Abstract
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is associated with pervasive social deficits as well as marked emotion dysregulation across the life span. Decreased social motivation accounts in part for social difficulties, but factors moderating its influence are not fully understood. In this paper, we (a) characterize social and emotional functioning among children and adolescents with ASD, (b) explore contributions of social motivation and emotion dysregulation to social skill, and (c) consider biological sex and intellectual functioning as moderators of these associations. In a sample of 2,079 children and adolescents with ASD from the Simons Simplex Collection, we document direct effects of social motivation, internalizing symptoms, aggression, attention problems, irritability, and self-injurious behavior on children’s social skills. Furthermore, dysregulation in several domains moderated the association between social motivation and social skill, suggesting a blunting effect on social motivation in the context of emotional difficulties. Moreover, when considering only individuals with intellectual skills in the average range or higher, biological sex further moderated these associations. Findings add to our understanding of social–emotional processes in ASD, suggest emotion dysregulation as a target of intervention in the service of social skill improvements, and build on efforts to understand sources of individual difference that contribute to heterogeneity among individuals with ASD.
Keywords: autism, emotion dysregulation, externalizing, internalizing, social motivation
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is currently defined by the presence of social communication deficits as well as behaviors and interests that are restricted or repetitive (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Theoretical models of the etiology of ASD frequently place social impairments squarely at the center, positing alterations to the neurobiology underlying social communication from very early in life. Within these models, the construct of social motivation is of particular interest, as several influential frameworks propose that social motivation deficits underlie social dysfunction in ASD (Chevalier, Kohls, Troiani, Brodkin, & Schultz, 2012; Dawson & Bernier, 2007; Dawson, Webb, & McPartland, 2005). In the context of typical development, they argue, social motivation is fundamental to social success, as it facilitates enhanced attention toward social stimuli (e.g., human voices over other sounds, and human faces over other images), promotes the development of neurobiological systems specialized for social information processing (e.g., face processing) and complex social cognition (e.g., perspective taking), and increases the frequency and quality of social approach and behavior (Dawson & Bernier, 2007). Among individuals with ASD, in contrast, alterations to early social motivation and reward undercut this developmental cascade, affecting the trajectory and outcome of a range of social and communication skills and behaviors (Dawson et al., 2005).
Not yet fully specified within social motivation models are the traits and characteristics that supplement and/or interact with social motivation to influence social cognition and behavior. Although ASD was once conceptualized as a rare, narrowly defined diagnosis, incredible heterogeneity among individuals with the same diagnosis has become increasingly clear in recent decades (Shen & Piven, 2017). This is evident across levels of analysis, from the genetic bases thought to underlie the disorder (Arnett, Trinh, & Bernier, 2018) to the range of cognitive profiles observed (e.g., Munson et al., 2008). Even within the core “social deficits” that define the disorder, there are individual differences in social interest, skill, and success (Wing & Gould, 1979). Wing and Gould, for example, described children with ASD who had apparently low social motivation (termed “aloof ”), others with a high degree of social motivation but poorly developed skills (termed “active”), and still others whose motivation fell in the middle (termed “passive”). Clearly, social skill and engagement are multiply determined, with contributions from social motivation as well as a host of other factors. Given the variability that characterizes ASD, understanding of the social skills and experiences of individuals with ASD requires elucidation of the individual differences that work in concert with social motivation to contribute to social functioning among children, adolescents, and adults with ASD.
Emotion Dysregulation in ASD
One source of variability that may influence social functioning relates to the emotional experiences of individuals with ASD. Alongside the core diagnostic deficits of ASD, differences in emotional experience (and dysregulation of emotion in particular) are commonly associated features of ASD, despite their absence from formal diagnostic criteria. As early as infancy, children later diagnosed with ASD display higher levels of parent-reported negative affect, including increased sadness and fear, less positive affect, and more difficulties with soothability than their peers without ASD (Filliter et al., 2015; Garon et al., 2009; Konstantareas & Stewart, 2006). With time, these features appear to manifest in a number of diagnostic outcomes. Among these is increased prevalence of anxiety disorders by school age (White, Oswald, Ollendick, & Scahill, 2009), with over 80% of children with ASD displaying significant symptoms of anxiety (Muris, Steerneman, Merckelbach, Holdrinet, & Meesters, 1998). Risk of depressive symptoms and formal mood disorder is similarly increased for children with ASD (Neuhaus, Bernier, & Beauchaine, 2014; Rieffe, De Bruine, De Rooij, & Stockmann, 2014; Simonoff et al., 2008), with increasing symptoms of depression and associated suicidal ideation and behavior as children move into adolescence and adulthood (Brereton, Tonge, & Einfeld, 2006; Cassidy et al., 2014).
Often in conjunction with internalizing symptoms, children and adolescents with ASD display increased emotion dysregulation in some forms of externalizing behavior as well (Vaillancourt et al., 2017). Although challenging behaviors such as tantrums, irritability, and aggressive behavior are normative for young children, these are often heightened beyond developmental expectations for children with ASD (Mazefsky & White, 2014). Externalizing symptoms are one of the most common presenting concerns for families of children with ASD seeking mental or behavioral health services (Arnold et al., 2003) and represent a prominent source of stress for parents (Estes et al., 2009). When such difficulties persist into adulthood, they have detrimental effects on quality of life and independence among adults with ASD (Ballaban-Gil, Rapin, Tuchman, & Shinnar, 1996).
Whether emotion dysregulation is an inherent feature within ASD itself or constitutes evidence of psychiatric comorbidity has yet to be resolved (Mazefsky et al., 2013; Mazefsky & White, 2014), but it is clear that such dysregulation is prevalent, long lasting, and clinically significant. As such, it almost certainly carries an impact for other areas of functioning, including the development and expression of social skill. Among children without ASD, symptoms of anxiety and depression correspond to poorer social skills and fewer friendships (Garaigordobil, Bernaras, Jaureguizar, & Machimbarrena, 2017; Motoca, Williams, & Silverman, 2012; Scharfstein, Alfano, Beidel, & Wong, 2011; Scharfstein & Beidel, 2015). Paralleling the co-occurrence of social and emotional difficulties at the behavioral level, these processes also share neurobiological substrates. For instance, a range of neural regions and systems that subserve social cognition and behavior, and have been shown to be altered in ASD, are also fundamental to psychiatric functioning across the life span (e.g., amygdala and associated limbic structures, prefrontal cortex, and serotonergic and dopaminergic systems; see Neuhaus, Beauchaine, & Bernier, 2010, for review). Alterations to many of these neural regions appear to underlie emotion dysregulation across a variety of diagnostic categories (e.g., Beauchaine, 2015; Beauchaine & Zisner, 2017), underscoring the transdiagnostic role of emotion dysregulation as it interacts with other influences to shape functioning across development.
