Skip to main content
Cambridge Open Access logoLink to Cambridge Open Access
. 2020 Aug 3;24(8):2085–2097. doi: 10.1017/S1368980020002311

Association between health literacy and dietary intake of sugar, fat and salt: a systematic review

Alessandra Buja *, Giulia Grotto 1, Laura Montecchio 1, Elisa De Battisti 1, Milena Sperotto 1, Chiara Bertoncello 1, Silvia Cocchio 1, Tatjana Baldovin 1, Vincenzo Baldo 1
PMCID: PMC8145460  PMID: 32744216

Abstract

Objectives:

To collect and summarise all current data from observational studies, generating evidence of the association between health literacy (HL) and the dietary intake of sugar, salt and fat, to analyse intervention studies on the promotion of an appropriate dietary intake of the above-mentioned nutrients and to ascertain whether HL moderates the efficacy of such intervention.

Design:

A systematic literature search of analytical observational studies on the association between HL and dietary intake of sugar, salt and fat was performed in Medline and Scopus databases. Intervention studies on the promotion of healthy nutrition that concerned the intake of sugar, salt and fat were also assessed.

Results:

Of the eight observational studies included in this review, five investigated dietary intake of sugar, one focused on salt, one assessed sugar and salt and one analysed the fat intake. The results of the five studies assessing sugar were mixed: three found an association between low levels of HL and a high sugar intake, one found this association only for boys and two found no evidence of any association. The two studies assessing salt and the one assessing fat found no evidence of any association with HL. One intervention study on the sugar intake concluded that HL was not a significant moderator of the intervention’s effectiveness.

Conclusion:

No evidence of any association between HL and salt and fat intake emerged, while for sugar, the results are mixed. More work is needed to better understand the moderating effects of HL on the outcomes of health promotion interventions.

Keywords: Prevention, Chronic disease, Sugar, Salt, Fat, Health literacy


In recent years, it has become overwhelmingly clear that regularly eating foods that are high in energy, such as saturated fats, with added sugar and salt, is among the key risk factors for developing weight problems, metabolic disorders and related diseases(13). In this regard, the WHO has produced specific recommendations on the appropriate dietary intake of the above-mentioned elements to prevent the rise of non-communicable diseases(4). In particular, total fat should not exceed 30 % of the total energy intake, saturated fat should be <10 % of the total energy intake and trans-fat <1 %. Fat consumption should shift away from saturated fats and trans-fats to unsaturated fats and towards the goal of eliminating industrially produced trans-fats. As regards the intake of free sugars, this should be limited to <10 % of the total energy intake, and <5 % is suggested for additional health benefits. Keeping salt intake to <5 g a day (corresponding to Na intake of <2 g a day) helps to prevent hypertension and reduces the risk of heart disease and stroke in the adult population(4).

By the mid-1900s, this trio of ingredients – salt, sugar and fat – had taken on a new psychosensory dimension because the processed food industry had discovered that they could be formulated to generate a sense of satiety and pleasure in consumers(5). Palatable food activates brain reward circuitry through fast sensory inputs and slow post-ingestive consequences (such as raising glucose concentration in the blood and brain). The repeated supraphysiological stimulation of reward pathways may make people’s behaviour increasingly compulsive and lead to further loss of control over food intake and ultimately to obesity(6,7). It is well known that the foods that are more palatable and that induce more cravings are typically processed foods or foods with a high salt, sugar and/or fat content(5). Today, crave-inducing foods and soft drinks are sold and consumed worldwide, creating an unfavourable food environment that has led to higher rates of obesity and its co-morbidities, diabetes and CVD(8,9).

From the above considerations, there is an abundantly clear need to implement all useful strategies to induce people to lower their consumption of sugar, salt and fat in an effort to improve the population’s general health and prevent the non-communicable diseases that are currently the main causes of death. Providing the population with appropriate dietary information and promoting a healthy eating behaviour are certainly essential to efforts in this sense.

One factor that has gained importance in recent years, as evidence has emerged of its association with health-related behaviours (such as diet), is health literacy (HL)(10), or an individual’s ability to access and understand health-related information and consequently make appropriate health-related decisions. HL empowers individuals, enabling them to take responsibility for their health(11,12). A limited HL has an adverse impact on health-related behaviour and disease-preventing strategies, such as participation in screening programmes(1316). In addition, people with a limited HL tend to make more use of healthcare services, including hospitalisations and emergency services, partly as a consequence of their often adopting unhealthy lifestyles(1719). HL can thus play a fundamental part in health promotion and prevention strategies(20), and its importance was emphasised at the Ninth Global Conference on Health Promotion (Shanghai, 2016)(21). Moreover, the theoretical model of health promotion indicated that improving HL is a way to improve people’s control and thereby change intermediate health outcomes such as dietary habits. However, no syntheses of empirical evidence have yet been provided. Concerning nutrition and dietary behaviour, it was recognised that higher levels of literacy, numeracy and HL are predictors of food label use, good portion-size estimation skills, lower BMI and better dietary quality(2225). However, there are still several aspects that remain to be clarified, for example, if HL affects the intake of specific nutrients.

The first aim of this systematic review was to shed more light on the link between HL and dietary intake of sugar, salt and fat. The second aim was to discover the role of baseline HL levels in influencing the effectiveness of health promotion programmes aiming to encourage appropriate sugar, salt and fat intake. To achieve the first aim, we collected and summarised all currently available data emerging from observational studies generating evidence of the association between HL and dietary intake of sugar, salt and fat. To assess the second aim, we considered intervention studies promoting a healthy diet that referred specifically to an appropriate sugar, salt and fat intake, and we analysed their outcomes to see whether HL moderated the efficacy of such intervention.

