s. |
verbal fluency | −0.78 (1.1) | −1.50 (1.4) | p = 0.16, n.s. |
attention | 0.06 (1.5) | −1.12 (1.4) | p = 0.063, n.s. |
executive function | −0.17 (0.96) | −0.94 (2.2) | p = 0.17, n.s. |
composite score | −0.50 (0.94) | −1.40 (1.25) | p = 0.038 * |
Amplitude (µV):T3 | 6.06 (3.0) | 6.30 (3.8) | p = 0.85, n.s. |
T4 | 7.18 (3.9) | 4.37 (4.4) | p = 0.099, n.s. |
Fz | 8.94 (6.6) | 6.96 (5.7) | p = 0.45, n.s. |
Cz | 14.14 (6.0) | 9.79 (6.7) | p = 0.094, n.s. |
Pz | 16.27 (5.2) | 13.76 (3.3) | p = 0.21, n.s. |
Latency (msec):T3 | 318.9 (30.5) | 346.5 (31.8) | p = 0.039 * |
T4 | 317.2 (40.0) | 329.0 (36.6) | p = 0.49, n.s. |
Fz | 315.9 (35.7) | 344.7 (41.3) | p = 0.65, n.s. |
Cz | 306.5 (34.5) | 342.7 (27.3) | p = 0.011 * |
Pz | 307.7 (29.3) | 337.5 (33.3) | p = 0.022 * |
Values represent mean (SD). ARMS; at-risk mental state, ARMS-p; ARMS patients later transitioned to overt psychosis, ARMS-NP; ARMS patients who did not transition to overt psychosis, JART; Japanese Adult Reading Test, PANSS; Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, mGAF; modified Global Assessment Functioning, SCoRS; Schizophrenia Cognitive Rating Scale, BACS; Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia. P300 data represent peak amplitudes (µV) and latencies (msec) for each group [mean (SD)]. Demographic differences between groups were examined with Student’s t-test or the chi-square test (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01).
No subject was on antipsychotic medications. All ARMS-NP patients (twenty-five), five of eight ARMS-P patients and thirty one out of thirty-nine schizophrenia patients were antipsychotic naïve. Other subjects were antipsychotic free at least 2 weeks.
This protocol was approved by the Committee on Medical Ethics of the University of Toyama. After complete and detail description of the study was provided, written informed consent was obtained from the participants in agreement with the Declaration of Helsinki. If a subject was under 20 years old, written consent was also obtained from a parent or legal guardian.
2.2. Electroencephalogram (EEG) Recording
ERPs were recorded at the time of clinical assessments using an auditory odd-ball paradigm, based on an established method [9,22,39]. Subjects were directed to lay awake on a bed, open their eyes, and watch a red circle shown on a display monitor. The patients were observed carefully, and if the patients were in poor conditions (asleep, too many eye blinks or eye movements, frequent body movement, unwilling to participate in the examination), we gave instructions again or stopped the recording.
EEGs were recorded with a 32-channel DC-amplifier (EEG-2100 version 2.22J, Nihon Kohden Corp., Tokyo, Japan). Recordings were performed using 19 or 32 channel Electrocap (Electrocap Inc., Eaton, OH) or in a wave-shielded and sound-attenuated room. Auditory stimuli were delivered binaurally through headphones with variable inter-stimulus intervals ranging from 1.5 to 2.5 s. Target tones of 2000 Hz were randomly presented in a series of standard tones of 1000 Hz, with the presentation probability of 0.2 for target tones. Numbers of the standard and target (deviant) tones were 200 and 50, respectively. All tones were 50 ms in duration with a rise–fall time of 10 ms. Subjects were requested to press a button as promptly and accurately as possible in response to infrequent target tones. EEGs were recorded at 19 (located at FP1, FP2, F3, F4, F7, F8, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, O2, T3, T4, T5, T6, Fz, Cz, and Pz) or 29 (Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, F8, FC3, FC4, C3, C4, T3, T4, CP3, CP4, TP7, TP8, P3, P4, T5, T6, O1, O2, FPz, Fz, FCz, Cz, CPz, Pz, and Oz) electrodes with average reference. In this paper, only midline (Fz, Cz, and Pz) and temporal lobe (T3, and T4) electrodes were presented according to a previous study [9]. The bandwidth was 0.16–120 Hz with a 60 Hz notch filter. Electrode impedance was less than 10 kΩ. Data were collected with a sampling rate of 500 Hz. Averaging of ERP waves and related procedures were performed using EPLYZER II software (Kissei Comtec, Co. Ltd. Nagano, Japan). The epoch was 700 ms, including a 100 ms pre-stimulus baseline. EEG responses with target tones were averaged off-line. We averaged all pre-stimulus amplitudes (from −100 to 0 ms) and defined it as zero-point. The peak of P300 was observed 250–400 ms after the target sound started. We defined it as time-window, and a positive maximum peak was detected within this time window by EPLYZAR II software. P300 amplitude was defined as the difference between the zero-point and peak, and its latency was defined as the interval from 0 ms to the timing of the peak. Before averaging ERPs, we manually checked the raw waveforms with care and removed all epochs without a typical P300 shape due to eye movement or blinking, facial muscle movement, sweating, or basic rhythms.