Given these links, emotion-related processes and social functioning are likely highly intertwined in both typical development and ASD, and disruptions in emotion-related processes likely impact social engagement and success in ASD. In this paper, we hypothesize that emotion dysregulation may have both direct and interactive effects on the social functioning of individuals with ASD. Whereas dysregulation likely decreases social skill and success directly, it may also moderate the associations between social motivation and social skill, such that it interferes with an individual’s ability to “translate” his or her motivation for social interaction into successful social engagement. For example, significant anxiety may inhibit social skill even when a child is highly motivated for social engagement, perhaps by decreasing social approach and thereby limiting opportunities to practice social skills (as well as other types of skills more broadly). Similarly, the presence of irritability may impede social interactions, even in the context of high social motivation, if a child displays frequent annoyance or negative reactions to his or her peers’ behavior. This may be especially relevant for children with ASD, for whom triggers for dysregulation can be idiosyncratic and may often appear unpredictable to peers without ASD (Mazefsky et al., 2013).
As these are complex constructs, and ASD is characterized by such heterogeneity, links between social and emotional processes in ASD are likely influenced by additional forms of individual differences. Two dimensions of difference are of particular interest in the current study. First is the role of biological sex. ASD is much more prevalent in males versus females (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), and profiles of ASD symptoms may differ on the basis of sex (Knutsen, Crossman, Perrin, Shui, & Kuhlthau, 2018). In addition, sex may influence the forms in which emotion dysregulation appears, as girls with ASD are more likely than boys to experience anxiety and depression, whereas boys are more likely to exhibit aggressive and repetitive behaviors (Werling & Geschwind, 2013). Moreover, developmental trajectories of internalizing and externalizing disorders over the course of early childhood may also differ by sex among children with and without ASD (Vaillancourt et al., 2017).
A second dimension of individual difference that warrants attention is cognitive and intellectual functioning. Early research suggested that nearly 70% of individuals with ASD experienced significant intellectual disability (Fombonne, 2003), but more recent research reveals a wide spectrum of cognitive levels and profiles that spans the full range of ability (Ankenman, Elgin, Sullivan, Vincent, & Bernier, 2014; Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 2010). As with biological sex, individuals’ intellectual ability may influence display of emotion dysregulation, both within diagnostic domains (e.g., differential distributions of anxiety disorders by IQ; White et al., 2009) and across domains (e.g., differential relations to internalizing vs. externalizing symptoms; Estes, Dawson, Sterling, & Munson, 2007; Neuhaus, Bernier, Tham, & Webb, 2018). In particular, increased cognitive ability may be a particular risk factor for internalizing disorders, as it may enhance one’s awareness of social difficulties (Sterling, Dawson, Estes, & Greenson, 2008).
Current Objectives
Our overarching objective in this paper was to characterize social and emotional functioning among children and adolescents with ASD, with particular attention to the links between these two domains. Within this objective, we aim to explore the contributions of social motivation and emotion dysregulation to children and adolescents’ social skill. Moreover, by capitalizing on a large cohort of well-characterized children with ASD, we aim to investigate the potential moderating role of emotion dysregulation in the association between social motivation and adaptive social skill, as emotion-related processes may blunt or facilitate social success. Furthermore, in keeping with the literature reviewed thus far, we sought to consider the roles of (a) child’s biological sex, given sex differences in prevalence and trajectory of emotion dysregulation within the general population, as well as (b) cognitive ability, as risk for mental health concerns among individuals with ASD appears to vary, in part, with cognitive skill. To represent the breadth of forms in which dysregulation frequently appears among children with ASD, we considered five different measures of emotion dysregulation: internalizing symptoms, aggression, attention difficulties, irritability, and self-injurious behavior.
Method
Participants
These goals were investigated using data from the Simons Simplex Collection (SSC), a compilation of comprehensive behavioral, medical, and genetic data gathered across 12 sites in the United States (Fischbach & Lord, 2010). Participating families had exactly one child with ASD between the ages of 4 and 18 years, with no additional family history of ASD in the immediate or extended family. Families underwent extensive screening and assessment procedures, and were excluded if they reported a diagnosis of any known genetic conditions (e.g., Down Syndrome or Fragile X), history of head injury or neurological disease, gestational age less than 36 weeks at birth, birth weight below 2000 g, extensive pregnancy or birth complications, sensory/motor impairments that could interfere with study measures, or a primary language other than English. All participating children and adolescents met Collaborative Program for Excellence in Autism criteria for ASD (Lainhart et al., 2006) as assessed by research reliable clinicians, based on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 2003) with revised algorithm scoring (Gotham, Risi, Pickles, & Lord, 2007), Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994), and expert clinical judgment.
From within the full SSC sample, we selected participants between the ages of 6 and 18 years for the current analyses. This age range was chosen to correspond with the school-age version of the Child Behavior Checklist/6–18 (Achenbach, 1991) to ensure a consistent assessment battery across our sample. This resulted in a sample of 2,079 individuals (276 female, 1,803 male) with an average age of 123.7 months (SD = 37.45, range 72.0–216.0). Self-reported racial and ethnic backgrounds for participating families were as follows: African American (3.8%), Asian (3.9 %), Native American or Hawaiian (0.3%), White (79.0%), more than one race (7.6%), and other/not reported (5.2%). The sample ranged in severity of ASD symptoms, with a mean calibrated severity score of 7.46 (SD = 1.70, range 4–10; Gotham, Pickles, & Lord, 2009) on the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule. Cognitive skills varied markedly as well, with a mean full scale IQ score of 81.75 (SD = 28.35, range 7–167).
Measures
Emotion dysregulation
In order to capture multiple aspects of dysregulation, five indices were extracted from the larger SSC data set. From the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach, 1991), we extracted T scores from the anxious/depressed subscale to index internalizing symptoms of anxiety and depressed mood. Although the CBCL yields a broadband internalizing score, several of its items overlap closely with symptoms of ASD itself (e.g., preference to be alone or social withdrawal) and so the anxious/depressed subscale score was selected as a purer measure of internalizing emotional processes. In addition, we extracted the aggressive behavior subscale from the CBCL in order to index disruptive, violent, or defiant behavior; and the attention problems subscale to index impulsive behavior. Although this third subscale is not strictly within the domain of “emotion dysregulation,” it captures dysregulation common to individuals with ASD. In addition to these subscales, we extracted the irritability subscale score from the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (Aman, Singh, Stewart, & Field, 1985), as well as the self-injurious behavior subscale from the Repetitive Behavior Scale—Revised (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000).