Methods

Search strategy

For the current study, a comprehensive and systematic literature search was conducted in the Medline and Scopus databases to identify:

  • 1.

    observational studies (cross-sectional, cohort and case–control studies) investigating the association between HL and dietary intake of sugar, fat and salt;

  • 2.

    intervention studies aiming to promote an appropriate intake of these three ingredients to see whether HL influences the efficacy of the intervention.

The search process involved the use of three different search strings obtained by combining the terms ‘health literacy’ or ‘literacy’ with the terms ‘sugar*’ or ‘sweet*’ or ‘sport drink’, and ‘salt*’ or ‘salty’ or ‘sodium’, and ‘fat*’ or ‘fatty’ using Boolean operators. Three different search strings were used (one for each of the nutrients considered), so the search yielded three different lists of papers. The search strings were defined as explained in online supplementary material, Appendix 1.

The records retrieved from the databases were imported in Endnote and duplicates were removed. Two reviewers (G.G. and L.M.) checked the search hits by reading the article titles and abstracts. If the results of a study were published more than once, only the most complete article was considered in the analysis. The authors also checked the reference lists of the papers included in the review for any articles not already considered.

Data extraction

The following data were extracted from each study: first author’s name, year of publication, journal, study design, sampling method, characteristics of the study sample (e.g., age range), measures of outcome and exposure, results, confounding factors, interactions and the author’s conclusions.

Eligibility criteria

The studies included in the review had to meet the following inclusion criteria: HL had to be measured using validated questionnaires; dietary intake of sugar, fat and salt had to be measured quantitatively; a measure of the association between HL and dietary sugar/fat/salt intake had to be reported and the paper had to have been published from January 2000 up until January 2019 (for sugar) or March 2019 (for salt and fat) and written in English. Studies involving patients with a specific disease and previous systematic reviews were ignored. For the intervention studies, only explicit interventions aimed at reducing salt, sugar or fat intake were considered in this systematic review.

Quality assessment criteria

Two different authors (G.G. and L.M.) independently judged the methodological quality of the studies using the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology approach(21) for observational studies and the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials approach(22) for randomised controlled trials. Total Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology and Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials scores were calculated for each study. A larger percentage of items conforming to the guidelines indicated a higher methodological quality: ≥80 % excellent quality, 60–79 % good quality, 50–59 % sufficient quality and <50 % poor quality. A Cohen’s kappa score was calculated to establish the level of agreement between the two reviewers’ assessments.

Results

The reference lists of the nine selected articles yielded no additional papers meeting our inclusion and exclusion criteria. The review was thus conducted on nine papers in all (one paper assessed the association between HL and both sugar and salt intake). Among these, eight were observational studies, while one was a randomised controlled trial.

Considering these nine articles, the number of participants enroled in each observational study ranged from 100 to 3165. Seven studies(2329) were conducted on adults (one of them(23) only enroled participants over 60 years old), while one study sample(30) consisted of dyads involving a female caregiver and a 3- to 5-year-old child, and one study was conducted on adolescents aged 13–15 years(31).

The studies were conducted in various parts of the world, including the USA (five studies), Iran (two studies), Switzerland (one study) and Indonesia (one study), and they were published between 2012 and 2018.

HL was measured with the Newest Vital Sign tool in three studies (one of them considered only three of the six items in the test) and with the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine – Short Form(32) in two studies. Other tools used in single studies were the Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Dentistry-30(33), the Nutrition Literacy Questionnaire (NLQ-20)(34), the Oral Health Literacy – Adults Questionnaire(35) and the Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults – Shortened version. Two tests were administered in one study: a Short Food Literacy Questionnaire for adults(36) and the sixteen items concerning the health promotion domain in the European Health Literacy questionnaire (HLS-EU-Q47, German version)(37).

The studies varied in their approach to measuring the outcomes and relied mostly on self-reported information. In the majority of cases, participants answered questionnaires about their nutritional habits; only one study estimated Na intake (in g/d) from urinary excretion. Tables 1 and 2 provide details of the observational studies identified and included in the review.

Table 1.