2.3. Statistical Methods
We used Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25 (SPSS Japan Inc., Tokyo, Japan) for statistical analyses. Groups were compared for demographic and clinical data using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) or Chi-square test. We used analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with group as a between-subject variable and age as a covariate to evaluate group differences in P300 amplitude and latency, SCoRS score and GAF score. BACS domains were corrected by age and gender, as reported previously [45]. Bonferroni’s tests were employed as post hoc analysis to follow-up results yielded by ANCOVA. Correlation analyses were carried out to study the relationship between the amplitude of P300 vs. performance on clinical and neuropsychological tasks by Spearman’s rank correlation test. In these correlational analyses, we used amplitudes at Pz as a representative parameter, according to previous studies [52,53].
For data from the six subtests on the BACS, Bonferroni’s tests were performed for multiple comparisons. In subjects with ARMS, P300 amplitude and latency were normally distributed, with no differences between ARMS-P and ARMS-NP demographics; therefore, they were compared by an independent sample t-test.
For all statistical analyses, Shapiro–Wilk analysis and Levene’s test were performed to test normality, and significance level was defined as p < 0.05.
3. Results
3.1. Profiles of Subjects
Demographic and clinical data of the participants with healthy controls, ARMS and schizophrenia is shown in Table 1. There was a significant group difference in age and PANSS positive symptom between ARMS and schizophrenia, while the male/female ratio and JART scores did not differ. mGAF scores in patients with schizophrenia were worse than those of subjects with ARMS. Scores on the composite score of the BACS was lowest for schizophrenia group, followed by ARMS, compared to healthy control group. As shown in Table 2, compared to ARMS-NP group, ARMS-P group was not different in gender, age, JART, PANSS, and mGAF, but ARMS-P had worse scores on the BACS and SCoRS at baseline.
3.2. Group Comparison of P300 in Healthy Control, ARMS, and Schizophrenia
Grand average waveforms of P300 for healthy control, ARMS, and schizophrenia are shown in Figure 1. Average P300 amplitudes and latencies of these three groups at the T3, T4, Fz, Cz, and Pz leads are shown in Table 1. Statistical analysis revealed that patients with schizophrenia showed smallest P300 amplitudes, followed by ARMS and healthy control, at midline electrodes. P300 latencies at Cz and Pz in schizophrenia were longer than those in healthy controls and ARMS. P300 latency in subjects with ARMS do not differ from those in healthy control.
3.3. P300 between ARMS-P and ARMS-NP
Grand average waveforms of P300 in ARMS-P and ARMS-NP are shown in Figure 2. For comparison, waveforms of schizophrenia and controls are also drawn. P300 amplitudes appeared to be most profoundly reduced in patient with schizophrenia, followed by ARMS-P and ARMS-NP, in comparison with healthy controls. However, the difference in amplitude between ARMS-P and ARMS-NP was not significant.
P300 latencies of patients with schizophrenia and ARMS-P appeared to be longer than those of ARMS-NP and healthy controls. Statistical analysis revealed that ARMS-P subjects elicited significantly more prolonged latencies at the T3, Cz, and Pz leads compared with those of ARMS-NP subjects (Table 2).
3.4. Relationship between P300 and Cognitive Functions
We examined correlations between P300 parameters and cognitive functions. As shown in Table 3, in entire (ARMS, schizophrenia, and healthy controls combined) subjects, P300 amplitudes at Pz were significantly positively correlated with mGAF scores, performance on some BACS domains (motor function, attention, and executive function) and BACS composite score. Furthermore, P300 latencies were negatively correlated with composite and attention scores of the BACS. In subjects with ARMS as a whole, scores on the mGAF were positively correlated with P300 amplitudes. Such relationship was found in ARMS-NP (rs = 0.57, p = 0.019) but not in ARMS-P subgroups (data not shown). No significant relationships were observed between performance on the BACS and P300 latency in ARMS subjects. In subjects with schizophrenia, BACS composite scores and PANSS general psychopathology mildly correlated with P300 amplitude, but no relationships were observed between latency and other clinical/cognitive measurements.