Social functioning
Two types of social behavior were extracted from the SSC data set. Social motivation was reflected through the Social Motivation raw score from the Social Responsiveness Scale (Constantino & Gruber, 2005). Widely used in research and clinic settings, this subscale includes items assessing social avoidance or disinterest, and higher scores indicate greater impairment in social motivation. Social skill/success was assessed through the socialization standard score from the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales—2nd edition (Vineland-2; Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005), a structured parent interview that contains items assessing social engagement and participation, peer play and interactions, and social understanding. Vineland-2 standard scores have a mean of 100 (SD = 15).
Cognitive skills
Participants’ cognitive skills were assessed with the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children—4th edition (Wechsler, 2003), the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Wechsler, 1999), the Differential Abilities Scale—2nd edition (Elliott, 2007), or the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1984), and yielded full-scale IQ scores.
Analytic approach
We first sought to better understand the nature and degree of dysregulation difficulties in our sample, with particular attention to differential symptom/skill levels by biological sex. These relations were assessed via group comparisons of symptom levels and percentage of male and female participants exceeding clinical thresholds (when specified by measures). Correlations were then used to investigate relations between social and emotional functioning.
Next, in order to test the relations between social motivation, emotion dysregulation, and social skill, we created a series of general linear models. For each measure of dysregulation, the corresponding model contained the child’s age, child’s biological sex, parent-reported social motivation, the respective dysregulation measure, and the Motivation × Dysregulation interaction term. In addition, because the potential moderating effect of sex was of interest, a Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation interaction term was included as well. In all models, participants’ socialization standard score on the Vineland-2 was entered as the dependent variable reflecting social success. Figure 1 depicts conceptual and statistical models for this approach.
Finally, given our goal of understanding the role of cognitive skill in social–emotional well-being, models were replicated in the subgroup of individuals with ASD with full-scale IQ scores assessed to be 85 or higher, corresponding to cognitive skills in the average range or above (N = 1,073; 115 female, 958 male).
Results
Emotion dysregulation
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for measures of emotion dysregulation in the sample by participant sex. As shown, male and female participants did not differ in mean symptom level for measures of internalizing symptoms or aggressive behavior. In contrast, female participants displayed significantly higher levels of attention problems, self-injurious behavior, and irritability than males, consistent with previous findings among a subset of other research samples (e.g., Antezana et al., 2018; Holtmann, Bolte, & Poustka, 2007).
Table 1.
Male (n = 1,803) | Female (n = 276) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Range | Mean (SD) | Range | t | Cohen’s d | |
CBCL anxious/depressed T score | 59.55 (9.2) | 50–98 | 59.17 (9.1) | 50–92 | t (2076) = 0.65 | 0.04 |
CBCL aggressive behavior T score | 59.47 (9.2) | 50–97 | 59.94 (8.7) | 50–91 | t (2077) = −0.79 | 0.05 |
CBCL attention problems T score | 66.29 (10.0) | 50–100 | 69.69 (10.5) | 50–100 | t (2077) = −5.23*** | 0.33 |
ABC irritability | 11.11 (8.7) | 0–45 | 12.78 (9.1) | 0–41 | t (2075) = −2.94** | 0.19 |
RBS-R self-injurious behavior | 2.08 (2.8) | 0–21 | 2.51 (3.1) | 0–18 | t (2075) = −2.33* | 0.15 |
Note: CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991). ABC, Aberrant Behavior Checklist (Aman, Singh, Stewart, & Field, 1985). RBS-R, Repetitive Behavior Scale—Revised (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000).
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Measures of dysregulation extracted from the CBCL were then considered with regard to clinical significance. For the three CBCL subscales included here (anxious/depressed, aggressive behavior, and attention problems), T scores of 65 or higher correspond to “borderline clinical” or “clinical” levels of concern. For the entire sample, 64.9% of participants had a T score of 65 or higher on at least one of these CBCL subscales, indicating at least one area of significant concern. In addition, 9.6% of participants had T scores of 65 or higher on all three CBCL subscales, indicating significant dysregulation across multiple domains. Table 2 presents percentages of participants by sex whose CBCL T scores were 65 or higher. As shown, female participants were more likely than males to exceed this threshold on at least one subscale. However, there was not a significant sex difference in percentage of participants exceeding borderline clinical thresholds on all three subscales. Table 2 also presents percentages exceeding thresholds on individual CBCL subscales. Approximately one-quarter of participants of both sexes fell above the threshold for anxious/depressed and for aggressive behaviors. However, female participants were significantly more likely than males to fall above the borderline clinical threshold on attention problems, with nearly two-thirds of T scores exceeding 65.
Table 2.
Male | Female | χ2 | Φ coefficient | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Exceeding on any of three CBCL subscales | 64.0% | 71.4% | χ2 (1) = 5.75* | .05 |
Exceeding on all three CBCL subscales | 9.5% | 9.8% | χ2 (1) = 0.02 | .00 |
Individual CBCL subscales | ||||
CBCL anxious/depressed | 26.9% | 24.3% | χ2 (1) = 0.86 | .02 |
CBCL aggressive behavior | 24.8% | 26.1% | χ2 (1) = 0.21 | .01 |
CBCL attention problems | 50.6% | 63.8% | χ2 (1) = 16.54** | .09 |
Next, we examined preliminary associations between social and emotional well-being among our participants. As displayed in Table 3, social motivation impairment was significantly positively correlated with all five measures of dysregulation. Greater impairment in social motivation was associated with greater dysregulation across all domains assessed. Social skill was negatively correlated with four of the five domains of dysregulation, such that poorer skill was associated with more aggressive behavior, attention problems, self-injurious behavior, and irritability.
Table 3.
SRS social motivation | Vineland-2 socialization | |
---|---|---|
CBCL anxious/depressed | .35* | .04 |
CBCL aggressive behavior | .23* | −.14* |
CBCL attention problems | .34* | −.17* |
ABC irritability | .31* | −.24* |
RBS-R self-injurious behavior | .24* | −−.23* |
Note: CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991). ABC, Aberrant Behavior Checklist (Aman, Singh, Stewart, & Field, 1985). RBS-R, Repetitive Behavior Scale—Revised (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000). SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale (Constantino & Gruber, 2005). Vineland-2, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales—2nd edition (Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005).
p < .001.
Dysregulation, social motivation, and social skill
In order to more fully understand links between dysregulation, social motivation, and social skills, we next created a series of general linear models as described earlier. Models are presented by domain of emotion dysregulation, first for the full sample and then for the subgroup with cognitive skills in the average range or higher. In keeping with recommendations (Pedhazur, 1997), we prioritize interpretation of interactive effects over concurrent main effects in these models. Table 4 displays full model results.