Overview of studies reviewed. Material and methods

Author
Year
Journal
Children (C)
Adult (A)
Study design
Sample methods
Sample
Age
Measure of exposure Measure of outcome
Sugar
  Divaris
2012
Acta Odontol Scand
Child–caregiver dyads Cross-sectional study
Data were taken from the Carolina Oral Health Literacy project. Purposeful quota sampling was used to ensure adequate study sample representation of minority groups
North Carolina, USA
Two hundred three female caregiver/child dyads
Inclusion criteria: caregivers with children aged 3–5 years who completed the Early Childhood Oral Health Impact Scale
Exclusion criteria: males, Asians and first language other than English
OHL of parents measured with Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Dentistry-30
Scores 0–30, with low OHL < 13
Consumption of sweets (never/occasionally, once a day, more than once a day)
  Irwan
2016
International Journal of Nursing Sciences
A Cross-sectional study
Participants were selected from a list of older adults drawn up by a nurse responsible for monthly health check-ups and a community leader in the district of Tammua
Indonesia 2014
One hundred forty older adults (over 60 years)
Inclusion criteria: age over 60 years, ability to hear and see, no mental disorders and independence in activities of daily living
REALM-SF: seven-item word recognition test consisting of health-related terms
Scores from 0 (= third-grade reading level or below might be unable to read most material) to 7 (HL level considered = high school education)
Having sugar and sugary food/beverage limitations (never, seldom, sometimes, always)
  Joulaei
2018
Progress in Nutrition
C Cross-sectional study
Adolescents aged 13–15 years were selected from secondary schools using a cluster random sampling method
Shiraz, Iran
Three hundred eighty-eight adolescents
Inclusion criteria: age between 13 and 15 years, attending school in Shiraz
Exclusion criteria: foreign students and adolescents with chronic diseases or special diets
Nutrition Literacy Questionnaire (NLQ-20): this comprises thirty-four items measured on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 5 (strongly disagree) including the three main domains of nutrition literacy
NLQ-20 scores ranged from 20 to 100
Added sugar consumption in % of total energy intake, measured using the Revised Children’s Diet Quality Index, and assessed with a validated FFQ
  Persoskie
2017
Prev Chronic Dis
A Cross-sectional study
Data from the Health Information National Trends Survey Round 4 Cycle 3 (mailed questionnaire). Households were randomly selected using a stratified sample from a listing of all non-vacant residential addresses in the USA. One adult per household was asked to complete the questionnaire
USA
Year 2013
Three thousand one hundred sixty-five participants
Inclusion criteria: age 18+ years, resident in USA
NVS
Score range 0–4
The study excluded two questions from the NVS (Would it be safe to eat the ice cream if you are allergic to penicillin, peanuts, latex gloves and bee stings? If not, why not?) due to space limitations on the study instrument
Dietary behaviours: the number of days respondents drank sugar-sweetened soda or pop each week (every day; 5–6 d a week; 3–4 d a week; 1–2 d a week; <1 d a week; I do not normally drink any soda or pop)
  Sistani
2017
Eur J Dent
A Cross-sectional population-based survey
Multistage random area served as the sampling method, and the sample size was calculated by estimating a single proportion
Tehran, Iran 2011
One thousand thirty-one adults
Inclusion criteria: 18–65 years old
Exclusion criteria: illiteracy in Persian
OHLAQ
Seventeen items in four sections: reading comprehension, numeracy, listening and decision-making. Correct answers = 1; incorrect or missing answers = 0. OHLAQ scores are divided into three levels: inadequate (0–9), borderline (10–11) and adequate (12–17)
Consumption of sugary snacks or beverages between daily meals, categorised as ‘once or more daily (≥1/d)’ and ‘less than once daily (<1/d)’ as an optimal oral health behaviour.
  Zoellner
2011
J Am Diet Assoc.
A Cross-sectional survey
Participants were recruited by seven indigenous Delta residents (community health workers and/or Extension Programme Assistants)
A proportional quota sampling plan based on educational achievement levels was developed and used to assure that participants were representative of the greater Delta region. Recruiters were also trained to recruit across all age ranges (>18 years of age), an equal number of men and women, and approximately 70 % African American and 30 % white participants
Arkansas and Louisiana, USA
Three hundred seventy-six participants
Inclusion criteria: age 18+ years, resident in the rural Lower Mississippi Delta, English speaking
NVS
Scores range 0–6
High likelihood of limited literacy 0–1 score
Possibility of limited literacy 2–3 score
Adequate literacy skills 4–6 score
Dietary intake: validated 158-item FFQ to construct HEI scores. Score range 0–100
The sum of twelve component scores including total fruit (including 100 % fruit juice) (5 points), whole fruit (5 points), total vegetables (5 points), dark green and orange vegetables and legumes (5 points), total grains (5 points), whole grains (5 points), milk (10 points), meat and beans (10 points), oils (10 points), saturated fat (10 points), Na (10 points) and energy content from solid fat, alcoholic beverages and added sugars (20 points).
Higher scores indicate better adherence to DGA recommendations
SSB consumption: summing kilocalories from five items on the FFQ, including carbonated soft drinks, fruit drinks, powdered drink mixes, coffee with sugar and tea with sugar
  Zoellner
2016
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
A Randomised controlled trial
Recruitment strategies: active (e.g., recruitment at health departments) and passive (e.g., flyers, newspaper ads, word of mouth). Randomisation at individual level. Participants engage in one of the two types of intervention (MoveMore or SipSmarter)
Southwest Virginia, USA
Years 2012–2014
Three hundred one low socio-economic status adults from medically underserved rural regions were enroled and randomised (155 allocated to SipSmarter; 146 allocated to MoveMore)
Two hundred ninety-six participants completed the programme
Inclusion criteria: English-speaking adults ≥18 years of age, with self-reported consumption of ≥200 SSB kcals/d, no self-reported contraindications for physical activity, regular access to a telephone and not concurrently enroled in another nutrition or physical activity programme. To minimise cross-contamination between intervention groups, only one member per household was allowed to enrol in the trial
NVS. Six-item questionnaire to assess HL based on the nutrition facts panel. Scores 0–3: high likelihood or possibility of limited literacy; scores 4–6: adequate HL BEVQ-15, a validated food frequency instrument that assesses the past month’s beverage consumption
Theory of Planned Behaviour questions related to SSB consumption (twenty items) and physical activity (twenty items)
Salt
  Irwan
2016
International Journal of Nursing Sciences
A Cross-sectional study
Participants were selected from a list of older adults prepared by a nurse responsible for MHC and a community leader in the district of Tammua
Indonesia 2014
One hundred forty older adults (over 60 years)
Inclusion criteria: age over 60 years, ability to hear and see, no mental disorders and independence in activities of daily living
REALM-SF: seven-item word recognition test consisting of health-related terms
Scores from 0 (= third-grade reading level or below might be unable to read most material) to 7 (HL level considered = high school education)
Internal self-care practices were measured with the Health Promoting Lifestyle Profile II questionnaire; out of fifty-two questions, six were related to salt consumption (e.g., ‘I limit use of salt and salty foods’). Response options were ‘never’, ‘seldom’, ‘sometimes’ or ‘always’.
  Luta
2018
Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis
A Cross-sectional study
Participants were employees of eight social services, production/service, university/research or public service organisations in the German-speaking part of Switzerland, all with staff canteens
The sample size of 145 employees exceeded a threshold of 112 participants, calculated to be necessary to detect a reduction in the primary end point (24-h urinary Na excretion) by 15 % with α 0·05 and 80 % power, which would also allow for a 10 % drop-out rate
Switzerland
April–October 2015
One hundred forty-five participants, 15–65 years old
Inclusion criteria: use of staff canteen at least once a week, availability every 3 months for nutrition workshops held during regular working hours and sufficient knowledge of German
Exclusion criteria: medical or nonmedical issues and/or use of medication that would influence urine collection and analysis
HL was measured selectively using the sixteen items in the health promotion domain of the validated European Health Literacy questionnaire (HLS-EU-Q47, German version). HL index (0–50 points): inadequate (≤25), problematic (>25–33), sufficient (>33–42) or excellent (>42–50)
Food literacy was measured using a validated, twelve-item questionnaire scored using four- or five-point Likert scales and covering crucial elements of nutrition literacy and FL definitions. A summary score was calculated that ranged from 7 to 52; the higher the score, the better the FL
Prediction of Na and K intake from urinary excretion (g/d)
Fat
  Guntzviller
2017
J Immigr Minor Health
A Cross-sectional study
Two bilingual university extension employees who routinely work with low-income, Spanish-speaking populations helped identify and recruit participants
Lake County, Indiana, USA
One hundred adults, Spanish-speaking with limited or no English proficiency and 200 % below the poverty line
Short version of the Spanish Test of Functional Health Literacy in Adults.
Two short exercises from the reading comprehension and two from the numeracy sections were used to calculate the scale. HL scores ranged from 1 to 100, with scores above 66 corresponding to an ‘adequate’ HL.
Answer to the question ‘Do you consistently avoid eating high-fat foods?’ Answer sets included: No, I do not intend to in the next 6 months (1), No, but I intend to in the next 6 months (2), No, but I intend to in the next 30 d (3), Yes, I have been, but for <6 months (4), Yes, I have been for more than 6 months (5)