Table 3.
Entire Subject a | ARMS (n = 33) | Schizophrenia (n = 39) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Amplitude (µV) | Latency (msec) | Amplitude (µV) | Latency (msec) | Amplitude (µV) | Latency (msec) | |||||||
rs | p | rs | p | rs | p | rs | p | rs | p | rs | p | |
JART | −0.03 | 0.77 | 0.08 | 0.44 | −0.08 | 0.65 | 0.07 | 0.71 | 0.10 | 0.56 | 0.01 | 0.95 |
PANSS: positive symptom | −0.15 | 0.20 | 0.02 | 0.86 | −0.08 | 0.68 | 0.05 | 0.78 | 0.12 | 0.47 | −0.11 | 0.50 |
negative symptom | −0.19 | 0.12 | 0.21 | 0.09 | −0.30 | 0.09 | 0.15 | 0.41 | −0.28 | 0.10 | 0.31 | 0.06 |
general psychopathology | −0.22 | 0.07 | 0.08 | 0.51 | −0.08 | 0.68 | 0.17 | 0.34 | −0.34 | 0.04 * | −0.022 | 0.90 |
mGAF | 0.46 | 0.003 ** | −0.14 | 0.29 | 0.48 | 0.02 * | 0.09 | 0.67 | 0.14 | 0.46 | −0.095 | 0.61 |
SCoRS | −0.22 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 0.16 | −0.11 | 0.54 | 0.16 | 0.36 | −0.17 | 0.33 | 0.11 | 0.52 |
BACS: verbal memory | 0.25 | 0.01 | −0.10 | 0.35 | 0.02 | 0.65 | −0.02 | 0.92 | 0.31 | 0.06 | −0.04 | 0.80 |
working memory | 0.21 | 0.04 | −0.19 | 0.06 | −0.21 | 0.23 | −0.15 | 0.40 | 0.28 | 0.10 | −0.12 | 0.48 |
motor function | 0.41 | <0.001 b | −0.22 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 0.86 | −0.28 | 0.10 | 0.27 | 0.10 | −0.14 | 0.54 |
verbal fluency | 0.28 | 0.007 | −0.24 | 0.02 | −0.03 | 0.87 | −0.17 | 0.34 | 0.17 | 0.31 | −0.85 | 0.62 |
attention | 0.50 | <0.001 b | −0.33 | 0.001 b | 0.06 | 0.74 | −0.16 | 0.36 | 0.43 | 0.01 | −0.24 | 0.14 |
executive function | 0.36 | 0.003 b | −0.15 | 0.14 | 0.06 | 0.74 | −0.002 | 0.99 | 0.37 | 0.03 | −0.20 | 0.22 |
BACS composite score | 0.43 | <0.001 ** | −0.28 | 0.006 * | 0.01 | 0.95 | −0.22 | 0.20 | 0.34 | 0.04 * | −0.16 | 0.33 |
Data were calculated by Spearman’s rank correlation (* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01). a “Entire subject” means schizophrenia, ARMS, and healthy control (n = 100). As shown in Table 1, PANSS, mGAF and SCoRS were measured only in ARMS and schizophrenia patients (n = 72). b BACS subdomains survived after Bonferroni’s correction for multiple comparison.
4. Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first report that subjects with ARMS who later developed overt schizophrenia (ARMS-P) elicited prolonged P300 latencies at baseline com-pared to those in subjects who did not develop psychosis. Progression to schizophrenia was also associated with disturbances of cognition and social functioning at baseline, as represented by poorer performances on the BACS in ARMS-P subjects compared to those of ARMS-NP. We further demonstrated a significant correlation between P300 amplitudes and global functioning in ARMS subjects.
As shown on Table 1 and Table 2, attention deficits in ARMS-P, but not ARMS-NP subjects, as measured by the BACS, were greater than −1 (z-score), which was comparable to those in overt schizophrenia. Consistent with previous studies [17,19], our results indicated that P300 amplitudes and latencies in the entire patients were significantly associated with performance on the digit symbol substitution test in the BACS (Table 3). These findings accord with the notion that P300 reflects attention-dependent information processing and provides a feasible marker of neurophysiological abnormalities shared by different clinical stages of schizophrenia [17,18,19,20,21,54]. P300 and other EEG parameters have been shown to be influenced by several factors, such as volition and medication status the latter including antipsychotic drugs [9,39,40,55]. Although some previous studies of P300 used subjects with ARMS receiving these drugs [34,35,56], all of the participants studied here were antipsychotic-free, which is one of the strengths of our study.