Table 4.
F | df | p | Overall model Adj. R2 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
CBCL anxious/depressed | |||||
Full sample | 114.24 | 6, 2071 | <.001 | .249 | 24.6% |
Age | 233.99 | 1, 2071 | <.001 | .102 | |
Sex | 1.77 | 1, 2071 | .184 | .001 | |
Social motivation | 38.39 | 1, 2071 | <.001 | .018 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 0.83 | 1, 2071 | .361 | .000 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 9.59 | 1, 2071 | .002 | .005 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 1.08 | 1, 2071 | .299 | .001 | |
Participants with IQ ≥ 85 | 50.69 | 6, 1066 | <.001 | .222 | 21.8% |
Age | 96.73 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .083 | |
Sex | 1.79 | 1, 1066 | .181 | .002 | |
Social motivation | 19.23 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .018 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 9.03 | 1, 1066 | .003 | .008 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 8.35 | 1, 1066 | .004 | .008 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 4.47 | 1, 1066 | .035 | .004 | |
CBCL aggressive behavior | |||||
Full sample | 95.61 | 6, 2072 | <.001 | .217 | 21.5% |
Age | 214.03 | 1, 2072 | <.001 | .094 | |
Sex | 0.97 | 1, 2072 | .326 | .000 | |
Social motivation | 28.86 | 1, 2072 | <.001 | .014 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 20.62 | 1, 2072 | <.001 | .01 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 9.89 | 1, 2072 | .002 | .005 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 0.45 | 1, 2072 | .502 | .000 | |
Participants with IQ ≥ 85 | 61.27 | 6, 1066 | <.001 | .256 | 25.2% |
Age | 122.09 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .103 | |
Sex | 2.28 | 1, 1066 | .132 | .002 | |
Social motivation | 15.13 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .014 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 22.82 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .021 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 7.06 | 1, 1066 | .008 | .007 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 5.69 | 1, 1066 | .017 | .005 | |
CBCL attention problems | |||||
Full sample | 95.07 | 6, 2072 | <.001 | .216 | 21.4% |
Age | 218.60 | 1, 2072 | <.001 | .095 | |
Sex | 0.17 | 1, 2072 | .68 | .000 | |
Social motivation | 16.70 | 1, 2072 | <.001 | .008 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 12.86 | 1, 2072 | <.001 | .006 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 3.22 | 1, 2072 | .073 | .002 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 0.073 | 1, 2072 | .787 | .000 | |
Participants with IQ ≥ 85 | 52.71 | 6, 1066 | <.001 | .229 | 22.4% |
Age | 109.49 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .093 | |
Sex | 1.39 | 1, 1066 | .238 | .001 | |
Social motivation | 7.10 | 1, 1066 | .008 | .007 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 6.70 | 1, 1066 | .01 | .006 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 1.68 | 1, 1066 | .196 | .002 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 4.65 | 1, 1066 | .031 | .004 | |
ABC irritability | |||||
Full sample | 110.35 | 6, 2070 | <.001 | .242 | 24.0% |
Age | 258.08 | 1, 2070 | <.001 | .111 | |
Sex | 1.20 | 1, 2070 | .274 | .001 | |
Social motivation | 86.39 | 1, 2070 | <.001 | .04 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 27.15 | 1, 2070 | <.001 | .013 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 3.88 | 1, 2070 | .049 | .002 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 1.26 | 1, 2070 | .263 | .001 | |
Participants with IQ ≥ 85 | 61.03 | 6, 1065 | <.001 | <.001 | 25.2% |
Age | 134.11 | 1, 1065 | <.001 | .112 | |
Sex | 0.02 | 1, 1065 | .899 | .000 | |
Social motivation | 50.65 | 1, 1065 | <.001 | .045 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 19.34 | 1, 1065 | <.001 | .018 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 5.30 | 1, 1065 | .022 | .005 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 4.20 | 1, 1065 | .041 | .004 | |
RBS-R self-injurious behavior | |||||
Full sample | 103.56 | 6, 2070 | <.001 | .231 | 22.9% |
Age | 206.62 | 1, 2070 | <.001 | .091 | |
Sex | 0.36 | 1, 2070 | .547 | .000 | |
Social motivation | 157.80 | 1, 2070 | <.001 | .071 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 15.05 | 1, 2070 | <.001 | .007 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 1.81 | 1, 2070 | .179 | .001 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 0.003 | 1, 2070 | .953 | .000 | |
Participants with IQ ≥ 85 | 50.69 | 6, 1066 | <.001 | .222 | 21.8% |
Age | 98.28 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .084 | |
Sex | 0.272 | 1, 1066 | .602 | .000 | |
Social motivation | 91.31 | 1, 1066 | <.001 | .079 | |
Emotion dysregulation | 4.21 | 1, 1066 | .04 | .004 | |
Motivation × Dysregulation | 1.79 | 1, 1066 | .181 | .002 | |
Sex × Motivation × Dysregulation | 1.33 | 1, 1066 | .249 | .001 |
Note: CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991). ABC, Aberrant Behavior Checklist (Aman, Singh, Stewart, & Field, 1985). RBS-R, Repetitive Behavior Scale—Revised (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000).
p < .05.
p < .01.
p < .001.
Internalizing symptoms
For the full sample (N = 2,079), the model investigating the role of internalizing symptoms as contributors to social skill accounted for 24.6% of the variance in social skill, F (6, 2071) = 114.24, p < .001, . Within the model, there was a significant interaction between social motivation and internalizing symptoms, F (1, 2071) = 9.59, p = .002, . This interaction was such that the association between social motivation and social skill was stronger for children with fewer internalizing difficulties. In addition to this interaction, main effects were significant for child age, F (1, 2071) = 233.99, p < .001, , and child social motivation, F (1, 2071) = 38.39, p < .001, . Social skills were relatively stronger for children who were younger and who experienced less impairment in social motivation.
Among participants with average or higher cognitive skills (N = 1,073), the model containing internalizing symptoms accounted for 21.8% of the variance in social skill, F (6, 1066) = 50.69, p < .001, . Unique to participants with higher cognitive skills, a significant Sex × Motivation × Internalizing interaction effect, F (1, 1066) = 4.47, p = .035, , indicated that internalizing symptoms may moderate the relation between social motivation and social skill differently according to participant sex. As shown in Figure 2a, the moderating effect of internalizing symptoms may be limited to male participants, and internalizing symptoms may not carry a similar effect for female participants among this subgroup with average or higher cognitive skills. Within this model, we also observed an effect of the Motivation × Internalizing interaction, F (1, 1066) = 8.35, p = .004, . As with the full sample, a number of main effects were observed as well, including those for child age, F (1, 1066) = 96.73, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 1066) = 19.23, p < .001, , and internalizing symptoms, F (1, 1066) = 9.03, p = .003, .