OHL, oral health literacy; REALM-SF, Rapid Estimate of Adult Literacy in Medicine, Short Form; HL, health literacy; NVS, Newest Vital Sign; OHLAQ, Oral Health Literacy − Adults Questionnaire; SSB, sugar-sweetened beverage; FL, food literacy; HEI, Healthy Eating Index; DGA, Dietary Guidelines for Americans; BEVQ, Beverage Intake Questionnaire; MHC, monthly health checkups.

Table 2.

Overview of studies reviewed. Results

Author
Year
Journal
Results Confounders Conclusion Additional comments (e.g., details of intervention)
Sugar
  Divaris
2012
Acta Odontol Scand
Although a positive trend emerged, the frequency of the children’s sweet consumption was not found significantly associated with the caregivers’ level of oral HL (only a bivariate distribution was performed). Age
Education level
Race
OHL was not significantly associated with child’s oral health-related quality of life
  Irwan
2016
International Journal of Nursing Sciences
No association was found between HL level and sugar limitation Gender
Occupation
Education level
Health status
Sugar limitation correlated with salt limitation (0·15, P < 0·05) and MHC – Monthly Health Check-ups (0·10)
  Joulaei
2018
Progress in Nutrition
Among boys, an increasing FNL (component of total nutrition literacy) was associated with higher sugar score quartile, which showed lower intake of sugar (OR 1·071, 95 % CI 1·002, 1·146, P < 0·05) Gender
BMI
Parents’ education level
Physical activity
A higher FNL was associated with lower sugar intake and a better energy balance in boys
  Persoskie
2017
Prev Chronic Dis
In unadjusted models, NFP label understanding was negatively associated with sugar-sweetened soda consumption: participants with a better understanding of the NFP reported consuming sugar-sweetened soda fewer days a week (OR 0·88; 95 % CI 0·81, 0·94; P = 0·001).
After adjusting for demographic characteristics, the association between NFP label understanding and sugar-sweetened soda consumption remained significant (OR 0·90; 95 % CI 0·81, 0·99; P = 0·03)
Age
Gender
Education level
Income
Race/ethnicity
Many consumers have difficulty interpreting nutrition labels and label understanding correlates with self-reported dietary behaviours (those with better label understanding tended to drink less sugar-sweetened soda)
Higher scores for label understanding were associated with consuming less sugar-sweetened soda, even after adjusting for demographic factors
  Sistani
2017
Eur J Dent
High OHL scores 12–17 correlated significantly with a lower consumption of sugary snacks or beverages (<1/d between meals) (OR = 1·56, 95 % CI 1·13, 2·15, P = 0·007) Age
Gender
Education level
Economic status
Age and OHL scores predict individuals’ consumption of sugary drinks and snacks (<1/day between meals). This result indicates that all adults, and especially youngsters and those with limited literacy skills, should have access to easily understood messages about a diet for oral health.
  Zoellner
2011
J Am Diet Assoc.
HL significantly predicted SSB beverage consumption (R 2 = 0·15; F = 6·3; P < 0·01), while accounting for demographic variables. For SSB, participants in the lowest HL category consumed about 119 kcal/d more than those with an adequate HL
Every 1 point in HL scores was associated with 34 fewer SSB kcal/d
Age
Gender
Education level
Household income
Race
SNAP participation
An important relationship exists between HL and SSB consumption and illustrates how understanding the causes and consequences of limited HL is an important factor in promoting compliance with the Dietary Guidelines for Americans
  Zoellner
2016
Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act
HL status did not significantly influence 6-month retention rates (low HL = 79 ± 41 %, high HL = 72 ± 45 %; P = 0·56) or class engagement rates (low HL = 2·14 ± 1·03, high HL = 1·97 ± 1·15; P = 0·20)
The 6-month SSB kcal reduction between participants in the SipSmarter condition with low HL v. high HL was not statistically significant (P = 0·21).
Age
Gender
Education level
Income
Race/ethnicity
Employment status
Number of children
Smoking status
BMI
HL
Baseline HL status did not moderate any of the primary or secondary outcomes. Between the low HL and high HL groups, the relative effect of the treatment was not statistically significant
(P = 0·31).
SipSmarter targeted decreasing SSB consumption, with the primary goal of achieving the SSB recommendation of <8 fluid ounces a day. To sufficiently target SSB reduction, participants were educated on recommendations for all beverage categories (including water, unsweetened beverages, milk). The comparison condition, MoveMore targeted PA promotion, with the primary goal of achieving 150 min of moderate-intensity aerobic activity and doing muscle-strengthening activities on two or more days a week. The final 6-month intervention structure, informed by the preliminary work, included three small-group classes, one live teach-back call and eleven Interactive Voice Response calls. SipSmarter and MoveMore conditions were matched in duration and contact. Each of the small-group classes was 90–120 min in duration and delivered in weeks 1, 6 and 17
Salt
  Irwan
2016
International Journal of Nursing Sciences
No association was found between HL level and salt limitation Gender
Occupation
Education level
Health status
The number of respondents who never limited their sugar and salt intake was especially surprising. An intervention programme should be developed to limit salt and sugar intake by the Indonesian elderly
  Luta
2018
Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis
In multiple regression analysis, neither HL nor FL was significantly associated with salt intake Age
Gender
There was no significant association between the HL index or FL scores and salt intake. The only variable significantly associated with salt intake (P = 0·005) was how salt content impacts food/menu choice. The analysis strongly emphasises the need to raise health-related knowledge, abilities and skills regarding Na/salt and K in the working population of Switzerland and, in particular, to bridge the gap between knowledge and salt-related dietary practices
Fat
  Guntzviller
2017
J Immigr Minor Health
HL and avoiding fatty foods were not significantly associated Age
Gender
Race
Education level
Self-efficacy interacted with HL in avoiding fatty foods although the interaction was only marginally significant. As participants’ HL increased, the positive relationship between self-efficacy and avoiding fatty foods became stronger