P300 parameters have been investigated in the context of prediction of people particularly vulnerable to developing psychosis [32,33,34,35,36,38,56,57]. Reduced P300 amplitudes in subjects with ARMS as a whole, studied here, compared to those in healthy controls is consistent with previous findings [32,33,34,35,36,38,56,57]. By contrast, the lack of difference in P300 amplitudes between ARMS-P and ARMS-NP differs from the findings in some previous studies [34,36]. Importantly, no study has reported prolonged latency in ARMS-P subjects compared to ARMS-NP subjects. Part of these discrepant results may be related to the difference in study conditions and/or backgrounds of subjects; one study [34] used longer (100 ms) duration for the target tone, unlike the present study used 50 ms duration of target tone. Further, other studies [34,35,56] used patients who were treated with antipsychotic drugs, while our study concerned only antipsychotic-free patients. The discrepancy in presence/absence of prolonged P300 latency between a previous study [33] that also used antipsychotic-free patients and the current one may be explained by the difference in demographic data, such as age, a factor known to prolong P300 latency [58,59].
The origins of the P300 are considered to involve the temporal-parietal junction, medial temporal cortex, and lateral prefrontal cortex [60,61]. For example, volume reductions of these brain regions have been found to be correlated with decreased P300 amplitudes in ARMS subjects [35,56]. Further, the supramarginal gyrus, a part of temporal-parietal junction whose function is impaired in schizophrenia [62], has been regarded as one of the main generators of P300, because its damage results in deterioration of P300 activity [63]. Thus, the aberrant functions of these brain areas, such as temporal gyrus, may cause prolongation of P300 latency in schizophrenia and ARMS. Further, re-ductions in P300 amplitudes and prolonged latency are also present in unaffected relatives of patients [64,65,66,67], suggesting genetic backgrounds for these neurophysiological phenotypes. Specifically, data from a meta-analysis performed by Bramon et al. (2005) indicate a significant prolongation of P300 latency in relatives of schizophrenia patients, while its amplitudes were not affected [64]. Further efforts to characterize these electro-physiological manifestations in the prodromal stage of schizophrenia are likely to broaden the views on trait markers of the illness.
Cognitive and social functioning has been recognized to provide an important target of early intervention for people susceptible to developing psychosis. The SCoRS was developed for measuring daily activity skills in patients with schizophrenia [49]. Further, we found this assessment tool is also useful for evaluation of real-world activities in people with ARMS [47]. Importantly, SCoRS scores in the ARMS-P group were worse than those in ARMS-NP group (Table 2). To our knowledge, our observation is the first to indicate that daily-living skills linked to cognition, evaluated in this way, are more profoundly impaired who later convert to schizophrenia compared to non-converters. These findings overall add to the concept that poor functionality provide a trait marker for psychosis.
There have been attempts to identify neural substrates for neuropsychological disturbances of individuals in early stages of schizophrenia. Accordingly, P300 indices at Pz elicited significant correlations with mGAF and BACS scores in entire subjects (healthy controls, ARMS subjects and patients with schizophrenia combined) (Table 3). These findings are consistent with the concept that changes of cognitive and social functions, measured behaviorally, are reflected by some electrophysiological parameters in individuals with schizophrenia or the clinical high-risk state [9,38,39]. On the other hand, P300 amplitudes were related to mGAF, but not BACS scores in the ARMS subjects. Further study with a larger number of subjects should clarify the contribution of P300 activities to functional outcomes in ARMS.
Finally, it would be worthwhile to discuss the clinical implications of our findings. The present results indicate prolonged P300 latency may provide a potential biomarker for schizophrenia. This is supported by the lack of a significant effect of antipsychotic treatments on P300 latency, unlike the case for its amplitudes, as demonstrated in a meta-analysis [68], suggesting the robustness of P300 latency as a trait-marker of schizophrenia. On the other hand, a number of other candidates for biomarkers of the disease have been reported, and attempts have been made to combine multiple indicators to improve predictive accuracy [69,70]. In this line, P300 latency may help improve diagnostic accuracy when used with other indices.
Several limitations of this study should be considered. First, the small sample size, especially for ARMS-P subjects, might have limited the statistical power and restricted the generalizability of our results. Second, given the evidence of age-related changes of P300 [59,71], the higher age of schizophrenia patients as compared with other groups might have biased our results. However, this may not be relevant to the difference in P300 laten-cy between the ARMS-P and -NP groups (the main find of the current study), as they shared similar ages.
In conclusion, the present study suggests, for the first time, the ability of P300 latency to predict the development of schizophrenia in vulnerable subjects. Our observations also confirm that poor cognitive function and daily-living skills are associated with the risk of development of schizophrenia. When combined with other indices of different modalities, P300 latency may provide a diagnostic marker. Further study is warranted to investigate if P300 latency would be linked to outcomes and other clinical features of individuals at high-risk for schizophrenia and related psychoses.