Aggressive behavior
Within the broader sample spanning all levels of cognitive skill, the model assessing aggressive behavior accounted for 21.5% of the variance in social skill, F (6, 2072) = 95.61, p < .001, . A significant interaction between social motivation and aggressive behavior, F (1, 2072) = 9.89, p = .002, , indicated a stronger relationship between social motivation and social skill when participants had fewer aggressive behaviors. We also observed significant main effects for child age, F (1, 2072) = 214.03, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 2072) = 28.86, p < .001, , and aggressive behavior, F (1, 2072) = 20.62, p < .001, . Social skills were stronger among children who were younger, had stronger social motivation, and had lower levels of aggressive behavior.
Consistent with this, the model for the subgroup with average or higher cognitive skills accounted for 25.2% of variance in social skills, F (6, 1066) = 61.27, p < .001, . As we found with internalizing symptoms above, there was again a significant interaction with sex. The Sex × Motivation × Aggressive Behavior interaction was significant, F (1, 1066) = 5.69, p = .017, , and was such that social motivation and social skill were more closely related at lower levels of aggressive behavior for female participants only. See Figure 2b. The model also indicated a Motivation × Aggressive Behavior interaction, F (1, 1066) = 7.06, p = .008, , as well as significant main effects for age, F (1, 1066) = 122.09, p < .001, , motivation, F (1, 1066) = 15.13, p < .001, , and aggressive behavior, F (1, 1066) = 22.82, p < .001, .
Attention problems
The full sample model for attention problems accounted for 21.4% of variance in social skills, F (6, 2072) = 95.07, p < .001, . Neither interaction term reached significance in this model. Main effects were significant for child’s age, F (1, 2072) = 218.60, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 2072) = 16.70, p < .001, , and attention problems, F (1, 2072) = 12.86, p < .001, . Stronger social skills were associated with younger age, more social motivation, and fewer attention problems.
Results from the model including only participants with average or higher cognitive skills were similar, with 22.4% of the variance in social skill accounted for, F (1, 1066) = 52.71, p < .001, . As with the other forms of dysregulation discussed thus far, this model revealed a significant interaction effect for Sex × Motivation × Attention Problems, F (1, 1066) = 4.65, p < .031, , such that the moderating effect of attention problems on the association between social motivation and social skill was stronger for male participants. See Figure 2c. Main effects of age, F (1, 1066) = 109.49, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 1066) = 7.10, p = .008, , and attention problems, F (1, 1066) = 6.70, p < .01, , remained significant.
Irritability
The model investigating irritability accounted for 24.0% of the variance in social skills, F (6, 2070) = 110.35, p < .001, . The interaction between social motivation and irritability was significant in this model, F (1, 2070) = 3.88, p = .049, , with a stronger association between social motivation and social skill when irritability was lower. We also observed main effects of age, F (1, 2070) = 258.08, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 2070) = 86.39, p < .001, , and irritability, F (1, 2070) = 27.15, p < .001, . Social skills were stronger for participants who were younger, had stronger social motivation, and had lower irritability ratings.
Among participants with cognitive skills in the average range or higher, we again observed significant variance in social skill accounted for by the model, 25.2%, F (6, 1065) = 61.03, p < .001, . The interaction between Sex × Motivation × Irritability was significant, F (1, 1065) = 4.20, p = .041, , such that a moderating effect of irritability was stronger for female participants than for male. See Figure 2d. Along with this, we observed a Motivation × Irritability interaction, F (1, 1065) = 5.30, p = .022, . Finally, consistent with the full sample, effects were significant for age, F (1, 1065) = 134.11, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 1065) = 50.65, p < .001, , and irritability, F (1, 1065) = 19.34, p < .001, .
Self-injurious behavior
For the full sample, the model investigating the role of self-injurious behavior accounted for 22.9% of variance in social skill, F (6, 2070) = 103.56, p < .001, , with main effects of child’s age, F (1, 2070) = 206.62, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 2070) = 157.80, p < .001, , and self-injurious behavior, F (1, 2070) = 15.05, p < .001, . In keeping with the other measures of dysregulation discussed, stronger social skills were associated with younger age, more social motivation, and less self-injurious behaviors. There were no significant interaction effects in this model, indicating that self-injurious behaviors did not moderate the links between social motivation and social skill.
Finally, the comparable model analyzed only with participants of average or higher cognitive skill was quite consistent with the full sample, accounting for 21.8% of variance, F (6, 1066) = 50.69, p < .001, . Main effects of age, F (1, 1066) = 98.28, p < .001, , social motivation, F (1, 1066) = 91.31, p < .001, , and self-injurious behavior, F (1, 1066) = 4.21, p = .04, , remained significant, with no evidence of interaction effects.
Summary
Taken together, analyses indicate complex relations between social and emotional constructs in this sample. For domains of dysregulation including internalizing, aggression, and irritability, we observed interactive effects such that greater dysregulation corresponded to weaker associations between a child’s social motivation and his or her social skills, suggesting that marked dysregulation blunts the facilitative effects of social motivation on social skill. When looking specifically at the subgroup of participants with average or higher cognitive skills, we observed additional moderating effects of participant sex, indicating differential effects of emotion dysregulation on social processes for males and females.
Discussion
From these results, it is clear that social and emotional functioning are tightly intertwined among children and adolescents with ASD, and that both biological sex and cognitive skill are important to understanding how these processes interact. As a whole, the participants in this sample experienced significant symptoms across the internalizing and externalizing spectra, with approximately two-thirds meeting or exceeding levels of borderline clinical concern on at least one measure (e.g., CBCL subscale), and approximately 10% demonstrating significant dysregulation across multiple measures by parent report. Symptom levels varied by participant sex in some domains of dysregulation, as girls displayed higher levels of attention problems, self-injurious behavior, and irritability than boys. Particularly striking, nearly two-thirds of girls in this sample demonstrated clinically elevated levels of attention problems. Moreover, the interactions between dysregulation in these domains and participants’ social functioning suggest that these symptoms are meaningful with respect to social engagement and skill.