HL, health literacy; OHL, oral health literacy; FNL, functional nutrition literacy; NFP, Nutrition Facts panel; SSB, sugar-sweetened beverage; FL, food literacy; SNAP, Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program.

Health literacy and sugar intake

Figure 1 shows the flow chart of the article selection process for the association between sugar intake and HL. Seven articles met our inclusion criteria and were included in this review: three studies found that participants with a higher HL had a lower sugar intake, one found the same association only for boys and two found no evidence of any association.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Article selection process (sugar), flow chart. HL, health literacy

All three studies finding an association were conducted on adults. Two of these studies were conducted in the USA and focused specifically on sugar-sweetened beverage (SSB) consumption, using the Newest Vital Sign tool to measure participants’ HL. Using nationally representative data, Huizinga et al. (24) found that participants with a better understanding of panels reporting nutritional facts reported consuming sugar-sweetened soda fewer times a week. Their findings were consistent with those of von Elm et al. (26), who reported that participants in the lowest HL category (as measured with the Newest Vital Sign tool, which measures understanding of food labels) ingested just over 100 kcal/d more from SSB than participants with an adequate HL. Each additional point in participants’ HL scores (0–6) was associated with a 34 kcal/d lower intake from SSB. The third study showing an association between a higher HL and a more limited consumption of sugar was conducted by Cha et al. (25), who investigated the association between oral health-related behaviour and oral HL among Iranian adults. They considered the consumption of sugary snacks or beverages between meals, finding that higher oral HL scores correlated significantly with a lower consumption of these foods (less than once a day).

Oral HL was assessed by Naghibi Sistani et al. too(30), who investigated the association between caregivers’ oral HL and their children’s oral health-related quality of life. The authors explored whether HL modified the association between children’s oral health status and oral health-related quality of life. The study was based on data from structured interviews with female caregivers of 3- to 5-year-old children to examine deleterious oral health practices in children. Although a trend emerged, the frequency of the children’s sweet consumption was not found significantly associated with the caregivers’ level of oral HL (only bivariate distribution was performed). The only study that we found concerning adolescents (13–15 years old) estimated their eating behaviour using a validated FFQ and examined its association with nutritional literacy(31). This construct was investigated specifically in three aspects – functional nutrition literacy, interactive nutrition literacy and critical nutrition literacy – using the Nutrition Literacy Questionnaire (NLQ-20)(34). It emerged that a higher functional nutrition literacy was associated with a lower sugar intake in boys, while there was no evidence of such an association in girls. Sugar intake was not associated with either of the other aspects of nutritional literacy (interactive nutrition literacy or critical nutrition literacy) in either sex.

A single cross-sectional study aimed to examine self-care practices and health-seeking behaviours of older adults (over 60 years)(23). Various self-care practices were investigated, including the dietary intake of sugar and salt. No evidence of any association emerged between HL and a limited consumption of these elements.

The only intervention study we found was a randomised and controlled trial aiming to examine the effects of a behavioural intervention to reduce SSB intake among adults in medically underserved rural communities(27). It emerged that baseline HL status did not moderate any outcomes.