Acknowledgments
Special thanks to our colleagues, Tomonori Seo, Tomohiro Miyanishi in Department of Neuropsychiatry, University of Toyama Graduate School of Medicine and Pharmaceutical Science to help recording EEG. Further, we would like to thank Kaori Sakata for coordinate subjects’ schedule and performing the neuropsychological tests. Kodai Tanaka, Tadasu Matsuoka and Mihoko Nakamura made big effort to evaluate ARMS patients’ by CAARMS. Yoichiro Takayanagi and Yasuhiro Kawasaki provided valuable advice about statistical methods and recruitment of patients.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, Y.H., T.S., T.T. (Tsutomu Takahashi), and M.S.; methodology, Y.H. and T.S.; investigation for EEG recording and analysis, T.T. (Takahiro Tateno), S.N. (Suguru Nakajima), and Y.H.; investigation for symptom evaluation, D.S., T.T. (Takahiro Tateno), and S.N. (Suguru Nakajima); investigation for neurocognitive evaluation, Y.M. and S.N. (Shimako Nishiyama); writing—original draft preparation, Y.H.; writing—review and editing, T.S., T.T. (Tsutomu Takahashi), and M.S.; project administration, M.S. and Y.H.; funding acquisition, M.S., T.T. (Tsutomu Takahashi), T.S., and Y.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was funded in part by the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science KAKENHI (grant numbers 18K07550, 16K10205, JP20H03598, JP20H03610 and 26461739), and by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (grant number JP18dk030708, JP19dk0307029 and JP20dk0307099).
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Committee on Medical Ethics of Toyama University approved the present study (No. I2013006) on 5 February 2014.
Informed Consent Statement
Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the subjects and their caregivers to publish this paper.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available since we do not have permission to share the data.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Green M.F. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McGlashan T.H., Zipursky R.B., Perkins D., Addington J., Miller T., Woods S.W., Hawkins K.A., Hoffman R.E., Preda A., Epstein I., et al. Randomized, double-blind trial of olanzapine versus placebo in patients prodromally symptomatic for psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2006;163:790–799. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.5.790. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.McGorry P.D., Yung A.R., Phillips L.J., Yuen H.P., Francey S., Cosgrave E.M., Germano D., Bravin J., McDonald T., Blair A., et al. Randomized Controlled Trial of Interventions Designed to Reduce the Risk of Progression to First-Episode Psychosis in a Clinical Sample With Subthreshold Symptoms. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2002;59:921–928. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Morrison A.P., French P., Walford L., Lewis S.W., Kilcommons A., Green J., Parker S., Bentall R.P. Cognitive therapy for the prevention of psychosis in people at ultra-high risk: Randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2004;185:291–297. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.4.291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fusar-Poli P., Borgwardt S., Bechdolf A., Addington J., Riecher-Rossler A., Schultze-Lutter F., Keshavan M., Wood S., Ruhrmann S., Seidman L.J., et al. The psychosis high-risk state: A comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:107–120. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fusar-Poli P., Bonoldi I., Yung A.R., Borgwardt S., Kempton M.J., Valmaggia L., Barale F., Caverzasi E., McGuire P. Predicting psychosis: Meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2012;69:220–229. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Fusar-Poli P., Bechdolf A., Taylor M.J., Bonoldi I., Carpenter W.T., Yung A.R., McGuire P. At Risk for Schizophrenic or Affective Psychoses? A Meta-Analysis of DSM/ICD Diagnostic Outcomes in Individuals at High Clinical Risk. Schizophr. Bull. 2013;39:923–932. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kawasaki Y., Suzuki M., Kherif F., Takahashi T., Zhou S.-Y., Nakamura K., Matsui M., Sumiyoshi T., Seto H., Kurachi M. Multivariate voxel-based morphometry successfully differentiates schizophrenia patients from healthy controls. NeuroImage. 2007;34:235–242. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Higuchi Y., Sumiyoshi T., Kawasaki Y., Matsui M., Arai H., Kurachi M. Electrophysiological basis for the ability of olanzapine to improve verbal memory and functional outcome in patients with schizophrenia: A LORETA analysis of P300. Schizophr. Res. 2008;101:320–330. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Lin Y.-T., Liu C.-M., Chiu M.-J., Liu C.-C., Chien Y.-L., Hwang T.-J., Jaw F.-S., Shan J.-C., Hsieh M.H., Hwu H.-G. Differentiation of Schizophrenia Patients from Healthy Subjects by Mismatch Negativity and Neuropsychological Tests. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e34454. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034454. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Takahashi T., Zhou S.-Y., Nakamura K., Tanino R., Furuichi A., Kido M., Kawasaki Y., Noguchi K., Seto H., Kurachi M., et al. A follow-up MRI study of the fusiform gyrus and middle and inferior temporal gyri in schizophrenia spectrum. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;35:1957–1964. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Takayanagi Y., Takahashi T., Orikabe L., Mozue Y., Kawasaki Y., Nakamura K., Sato Y., Itokawa M., Yamasue H., Kasai K., et al. Classification of First-Episode Schizophrenia Patients and Healthy Subjects by Automated MRI Measures of Regional Brain Volume and Cortical Thickness. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e21047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Niznikiewicz M.A. Neurobiological approaches to the study of clinical and genetic high risk for developing psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2019;277:17–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Näätänen R., Todd J., Schall U. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as biomarker predicting psychosis in clinically at-risk individuals. Biol. Psychol. 2016;116:36–40. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Higuchi Y., Sumiyoshi T., Ito T., Suzuki M. Perospirone Normalized P300 and Cognitive Function in a Case of Early Psychosis. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2013;33:263–266. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0b013e318287c527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Higuchi Y., Sumiyoshi T., Seo T., Miyanishi T., Kawasaki Y., Suzuki M. Mismatch Negativity and Cognitive Performance for the Prediction of Psychosis in Subjects with At-Risk Mental State. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e54080. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Polich J., Kok A. Cognitive and biological determinants of P300: An integrative review. Biol. Psychol. 1995;41:103–146. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(95)05130-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Johnson R., Jr. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. Psychophysiology. 1986;23:367–384. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00649.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Nieman D., Koelman J., Linszen D., Bour L., Dingemans P., de Visser B.O. Clinical and neuropsychological correlates of the P300 in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2002;55:105–113. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00184-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sutton S., Braren M., Zubin J., John E.R. Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty. Science. 1965;150:1187–1188. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3700.1187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Bashore T.R., van der Molen M.W. Discovery of the P300: A tribute. Biol. Psychol. 1991;32:155–171. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(91)90007-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kawasaki Y., Maeda Y., Higashima M., Nagasawa T., Koshino Y., Suzuki M., Ide Y. Reduced auditory P300 amplitude, medial temporal volume reduction and psychopathology in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 1997;26:107–115. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00055-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Roth W.T., Pfefferbaum A., Horvath T.B., Berger P., Kopell B.S. P3 reduction in auditory evoked potentials of schizophrenics. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1980;49:497–505. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90392-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bruder G.E., Tenke C.E., Towey J.P., Leite P., Fong R., Stewart J.E., McGrath P.J., Quitkin F.M. Brain ERPs of depressed patients to complex tones in an oddball task: Relation of reduced P3 asymmetry to physical anhedonia. Psychophysiology. 1998;35:54–63. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3510054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Qiu Y.-Q., Tang Y.-X., Chan R.C.K., Sun X.-Y., He J. P300 Aberration in First-Episode Schizophrenia Patients: A Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e97794. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hünerli D., Emek-Savaş D.D., Çavuşoğlu B., Çolakoğlu B.D., Ada E., Yener G.G. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease is associated with decreased P300 amplitude and reduced putamen volume. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2019;130:1208–1217. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.04.314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Yilmaz F.T., Ozkaynak S.S., Barcin E. Contribution of auditory P300 test to the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol. Sci. 2017;38:2103–2109. doi: 10.1007/s10072-017-3106-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wada M., Kurose S., Miyazaki T., Nakajima S., Masuda F., Mimura Y., Nishida H., Ogyu K., Tsugawa S., Mashima Y., et al. The P300 event-related potential in bipolar disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Affect. Disord. 2019;256:234–249. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Zhong B.-L., Xu Y.-M., Xie W.-X., Li Y. Can P300 aid in the differential diagnosis of unipolar disorder versus bipolar disorder depression? A meta-analysis of comparative studies. J. Affect. Disord. 2019;245:219–227. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Peisch V., Rutter T., Wilkinson C.L., Arnett A.B. Sensory processing and P300 event-related potential correlates of stimulant response in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A critical review. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2021;132:953–966. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2021.01.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Cui T., Wang P.P., Liu S., Zhang X. P300 amplitude and latency in autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2016;26:177–190. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0880-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ozgürdal S., Gudlowski Y., Witthaus H., Kawohl W., Uhl I., Hauser M., Gorynia I., Gallinat J., Heinze M., Heinz A., et al. Reduction of auditory event-related P300 amplitude in subjects with at-risk mental state for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2008;105:272–278. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2008.05.