Consistent across the domains of dysregulation assessed here were robust direct effects of dysregulation on social skills, such that more dysregulation (of any form assessed here) was directly associated with poorer social skills. This is consistent with findings of social difficulties among children with internalizing and externalizing difficulties but without ASD (Garaigordobil et al., 2017; Motoca et al., 2012; Scharfstein et al., 2011; Scharfstein & Beidel, 2015). More novel, however, are our findings that dysregulation interacts with social motivation, effectively blunting or masking the facilitative effect of social motivation on social skill. This moderating effect was evident for a number of forms of dysregulation (i.e., internalizing, aggressive behavior, and irritability) across the full sample. Our analyses did not address possible mechanisms for these effects, but numerous possibilities exist. In one scenario, a child with relatively strong interest in others (high social motivation) might approach peers for interactions, but struggle with outbursts or aggression (high emotion dysregulation) when normative disagreements arise, resulting in negative peer interactions and decreasing receptiveness from peers (poor social success). Similarly, a child high in social motivation but also high in anxiety might avoid peer interactions, resulting in fewer opportunities to practice social skills and form relationships, resulting in relatively poor social skills despite high social motivation.
Adding another layer of nuance, within the subgroup of participants whose cognitive skills fell within the average range or higher, the models indicated that the moderating effect of emotion dysregulation on social processes might operate differently for male versus female participants. Across four of the five domains assessed—internalizing, aggression, attention problems, and irritability—we observed an interaction with participant sex. For the domains of aggressive behavior and irritability, moderating effects of dysregulation were stronger in females; aggression and irritability blunted the facilitative effect of social motivation on social skills for female more so than for male participants. In contrast, moderating effects of internalizing and attention problems were stronger for males. To some extent, these moderating effects occur in domains that are counter to typical sex-based behavioral norms. For example, among the general population, girls tend to have higher levels of internalizing symptoms than boys (Altemus, Sarvalya, & Epperson, 2014; Breslau et al., 2017; Cohen, 1989). It might be that displaying increased symptoms in the internalizing domain is more detrimental to social success for boys, precisely because those behaviors run counter to normative sex-based patterns. Likewise, we observed a stronger moderating effect of aggression for girls; perhaps heightened symptoms of aggression more strongly interfere with social processes in girls than in boys, again because they contrast with typical sex-based patterns of behavior. Our results suggest that having more symptoms in sex-congruent domains (aggression for male, internalizing for female) might be less detrimental to social success, at least with respect to a child’s ability to act on their social motivation.
That sex-related effects emerged only for participants with relatively stronger cognitive skills is intriguing, and could suggest that dysregulation interferes more strongly with the ability to act on social motivation in a successful way in the context of average or higher cognitive skills. Previous research indicates that higher cognitive skills among individuals with ASD confers increased vulnerability to internalizing disorders, with positive correlations between children’s IQ and anxiety (e.g., Sukhodolsky et al., 2008) and meta-analytic findings that elevations in anxiety among children with ASD compared to peers without ASD become more marked as IQ increases (van Steensel & Heeman, 2017). Even among young children with ASD, those with higher IQs exhibit higher levels of anxiety, which appear to be mediated in part by social understanding (Niditch, Varela, Kamps, & Hill, 2012). Our findings that social–emotional mechanisms may function differently according to sex among individuals with higher IQs adds to the literature suggesting a unique set of vulnerabilities among this “higher functioning” population.
Findings of heightened vulnerability to detrimental social effects of emotion dysregulation among children with stronger cognitive skills may also reflect the behavioral expectations placed on this group relative to those with lower cognitive skills, and/or the moderating role of social supports provided to them by adults when dysregulation does arise. One might anticipate that children with average or stronger cognitive skills are more likely to participate in general education or integrated classroom settings in which behavioral expectations are less tolerant of emotional difficulties and social interactions receive less direct oversight than self-contained classroom settings. As part of this, teachers and caregivers may take a less forward role in scaffolding social interactions, resolving conflictual interactions (e.g., for children with impulsive or aggressive behaviors), motivating social approach (e.g., for children with anxiety), and tempering negative social consequences following episodes of dysregulation. In contrast, children with IQs below the average range may spend more time in self-contained or highly structured settings, in which adults may more readily supervise social interactions and provide emotional support, and in which episodes of significant dysregulation may be less unusual and therefore carry fewer social consequences.
Implications
To our knowledge, this study represents a first demonstrated interaction between social motivation and emotion dysregulation, particularly in such a large and diverse sample of individuals with ASD. Previous literature investigating social motivation and dysregulation in tandem have thus far approached these constructs in a number of ways, but none have previously shed light on their interaction in the prediction of social skill. For example, among adults with ASD, social motivation and emotion dysregulation appear to make independent but not interactive contributions to social anxiety (Swain, Scarpa, White, & Laugeson, 2015). Among children, social motivation appears to partially mediate the relationship between anxiety and children’s insistence on sameness, a core feature of ASD (Factor, Condy, Farley, & Scarpa, 2016), and difficulties with social motivation are associated with the presence of symptoms of anxiety and ADHD (Factor, Ryan, Farley, Ollendick, & Scarpa, 2017). Clearly, relationships between social motivation and emotion dysregulation across many forms are multifaceted, with much yet to learn. Consistent across findings, however, are suggestions that difficulties with social motivation, social skills, and emotion dysregulation cluster together for individuals with ASD, such that difficulties in any portend risk for poorer outcomes in the others.
Findings discussed here not only delineate vulnerabilities for poorer social and emotional outcomes among children and adolescents with ASD but also highlight points of intervention. Whereas the main effects of dysregulation on social skill suggest a clear need to intervene in emotion-related processes in order to facilitate social functioning, the moderating role of emotion dysregulation on social functioning suggests that such support may be beneficial even when difficulties with dysregulation are not readily apparent. Although a substantial proportion of individuals in this sample displayed clinically elevated symptoms of dysregulation, many did not; yet, the moderating role of dysregulation was robust and consistent across domains. As such, emotion dysregulation even within the normative or subclinical ranges still merits consideration as a target of intervention as a vehicle to increasing social success by allowing the facilitative effects of social motivation on social skill. This may be particularly relevant for intervention approaches that aim to capitalize on or promote social motivation (e.g., pivotal response training), as they may benefit from adjunctive intervention to promote emotion regulation.
Despite the strengths of the sample investigated here, findings must be considered in the context of the sample’s limitations. A primary limitation of the SSC data set is its cross-sectional nature, in which all data were gathered within a tight timeframe rather than across a developmental span. As a result, data presented here are not longitudinal in nature and associations cannot determine the direction of effects between variables. In addition, by design, the SSC contains only families who met very strict inclusion/exclusion criteria in order to enrich the frequency of de novo genetic events as the etiology of ASD (Fischbach & Lord, 2010), yielding approximately 35% of the SSC sample with an identified genetic event (Sanders et al., 2015). Families in which ASD occurs in multiple individuals within the family or who may be less likely to meet the strict diagnostic and inclusion criteria for the SSC (e.g., Neuhaus, Beauchaine, Bernier, & Webb, 2018) may be less well represented in such data sets and resulting analyses.