Health literacy and salt intake

Figure 2 shows the flow chart of the article selection process for the association between salt intake and HL. Two cross-sectional studies were selected. Neither found any significant association between salt intake and HL. Irwan et al. (28) sought associations between HL, food literacy (FL) and salt awareness, on the one hand, and salt intake, K intake and Na:K ratio, on the other hand, using baseline data from a workplace intervention trial conducted in Switzerland. Na and K intakes were estimated from a single 24-h urine collection. The authors found a tendency towards a lower salt intake among the more health-literate and food-literate participants, but on multiple regression analysis, neither HL nor FL was significantly associated with salt intake, K intake or Na:K ratio. The only variable significantly associated with salt intake was food salt awareness when it came to the choice of foods.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Article selection process (salt), flow chart. HL, health literacy

Health literacy and fat intake

Figure 3 shows the flow chart of the article selection process for the association between fat intake and HL. The only study investigating this issue aimed to examine HL, self-efficacy and eating and exercising behaviour in a low-income Hispanic population in the USA(29). The results produced no evidence of any statistically significant association between HL and the intake of fatty foods. Self-efficacy interacted with HL as regards to the three types of behaviour investigated (eating fruits and vegetables, avoiding fatty foods and exercising), but the interaction was only marginally significant for avoiding fatty foods.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Article selection process (fat), flow chart. HL, health literacy

Appendix 2 in the online supplementary material outlines the methodological quality of the studies. Overall, of the eight observational studies considered, three were of good, three of moderate and two of poor methodological quality. The single randomised controlled trial was compliant for 77 % of the items. A Cohen’s kappa of 0·66 reflected a good agreement between the two reviewers.

Discussion

Three of the six observational studies that considered sugar intake found that a greater HL coincided with a lower sugar intake, providing weak evidence of HL being a determinant of sugar intake, mainly in adults. As for salt and fat intake, our review goes to show that the evidence to support any empirical associations between HL and salt and fat is still very scanty, and no studies have found an association between HL and these nutrients to date.

Returning to the association between HL and sugar intake, the three studies revealing an association between a greater HL and a lower sugar intake (especially in the form of SSB) were conducted on adults. These findings indicate that healthier dietary practices are associated with a better understanding of food labels and suggest that HL might have a role in improving adult people’s diet. Given that SSB consumption has doubled over the past three decades in the USA(3840), the relationship between these drinks and obesity(4143), and the evidence of their consumption being inversely related to the level of schooling(44), and to HL, would warrant major targeted intervention to reverse the trends of their consumption(26). This consideration would not apply to elderly people, as the only study on this age group found no association between HL and sugar intake.

While children and young adults are a core target group for HL research and practice, there are limited knowledge and academic consensus regarding the abilities and knowledge a child or young person should possess in order to make sound health-related decisions. The only study that investigated the link between nutritional literacy and eating behaviour in adolescents found that a higher nutritional literacy is associated with a lower sugar intake in boys, but not in girls(36).

Only one randomised and controlled trial examined the effects of a behavioural intervention to reduce SSB intake and see if HL influenced retention, engagement or outcomes(27). It emerged that baseline HL status did not moderate any of the primary or secondary outcomes, suggesting that the intervention strategies achieved much the same benefits irrespective of participants’ level of HL. In other words, health promotion interventions to improve the population’s knowledge of health-related topics can apparently overcome inequalities relating to participants’ baseline HL levels. In one systematic review of health promotion interventions targeting disease self-management conducted in 2010, only eight of the twenty-four trials analysed the modifying effect of different levels of HL, and findings were mixed regarding how much people’s level of HL influenced the effectiveness of such interventions(45).

Studies assessing HL and salt intake found no evidence of any association between the two variables(23,28). As for HL, FL and salt awareness vis-à-vis salt intake, the only variable significantly associated with the salt intake (in one study) was food salt awareness, when it came to the choice of foods. This might seem paradoxical because applying a knowledge of salt content when choosing what food to eat is considered an advanced form of (interactive and critical) FL behaviour. It demands the ability to judge whether a food contributes to healthy nutrition and to distinguish between more healthy and less healthy options(46). This requires a good functional FL, such as knowing the recommendations and being able to read and understand food labels(46). The authors nevertheless conclude that, whatever the potential of HL, FL and specific salt awareness, the high Na content in processed foods may hamper consumers’ efforts to reduce their overall Na intake(28). In fact, previous studies have demonstrated that hedonic response to salt preference depends on a complex interplay of physiological, genetic, psychological and developmental factors and that high salt intake could be the expression of sensory and behavioural factors(47,48). On the other hand, an experimental study(49) found salt taste sensitivity unrelated to the real salt intake, whereas self-reported eating habits were associated with the actual salt intake. This would support the feasibility of limiting individuals’ salt intake effectively through education and training.

The only study based on social cognitive theory hypothesised that self-efficacy and capability (e.g., HL) may interact in influencing preventive health behaviours, such as fat intake, but found no such association. Previous studies had found that overeaters feature a strong liking or preference for fat and a strong attraction to more palatable food, the palatability consequently overriding any satiety signal being triggered by the fat(50). Such a scenario suggests a weak satiety response to fat. It was also demonstrated that high-fat eaters had higher levels of diet-induced thermogenesis and higher leptin levels, suggesting that some individuals stay lean on high-fat diets due to a particular genotype(51). Both these studies suggest that physiological mechanisms could explain dietary fat intake, but also in a complex interplay with psychological aspects. For instance, fat addiction could have important psychological determinants relating to motivation, depression, anxiety and reasoning that need to be carefully examined(52). These considerations and complex relationships should have important implications in how we interpret the influence of HL on dietary behaviour and how we devise intervention to address eating problems and promote healthy lifestyles, especially in adults(53,54).

Strengths and limitations

While a low HL has been linked to numerous unhealthy behaviours and poor health outcomes, this is the first study (to our knowledge) to systematically review the evidence of the association between people’s HL and their dietary intake of sugar, salt and fat – the three main elements considered in recommendations for a proper diet to prevent non-communicable diseases.