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bramon E., Shaikh M., Broome M., Lappin J., Bergé D., Day F., Woolley J., Tabraham P., Madre M., Johns L., et al. Abnormal P300 in people with high risk of developing psychosis. NeuroImage. 2008;41:553–560. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Van Tricht M.J., Nieman D.H., Koelman J.H., van der Meer J.N., Bour L.J., de Haan L., Linszen D.H. Reduced parietal P300 amplitude is associated with an increased risk for a first psychotic episode. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;68:642–648. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Fusar-Poli P., Crossley N., Woolley J., Carletti F., Perez-Iglesias R., Broome M., Johns L., Tabraham P., Bramon E., McGuire P. White matter alterations related to P300 abnormalities in individuals at high risk for psychosis: An MRI–EEG study. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 2011;36:239–248. doi: 10.1503/jpn.100083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Nieman D.H., Ruhrmann S., Dragt S., Soen F., Van Tricht M.J., Koelman J.H.T.M., Bour L.J., Velthorst E., Becker H.E., Weiser M., et al. Psychosis Prediction: Stratification of Risk Estimation With Information-Processing and Premorbid Functioning Variables. Schizophr. Bull. 2014;40:1482–1490. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Tang Y., Wang J., Zhang T., Xu L., Qian Z., Cui H., Tang X., Li H., Whitfield-Gabrieli S., Shenton M.E., et al. P300 as an index of transition to psychosis and of remission: Data from a clinical high risk for psychosis study and review of literature. Schizophr. Res. 2020;226:74–83. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kim M., Lee T.H., Kim J.-H., Hong H., Lee T.Y., Lee Y., Salisbury D.F., Kwon J.S. Decomposing P300 into correlates of genetic risk and current symptoms in schizophrenia: An inter-trial variability analysis. Schizophr. Res. 2018;192:232–239. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sumiyoshi T., Higuchi Y., Itoh T., Matsui M., Arai H., Suzuki M., Kurachi M., Sumiyoshi C., Kawasaki Y. Effect of perospirone on P300 electrophysiological activity and social cognition in schizophrenia: A three-dimensional analysis with sLORETA. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2009;172:180–183. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.07.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Umbricht D., Javitt D., Novak G., Bates J., Pollack S., Lieberman J., Kane J. Effects of clozapine on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 1998;44:716–725. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00524-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mizuno M., Suzuki M., Matsumoto K., Murakami M., Takeshi K., Miyakoshi T., Ito F., Yamazawa R., Kobayashi H., Nemoto T., et al. Clinical practice and research activities for early psychiatric intervention at Japanese leading centres. Early Interv. Psychiatry. 2009;3:5–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-7893.2008.00104.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Yung A.R., Yuen H.P., McGorry P.D., Phillips L.J., Kelly D., Dell’Olio M., Francey S.M., Cosgrave E.M., Killackey E., Stanford C., et al. Mapping the Onset of Psychosis: The Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2005;39:964–971. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2005.01714.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kay S.R., Fiszbein A., Opler L.A. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Matsuoka K., Uno M., Kasai K., Koyama K., Kim Y. Estimation of premorbid IQ in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease using Japanese ideographic script (Kanji) compound words: Japanese version of National Adult Reading Test. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2006;60:332–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2006.01510.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Kaneda Y., Sumiyoshi T., Keefe R., Ishimoto Y., Numata S., Ohmori T. Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Validation of the Japanese version. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2007;61:602–609. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01725.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Keefe R.S., Goldberg T., Harvey P.D., Gold J.M., Poe M.P., Coughenour L. The Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia: Reliability, sensitivity, and comparison with a standard neurocognitive battery. Schizophr. Res. 2004;68:283–297. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Higuchi Y., Sumiyoshi T., Seo T., Suga M., Takahashi T., Nishiyama S., Komori Y., Kasai K., Suzuki M. Associations between daily living skills, cognition, and real-world functioning across stages of schizophrenia; a study with the Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale Japanese version. Schizophr. Res. Cogn. 2017;7:13–18. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2017.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Kaneda Y., Ueoka Y., Sumiyoshi T., Yasui-Furukori N., Ito T., Higuchi Y., Suzuki M., Ohmori T. Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale Japanese version (SCoRS-J) as a co-primary measure assessing cognitive function in schizophrenia. Nihon Shinkei Seishin Yakurigaku Zasshi. 2011;31:259–262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Keefe R.S., Poe M., Walker T.M., Kang J.W., Harvey P.D. The Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale: An Interview-Based Assessment and Its Relationship to Cognition, Real-World Functioning, and Functional Capacity. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2006;163:426–432. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Hall R.C. Global Assessment of Functioning. J. Psychosom. Res. 1995;36:267–275. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(95)71666-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Kaneda Y., Ohmori T., Okahisa Y., Sumiyoshi T., Pu S., Ueoka Y., Takaki M., Nakagome K., Sora I. Measurement and Treatment Research toImprove Cognition in Schizophrenia Consensus Cognitive Battery: Validation of the Japanese version. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2013;67:182–188. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Takahashi S., Tanabe E., Yara K., Matsuura M., Matsushima E., Kojima T. Impairment of exploratory eye movement in schizophrenia patients and their siblings. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2008;62:487–493. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2008.01840.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hall M.-H., Jensen J.E., Du F., Smoller J.W., O’Connor L., Spencer K.M., Öngür D. Frontal P3 event-related potential is related to brain glutamine/glutamate ratio measured in vivo. Neuroimage. 2015;111:186–191. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Michie P.T., Kent A., Stienstra R., Castine R., Johnston J., Dedman K., Wichmann H., Box J., Rock D., Rutherford E., et al. Phenotypic markers as risk factors in schizophrenia: Neurocognitive functions. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2000;34:74–85. doi: 10.1177/000486740003401S12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Jackson A., Seneviratne U. EEG changes in patients on antipsychotic therapy: A systematic review. Epilepsy Behav. 2019;95:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.02.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Fusar-Poli P., Crossley N., Woolley J., Carletti F., Perez-Iglesias R., Broome M., Johns L., Tabraham P., Bramon E., McGuire P. Gray matter alterations related to P300 abnormalities in subjects at high risk for psychosis: Longitudinal MRI-EEG study. Neuroimage. 2011;55:320–328. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Van der Stelt O., Lieberman J.A., Belger A. Auditory P300 in high-risk, recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2005;77:309–320. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.04.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Mori Y., Kurosu S., Hiroyama Y., Niwa S.-I. Prolongation of P300 latency is associated with the duration of illness in male schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2007;61:471–478. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01695.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Van Dinteren R., Arns M., Jongsma M.L., Kessels R.P. P300 development across the lifespan: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e87347. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Soltani M., Knight R.T. Neural origins of the P300. Crit. Rev. Neurobiol. 2000;14:199–224. doi: 10.1615/CritRevNeurobiol.v14.i3-4.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Linden D.E.J. The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? Neuroscience. 2005;11:563–576. doi: 10.1177/1073858405280524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Shenton M.E., Dickey C.C., Frumin M., McCarley R.W. A review of MRI findings in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2001;49:1–52. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00163-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Yamaguchi S., Knight R.T. Anterior and posterior association cortex contributions to the somatosensory P300. J. Neurosci. 1991;11:2039–2054. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-07-02039.1991. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bramon E., McDonald C., Croft R.J., Landau S., Filbey F., Gruzelier J.H., Sham P.C., Frangou S., Murray R.M. Is the P300 wave an endophenotype for schizophrenia? A meta-analysis and a family study. NeuroImage. 2005;27:960–968. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Blackwood D.H.R., Clair D.M.S., Muir W.J., Duffy J.C. Auditory P300 and Eye Tracking Dysfunction in Schizophrenic Pedigrees. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1991;48:899–909. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810340031004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Turetsky B.I., Cannon T.D., Gur R.E. P300 subcomponent abnormalities in schizophrenia: III. Deficits In unaffected siblings of schizophrenic probands. Biol. Psychiatry. 2000;47:380–390. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00290-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Winterer G., Egan M.F., Raedler T., Sanchez C., Jones D.W., Coppola R., Weinberger D.R. P300 and Genetic Risk for Schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2003;60:1158–1167. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.11.1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Bramon E., Rabe-Hesketh S., Sham P., Murray R.M., Frangou S. Meta-analysis of the P300 and P50 waveforms in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2004;70:315–329. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Lieberman J.A., Small S.A., Girgis R.R. Early Detection and Preventive Intervention in Schizophrenia: From Fantasy to Reality. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2019;176:794–810. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19080865. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Schmidt A., Cappucciati M., Radua J., Rutigliano G., Rocchetti M., Dell’Osso L., Politi P., Borgwardt S., Reilly T., Valmaggia L., et al. Improving Prognostic Accuracy in Subjects at Clinical High Risk for Psychosis: Systematic Review of Predictive Models and Meta-analytical Sequential Testing Simulation. Schizophr. Bull. 2016;43:375–388. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bourisly A.K. Effects of aging on P300 between late young-age and early middle-age adulthood: An electroencephalogram event-related potential study. Neuroreport. 2016;27:999–1003. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available since we do not have permission to share the data.