Finally, the measures included in this study were all parent report in format. Although well established and used frequently in both research and clinical realms, such measures would be enriched by incorporation of complementary observational, self-report, and psychophysiological measures of social and emotional functioning. This may be particularly relevant for the construct of social motivation, which is almost certainly a multifaceted rather than a unitary construct, and might be best represented as a multidimensional construct in future research. In the same vein, the construct of emotion dysregulation itself is complex (as evidenced in literatures across genetic, psychophysiological, and behavioral approaches; Beauchaine, 2015) and thus is likely best assessed and modeled using multiple concurrent methods that span levels of analysis (Cole, Hall, & Hajal, 2017). Given the varied neurobiological systems that interact over time to result in significant dys-regulation, increased methodological sophistication in the assessment of dysregulation, particularly approaches that allow integration of simultaneous sources of information (e.g., self-report with concurrent psychophysiological monitoring), would allow for greater richness and ecological validity in modeling of associated processes and risk factors (Adrian, Zeman, & Veits, 2011).
Future directions
On a larger scale, the approach presented here represents an effort toward the field’s larger goal of identifying factors and individual differences that influence qualitative variability in the phenotype of ASD, rather than looking solely at factors that contribute to the presence versus absence of the disorder. As we note earlier, one of the most distinctive characteristics of ASD is its heterogeneity—the variability in skills, subjective experiences, and developmental trajectories among individuals all receiving the same diagnosis. Further exploration of individual differences that shape trajectories related to social and emotional outcomes remains essential, particularly over the course of development (see Finucane, Challman, Martin, & Ledbetter, 2016). Our findings present evidence that sex and cognitive skill are part of this variety of factors, but countless others remain to be explored.
Decades of research with families and youth without ASD have identified many factors that interact with individuals’ temperament and personality traits to determine social–emotional outcomes, from peer interactions (Snyder et al., 2005), to family relationship patterns (e.g., coercive escalation; Patterson, 1982), to adverse life experiences (Mead, Beauchaine, & Shannon, 2010). Over time, these factors interact in transactional and sometimes subtle ways to buffer against or exacerbate difficulties in social–emotional functioning, such as more extreme externalizing and internalizing symptoms (e.g., nonsuicidal self-injury; Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge, & Gatzke-Kopp, 2009). Extrapolation from research with these non-ASD populations provides intriguing hypotheses for how similar mechanisms might operate among populations with ASD. To this end, empirical understanding of (a) how these factors operate in the context of ASD, and (b) how the unique neurobiological and psychosocial aspects of ASD alter their influence will be essential to elucidating the nature, developmental processes, and intervention points for social and emotional difficulties.
Acknowledgments.
We thank all of the families at the participating Simons Simplex Collection (SSC) sites, as well as the principal investigators (A. Beaudet, J. Constantino, E. Cook, E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, E. Hanson, D. Grice, A. Klin, R. Kochel, D. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. Martin, D. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, K. Pelphrey, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. State, W. Stone, J. Sutcliffe, C. Walsh, Z. Warren, and E. Wijsman). We appreciate obtaining access to phenotypic data on SFARI Base.
Footnotes
Data. Approved researchers can obtain the SSC population data set described in this study (SSC distribution 15, obtained from SFARI Base) by applying at https://base.sfari.org.
References
- Achenbach TM (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry. [Google Scholar]
- Adrian M, Zeman J, & Veits G (2011). Methodological implications of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 171–197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Altemus M, Sarvalya N, & Epperson CN (2014). Sex differences in anxiety and depression clinical perspectives. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 35, 320–330. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aman MG, Singh NN, Stewart AW, & Field CJ (1985). The Aberrant Behavior Checklist: A behavior rating scale for assessment of treatment effects. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89, 485–491. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Ankenman K, Elgin J, Sullivan K, Vincent L, & Bernier R (2014). Nonverbal and verbal cognitive discrepancy profiles in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of age and gender. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 119, 84–99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antezana L, Factor RS, Condy EE, Strege MV, Scarpa A, & Richey JA (2018). Gender differences in restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests in youth with autism. Autism Research. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnett AB, Trinh S, & Bernier RA (2018). The state of research on the genetics of autism spectrum disorder: Methodological, clinical and conceptual progress. Current Opinion in Psychology, 27, 1–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arnold LE, Vitiello B, McDougle C, Scahill L, Shah B, Gonzalez NM, … Tierney E (2003). Parent-defined target symptoms respond to risperidone in RUPP autism study: Customer approach to clinical trials. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1443–1450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 63, 1–21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ballaban-Gil K, Rapin I, Tuchman R, & Shinnar S (1996). Longitudinal examination of the behavioral, language, and social changes in a population of adolescents and young adults with autistic disorder. Pediatric Neurology, 15, 217–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, 875–896. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP, Klein DN, Crowell SE, Derbidge C, & Gatzke-Kopp L (2009). Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A Biology × Sex × Environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 735–770. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP, & Zisner A (2017). Motivation, emotion regulation, and the latent structure of psychopathology: An integrative and convergent historical perspective. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 119, 108–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Parker DE, & Lewis MH (2000). Varieties in repetitive behavior in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 237–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brereton AV, Tonge BJ, & Einfeld SL (2006). Psychopathology in children and adolescents with autism compared to young people with intellectual disability. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 863–870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslau J, Gilman SE, Stein BD, Ruder T, Gmelin T, & Miller E (2017). Sex differences in recent first-onset depression in an epidemiological sample of adolescents. Translational Psychiatry, 7, e1139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cassidy S, Bradley P, Robinson J, Allison C, McHugh M, & Baron-Cohen S (2014). Suicidal ideation and suicide plans or attempts in adults with Asperger’s syndrome attending a specialist diagnostic clinic: A clinical cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry, 1, 142–147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, & Schultz RT (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 231–239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen NJ (1989). Sex differences in child psychiatric outpatients: Cognitive, personality, and behavioral characteristics. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 20, 113–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cole PM, Hall SE, & Hajal NJ (2017). Emotion dysregulation as a vulnerability to psychopathology. In Beauchaine TP & Hinshaw SP (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathology (3rd ed., pp. 346–386). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Constantino JN, & Gruber CP (2005). Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Dawson G, & Bernier R (2007). Social brain circuitry in autism. In Coch D, Dawson G, & Fischer K (Eds.), Human behavior and the developing brain: Atypical development (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dawson G, Webb SJ, & McPartland J (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 403–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott CD (2007). Differential Ability Scales: Introductory and technical handbook (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Estes AM, Dawson G, Sterling L, & Munson J (2007). Level of intellectual functioning predicts patterns of associated symptoms in school-age children with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 439–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estes A, Munson J, Dawson G, Koehler E, Zhou XH, & Abbott R (2009). Parenting stress and psychological functioning among mothers of preschool children with autism and developmental delay. Autism, 13, 375–387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Factor RS, Condy EE, Farley JP, & Scarpa A (2016). Brief report: Insistence on sameness, anxiety, and social motivation in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2548–2554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Factor RS, Ryan SM, Farley JP, Ollendick TH, & Scarpa A (2017). Does the presence of anxiety and ADHD symptoms add to social impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 47, 1122–1134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Filliter JH, Longard J, Lawrence MA, Zwaigenbaum L, Brian J, Garon N, … Bryson SE (2015). Positive affect in infant siblings of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 567–575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finucane B, Challman TD, Martin CL, & Ledbetter DH (2016). Shift happens: Family background influences clinical variability in genetic neurodevelopmental disorders. Genetics in Medicine, 18, 302–304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fischbach GD, & Lord C (2010). The Simons Simplex Collection: A resource for identification of autism genetic risk factors. Neuron, 68, 192–195. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fombonne E (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 365–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garaigordobil M, Bernaras E, Jaureguizar J, & Machimbarrena JM (2017). Childhood depression: Relation to adaptive, clinical and predictor variables. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00821 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garon N, Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, Smith IM, Brian J, Roberts W, & Szatmari P (2009). Temperament and its relationship to autistic symptoms in a high-risk infant sib cohort. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 59–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotham K, Pickles A, & Lord C (2009). Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders, 39, 693–705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, & Lord C (2007). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: Revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 613–627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holtmann M, Bolte S, & Poustka F (2007). Autism spectrum disorders: Sex differences in autistic behavior domains and coexisting psychopathology. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49, 361–366. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutsen J, Crossman M, Perrin J, Shui A, & Kuhlthau K (2018). Sex differences in restricted repetitive behaviors and interests in children with autism spectrum disorder: An Autism Treatment Network study. Autism. Advance online publication. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konstantareas MM, & Stewart K (2006). Affect regulation and temperament in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 143–154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lainhart JE, Bigler ED, Bocian M, Coon H, Dinh E, Dawson G, … Volkmar F (2006). Head circumference and height in autism: A study by the collaborative program of excellence in autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part A, 140A, 2257–2274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, & Risi S (2003). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Rutter M, & Le Couteur A (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazefsky CA, Herrington J, Siegel M, Scarpa A, Maddox BB, Scahill L, & White SW (2013). The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 679–688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazefsky CA, & White SW (2014). Emotion regulation: Concepts and practice in autism spectrum disorder. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 23, 15–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mead HK, Beauchaine TP, & Shannon KE (2010). Neurobiological adaptations to violence across development. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 1–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Motoca LM, Williams S, & Silverman WK (2012). Social skills as a mediator between anxiety symptoms and peer interactions among children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 329–336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen EM (1997). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Munson J, Dawson G, Sterling L, Beauchaine T, Zhou A, Koehler E, … Abbott R (2008). Evidence for latent classes of IQ in young children with autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 113, 439–452. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muris P, Steerneman P, Merckelbach H, Holdrinet I, & Meesters C (1998). Comorbid anxiety symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12, 387–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuhaus E, Beauchaine TP, & Bernier R (2010). Neurobiological correlates of social functioning in autism. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 733–748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuhaus E, Beauchaine TP, Bernier RA, & Webb SJ (2018). Child and family characteristics moderate agreement between caregiver and clinician reports of autism symptoms. Autism Research, 11, 476–487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuhaus E, Bernier RA, & Beauchaine TP (2014). Social skills, internalizing and externalizing symptoms, and respiratory sinus arrhythmia in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 730–737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neuhaus E, Bernier RA, Tham SW, & Webb SJ (2018). Gastrointestinal and psychiatric symptoms among children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 9, 1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niditch LA, Varela RE, Kamps JL, & Hill T (2012). Exploring the association between cognitive functioning and anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorders: The role of social understanding and aggression. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 41, 127–137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patterson GR (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia. [Google Scholar]
- Pedhazur EJ (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research (3rd ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace. [Google Scholar]
- Rieffe C, De Bruine M, De Rooij M, & Stockmann L (2014). Approach and avoidant emotion regulation prevent depressive symptoms in children with an autism spectrum disorder. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 39, 37–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanders SJ, He X, Willsey AJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Samocha KE, Cicek AE, … State MW (2015). Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron, 87(6), 1215–1233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharfstein L, Alfano C, Beidel D, & Wong N (2011). Children with generalized anxiety disorder do not have peer problems, just fewer friends. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 42, 712–723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scharfstein LA, & Beidel DC (2015). Social skills and social acceptance in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44, 826–838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shen MD, & Piven J (2017). Brain and behavior development in autism from birth through infancy. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 19, 325–333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, & Baird G (2008). Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 921–929. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder J, Schrepferman L, Oeser J, Patterson G, Stoolmiller M, Johnson K, & Snyder A (2005). Deviancy training and association with deviant peers in young children: Occurrence and contribution to early-onset conduct problems. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 397–413. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow S, Cicchetti D, & Balla D (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (2nd ed.). Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. [Google Scholar]
- Sterling L, Dawson G, Estes A, & Greenson J (2008). Characteristics associated with presence of depressive symptoms in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1011–1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sukhodolsky DG, Scahill L, Gadow KD, Arnold LE, Aman MG, McDougle CJ, … Vitiello B (2008). Parent-rated anxiety symptoms in children with pervasive developmental disorders: Frequency and association with core autism symptoms and cognitive functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 117–128. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swain D, Scarpa A, White S, & Laugeson E (2015). Emotion dysregulation and anxiety in adults with ASD: Does social motivation play a role? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3971–3977. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaillancourt T, Haltigan JD, Smith I, Zwaigenbaum L, Szatmari P, Fombonne E, … Bryson S (2017). Joint trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 29, 203–214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Steensel FJA, & Heeman EJ (2017). Anxiety levels in children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26, 1753–1767. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th ed.). London: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Werling DM, & Geschwind DH (2013). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 26, 146–153. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White SW, Oswald D, Ollendick T, & Scahill L (2009). Anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 216–229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wing L, & Gould J (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 9, 11–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]