Our systematic review has a few limitations. First, the various studies adopted different tools to measure both HL and dietary intake of the elements considered, making the results difficult to compare and preventing any meta-analysis. The use of a limited number of standardised measuring tools in future studies would facilitate comparisons between international scenarios. Second, some of the samples considered in our review were drawn from particular categories of the population that might be seen as vulnerable (i.e., ethnic minorities) and may not be representative of the general population. The characteristics of the target population could influence the level of association between the variables analysed. Further studies could try to clarify the possible influence of different socio-demographic characteristics, such as age, in mediating the association between HL and health-related behaviour. We also found only one publication describing an intervention programme that enabled us to examine whether HL mediated the efficacy of a health-promoting intervention. Further studies on HL and health-related behaviour would be useful to shed more light on these topics.

Conclusion

This systematic review concerning the association between HL and the dietary intake of sugar, salt and fat revealed no evidence of any such association for salt and fat, while for sugar, the results are mixed: only about half of the studies considered found an association between a higher level of HL and a more limited sugar consumption. The hypothesised link between health promotion and education strategies and an improvement in HL – with a view to influencing intermediate outcomes such as eating habits – should be tested in further research because this first attempt to summarise the early empirical evidence is inconclusive. More work is also needed to better understand the moderating effects of HL on the outcomes of health promotion interventions.

Acknowledgements

Acknowledgements: None. Financial support: The current research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. Conflict of interest: There are no conflicts of interest. Authorship: A.B. conceptualised the study and approved the final manuscript as submitted. M.S. designed the data collection tools and approved the final manuscript as submitted. A.B., G.G., E.D. and L.M. drafted the manuscript and approved the final manuscript as submitted. V.B., C.B., S.C. and T.B. critically reviewed the interpretation of the data, revised the manuscript and approved the final manuscript as submitted. Ethics of human subject participation: Not applicable.

Supplementary material

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980020002311.

S1368980020002311sup001.docx (39.3KB, docx)

click here to view supplementary material

References

  • 1. Hooper L, Abdelhamid A, Bunn D et al. (2015) Effects of total fat intake on body weight. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 8, CD011834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Mozaffarian D, Fahimi S, Singh GM et al. (2014) Global sodium consumption and death from cardiovascular causes. N Engl J Med 371, 624–634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Te Morenga LA, Howatson A, Jones RM et al. (2014) Dietary sugars and cardiometabolic risk: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of the effects on blood pressure and lipids. AJCN 100, 65–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. World Health Organization (2018) Healthy diet. Fact Sheet No. 394 WHO; available at https://www.who.int/who-documents-detail/healthy-diet-factsheet394 (accessed April 2020).
  • 5. Rao P, Rodriguez RL & Shoemaker SP (2018) Addressing the sugar, salt, and fat issue the science of food way. NPJ Sci Food 2, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Volkow ND & Wise RA (2005) How can drug addiction help us understand obesity? Nat Neurosci 8, 555–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Yeomans MR & Gray RW (2002) Opioid peptides and the control of human ingestive behaviour. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26, 713–728. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Traill WB, Mazzocchi M, Shankar B et al. (2014) Importance of government policies and other influences in transforming global diets. Nutr Rev 72, 591–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. French SA, Story M & Jeffery RW (2001) Environmental influences on eating and physical activity. Annu Rev Public Health 22, 309–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. von Wagner C, Knight K, Steptoe A et al. (2007) Functional health literacy and health-promoting behaviour in a national sample of British adults. J Epidemiol Community Health 61, 1086–1090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Kickbusch IS (2001) Health literacy: addressing the health and education divide. Health Promot Int 16, 289–297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Nutbeam D (2008) The evolving concept of health literacy. Soc Sci Med 67, 2072–2078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Davis TC, Williams MV, Marin E et al. (2002) Health literacy and cancer communication. CA Cancer J Clin 52, 134–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Bennett IM, Chen J, Soroui JS et al. (2009) The contribution of health literacy to disparities in self-rated health status and preventive health behaviors in older adults. Ann Fam Med 7, 204–211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Miller DP, Brownlee CD, McCoy TP et al. (2007) The effect of health literacy on knowledge and receipt of colorectal cancer screening: a survey study. BMC Fam Pract 8, 16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Pappas G, Siozopoulou V & Saplaoura K et al. (2007) Health literacy in the field of infectious diseases: the paradigm of brucellosis. J Infect 54, 40–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Lynn Nielsen-Bohlman AMP & Kindig DA (2004) Health Literacy: A Prescription to End Confusion. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. DeWalt DA, Berkman ND, Sheridan S et al. (2004) Literacy and health outcomes: a systematic review of the literature. J Gen Intern Med 19, 1228–1239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Cho YI, Lee SYD, Arozullah AM et al. (2008) Effects of health literacy on health status and health service utilization amongst the elderly. Soc Sci Med 66, 1809–1816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Nutbeam D (2017) Health literacy as a population strategy for health promotion. JJHEP 25, 210–222. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. World Health Organization (2017) Shanghai declaration on promoting health in the 2030 Agenda for sustainable development. Health Promot Int 32, 7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Malloy-Weir L & Cooper M (2017) Health literacy, literacy, numeracy and nutrition label understanding and use: a scoping review of the literature. J Hum Nutr Diet 30, 309–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Huizinga MM, Carlisle AJ, Cavanaugh KL et al. (2009) Literacy, numeracy, and portion-size estimation skills. Am J Prev Med 36, 324–328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Huizinga MM, Beech BM, Cavanaugh KL et al. (2008) Low numeracy skills are associated with higher BMI. Obesity (Silver Spring) 16, 1966–1968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Cha E, Kim KH, Lerner HM et al. (2014) Health literacy, self-efficacy, food label use, and diet in young adults. Am J Health Behav 38, 331–339. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. von Elm E, Altman DG, Egger M et al. (2008) The strengthening the reporting of observational studies in epidemiology (STROBE) statement: guidelines for reporting observational studies. J Clin Epidemiol 61, 344–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D et al. (2010) CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomized trials. Ann Intern Med 152, 726–732. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Irwan AM, Kato M, Kitaoka K et al. (2016) Self-care practices and health-seeking behavior among older persons in a developing country: theories-based research. Int J Nurs Sci 3, 11–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Persoskie A, Hennessy E & Nelson WL (2017) US consumers’ understanding of nutrition labels in 2013: the importance of health literacy. Prev Chronic Dis 14, E86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Naghibi Sistani MM, Virtanen JI, Yazdani R et al. (2017) Association of oral health behavior and the use of dental services with oral health literacy among adults in Tehran, Iran. Eur J Dent 11, 162–167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Zoellner J, You W, Connell C et al. (2011) Health literacy is associated with healthy eating index scores and sugar-sweetened beverage intake: findings from the rural Lower Mississippi Delta. J Am Diet Assoc 111, 1012–1020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Zoellner JM, Hedrick VE & You W (2016) Effects of a behavioral and health literacy intervention to reduce sugar-sweetened beverages: a randomized-controlled trial. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 13, 38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Luta X, Hayoz S, Gréa Krause C et al. (2018) The relationship of health/food literacy and salt awareness to daily sodium and potassium intake among a workplace population in Switzerland. Nutr Metab Cardiovas Dis 28, 270–277. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Guntzviller LM, King AJ, Jensen JD et al. (2017) Self-efficacy, health literacy, and nutrition and exercise behaviors in a low-income, Hispanic population. J Immigr Minor Health 19, 489–493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Divaris K, Lee JY, Baker AD et al. (2012) Caregivers’ oral health literacy and their young children’s oral health-related quality of life. Acta Odontol Scand 70, 390–397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Joulaei H, Keshani P & Kaveh MH (2018) Nutrition literacy as a determinant for diet quality amongst young adolescents: a cross-sectional study. Progr Nutr 20, 455–464. [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Arozullah AM, Yarnold PR, Bennett CL et al. (2007) Development and validation of a short-form, rapid estimate of adult literacy in medicine. Med Care 45, 1026–1033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Lee JY, Rozier RG, Lee SY et al. (2007) Development of a word recognition instrument to test health literacy in dentistry: the REALD-30 – a brief communication. J Public Health Dent 67, 94–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Ndahura NB (2019) Nutrition literacy status of adolescent students in Kampala district, Uganda. Master thesis, Lillestrøm, Norway: Oslo and Akershus University College of Applied Sciences; available at https://oda.hioa.no/en/nutrition-literacy-status-of-adolescent-students-in-kampala-district-uganda/asset/dspace:3744/maeh_ndahura_2012.pdf (accessed July 2019).
  • 40. Naghibi Sistani MM, Montazeri A, Yazdani R et al. (2014) New oral health literacy instrument for public health: development and pilot testing. J Investig Clin Dent 5, 313–321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Krause C, Beer-Borst S, Sommerhalder K et al. (2018) A short food literacy questionnaire (SFLQ) for adults: findings from a Swiss validation study. Appetite 120, 275–280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Sorensen K, Pelikan JM, Rothlin F et al. (2015) Health literacy in Europe: comparative results of the European health literacy survey (HLS-EU). Eur J Public Health 25, 1053–1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Brownell KD, Farley T, Willett WC et al. (2009) The public health and economic benefits of taxing sugar-sweetened beverages. N Engl J Med 361, 1599–1605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Malik VS, Willett WC & Hu FB (2009) Nutritively sweetened beverages and obesity. J Am Med Assoc 301, 2210–2210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Duffey KJ & Popkin BM (2007) Shifts in patterns and consumption of beverages between 1965 and 2002. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 2739–2747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Malik VS, Schulze MB & Hu FB (2006) Intake of sugar-sweetened beverages and weight gain: a systematic review. Am J Clin Nutr 84, 274–288. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Beauchamp GK & Engelman K (1991) High salt intake: sensory and behavioral factors. Hypertension 17, I176–I181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Leshem M (2009) Biobehavior of the human love of salt. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33, 1–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Lee H, Cho HJ, Bae E et al. (2014) Not salt taste perception but self-reported salt eating habit predicts actual salt intake. J Korean Med Sci 29, Suppl. 2, S91–S96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Erlanson-Albertsson C (2005) How palatable food disrupts appetite regulation. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 97, 61–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Blundell JE & Cooling J (1999) High-fat and low-fat (behavioural) phenotypes: biology or environment? Proc Nutr Soc 58, 773–777. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Sarkar S, Kochhar KP & Khan NA (2019) Fat addiction: psychological and physiological trajectory. Nutrients 11, E2785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Smilowitz JT, German JB & Zivkovic AM (2010) Food intake and obesity: the case of fat. In Fat Detection: Taste, Texture, and Post Ingestive Effects; Frontiers in Neuroscience [Montmayeur J-P & le Coutre J, editors]. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis Group. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Meule A (2019) A critical examination of the practical implications derived from the food addiction concept. Curr Obes Rep 8, 11–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980020002311.

S1368980020002311sup001.docx (39.3KB, docx)

click here to view supplementary material


Articles from Public Health Nutrition are provided here courtesy of Cambridge University Press

RESOURCES